J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. Vol. 42, No. 5, pp. 623–636, 2001 Cambridge University Press ' 2001 Association for Child Psychology and Psychiatry Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved 0021–9630\01 $15n00j0n00 Effects of Maternal Depression on Cognitive Development of Children Over the First 7 Years of Life Sophie Kurstjens Institut fu$ r Pa$ dagogische Psychologie und Empirische Pa$ dagogik, LMU Mu$ nchen, Germany Dieter Wolke University of Hertfordshire, Hatfield, U.K. The effects of postnatal depression on cognitive test scores at 20 months and 4 ; 8 years of age as well as the timing (onset in the early postnatal period versus later), severity, number of episodes, duration of longest phase, recency, and chronicity of material depression on children’s cognitive scores at 6 ; 3 years was investigated. In South Bavaria, Germany, 1329 mothers of singletons were screened when the children were 6 ; 3 years of age for the presence of depressive symptoms since the birth of their infant. A standard interview (SADS-L) was used to ascertain DSM-IV diagnosis and details of depressive episodes. Ninety-two mothers were diagnosed as having suffered DSM-IV defined depression (7 %). Seven hundred and twenty-one mothers had no depressive episodes or symptoms from their children’s birth until 6 ; 3 years and were used as control group. The children had been assessed with the Griffiths Scales of Babies’ Abilities (20 months), the Columbia Mental Maturity Scales (CMM) at 4 ; 8 years, and the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) at 6 ; 3 years. No significant main effects of severity, timing of onset, duration, or chronicity of depression of the child’s cognitive development were found. Significant interactions of gender with chronicity of maternal depression (i.e. early-onset major and repeated episodes) were detected. Low SES boys or boys born at neonatal risk of mothers with chronic depression had lower Achievement Scores in the K-ABC at 6 ; 3 years than children of mothers with less severe depression or controls. It is concluded that maternal depression per se has negligible effects on children’s cognitive development. Long-term effects may be found when maternal depression is chronic, the child is a boy and neonatal risk-born, or the family suffers other social risks. Keywords : Postnatal depression, maternal depression, cognitive development, chronicity. Abbreviations : AS : Achievement Scales ; BLS : Bavarian Longitudinal Study ; CMM : Columbia Mental Maturity Scale ; DQ : developmental quotient ; GCI : General Cognitive Index ; K-ABC : Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children ; MPC : Mental Processing Composite ; RDCS : Research Diagnostic Criteria Symptomlist ; SADS : Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia ; SES : socioeconomic status. been hypothesised that the sadness, irritability, and social withdrawal that characterise depressed women compromise their ability to provide a responsive, sensitive, and nurturing environment for their infants and children (Cohn & Campbell, 1992 ; Field, 1992 ; Murray, Kempton, Woolgar, & Hooper, 1993 ; Tronick, 1989). Theoretically, then, one of the major mechanisms through which maternal depression could influence infant and child cognitive development is parenting behaviour (Cummings & Davies, 1994). For example, the symptoms of depression and the associated negative self-cognitions (Teti & Gelfand, 1997) lead to less ability to support the infant in regulating affect, less positive and synchronized affect expression, and lower levels of maternal stimulation and responsiveness (DeMulder & Radke-Yarrow, 1991 ; Field, 1992 ; Murray, Fiori-Cowley, Hooper, & Cooper, 1996 ; Murray, Stanley, Hooper, King, & Fiori-Cowley, 1996 ; Stein et al., 1991). The infant is deprived of the experience of expanding his or her states of consciousness and intersubjectivity with the mother (Tronick & Field, 1987 ; Winnicott, 1960). This deprivation limits the Introduction Maternal depression, whether postpartum or later in the life of the child, has been considered as a risk factor for children’s development (Murray & Cooper, 1996 ; Puckering, 1989 ; Rutter, 1990). Different pathways have been suggested for the way in which an emotional disorder of the mother with primarily social disabilities might exert adverse effects on children’s cognitive development (Cummings & Davies, 1994 ; Hay, 1997 ; Rutter, 1990, 1997). Central to most of the proposals are that maternal depression, particularly in the postpartum period, has a deleterious effect on mother-infant interaction insofar as the symptoms of depression interfere with the mothering role (Murray, 1992 ; Radke-Yarrow, Cummings, Kuczynski, & Chapman, 1985). It has thus Requests for reprints to : Professor Dieter Wolke, University of Hertfordshire, Department of Psychology, DWRU, College Lane, Hatfield, Herts AL10 9AB, U.K. (E-mail : D.F.H.Wolke!herts.ac.uk). 623 624 S. KURSTJENS and D. WOLKE infant’s experience and forces the infant into self-regulatory patterns that eventually compromise the child’s development (Tronick & Weinberg, 1997). Provision and lack of contingency experiences and less emotional and attention regulation has been speculated to be one route to poorer learning experience and cognitive development in infants of depressive mothers (Hay, 1997 ; Kaplan, Bachorowski, Hoff, & Zarlengo-Strouse, in press). Others suggest that it may be the lack of a variety of activities and opportunities for play and conversation (Rutter, 1990). The mother is considered as the major framework for these early learning experiences. Although the interactions of the infant are mutual they are not symmetrically regulated (Murray & Cooper, 1997). Moreover, maternal depression is often accompanied by social adversities and conflictual environments, which could discourage learning (e.g. marital conflict, lack of social support) (Sameroff, Seifer, Baldwin, & Baldwin, 1993). Although these suggestions are plausible, there is still a paucity of longitudinal studies of whether maternal depression impacts on cognitive development and how persistent these effects are in the offspring. As far as we are aware there is only one study that assessed the cognitive development of children of depressed mothers repeatedly during the first 5 years of life. Murray and colleagues (Murray, 1992 ; Murray & Cooper, 1996 ; Murray, Fiori-Cowley, et al., 1996 ; Murray, Hipwell, Hooper, Stein, & Cooper, 1996 ; Murray et al., 1993), in their Cambridge Longitudinal Study, assessed the cognitive development of the children of depressed and control mothers at 9 months, 18 months, and 5 years of age. At 9 months children of postnatally depressed mothers showed significantly reduced performance on Piaget’s Object Permanence task. Children of mothers who experienced a major episode were more likely to fail than children whose mothers suffered from a minor episode (Murray, 1992). At 18 months there was a significant overall effect of maternal group on Object Concept tasks, but not on the Bayley Scales of Infant Mental Development, a general measure of infant cognitive development. However, there was a significant interaction between maternal depression and infant gender, with boys of depressed mothers performing poorly (Murray et al., 1993). Similarly, there was no overall effect of maternal group on the Reynell Scales, a test of expressive language and comprehension, but again there was a significant interaction between maternal group and infant gender as well as maternal group and social class. Boys of depressed mothers and infants of depressed mothers of lower social class performed poorly compared to the others (Murray, 1992). Finally, assessment of cognitive functioning at 5 years of age using the McCarthy Scales of Children Abilities (Murray, Hipwell, et al., 1996) showed no association with maternal depression. Neither of the five subscales nor the General Cognitive Index (GCI) showed an overall effect of maternal depression. Furthermore there was no interaction between maternal group and timing of depression (postnatal depression vs. maternal depression at any other time in the child’s lifetime), duration of depressive episode, sex of infant, or socioeconomic status of family. Cogill, Caplan, Alexandra, Robson, and Kumar (1986) identified maternal depression through repeated interviews in pregnancy and throughout the year following the birth of the child. Cognitive development of the children was assessed at 4 years of age compared to a control group. Cogill et al. reported a significant main effect of maternal depression on overall GCI score and significant decrements for children of depressed mothers on the perceptual, memory, and motor subscales. The adverse effects were only found for children whose mothers became depressed in the first year postpartum. There were no significant interactions between maternal group and sex of child or maternal education. A second, more detailed analysis of these data revealed that maternal education also showed a significant main effect on child’s cognitive development and the difference between the children of depressed and nondepressed mothers was only reliable when mothers were postnatally depressed and had low educational level (Hay & Kumar, 1995). Sharp et al. (1995) considered mothers to have postnatal depression if the mothers reported depression in the first year of life (point prevalence) or retrospectively with the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (SADS-L) when the child was 4 years old. They considered the retrospective SADS-L as accurate (p. 1322) because the point prevalence interviews at 3 and 12 months may have missed episodes of illness between 3 and 12 months. Controls were children of mothers who had reported no depressive episodes in the first year and at the 4-year retrospective SADS-L interview. They found an overall significant effect of depression on the McCarthy Scales at 4 years of age, which was explained by an interaction of gender and maternal mental illness. Boys whose mothers were