Distribution of the Relatives of Children with SLI and their MLU-Matched Peers Brian Weiler Magdalene Jacobs Lisa Wisman Weil C. Melanie Schuele Vanderbilt University School of Medicine Purdue University INTRODUCTION RESULTS PURPOSE • Understanding the complex syntax profile of children with SLI is critical to designing appropriate interventions, especially those related to classroom dialogue. This study was conducted to determine whether the proportional distribution of relative types produced by children with SLI differs from that produced by children with typical language. • Children with specific language impairment (SLI) have weaker production of complex syntax than age-matched peers (e.g., Barako Arndt & Schuele, 2012; Owen & Leonard, 2006; Eisenberg, 2004). Given that children with SLI have difficulties with syntactically complex productions, the current study specifically considers whether children with SLI, when using relatives, show greater proportional use of propositionally less complex structures than their MLU-matched peers. • Children with SLI struggle with a grammatical requirement of relative clauses. • They omit obligatory relative markers to a much greater extent and for a protracted period of time compared to MLUmatched peers. • In an elicited language task, children with SLI between 5 7 years of age often omitted obligatory relative markers (e.g., that) in subject relative clauses (e.g., I like the dog ___ is brown.); whereas, a group of typical language learners between 3 - 5 years of age never omitted obligatory relative markers (Schuele & Tolbert, 2001). • Less understood is the extent to which children with SLI might also differ from MLU-matched peers in the versatility of their production of different relative clause types. • Relative types can be categorized according to the syntactic role of either the head noun or the relativized noun phrase (Diessel and Tomasello, 2000). • The current study focuses on five different types of head nouns defined in the below table, where cat is the head noun of the relative clause. FINDING 1 The proportional distribution of total relative types used by children with SLI and MLU-matched peers was comparable. PARTICIPANTS SLI M SD Range Age SPELT-2 CMMS (Z-score) (SS) Finding 2 • The proportion of PN-relatives produced by the MLU group decreased with age in a manner consistent with that of the four children with typical language reported on by Diessel and Tomasello (2000).The development of relatives in the spontaneous language of children with typical language appears to follow a pattern of relative clause expansion beyond an early reliance on PN-relatives. Number of Utterances 6;6 10 mos. 5;2 – 7;10 -3.5 2.11 -7.70 to -1.25 99.48 7.15 87-116 200 68.29 97 – 364 M 4;7 -0.01 108.14 207.4 SD 7 mos. 0.52 8.26 61.45 Range 3;2-5;6 -1.00 to 1.00 87 - 124 96 – 364 Percent of Total Utterances Containing Relatives SLI: 1.7% (73/4200) MLU: 1.4% (68/4770) MLU METHODS • A logical next step in this line of research would involve investigation of the spontaneous production of relative types by children with typical language who are age-matched to the children with SLI. It is important to determine whether, at the age of moving into academic language tasks, the range and versatility with which children with SLI modify noun phrases is different from their classmates. FINDING 2 • Within the spontaneous language of typical language learners, Diessel and Tomasello (2000) reported a 71% to 37% decrease in the proportion of PN-relatives to all other relative types between the ages of 3 to 5. • They speculate that the early proportional frequency of PNrelatives may be related to three factors: Whereas the proportional frequency of PN-relatives decreased with age for MLU-matches, a finding consistent with Diessel and Tomasello (2000), it remained unchanged for children with SLI. Children also completed an elicited subject relative clause production task (adapted from Hamburger and Crain, 1982; Schuele, 1995). The findings from this task reflect those from Schuele and Tolbert (2001), namely that children with SLI omit obligatory relative markers more frequently than younger MLU-matched peers with typical language. • Children with SLI, on the other hand, did not show an agerelated decline in PN productions. Whether this finding is related to a prolonged dependence on propositionally simple relative constructions among children with SLI or is simply related to the specific pragmatic demands of the conversational context is a question worthy of future investigation. FUTURE DIRECTIONS Relative clause types were classified by consensus between all four authors using a coding scheme adapted from Diessel (2004) and Diessel and Tomasello (2000). • Diessel and Tomasello (2005) posit a bottom-up sequence of relative clause acquisition which begins with single proposition constructions where the predicate nominal (PN) of a presentational copular clause is modified by the relative clause (e.g., This is the cat that chases mice). Children’s repertoire of relative productions later expands to more complex constructions containing two propositions (e.g., The cat that chases mice eats cheese). Finding 1 • Although children with SLI struggle with the grammatical requirements of relative clauses – omitting obligatory relative markers in subject relatives to a greater extent than MLUmatched peers – they show similar range and versatility in production across relative types, suggesting relative clause structure on a broad level may not be problematic. • Complexity of propositional structure in relatives did not readily distinguish the spontaneous productions of children with SLI from MLU-matched peers (PN: χ2 (1) = 2.40, p > .10). Examiner-child conversational language samples were collected (Hadley, 1998) and analyzed to study the complex syntax development of children with SLI. • 21 children with SLI • 23 typically developing children matched on MLU (+/-.2) Group DISCUSSION • Longitudinal case studies would permit examination of complex syntax trajectories over time within subjects rather than between subjects. • Future studies should be designed with a methodology that considers the influence of adult input, propositional complexity, and pragmatic communicative function on the production of relative types. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Data collection was supported by the ASHFoundation and the Schubert Child Development Center at Case Western Reserve University (PI: Schuele). Completion of this study and preparation of this poster were supported by a Preparation of Leadership Personnel grant (H325D080075; PI: Schuele), U.S. Department of Education. 1. PNs are frequently occurring in adult input provided to children. 2. The pragmatic function of PNs may correspond to early communicative needs. 3. PNs are less propositionally complex than other relative types that express relationships between two situations. LIST OF CITED REFERENCES AVAILABLE ON REQUEST http://www.mc.vanderbilt.edu/languagelab Poster Presented at the Symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders Madison, WI 2013