A rodent model for detection of oxygen based explosives and propellants Marina Vilardo, Ben Katz and Kimberly Kirkpatrick Rearing response (RR) training, and search and odor detection (SOD) training: Butyric acid group (n = 4) and MEK group (n = 4) Phase Pre-Training RR1 RR2 Baseline SOD1a/SOD1b SOD2 SOD3 Description Acclimation to the apparatus Shaping of the rearing response Target odor training (with sucrose) Target odor alone; 6 sessions x 6 trials Target + one distracter odor; 8 sessions x 6 trials Target + 2 distracter odors; 8 sessions x 6 trials Target + 5 distracter odors; 8 sessions x 6 trials Results (Continued) 80 H (Butyric acid) FA (Propionic acid) FA (Acetic acid) FA (MEK) FA (Acetone) FA A ((Hydrogen d peroxide) id ) FA (empty) 60 40 20 0 Percent Hits/False Alarms 80 60 H (Butryic acid) FA (empty) 40 20 100 Peercent Hits/False Alarmss Baseline: Butyric acid 100 Baseline: MEK 80 60 H (MEK) FA (empty) 40 20 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 3 4 5 6 Sessions Sessions Figure 1. Rats in both groups showed initial false alarms to empty cups, but by Session 3 were performing at a high level of accuracy in identifying the target odor. 0 1 2 SOD 1a: Butryic acid 100 80 60 H (Butyric acid) FA (Acetic acid) 40 FA (empty) 20 100 SOD 1a: MEK 80 60 40 H (MEK) FA (Acetone) FA (empty) 20 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 4 5 6 7 8 Sessions Sessions Figure 2. Both the groups showed initial false alarms to the novel distracters, but then acquired high hit rates by the end of SOD 1a. There were very few false alarms to empty cups. 0 1 2 3 SOD 3: MEK 80 60 40 20 H (MEK) FA (Acetone) FA (Hydrogen peroxide) FA (Butyric acid) FA (Propionic acid) FA (A (Acetic i acid) id) FA (empty) 0 4 5 6 7 8 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Sessions Sessions Figure 3. During the final test phase (SOD 3) with all five distracters, both groups maintained high-level accuracy, showing few false alarms even to the novel odors from the other group of compounds. This indicates that the rats can generalize li their th i performance f to t the th target t t compounds d even when h challenged with novel distracters. 1 Results 100 Percent Hits/False Alarms SOD 3: Butyric Acid 100 Percent Hits/False Alarms Apparatus: •4 x 4 (16 holes) open field maze matrix with four entry doors •Cups recessed in the holes •Randomly selected cup(s) contained a vial with an odor-infused odor infused cotton ball •All cups filled with bedding Perccent Hits/False Alarms Method Method (Continued) Perccent Hits/False Alarms Introduction Explosives containing peroxide, such as acetone peroxide and methyl ethyl ketone peroxide (MEKP) have been used in a number of recent terrorist attacks. Liquid explosives and explosives buried in plastic containers, such as improvised explosive devices (IEDs) used by insurgents in Afghanistan, Afghanistan are hard to detect with metal detectors. Currently the Hero Rat project (www.herorat.org) uses Gambian pouched rats to search and alert for buried landmines in several African countries. Previous studies have shown that laboratory rats can be trained to detect nitrogen-based explosive l i odors d (Marshall, (M h ll Doty, D L Lucero, and d Slotnick, Sl i k 1981; Nolan, Weinstein, and Weinstein, 1978; Weinstein, Weinstein, and Drozdenko, 1992), and contraband items (Otto, Brown, and Long, 2002). The present study examined the ability of Sprague-Dawley rats to successfully y search, discriminate, and alert for several explosives and propellants - Butyric acid (10%), Propionic acid (10%), Acetic acid (10%), MEK (10%), Acetone (20%), and Hydrogen peroxide (20%). KANSAS STATE UNIVERSITY 2 3 Conclusions The present results indicated that laboratory rats can be trained to detect and discriminate among substances that are relatives of ingredients g used in liquid q explosives. Initially, the rats made false alarm errors to empty cups (Figure 1) and to the first distracter odor (Figure 2) they encountered. However, with a relatively small amount of training (18-36 trials), they were able to inhibit errors and display a high hit-rate for both targets. targets Once they had learned to inhibit false alarms in SOD 1a (Figure 2), they were able to transfer their training to previously non-presented odors (SOD3; Figure 3). The results indicate that laboratory rats are a potentially viable model for training search and alert behaviors for oxygen-based explosives. R f References Marshall, D., Doty, R., Lucero, D., & Slotnick, B. (1981). Odor detection in the rat for the vapours of three related perflourocarbons and ethylene glycol dinitrate. Chemical Senses, 6, 421-433. Nolan, R., Weinstein, S., & Weinstein, C. (1978). Electroencephalographic studies of specifically-conditioned explosives detecting rats. In: Proceedings of the New Concepts Symposium and Workshop on Detecting and Identification of Explosives, pp. 201-205. Otto, J., Brown, M. F., & Long III, W. (2002). Training rats to search and alert on contraband odors. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 77, 217-232. Weinstein, S., Weinstein, C., & Drozdenko, R. (1992). The challenge of biodetection for screening persons carrying explosives. In: Proceedings of the First International Symposium on Explosive DetectionTechnology. FAA Technical Center, Alantic City International Airport, NJ. Correspondence may be addressed to Kimberly Kirkpatrick (kirkpatr@ksu.edu) or Marina Vilardo (chaton@ksu.edu), Department of Psychology, Kansas State University, Manhattan, KS 66506