depressed in the first year postpartum had lower scores than boys in the control group or girls in either the depressive or the control group. This interaction effect was found independent of whether the mother was only postpartum depressed or had repeated episodes of depression. It reduced somewhat but was generally robust when other additional factors like socioeconomic status of the family, maternal education, child behavior during the test, parental IQ, social and intellectual situation at home (HOME-Inventory), or mother-infant interaction were considered individually. Social factors and intellectual stimulation at home as well as harmony of mother-infant interaction also predicted boys’ IQ scores on the McCarthy Scales. Further analysis by Hay (1997, p. 95–96) indicated, however, that the effect on cognitive scores was only discerned in boys who came from families considered as working class. Cross-sectional studies also reported about the cognitive development of children of depressed mothers. Whiffen and Gotlib (1989) assessed 2-month-old infants of postnatally depressed and control mothers with the Bayley Scales of Infant Development. The infants of the depressed women performed significantly less well on the cognitive tasks than did the infants of the nondepressed women. Weissman et al. (1986), in their Yale Family Study of Major Depression, examined children between the ages of 6 and 23 years, whose parents (either one or both) had a history of a treated major depression. There were no significant differences between the IQs of the children of depressed and normal parents. Furthermore, Stevenson and Fredman (1990) reported that maternal depression and anxiety was a significant predictor of reading problems in preadolescent twins. The findings so far are inconsistent across different studies or over time within the same sample (e.g., Murray, Fiori-Cowley, et al., 1996 ; Murray, Hipwell, et al., 1996). These can partly be accounted for by differences between studies and methodological limitations. The sample sizes of the existing studies lie between only 20 (Cogill et al., 1986 ; Whiffen & Gotlib, 1989) and 60 depressed mothers MATERNAL DEPRESSION AND CHILD COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT (Murray, Hipwell, et al., 1996 ; Sharp et al., 1995 ; Weissman et al., 1986). When subsamples such as boys with mothers with only postpartum depression were considered in analyses (e.g. N l 13 in the Sharp et al. sample), the cell sizes became very small. Comparisons in small subsamples are susceptable to outliers. For example, of the 135 children studied by Sharp et al., 8 had IQ scores 70 ; 7 of these were children of depressed mothers and 5 of the 7 were boys. The interaction effect with sex may be related to these ‘‘ outliers ’’. Other samples were socially homogeneous. The Murray et al. longitudinal study and the Cogill et al. study included only a few subjects of lower socioeconomic background whereas the Sharp et al. participants were nearly all of lower socioeconomic background. Testing of the effects of SES and the interactions of SES with depression status was thus limited. Most significantly, Murray et al. report repeated longitudinal observations. However, cognitive assessments into middle childhood are necessary to conclude that there are lasting and stable effects of maternal depression on cognitive development (McCall, Appelbaum, & Hogarty, 1973 ; Rutter, 1997 ; Moffitt, Caspi, Harkness, & Silva, 1993). Nevertheless, the following risk factors are promising but require further replication in longitudinal research. First, timing of depression may be crucial (Hay, 1997 ; Rutter, 1997). Both Sharp et al. (1995) and Cogill et al. (1986) found cognitive decrements only for children whose mothers were postnatally depressed. Murray (1992) reported the same for infants’ cognitive development until 18 months of age, however, reassessment at 5 years of age could not find any association with timing of maternal depression. Second, children of depressed mothers who belong to the lower social class may be at greater risk than children of depressed mothers of middle or high social class. However, findings regarding this issue vary considerably : some found main effects of socioeconomic status on cognitive development (Murray, 1992 ; Sharp et al., 1995), others interactions with maternal group (Murray, 1992 ; Cogill et al., 1986), and others again found no association with social class (Whiffen & Gotlib, 1989). Third, the differential effects of maternal depression according to infant gender have led to much attention. Sharp et al. (1995) found effects of maternal depression only for boys although Murray et al. (1993) did not find a significant interaction between maternal group and sex of child at 9 months or 18 months of age on Object Concept Task. However, performance on Bayley Scales (Murray et al., 1993) as well as on the Reynell Scales at 18 months of age (Murray, 1992) did show significant interactions between maternal group and sex of child with boys of depressed mothers scoring lower than girls. Finally, the same children reassessed at 5 years failed to show any association between maternal depression and sex of child (Murray, Hipwell, et al., 1996). Cogill et al. (1986) and the reanalysis of Hay and Kumar (1995) reported no interaction effects with infant gender. Finally, Cogill et al. (1986 ; Hay, 1997 ; Hay & Kumar, 1995) suggested that infant risk factors such as low birthweight may potentiate the effect of postpartum depression on cognitive development. However, this finding is based on only six low birthweight infants in an already small sample (Hay, 1997). The heterogeneity of depressive illness and differential effects on cognitive development have not been considered consistently within one or across studies 625 (Cummings & Davies, 1994 ; Rutter, 1990). Such features are the timing of depression (Murray, Fiori-Cowley, et al., 1996 ; Murray, Hipwell, et al., 1996 ; Sharp et al., 1995) and the severity (Murray, 1992), duration, and number of depressive episodes in the child’s lifetime. Recently, it has been suggested that the chronicity rather than the experience of depressive illness per se may be the crucial factor for the child’s development (S. B. Campbell & Cohn, 1997). Studies often have combined women with short-lived minor depressions with those experiencing major, repeated, and long-term depressions (Hopkins, Campbell, & Marcus, 1987 ; O ’Hara, 1997). Campbell et al. (S. B. Campbell, Cohn, & Meyers, 1995) demonstrated less positive maternal interactions only for depressed mothers who had chronic depression in the first 6 months compared to controls. Depressed mothers with shorter and minor depression did not differ from the control group. It is thus likely that long-term effects on intellectual development may only be found where there is chronicity or an accumulation of adverse social and family factors (Blanz, Schmidt, & Esser, 1991 ; Sameroff et al., 1993 ; Stanton, McGee, & Silva, 1991). The objective of the present study was to examine the effects of different features of maternal depression on the intellectual development of children, assessed on three occasions over the first 7 years in the children’s life (at 20 months, 4 ; 8 years, and 6 ; 3 years). The features of depression include the timing, recency, severity, duration, and number of depressive episodes as well as an index of chronicity of depression. Furthermore, interactions of child’s gender, infant neonatal risk, and family socioeconomic status with features of depression on child cognitive development are evaluated. Methods Population Sample The Bavarian Longitudinal Study (BLS ; Wolke & Meyer, 1999a, b)" investigates the development of children who were born during the period 1 February 1985–31 March 1986 and required admission to special care baby units within the first 10 days of life in South Bavaria as well as control children cared for on normal postnatal wards. A total sample of N l 8421 children and their mothers (N l 7505 at-risk children and N l 916 healthy born children) have been followed since birth and were assessed several times until the 5th year of the children’s life (Wolke, Meyer, Ohrt, & Riegel, 1995). The population for the investigation reported here was a subsample of the total study population of 1329 mothers and their singleton offspring who were randomly selected from the total sample within the stratification variables : gender (2), SES (3), and neonatal risk (3), and assessed at 6 ; 3 years. Of these children, 1011 (76n1 %) had been admitted to a children’s hospital for neonatal special care (at-risk children : very preterm 32 weeks gestation N l 200 ; preterm 32–36 weeks gestation N l 273 ; full-term 36 weeks gestation N l 538) and 318 (23n9 %) were full-term children who had normal postnatal care in obstetric hospitals. Maternal Psychiatric Interview When the children were 6 ; 3 years of age, a structured and standardised interview regarding maternal physical and mental health was conducted with all mothers by trained clinical psychologists, all of whom were unaware of children’s neonatal risk status (Kurstjens & Wolke, 2001). This interview included a screening for depressive symptoms from the SADS " See ‘‘ Bavarian-Finnish Longitudinal Study ’’ : www-homepage: http:\\psy.herts.ac.uk\DWRU\hmpage.html 626 S. KURSTJENS and D. WOLKE (Endicott & Spitzer, 1978), a reliable and well-validated instrument for assessing psychiatric history (O’Hara & Swain, 1996). The screening took 10 minutes and asked about depressive symptoms (depressive mood, irritability, crying, and loss of interest) in the last 7 years since pregnancy and birth of the study child. Each screening question was rated on 4-point scales (no, doubtful, moderate, definite\severe). Furthermore, it was rated whether the symptoms led to significant social and work dysfunction (‘‘ no social and work dysfunction ’’, ‘‘ social and work dysfunction for 2–6 days ’’, ‘‘ social and work dysfunction 7 days or more ’’). If the screening was positive (moderate or definite\severe in at least one of the four screening questions and social and work dysfunction), a detailed depression interview of about 30 minutes duration followed to establish timing, number, severity, and duration of the depressive episodes and treatment experiences (German version of the SADS of Endicott & Spitzer, 1978 ; translated and adapted by Wolke, Leo! n-Villagra! , & Meyer, 1993). According to the Research Diagnostic Criteria Symptomlist (RDCS ; Spitzer, Endicott, & Robins, 1978), 24 symptoms (e.g. insomnia, lack of appetite, thoughts of suicide) were explored on 3-point scales (no, moderate, definite\severe). Based on these data a diagnosis of Minor or Major episodes of depression according to DSMIV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) was made. Features of Maternal Depression The features of maternal depression were distinguished as follows (O’Hara, 1997). Timing. Those mothers who had a depressive episode within the first year of the child’s life were labeled as ‘‘ postnatal depression ’’, whereas those mothers who had a depressive episode after the first year of the child’s life were labeled as ‘‘ later depression ’’. Recency. Those mothers who were depressed during the last year before the 6 ; 3-year assessment had a ‘‘ recent depression ’’ and those mothers whose last depressive episode was more than 1 year before the 6 ; 3-year assessment had ‘‘ no recent depression ’’. Severity. According to DSM-IV criteria (specific core symptoms have to be present for a defined duration), ‘‘ Major ’’ (five and more core symptoms) and ‘‘ Minor ’’ depressive episodes (less than five core symptoms) were assessed. Number. Mothers did experience a variable number of episodes ranging from 1 episode only to more than 10 episodes in the first 7 years of the child’s life. Number of episodes was dichotomised as experience of one depressive episode only (‘‘ single episode ’’) versus experience of two or more episodes (‘‘ multiple episodes ’’). Duration. The longest recorded depressive episode was dichotomised as ‘‘ short ’’ when lasting 6 months or less and ‘‘ long ’’ when it lasted for more than 6 months. Severe-chronically depressed group. To account for the possibility that not one but the cumulation of several features of depression may be related to cognitive development in the children (S. B. Campbell & Cohen, 1997), we defined a severely affected group of depressed mothers. The ‘‘ severe-chronic ’’ depressed mothers had to have had a major depression which started in the postnatal period and involved multiple episodes. Assessment of Child Cognitive Status When children were 20 months of age (corrected for prematurity) they were assessed using the Griffiths Scales of Babies’ Abilities (1976) in its German version (Brandt, 1983). These scales allow the computation of a General Developmental quotient ; DQ l (Developmental age score\age at assessment)i100. At 4 ; 8 years of age (chronological age) cognitive development was assessed with the Columbia Mental Maturity Scales (CMM ; Burgemeister, Blum, & Lorge, 1972 ; German version : Eggert, 1972). The CMM is a frequently used test assessing the performance and reasoning component of in- telligence and is similar to the Raven Progressive Matrices allowing the computation of a Performance Score (100p15). At 6 ; 3 years of age children’s intellectual development was assessed with the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC ; Kaufman & Kaufman, 1983) in its German version (Melchers & Preuß, 1991) (see Wolke & Meyer, 1999a). This cognitive assessment battery is based on neuropsychological theories of intellectual functioning. General intelligence is measured with the Mental Processing Composite (MPC ; eight subsets at this age ; 100p15). The Achievement Score (AS ; 100p15) included three subsets designed to measure what has been learned by the child. The K-ABC is a widely accepted scale for measurement of intellectual development in at-risk children (e.g. Achenbach, Howell, Aoki, & Rauh, 1993 ; Li, Sauve, & Creighton, 1990 ; Weisglass-Kuperus, Baerts, Smrkovsky, & Sauer, 1993)#. All assessments were carried out by trained postgraduate psychologists, all of whom were blind to maternal group and children’s risk status. Statistical analysis. To test for effects of maternal depression on children’s cognitive development, group comparisons were carried out using three-way univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA). Independent factors for testing for the effect of postnatal depression were ‘‘ maternal group ’’ (postnatally depressed vs. controls consisting of mothers who were never depressed until their child was aged 6 ; 3 years), ‘‘ sex of child ’’ (boys and girls), ‘‘ socioeconomic status (SES) ’’ of family (low, middle, and high) and ‘‘ neonatal risk status ’’ of the child (no risk and risk), respectively. All factors could not be tested simultaneously but only in three-way ANOVAs due to cell size and statistical power limitations. Thus maternal group was tested with all the combinations of sex, SES, and neonatal risk status separately for interaction effects. Dependent variables were the measures of cognitive development at 20 months (Griffiths Scales of Babies’ Development) and the CMM at 4 ; 8 years. At 6 ; 3 years tests for the effects of the different features of depression on K-ABC MPC and AS were carried out and these comprised the following factors : timing : postnatal depression, later depression, controls ; recency : recent depression, no recent depression, controls ; number of episodes : single episode, multiple episodes, controls ; duration of longest episode : short, long, controls. To test for interaction effects of each feature of depression, three-way analyses of variance of depression feature by sex, SES, and risk status were carried out. Cognitive scores over time are shown as z-scores in figures and additionally in tables as raw IQ scores. Results Prevalence of Depression The depression screening was positive for 109 (8n2 %) of the 1329 mothers and required a full depression interview according to criteria (Fig. 1). One hundred and two mothers (7n7 %) received the full depression interview, however 7 cases (0n5 %) were missed due to interviewer errors. For 1220 mothers (91n8 %) screening was negative and therefore no depression interview was necessary. Seven hundred and twenty-one mothers had no depressive symptoms (none or only mild) until the seventh year of their child’s life and served as controls. Of the 102 mothers with the complete depression interview, 92 (7n0 %) met diagnostic criteria for a minor or major depressive episode. Ten mothers (0n7 %) had less # A representative sample for Bavaria (the ‘‘ Normative Sample ’’), sampled from the total BLS sample (see Riegel, Ohrt, Wolke, & O$ sterlund, 1995 ; Wolke, Ratschinski, Ohrt, & Riegel, 1994 ; Wolke & Meyer, 1999a, b), provided cohortspecific norms for the cognitive assessments (e.g. means and standard deviations for computing z-scores). MATERNAL DEPRESSION AND CHILD COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT Figure 1. 627 Prevalence of depression. Table 1 Features of Depression Features of depression Timing Postnatal Later Recency Nonrecent Recent Severity Minor Major Number Single episode Multiple episodes Duration Short ( 6 mths) Long ( 6 mths) Chronic-severe Less severe Chronic-severe Depressed mothers (N l 92) Birth risk\no birth risk (68\24) 44 (47n8 %) 48 (52n2 %) 35\9 33\15 40 (43n5 %) 52 (56n5 %) 30\10 38\14 26 (28n3 %) 66 (71n7 %) 23\3 45\21 29 (31n5 %) 63 (68n5 %) 19\10 49\14 53 (57n6 %) 39 (42n4 %) 39\14 29\10 68 (73n9 %) 24 (26n1 %) 50\18 18\6 severe depressive symptoms, which did not reach diagnostic status (depressive mood only, d.m. in Fig. 1). Features of Depression Frequency distributions of the features of depression are presented in Table 1. Twenty-six (28n3 %) out of 92 depressed mothers met diagnostic criteria for a minor depressive episode, and 66 (71n7 %) for at least one major episode. Forty-four mothers (47n8 %) experienced the first or only episode during the first year of the child (postnatal depression, p.d.), 48 mothers (52n2 %) suffered from a depressive episode for the first time in their life after the first year of the child (later depression, l.d.). 628 S. KURSTJENS and D. WOLKE Table 2 Compliance of Children of Depressed (Dep) and Control (Con) Mothers from 20 Months to 6 ; 3 Years of Age with Cognitive Assessment Result (N ) Group 20 months Griffiths 4 ; 8 years CMM 6 ; 3 years MPC AS Missing Con 4 4 23 20 Dep 3 0 0 Con Dep 711 90 695 87 715 715 90 90 Fifty-two (56n5 %) mothers had a depressive episode in the year before the 6 ; 3 years assessment, 63 of depressed mothers (68n5 %) had more than one depressive episode (multiple episodes), and 39 mothers (42n4 %) had at least one episode which lasted for more than 6 months. The severe-chronic depressed group of mothers comprised 24 (26n1 %) women. They had already suffered from a major depressive episode in the first year of life of their child and had multiple depressive episodes. Outliers and Dropouts Two children of the depressed group and six children of the control group scored more than 3n3 SDs below the population mean (according to the normative sample of the BLS) on the MPC scale of the K-ABC at 6 ; 3 years of age as well as more than 3n3 SDs below the population mean on the Griffiths Baby Scales, i.e. were moderately to severely mentally retarded. All children had a high birth risk and were significant outliers for statistical analysis. They were excluded from analysis of IQ data as they could have a biasing effect on the test of the effects of maternal depression (see Hay & Kumar, 1995). Therefore the IQ analysis is based on a maximum of N l 90 children of depressed and N l 715 children of control mothers. Individual children missed either the 20 months or 4 ; 8 years cognitive assessment (see Table 2). No child participated in less than two of the three assessments. At 4 ; 8 years of age there were no differences in CMM IQ scores between those children who had missed the assessment at 20 months (N l 4) compared to those with all assessments (N l 779) (t lk0n99, n.s.). At 20 months of age there were also no significant differences in Griffiths Baby Scales scores of those children who missed the 4 ; 8 years assessment (N l 23) and those children with all assessments (N l 779) (t lk0n89 ; n.s.). At 6 ; 3 years of age there were significant differences between the two groups on the MPC scale and AS scale of the K-ABC (MPC scale : t lk2n80, p n01 ; AS scale : t lk3n29, p .01). On both scales children who did not participate at 20 months assessment (MPC mean l 90 ; AS mean l 88) scored lower than children without missing data (MPC mean l 97 ; AS mean l 98). However, in the group of children with missing data, at 20 months or 6 ; 3 years there was no statistically significant difference in IQ scores between children of the depressed (N l 3) versus Table 3 Sociodemographic Characteristics of the Final Samples Depressed mothers (N l 90) Sex of child Boys Girls Gestation (weeks) Mean (SD) Birthweight Mean (SD) SES at birth Low Middle High SES at 6 ; 3 years Low Middle High Maternal age Mean (SD) Marital status Married Education Basic ( 10 yrs) Moderate (10–12 yrs) A-level\Univ.( 12 yrs) Parity Primiparous 48 (53 %) 42 (47 %) 37n7 (3n6) 2924n7 (841n8) Control mothers (N l 715) 383 (54 %) 332 (46 %) p n.s. 37n3 (3n6) n.s. 2808n2 (879n9) n.s. 34 (37n8 %) 27 (30n0 %) 29 (32n2 %) 258 (36n1 %) 273 (38n2 %) 184 (25n7 %) n.s. 38 (42n2 %) 28 (31n1 %) 24 (26n7 %) 254 (35n5 %) 258 (36n1 %) 203 (28n4 %) n.s. 28n1 (4n6) 28n4 (5n1) n.s. 88 (97n8 %) 683 (95n9 %) n.s. 12 (13n3 %) 58 (64n4 %) 20 (22n2 %) 85 (11n9 %) 503 (70n3 %) 127 (17n8 %) n.s. 51 (56n7 %) 427 (59n7 %) n.s. MATERNAL DEPRESSION AND CHILD COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT Table 4 Impact of Postnatal Maternal Depression on Griffiths Baby Scales General DQ at 20 months and CMM Performance Score at 4 ; 8 Years of Age (Main Effects) 20 months Controls Postnatal depression 4 ; 8 years N Griffiths N CMM 711 43 104n8 (8n6) 104n7 (6n5) 695 42 97n1 (16n3) 97n0 (20n7) control group (N l 20) (20 months Griffiths scales : t l 0n16, n.s. ; 6n3 years MPC scale : t l 0n01, n.s. ; 6n3 years AS scale : t l 1n04, n.s.). Furthermore, there were no significant differences between children with and without missing assessments according to social class, χ#(2) l 0n91, n.s. Children with a missing assessment had significantly more often a birth risk than children without missing data, χ# l 7n14 ; p n01. Subject Characteristics Means and standard deviations for the depressed and control subjects with respect to demographic characteristics of the subjects are presented in Table 3. There were no statistically significant differences between the depressive and control group in terms of sex of child, χ# (1) l 0n00 ; n.s., gestation, t(809) lk1n09 ; n.s., birthweight t(809) lk1n19 ; n.s., SES at time of the birth of the study child, χ#(2) l 2n77 ; n.s., or SES at 6 ; 3 years assessment, χ#(2) l 1n63 ; n.s. Nor did the two groups differ with regard to maternal age, t(806) l 0n57 ; n.s., marital status at birth of study child, χ#(1) l 0n74 ; n.s., maternal education, χ#(2) l 1n41 ; n.s., or parity, χ#(1) l 0n31 ; n.s. The two groups were thus highly comparable according to child and sociodemographic indices. Maternal Depression and Risk Status of the Child To test whether birth complications and neonatal risk of the child lead to a higher prevalence of maternal depression, we compared the rate of maternal depression in the two subsamples (neontal risk vs. no neontal risk) in the total sample. There were no differences in rates of depression between mothers of neonatal at-risk children, N l 1011 ; 6n7 %, and mothers of healthy born children, N l 318 ; 7n5 % ; χ#(1) l 3n50 ; n.s., and no differences in the composition of healthy born and neonatal at-risk children in the depression and control group (see Fig. 1). Infant Cognitive Development Table 4 shows the means and standard deviations of the postnatal depression group compared to the controls. At 20 months there was no significant main effect of maternal group, F(1) l 0n47, n.s. (see Table 4) and no significant interaction effect between maternal group and any of the other factors—maternal group by sex of child : F(1) l 0n40, n.s. ; by SES at time of child’s birth : F(2) l 0n20, n.s. ; by risk status of the child : F(1) l 0n41, n.s.— on general developmental quotient (DQ). Neither boys of postnatally depressed mothers (M l 103n5, SD l 6n2), nor children of socially disadvantaged families with a postnatally depressed mother (M l 103n5, SD l 6n6), nor children with birth risk and postnatally depressed mother (M l 104n6, SD l 6n6) had lower scores than 629 children of control mothers (boys : M l 103n6, SD l 8n9 ; low SES : M l 103n8, SD l 8n9 ; birth risk children : M l 104n3, SD l 9n1). There was also no significant three-way interaction effect. Similarly at 4 ; 8 years assessment, there was no significant main effect of maternal group, F(1) l 0n12, n.s. (see Table 4) nor any significant interaction effect between maternal group and other factors—maternal group by sex of child : F(1) l 0n43, n.s. ; by SES at time of child’s birth : F(2) l 0n32, n.s. ; by risk status of the child : F(1) l 0n01, n.s. Neither boys (M l 97n1, SD l 23n4), nor socially disadvantaged children (M l 91n7, SD l 21n0), nor children with birth risk (M l 96n0, SD l 22n4) of postnatally depressed mothers had lower CMM scores than children of the control group (boys : M l 95n5, SD l 15n9 ; low SES : M l 91n9, SD l 17n7 ; birth risk children : M l 95n9, SD l 17n0). At 6 ;3 years neither timing [postnatal depression vs. later depression vs. controls ; MPC : F(2) l 0n98, n.s ; AS : F(2) l 0n45, n.s.], nor recency [MPC : F(2) l 0n24, n.s. ; AS : F(2) l 0n81, n.s.], severity [MPC : F(2) l 0n42, n.s. ; AS : F(2) l 0n29, n.s.], nor number of episodes [MPC : F(2) l 0n98, n.s. ; AS : F(2) l 1n01, n.s], nor duration of longest depressive episode [MPC : F(2) l 0n22, n.s. ; AS : F(2) l 0n39, n.s.] showed any significant influence on cognitive development at 6 ; 3 years of age (Table 5). Neither children of postnatally depressed mothers, nor children with mothers who had a depressive episode in the last year before 6 ; 3 years cognitive assessment, nor children of mothers with major depression or with multiple episodes, nor children of mothers who had episodes that lasted longer than 6 months, had lower MPC or AS scores in the K-ABC than children of the later depressed, not recently depressed, minor depressed, or single episode or short episode depressed or control mothers, respectively (see tables). Furthermore, neither timing, recency, severity, number, nor duration of depression had significant two- or three-way interactions with sex of child, SES, or risk status of the child. Chronic-severe Depression The chronically affected group of depressed mothers did not differ from less affected depressed mothers or controls on the 6 ; 3 years cognitive assessment, neither on the MPC nor on the AS scale of the K-ABC [MPC : F(2) l 1n41, n.s. ; AS : F(2) l 0n54, n.s.] (see Table 5). There were also no significant two-way interaction effects between maternal group and sex of child [MPC : F(1) l 0n09, n.s. ; AS : F(2) l 1n22, n.s.), SES of family at time of 6 ;3 years assessment [MPC : F(2) l 0n74, n.s. ; AS : F(4) l 2n05, n.s.] or risk status of the child [MPC : F(1) l 0n23, n.s., AS : F(2) l 0n08, n.s.]. There was also no significant three-way interaction effect for the MPC scale. However, on the AS scale there were two significant three-way interaction effects : chronicity of maternal depression by sex of child by SES of family, F(6) l 2n27, p n05, and chronicity of maternal depression by sex of child by neonatal risk status of child, F(3) l 2n53, p l n05. Boys of chronically depressed mothers of low SES families (N l 9 ; M l 88n4, SD l 20n4) showed the lowest scores compared to boys of chronically depressed mothers in upper SES families (middle SES : N l 4 ; M l 90n3, SD l 6n0 ; upper SES : N l 4 ; M l 100n8, SD l 24n8) or boys and girls of the control group of any SES status (boys : lower SES : N l 135 ; M l 93n2, SD l 13n8 ; middle SES : N l 138 ; M l 97n9, SD l 13n5 ; upper SES : 630 S. KURSTJENS and D. WOLKE Table 5 Different Features of Maternal Depression and K-ABC Mental Processing Component (MPC ) and Achievement Score (AS ) Scores at 6 ; 3 Years of Age (Main Effects) Controls Timing Postnatal depression Later depression Recency Nonrecent Recent Severity Minor Major Number Single Multiple Duration Short Long Chronicity Less severe Chronic-severe N MPC 715 96n7 (12n0) p AS 97n9 (14n6) n.s. 43 47 95n5 (12n0) 98n2 (13n0) 40 50 99n2 (11n6) 95n1 (13n1) 25 26 99n8 (11n1) 95n8 (13n0) 29 61 99n6 (10n9) 95n6 (13n2) 53 37 96n0 (13n1) 98n2 (11n8) 67 23 98n5 (11n9) 92n3 (13n4) p n.s. 98n6 (16n5) 97n8 (15n7) n.s. n.s. 101n2 (15n9) 95n8 (15n8) n.s. n.s. 102n4 (15n4) 96n5 (16n0) n.s. n.s. 102n4 (13n0) 96n2 (16n9) n.s. n.s. 96n0 (17n1) 101n2 (13n9) n.s. n.s. 99n3 (14n9) 94n8 (18n7) n.s. l not significant. Figure 3. DQ\IQ scores of children of mothers of low, middle, and high SES at 20 months, 4 ; 8 years, and 6 ; 3 years of age (z-scores) Figure 2. Three-way interaction of chronic depression, neonatal risk, and sex of child on the Achievement Score of the K-ABC at 6 ; 3 years of age. N l 110 ; M l 104n8, SD l 13n3 ; girls : lower SES : N l 118 ; M l 93n7, SD l 16n4 ; middle SES : N l 120 ; M l 97n2, SD l 15n0 ; upper SES : N l 93 ; M l 102n8, SD l 10n8). Furthermore, boys with neonatal risk and a chronically depressed mother had significantly lower scores (N l 14 ; M l 88n8 ; SD l 18n1) than boys with neonatal risk and less severely depressed mothers (N l 3 ; M l 105n7 ; SD l 19n5) or boys of control group mothers with (N l 282 ; M l 97n1 ; SD l 14n0) or without birth risk (N l 101 ; M l 101n2 ; SD l 14n6) (see Fig. 2) DQ\IQ scores in different SES groups. To provide a comparison for judging the relative impact of maternal depression on children’s cognitive development, we deter- mined the influence of SES, a factor known to affect DQ\IQ scores over time. Figure 3 shows the average DQ\IQ z-scores between 20 months and 6 ; 3 years of age for the total sample (N l 804) of children (children of depressed and control mothers) according to SES. SES differences are statistically significant at all assessment points. Children of low SES scored, on average, about 0n2 to 0n5 SD units lower than children of middle SES and about 0n5 to 0n8 SD units below the children of high SES. Discussion In the present study we evaluated the effects of postnatal depression on children’s cognitive development over a 7-year period in a large sample of children. Moreover we examined the effects of different features of depression on children’s cognitive development at 6 ; 3 years of age. Our major findings are that (a) postnatal depression per se has no adverse effects on cognitive MATERNAL DEPRESSION AND CHILD COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT development of children during the first 7 years of life and (b) no main effects of different features of depression are found at 6 ; 3 years of age. However, (c) adverse effects on scores were found for low SES boys or boys born at neonatal risk of chronically depressed mothers in the Achievement Score (AS) of the K-ABC at 6 ; 3 years. Hay (1997) proposed that the effects on cognitive development may be more pronounced if the onset of maternal depression is in early infancy, a sensitive period for the formation of the mother-infant relationship and for learning to regulate arousal and attention mutually (Tronick & Weinberg, 1997) and the acquisition of awareness for contingencies (Dunham & Dunham, 1990). Hay (1997) bases her proposal on findings of the Cogill et al. (1986) study and her reanalysis of these data (Hay & Kumar, 1995). This showed that children of mothers who were depressed in the first year of the life had lower McCarthy General Cognitive scores than did children whose mothers were not ill at that time. However, no long-term main effects of postnatal depression compared to controls as found here were reported in two other large studies (Murray, Hipwell, et al., 1996 ; Sharp et al., 1995). Although Murray (1992) found an overall effect of postnatal depression on an Object Concept task when children were 9 and 18 months of age, other measures of cognitive development at 18 months (Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Reynell Language Scales ; Murray, 1992 ; Murray et al., 1993) or at 5 years of age (McCarthy Scales ; Murray, Hipwell, et al., 1996) did not indicate a difference between children of postnatally depressed and control mothers. Our finding that at no time from 20 months onwards did children of postnatally depressed mothers show lower scores than children of mothers who were later or not at all depressed is highly consistent with Murray, Hipwell, et al. (1996), who also found no evidence for a sensitive period of the impact of maternal depression for longer-term cognitive development. However, Murray, Hipwell, et al. found evidence that insensitive maternal interactions at 2 months (e.g. mother’s speech), whether by depressed or not depressed mothers, early developmental delay, and poorer home environment were related to 5-year IQ scores. This finding suggests that early maladaptive interaction and parenting that can also occur in motherinfant dyads without postnatal depression affects cognitive development. Campbell (S. B. Campbell & Cohen, 1997 ; S. B. Campbell et al., 1995) reported that most mothers, despite their postnatal depression, provide ‘‘ good enough mothering ’’ and depression does not indicate an inevitable risk. Marked differences are mostly observed in high-risk samples where postnatal depression is accompanied by other family and social problems and reflected in marked differences in maternal communication, infant distress, and avoidance (Field, Healy, Goldstein, & Guthertz, 1990). The reanalysis of the relatively affluent Cogill et al. (1986) sample (Hay, 1997 ; Hay & Kumar, 1995) showed that the difference between the children of depressed mothers and well mothers was only reliable when the mothers themselves were less well educated, providing further evidence for this interpretation. Differences in sensitivity and parenting styles have been repeatedly reported to differ according to SES status (Aylward, 1992 ; F. A. Campbell & Ramey, 1994 ; Cichetti, 1996 ; Crittenden & Bonvillian, 1984 ; Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1994 ; Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, & Klebanov, 1994 ; Felner et al., 1995 ; Hashima & Amato, 1994 ; Morisset, Barnard, Greenberg, Booth, & Spiker, 631 1990 ; Rutter, 1981 ; Steele, 1990). We found, like many other studies (e.g. Hernstein & Murray, 1994 ; Molfese, DiLalla, & Bunce, 1997 ; Parker, Greer, & Zuckermann, 1988 ; Sameroff et al., 1993 ; Walker, Greenwood, Hart, & Carta, 1994 ; Wolke & Meyer, 1999a), highly significant effects of SES on cognitive scores from 20 months to 6 ; 3 years of age that were independent of postnatal depression. Although a number of studies of postnatal depression have found an increase in insecure attachment classification in infant-mother dyads where the mother suffered postnatal depression (Murray, 1992 ; Radke-Yarrow et al., 1985 ; Teti, Gelfand, Messinger, & Isabella, 1995), there is little evidence for lower general cognitive abilities in infants who are insecurely attached (Van Ijzendoorn, Dijkstra, & Bus, 1995). Also, no long-term effects of attachment security on academic performance have been documented (Grossmann, Grossman, & Zimmermann, in press). Cognitive development appears to be more robust and less affected than emotional and behavioural functioning under less than optimal caretaking conditions (Rutter, 1985, 1987 ; Rutter & The ERA Study Team, 1998 ; Skuse, 1984). A second issue previously raised is whether certain infants are more vulnerable and affected by exposure to maternal depression. Sharp et al. (1995) reported that boys of mothers who had postnatal depression had IQ scores (GCI) almost 1 SD below those of sons of women who had been well. Cogill et al. (1986) further reported that low birthweight children may be particular vulnerable to postnatal depression in their mothers. In the present study we tested for interaction effects with gender, neonatal risk status of the infant, and SES. Two significant interaction effects were detected in our study and both were interactions between gender and the severe-chronic form of depression. Low SES boys and boys with neonatal complications of severe-chronically depressed mothers showed lower cognitive scores at 6 ; 3 years in the Achievement Scale of the K-ABC than children of other subgroups. This result indicates a similar finding to that by Sharp et al. (1995) that boys from lower SES (Hay, 1997) or with neonatal complications (Cogill et al. 1986), when growing up with a depressed mother, are at increased risk for adverse affects on cognitive development. At 18 months assessment, Murray (1992) also found significant interaction effects between maternal group and sex of child as well as maternal group and social class of family. Boys of postnatal depressed mothers as well as children of lower social class families reached significantly lower cognitive scores. However, at 5 years of age there were no more significant interactions between maternal depression and sex of child or social class (Murray, Hipwell, et al., 1996). Although this is further evidence for the higher vulnerability of boys, there is an important difference compared to the findings reported by Sharp et al. (1995). We did not find that the timing of maternal depression was important for boys’ cognitive development ; rather it was the chronicity of depression of socially or neonatally compromised boys that mattered. Furthermore, the synergistic effect of gender, SES, or neonatal complications and chronicity of depression was only found at 6 ; 3 years and confined to the Achievement Scale of the K-ABC and not found for Mental Processing tapping the g-factor of IQ (Melchers & Preuß, 1991). Thus the aspect of cognitive function (achievement) that is more dependent on parental stimulation is affected. It appears 632 S. KURSTJENS and D. WOLKE that the effects become only apparent after years of exposure to maternal depression. Notable is that the German children, although mostly attending half-day kindergarten, had not yet started primary school. A further follow-up in middle childhood should indicate whether schooling provides new opportunities of interaction with adults and turning points for these boys (Rutter & Rutter, 1992) or whether achievement scores remain low (Moffit et al., 1993). Furthermore, although Cogill et al. (1986) indicated that postnatal depression may affect low birthweight children more than normal weight infants we did not find that the timing of depression mattered. Low birthweight has an adverse main effect on cognitive development (Wolke, 1998 ; Wolke & Meyer, 1999a, b) but mothers of children with neonatal complications were neither more often clinically depressed nor did neonatal complications interact with postnatal depression for predicting cognitive scores. Rather, achievement scores are only found to be lower in boys requiring neonatal special care of mothers with chronic depression. Despite the finding that boys are a vulnerable group as reported by others previously, the practical significance of the three-way interactions detected here needs to be considered with some caution. Considering the number of comparisons carried out in our study and the fact that these interactions were only found at the 6 ; 3 year assessment and not in the earlier cognitive assessments, they could also be accounted for by chance. Furthermore, the number of children in the individual cells was small although the differences were statistically significant. This is also true for previous studies. Sharp et al.’s (1995) study had 26 boys altogether, of whom 13 had mothers who were only postnatally depressed. Altogether 7\26 boys had significant cognitive deficits (GCI 70) and 5 of these 7 were in the postnatal depression group (p. 1328). Cogill et al. (1986) had only six infants of low birthweight. Interactions in small samples are liable to outliers and this should be considered in the interpretation of findings. Similarly, it is important to consider methodological factors that may explain differences of our findings to previous reports of adverse effects of postnatal depression on cognitive development (Hay, 1997 ; Murray & Cooper, 1996 ; Sharp et al., 1995). First, although the cognitive assessments were carried out prospectively, the mother reports of depression were retrospective when the children were 6 ; 3 years of age and the validity of recall data could be questioned. First, we used a reliable and well-validated instrument (SADS, Endiott & Spitzer, 1978 ; Murray, Hipwell, et al., 1996 ; O’Hara & Swain, 1996) and had specially trained interviewers with regular review meetings of tape-recorded interviews. For example, Sharp et al. (1995) used the SADS-L when the children were 4 years of age to identify most of the mothers with postnatal depression. They concluded that ‘‘ the retrospective reports may well have been accurate ’’ (p. 1322) compared to the lower point prevalences found in prospective interviews at 3 and 12 months that may have missed cases occurring in between these times. Second, comparison of prospective data on psychological problems from the neonatal period until 4 ; 8 years of age (see Kurstjens & Wolke, 2001) indicated good convergence of early self-reported (Psychological Stress Index, PSI, Riegel et al., 1995 ; Wolke, 1997 ; Wolke & Meyer, 1999a, b) and interviewer-rated psychological problems (Family Adversity Index, FAI, Riegel et al., 1995 ; Wolke, 1997 ; Wolke & Meyer, 1999a, b) with diagnosis of postnatal or later depression (see Appendix). Mothers who at 6 ; 3 years reported a depressive episode in the postnatal period had at that time (neonatal or at 5 months) significantly more psychological problems than control mothers and mothers who got depressed only later. There is a good match between the pattern of prospective and retrospective data equivalent or exceeding those reported by Sharp et al. (1995, p. 1321). Moreover, during the lifetime of the child, 70 % of mothers in the depressed group did receive treatment for depression by a professional, 45 % had had pharmacological treatment for depression, and 23 % had been admitted to hospital for psychiatric treatment. Third, the lower prevalence rate for maternal depression in our sample compared to British and North American studies may suggest that the interview missed true cases (i.e. false negatives). O’Hara and Swain (1996) found in a meta-analysis of rates of postpartum depression of mainly Anglo-American studies a mean prevalence of 13 % (first year) for screening questionnaires but significantly lower rates (between 3n7 % to 10n7 %) in investigator-based interviews. There are several pieces of evidence that show that there are striking differences in depression rates across countries (Weissman et al., 1996 ; Wickberg-Johannssen, Erlandsson, & Hwang, 1996) and they are lower in Germany than, for example, the United States. The differences in rates for depression across countries suggest that cultural differences affect the expression of the disorder (Weissman et al., 1996). The American National Comorbidity Survey (NCS ; Kessler et al., 1994) found a lifetime prevalence of major depressive episode of 21n3 % and a 12-month prevalence of major depression of 12n9 %. A German study carried out by the Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry (EDSP ; Lieb et al., 1998) used the same interview as the Kessler et al. (1994) study in a South German community sample of 1023 mothers. The lifetime rates (11n9 % in South Germany vs. 21n3 % in the NCS) and 12-month prevalence rate for major depression (2n6 % vs. 12n9 %) were substantially lower than those in the U.S. sample. In our study, of the mothers identified postnatally as depressed, 2n3 % (30\1329) had a major depression and of the mothers with depression in the 12 months before the 6 ;3-year interview, 2n5 % (33\1329) had a major depression. These prevalence rates for major depression are so close to those of the German EDSP study of mothers that we can be fairly confident not to have missed cases of DSM-IV depression. Fourth, results from this sample reported elsewhere (Kurstjens & Wolke, 2001) impressively replicate previous findings by other groups (Cooper, Campbell, Day, Kennerley, & Bond, 1988 ; Cooper & Murray, 1995 ; Hobfoll, Ritter, Lavin, Hulsizer, & Cameron, 1995 ; Nieland & Rogers, 1997 ; O’Hara & Swain, 1996) that depression, whether postnatal or later, is best predicted by previous depressive illness (Bifulco, Brown, Morgan, Ball, & Campbell, 1998), life events, and family adversity. As found by Murray and Cartwright (1993) we also found that only women who had previously been depressed and had perinatal complications were more likely to develop postnatal depression (Kurstjens & Wolke, 2001). There are striking similarities across countries in patterns of depression despite widely different rates as noted previously (Weisman et al., 1996). In summary, our findings using a retrospective interview method match up well with previous findings on factors related to depression, rates of depression detected MATERNAL DEPRESSION AND CHILD COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT using interview methods in South Germany, and the effect of obstetric factors, life events, and family adversity on postnatal depression, underlining the validity of the interview used in this study. Finally, similarly to Sharp et al. (1995), we contrasted the mothers who experienced depression to those ‘‘ who were definitely free of illness ’’ (p. 1323), i.e. mothers who had no or single mild symptoms without these ever affecting their social functioning. Our sample included 76 % of children who were admitted for special care observation in the first 10 days of life. The large group of children born after obstetric or neonatal complications allowed for the assessment of whether and how obstetric factors are related to the occurrence of postnatal depression (Murray & Cartwright, 1993). No differences in DSM-IV clinical depression rates between mothers of at-risk children (6n7 %) and mothers of full-term, healthy-born children (7n5 % ; Kurstjens & Wolke, 2001) were found. Neonatal risk was controlled for in the design (i.e. the control group did not differ in regard to neonatal risk status and on any of the family variables tested) and in the statistical analysis and should thus not have affected findings. Compared to other studies (Cogill et al., 1986 ; Murray, 1992 ; Sharp et al., 1995 ; Whiffen & Gotlib, 1989) the sample size is large and the duration of follow-up long. As far as we are aware this is the largest study on maternal depression and cognitive development to date. DQ\IQ assessments were standard and carried out by qualified examiners and regularly checked for administrative objectivity via video recordings (Wolke & Meyer, 1999a). At each age, different trained examiners assessed the children to ensure that they were blind to children’s risk and mother’s diagnostic status. We are thus confident that the results are reliable and valid. In conclusion, adverse effects of maternal depression on cognitive development were only found in lower-SES boys and neonatal risk-born boys if the depression started early and was severe and chronic. Most women who become depressed are less chronically depressed and the effects on cognitive development in the children are negligible or small. Furthermore, even if the mothers are severely and chronically depressed the impact is smaller than the impact of SES on the children’s cognitive development. SES effects are already found at 20 months and continue to be influential in childhood (Fig. 3). It appears that the accumulation of adverse conditions (low SES ; chronic-severe depression, neonatal risk) rather than depression per se has a major impact on children’s cognitive development (Sameroff et al., 1993). Acknowledgements—Data collection was supported by the Bundesministerium fu$ r Forschung und Technik (BMBF) program grants PKE24 und JUG 14 (01EP9504) to K. Riegel, D. Wolke, und B. Ohrt and the data analysis and writing of the manuscript by the German Research Council (DFG) to R. Oerter und D. Wolke (Fkz Oe 71\14-1\14-2). Thanks are due to the participating neonatal hospitals in South Bavaria and the University of Munich Children’s Hospital (study centre), the psychologists who conducted the screening and depression interviews or carried out the cognitive assessments. We especially thank Patricia Rios, who checked for objectivity and reliability of psychometric assessments, Sabine Rieder, who reviewed the tape-recorded interviews for objectivity and reliability of administration and scoring, Renate Meyer, Brigitte So$ hne, and Norbert Link, who helped with database maintenance in the BLS, and the parents and children who 633 participated so willingly. Part of the data presented has been included in a Doctoral thesis (Dr Biol) by the first author under the supervision of the second author to the Medical Faculty of the Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich. References Achenbach, T. M., Howell, C. T., Aoki, M. F., & Rauh, V. A. (1993). Nine-year outcome of the Vermont Intervention Program for low birthweight infants. Pediatrics, 91, 45–55. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC : Author. Aylward, G. P. (1992). The relationship between environmental risk and developmental outcome. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 13, 222–229. Bifulco, A., Brown, G. W., Morgan, P., Ball, C., & Campbell, C. (1998). Predicting depression in women : The role of past and present vulnerability. Psychological Medicine, 28, 39–50. Blanz, B., Schmidt, M. H., & Esser, G. (1991). Familial adversities and child psychiatric disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 939–950. Brandt, I. (1983). Griffiths Entwicklungsskalen (GES ) zur Beurteilung der Entwicklung in den ersten beiden Lebensjahren. Weinheim, Germany : Beltz. Burgemeister, B., Blum, L., & Lorge, J. (1972). Columbia Mental Maturity Scale. New York : Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Campbell, F. A., & Ramey, C. T. (1994). Effects of early intervention on intellectual and academic achievement : A follow-up study of children from low-income families. Child Development, 65, 684–698. Campbell, S. B., & Cohn, J. F. (1997). The timing and chronicity of postpartum depression : Implications for infant development. In L. Murray & P. J. Cooper (Eds.), Postpartum depression and child development (pp. 165–197). London : Guilford Press. Campbell, S. B., Cohn, J., & Meyers, T. (1995). Depression in first-time mothers : Mother-infant interaction and depression chronicity. Developmental Psychology, 31, 349–357. Cicchetti, D. (1996). Regulatory processes in development and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 1–2. Cogill, S., Caplan, H., Alexandra, H., Robson, K., & Kumar, K. (1986). Impact of maternal postnatal depression on cognitive development of young children. British Medical Journal, 292, 1165–1167. Cohn, J. F., & Campbell, S. B. (1992). Influence of maternal depression on infant affect regulation. In D. Cichetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychopathology : Vol. 4. A developmental approach to affective disorders (pp. 103–130). Rochester, NY : University of Rochester Press. Cooper, P., Campbell, E., Day, A., Kennerley, H., & Bond, A. (1988). Non-psychotic psychiatric disorder after childbirth. A prospective study of prevalence, incidence, course and nature. British Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 799–806. Cooper, P., & Murray, L. (1995). Course and recurrence of postnatal depression : Evidence of the specifity of the diagnostic concept. British Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 191–195. Crittenden, P. M., & Bonvillian, J. D. (1984). The relationship between maternal risk status and maternal sensitivity. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 54, 250–262. Cummings, E., & Davies, P. (1994). Maternal depression and child development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 73–112. DeMulder, E. K., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1991). Attachment with affectively ill and well mothers : Concurrent behavioral correlates. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 227–242. Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1994). Socialization mediators of the relation between socioeconomic status and child conduct problems. Child Development, 65, 649–665. Duncan, G. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, P. K. (1994). Economic deprivation and early childhood development. Child Development, 65, 296–318. 634 S. KURSTJENS and D. WOLKE Dunham, P. J., & Dunham, F. (1990). Effects of mother-infant social interactions on infants’ subsequent contingency task performance. Child Development, 61, 785–793. Eggert, D. (1972). Die Columbia Mental Maturity Scale als Individualtest fu$ r normalentwickelte Kinder im Alter von 3–10 Jahren. In D. Eggert (Ed.), Zur Diagnose der Minderbegabung (pp. 185–201). Weinheim, Germany : Beltz. Endicott, J., & Spitzer, R. (1978). A diagnostic interview: The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 837–844. Felner, R. D., Brand, S., DuBois, D. L., Adan, A. M., Mulhall, P. F., & Evans, E. G. (1995). Socioeconomic disadvantage, proximal environmental experiences, and socioemotional and academic adjustment in early adolescence: Investigation of a mediated effects model. Child Development, 66, 774–792. Field, T. (1992). Infants of depressed mothers. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 49–66. Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., & Guthertz, M. (1990). Behavior-state matching and synchrony in mother-infant interactions of nondepressed versus depressed dyads. Developmental Psychology, 26, 7–14. Griffiths, R. (1976). The abilities of babies: A study in mental measurement. Amersham, U.K. : Association for Research in Infant and Child Development. Grossmann, K. E., Grossmann, K., & Zimmermann, P. (in press). A wider view of attachment and exploration. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment : Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 760–786). New York : Guilford Press. Hashima, P. Y., & Amato, P. R. (1994). Poverty, social support, and parental behavior. Child Development, 65, 394–403. Hay, D. F. (1997). Postpartum depression and cognitive development. In L. Murray & P. J. Cooper (Eds.), Postpartum depression and child development (pp. 85–110). New York : Guilford Press. Hay, D., & Kumar, R. (1995). Interpreting the effects of mothers’ postnatal depression on children’s intelligence : A critique and re-analysis. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 25, 165–181. Hernstein, R., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve : Intelligence and class structure in American life. New York : Free Press. Hobfoll, S. E., Ritter, C., Lavin, J., Hulsizer, M. R., & Cameron, R. P. (1995). Depression prevalence and incidence among inner-city pregnant and postpartum women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 445–453. Hopkins, J., Campbell, S. B., & Marcus, M. (1987). The role of infant-related stressors in postpartum depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 237–241. Kaplan, P., Bachorowski, J., Hoff, A., & Zarlengo-Strouse, P. (in press). A learning deficit in the infants of depressed mothers. Child Development. Kaufman, A., & Kaufman, N. (1983). Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children. Circle Pines, MN : American Guidance Service. Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M., Eshleman, S., Wittchen, H. U., & Kendler, K. S. (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 8–19. Kumar, R., & Robson, K. (1984). A prospective study of emotional disorders in childbearing women. British Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 35–47. Kurstjens, S., & Wolke, D. (2001). Postnatale und spa$ ter auftretende Depressionen bei Mu$ ttern: Pra$ valenz und Zusammenha$ nge mit obstetrischen, soziodemographischen sowie psychosozialen Faktoren. Zeitschrift fuW r Klinische Psychologie, 30, 33–41. Li, A. K. F., Sauve, R. S., & Creighton, D. E. (1990). Early indicators of learning problems in high-risk children. Journal of Development and Behavioral Pediatrics, 11, 1–6. Lieb, R., Lachner, G., Sonntag, H., Pfister, H., Hoefler, M., Gander, F., & Wittchen, H. U. (1998). Projektteil ‘‘ Familiengenetik und familiaW re Sozialisation ’’ (Projekt gefoW rdert durch das BMFBW, F-Kz. 01 EB9405) (Interim report). Munich : Max-Planck-Institute for Psychiatry, Clinical Psychology and Epidemiology, Kraepelinstr. 10, D80804. Lu$ tkenhaus, P., Grossmann, K. E., & Grossmann, K. (1985). Infant-mother attachment at twelve months and style of interaction with a stranger at the age of three years. Child Development, 56, 1538–1542. Matas, L., Arend, R., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Continuity of adaptation in the second year : The relationship between quality of attachment and later competence. Child Development, 49, 547–556. McCall, R. B., Appelbaum, M. I., & Hogarty, P. S. (1973). Developmental changes in mental performance. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38 (Serial No. 150). Meins, E. (1997). Security of attachment and social development of cognition. Hove, U.K. : Psychology Press. Melchers, P., & Preuß, U. (1991). Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children K.ABC (German Version). Amsterdam : Swets & Zeitlinger. Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Harkness, A. R., & Silva, P. A. (1993). The natural history of change in intellectual performance : Who changes? How much? Is it meaningful? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 441–453. Molfese, V. J., DiLalla, L. F., & Bunce, D. (1997). Prediction of the intelligence test scores of 3- to 8-year old children by home environment, socioeconomic status and biomedical risks. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 43, 219–234. Morisset, C. E., Barnard, K. E., Greenberg, M. T., Booth, C. L., & Spiker, S. J. (1990). Environmental influences on early language development: The context of social risk. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 127–149. Murray, L. (1992). The impact of postnatal depression on infant development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 543–561. Murray, L., & Cartwright, W. (1993). The role of obstetric factors in postpartum depression. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 11, 215–219. Murray, L., & Cooper, P. (1996). The impact of postpartum depression on child development. International Review of Psychiatry, 8, 55–63. Murray, L., & Cooper, P. (1997). Postpartum depression and child development. New York : Guilford Press. Murray, L., Fiori-Cowley, A., Hooper, R., & Cooper, P. (1996). The impact of postnatal depression and associated adversity on early mother–infant interactions and later infant outcome. Child Development, 67, 2512–2526. Murray, L., Hipwell, A., Hooper, R., Stein, A., & Cooper, P. (1996). The cognitive development of five year old children of postnatally depressed mothers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 927–936. Murray, L., Kempton, C., Woolgar, M., & Hooper, R. (1993). Depressed mothers’ speech to their infants and its relation to infant gender and cognitive development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1083–1101. Murray, L., Stanley, C., Hooper, R., King, F., & Fiori-Cowley, A. (1996). The role of infant factors in postnatal depression and mother-infant interactions. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 38, 109–119. Nieland, M., & Rogers, D. (1997). Symptoms in postpartum and non-postpartum samples : Implications for postnatal depression. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 15, 31–42. O’Hara, M. W. (1997). The nature of postpartum depression. In L. Murray & P. J. Cooper (Eds.), Postpartum depression and child development (pp. 3–31). London : Guilford Press. O’Hara, M. W., Schlechte, J. A., Lewis, D. A., & Varner, M. (1991). A controlled study of postpartum mood disorders : Psychological, environmental, and hormonal variables. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 63–73. MATERNAL DEPRESSION AND CHILD COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT O’Hara, M., & Swain, A. (1996). Rates and risk of postpartum depression—A meta-analysis. International Review of Psychiatry, 8, 37–54. Parker, S., Greer, S., & Zuckerman, B. (1988). Double jeopardy : The impact of poverty on early child development. The Pediatric Clinics of North America, 35, 1227–1240. Puckering, C. (1989). Annotation : Maternal depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 807–817. Radke-Yarrow, M., Cummings, M., Kuczynski, L., & Chapman, M. (1985). Patterns of attachment in two- and threeyear-olds in normal families and in families with parental depression. Child Development, 56, 884–893. Riegel, K., Ohrt, B., Wolke, D., & O$ sterlund, K. (1995). Die Entwicklung gefaW hrdet geborener Kinder bis zum fuW nften Lebensjahr. Die Arvo-YlppoW -Neugeborenen-Nachfolgestudie in SuW dbayern und SuW dfinnland. Stuttgart, Germany : Enke Verlag. Rutter, M. (1981). Maternal deprivation reassessed (2nd ed.). Harmondsworth, U.K. : Penguin. Rutter, M. (1985). Family and school influences on cognitive development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 683–704. Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57, 316–331. Rutter, M. (1990). Commentary : Some focus and process considerations regarding effects of parental depression on children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 60–67. Rutter, M. (1997). Maternal depression and infant development : Cause and consequence ; sensitivity and specificity. In L. Murray & P. J. Cooper (Eds.), Postpartum depression and child development (pp. 295–315). London : Guilford Press. Rutter, M., & Rutter, M. (1992). Developing minds : Challenge and continuity across the life span. Harmondsworth, U.K. : Penguin. Rutter, M., & The English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA) Study Team. (1998). Developmental catch-up, and deficit, following adoption after severe global early privation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 465–476. Sameroff, A. J., Seifer, R., Baldwin, A., & Baldwin, C. (1993). Stability of intelligence from preschool to adolescence : The influence of social and family risk factors. Child Development, 64, 80–97. Sharp, D., Hay, D., Pawlby, S., Schmu$ cker, G., Allen, H., & Kumar, R. (1995). The impact of postnatal depression on boys’ intellectual development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 1315–1336. Shaw, D. S., & Emery, R. E. (1987). Parental conflict and other correlates of the adjustment of school-age children whose parents have separated. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 269–281. Skuse, D. (1984). Extreme deprivation in early childhood—II. Theoretical issues and a comparative review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 25, 543–572. Spitzer, R., Endicott, J., & Robbins, E. (1978). Research diagnostic criteria : Rationale and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 773–782. Stanton, W. R., McGee, R., & Silva, P. A. (1991). Indices of perinatal complications, family background, child rearing, and health as predictors of early cognitive and motor development. Pediatrics, 88, 954–959. Steele, S. (1990). Quantitative and qualitative home assessment of primary caretaker-child interactions in lower socioeconomic families. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 13, 127–140. Stein, A., Gath, D., Bucher, J., Bond, A., Day, A., & Cooper, P. (1991). The relationship between post-natal depression and mother-child interaction. British Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 46–52. Stevenson, J., & Fredman, G. (1990). The social environmental correlates of reading abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 681–698. Teti, D. M., & Gelfand, D. M. (1997). Maternal cognitions as 635 mediators of child outcomes in the context of postpartum depression. In L. Murray & P. J. Cooper (Eds.), Postpartum depression and child development (pp. 136–164). London : Guilford Press. Teti, D. M., Gelfand, D. M., Messinger, D. S., & Isabella, R. (1995). Maternal depression and the quality of early attachment : An examination of infants, preschoolers, and their mothers. Developmental Psychology, 31, 364–376. Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–119. Tronick, E. Z., & Field, T. (1987). Maternal depression and infant disturbance. San Francisco : Jossey-Bass. Tronick, E. Z., & Weinberg, M. K. (1997). Depressed mothers and infants : Failure to form dyadic states of consciousness. In L. Murray & P. J. Cooper (Eds.), Postpartum depression and child development (pp. 54–81). London : Guilford Press. Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Dijkstra, J., & Bus, A. G. (1995). Attachment, intelligence, and language : A meta-analysis. Social Development, 4, 115–128. Van Ijzendoorn, M., Goldberg, S., Kroonenberg, P. M., & Frenkel, O. J. (1992). The relative effects of maternal and child problems on the quality of attachment : A meta-analysis of attachment in clinical samples. Child Development, 63, 840–858. Walker, D., Greenwood, C., Hart, B., & Carta, J. (1994). Prediction of school outcomes based on early language production and socioeconomic factors. Child Development, 65, 606–621. Weinberg, W. A., Dietz, S. G., Penick, E. C., & McAlister, W. H. (1974). Intelligence, reading achievement, physical size, and social class. The Journal of Pediatrics, 85, 482–489. Weisglas-Kuperus, N., Baerts, W., Smrkovsky, M., & Sauer, P. J. J. (1993). Effects of biological and social factors on the cognitive development of very low birthweight children. Pediatrics, 92, 658–665. Weissman, M. M., Bland, R. C., Canino, G. J., Faravelli, C., Greenwald, S., Hwu, H. G., Joyce, P. R., Karam, E. G., Lee, C. K., Lellouch, J., Lepine, J. P., Newman, S. C., RubioStipec, M., Wells, J. E., Wickramaratne, P. J., Wittchen, H., & Yeh, E. K. (1996). Cross-national epidemiology of major depression and bipolar disorder. JAMA, 276, 293–299. Weissmann, M., John, K., Merikangas, K., Prusoff, B., Wickramaratne, P., Gammon, G., Angold, A., & Warner, V. (1986). Depressed parents and their children. General health, social, and psychiatric problems. AJDC, 140, 801–805. Whiffen, V., & Gotlib, I. (1989). Infants of postpartum depressed mothers : Temperament and cognitive status. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 274–279. Wickberg-Johansson, B., Erlandsson, B., & Hwang, C. (1996). Primary health care management of postnatal depression in Sweden. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 14, 69–76. Winnicott, D. W. (1960). The theory of the parent-infant relationship. In M. Khan (Ed.), The international psychoanalytical library. The maturational processes and the facilitating environment : Studies in the theory of emotional development. London : Hogarth. Wolke, D. (1997). Entwicklung sehr Fru$ hgeborener bis zum 7. Lebensjahr. In T. Horstmann & C. Leyendecker (Eds.), FruW hfoW rderung und FruW hbehandlung—wissenschaftliche Grundlagen, praxisorientierte AnsaW tze und Perspektiven interdisziplinaW rer Zusammenarbeit (pp. 271–288). Heidelberg, Germany : Universita$ tsverlag C. Winter. Wolke, D. (1998). The psychological development of prematurely born children. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 78, 567–570. Wolke, D., Leo! n-Villagra! , J., & Meyer, R. (1993). Psychologisches Eltern-Interview zur Kindesentwicklung im Grundschulalter PIK-G. Unpublished manuscript. Wolke, D., & Meyer, R. (1999a). Cognitive status, language attainment and pre-reading skills of 6 year-old very preterm children and their peers: The Bavarian Longitudinal Study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41, 94–109. 636 S. KURSTJENS and D. WOLKE Wolke, D., & Meyer, R. (1999b). Ergebnisse der Bayerischen Entwicklungsstudie: Implikationen fu$ r Theorie und Praxis. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 8, 24–36. Wolke, D., Meyer, R., Ohrt, B., & Riegel, K. (1995). The incidence of sleeping problems in preterm and full-term infants discharged from neonatal special care units : An epidemiological longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 203–223. Wolke, D., Ratschinski, G., Ohrt, B., & Riegel, K. (1994). The cognitive outcome of very preterm infants may be poorer than often reported : An empirical investigation of how methodological issues make a big difference. European Journal of Pediatrics, 153, 906–915. Wolke, D., So$ hne, B., Riegel, K., Ohrt, B., & O$ sterlund, K. (1998). An epidemiological study of sleeping problems and feeding experiences of preterm and full-term children in South Finland : Comparison to a South German population sample. Journal of Pediatrics, 133, 224–231. Zimmermann, P., Suess, G. J., Scheuerer-Englisch, H., & Grossmann, K. E. (1999). Bindung und Anpassung von der fruehen Kindheit bis zum Jugendalter : Ergebnisse der Bielefelder und Regensburger Laengsschnittstudie. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 8, 36–48. Manuscript accepted 4 December 2000 Appendix Psychological Symptoms in the Family Adversity Index (FAI ) or the Psychological Stress Index (PSI ) (1 item), or the FAI and PSI (Both items) at 5 Months, 20 Months, and 4 ; 8 years According to Timing of Depression and Control Mothers Psychological problems At birth or 5 No item 1 item Both items 20 monthsb No item 1 item Both items 4 ; 8 yearsc No item 1 item Both items a b c Postnatal depression (N l 44) Late depression (N l 48) Control mothers (N l 721) 14 (31n8 %) 6 (13n6 %) 24 (54n5 %) 25 (52n1 %) 10 (20n8 %) 13 (27n1 %) 484 (67n7 %) 124 (17n3 %) 107 (15n0 %) n001 12 (27n3 %) 16 (36n4 %) 16 (36n4 %) 21 (44n7 %) 17 (36n2 %) 9 (19n1 %) 464 (65n0 %) 199 (27n9 %) 51 (7n1 %) n001 15 (34n9 %) 15 (34n9 %) 13 (30n2 %) 20 (44n4 %) 8 (17n8 %) 17 (37n8 %) 539 (77n0 %) 114 (16n3 %) 47 (6n7 %) n001 monthsa Missing : 6. Missing : 7. Missing : 20. p