Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Case Study 1: An Evidence-Based Practice Review Report Theme: Interventions for children with Special Educational Needs From a parent’s perspective, is cognitive-behavioural treatment an effective intervention for children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder? Summary Cognitive-behavioural treatment is a theoretical and evidence-based intervention that addresses dysfunctional thoughts and attributions, maladaptive behaviours and cognitive processes through goal-orientated and systematic procedures in either group or individual therapy sessions. Cognitive-behavioural treatment aims to teach children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) strategies to help them increase their self-control and problem-solving abilities, through modelling, role playing and self-instruction. effectiveness regarding Cognitive-behavioural treatment has shown mixed ADHD behaviours, yet still remains a consistent recommendation of the National Collaborating Centre of Mental Health (NICE) guidelines when treating children and young people with ADHD. In light of these discordant views, combined with the fact that some research is now dated, a reconsideration of the area is necessary. In particular, this review will focus on the parental perspective. Five studies were evaluated in this review. All five studies found that the intervention had a positive effect on ADHD behaviours from the parent’s point of view. Those that followed up at later points also reported encouraging results. A number of issues including methodological quality and generalisability are raised, but overall it is recommended that cognitive-behavioural treatment continue to be considered appropriate for treating children with ADHD. 1 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Introduction Essential elements of cognitive-behavioural treatment The cognitive-behavioural model highlights the central role of thoughts and attributions in understanding an individual’s emotional and behavioural life. The core techniques involve helping individuals to identify patterns of thinking that interfere with their optimal functioning (Levine & Anshel, 2011). No single set of methods defines cognitive-behavioural treatment. However, treatment is governed by two overarching main themes: the conviction that cognitive processes influence emotion, motivation and behaviour and the use of cognitive and behaviour-change techniques in a pragmatic (hypothesis-testing) manner (Butcher, Mineka, & Hooley, 2010). The paradigm also includes an emphasis on education, arguing that giving people information about their diagnosis encourages the development of independent problem-solving skills (Flanagan & Miller, 2010). Cognitive-behavioural treatment in relation to children with ADHD Due to some limited effects of psychostimulant medications on decreasing the cardinal symptoms of ADHD (Hechtman, Weiss, & Perlman, 1984; Gittelman & Kanner, 1986) researchers explored alternative treatment approaches such as cognitive-behavioural treatment. Carried out by a therapist, cognitive-behavioural treatment sessions attempt to teach children strategies to help them increase their self-control and problem-solving abilities, through modelling, role playing and selfinstruction (Kendall & Braswell, 1985; Kendall, Padever, & Zupan, 1980) to: (1) define the nature of the problem; (2) reflect on all the possible solutions; and (3) choose one solution and evaluate its outcome (Kendall & Braswell, 1985). 2 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Most current models of ADHD emphasize the deficiencies in the executive functioning skills of behavioural inhibition and self-regulation (Barkley, 2006). These deficiencies in turn manifest themselves in overt behaviours, such as sustaining attention to academic tasks and inhibiting excessive motor activity. Cognitivebehavioural proponents recognise the goodness of fit between these highlighted deficits in children with ADHD and the self-regulatory focus of cognitive-behavioural treatment (Hinshaw & Melnick, 1992). Relatedly, the proponents of cognitivebehavioural approach contend that the maintenance of treatment gains can be achieved only through teaching a generalised set of cognitive mediational (self-talk) skills that children with ADHD can internalise. Basis in psychological theory The cognitive-behavioural perspective can take a variety of forms and draws on a range of psychological theories. For example, Bandura’s theory of self-efficacy - the view that one can achieve desired goals (1986, 1997) and that the beliefs individuals hold about their capabilities have a strong influence on the ways in which they behave (Usher & Pajares, 2008) - is an early example of a cognitive-behavioural perspective. He posited that cognitive-behavioural treatments work in large part by improving self-efficacy. Today, the cognitive-behavioural paradigms often follow the model proposed by Beck (1964), which emphasises the central role of thoughts and attributions in understanding an individual’s emotional and behavioural life. Beck’s model makes the assumption that problems result from biased processing of external events or internal stimuli. These biases distort the way that person makes sense of the experiences he or she has in the world, leading to cognitive errors. Beck (2005) argues that underlying these biases is a relatively stable set of schemas that contain 3 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth dysfunctional beliefs. When the schemas become activated they bias information processing. The central focus in treatment is therefore to alter distorted and maladaptive cognitions and their underlying schemas; this is achieved through making individuals aware of and exploring the connections between thoughts and emotional responses and later learning and practicing strategies to better deal with difficult external events or internal stimuli. In the case of ADHD for example, the cognitive-behavioural approach helps pupils to understand links between thoughts, feelings and behaviours and that these may result in unhelpful, inappropriate or maladaptive consequences. The therapy also explores learning to change these thoughts, feelings and behaviours to produce more desirable outcomes (NICE, 2009). Rationale ADHD is repeatedly singled out as a challenge for teachers, parents and psychologists, because of the behavioural characteristics commonly demonstrated by children with this disorder (Langberg, Froelich, Loren, Martin, & Epstein, 2008). Because ADHD is associated with poor peer relations and negative self-image (Horn, Ialongo, Greenberg, Packard, & Smith-Winberry, 1990), children with ADHD are at greater risk as adolescents and adults of several social and emotional problems, including substance abuse and depression (Gray, Riggs, Min, MikulichGilbertson, Bandyopadhyay, & Winhusen, 2011; Tamm, Trello-Rishel, Riggs, Nakonezny, Acosta, Bailey, & Winhusen, 2013), and the frequency of academic and career problems is significantly higher in these children compared with the normal population (Jensen, Mrazek, Knapp, Steinberg, Pfeffer, Schowalter, & Shapiro, 1997; Springer, Phillips, Phillips, Cannady, & Kerst-Harris, 1992). 4 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Findings on the effectiveness of cognitive-behavioural interventions on ADHD behaviours have been mixed. Literature demonstrates the limitations of cognitivebehavioural interventions in directly targeting central ADHD impairments (Abikoff, 1987; Abikoff, 1991). Indeed, despite some early claims of success (e.g. Cameron & Robinson, 1980), systematic investigations aimed at comparing the benefits of cognitively based interventions with stimulant medications have demonstrated the superiority of the latter (Abikoff, Ganeles, Reiter, Blum, Foley, & Klein, 1988). However, other studies show that cognitive-behaviour techniques are effective in moderating impulsivity (Kendall & Braswell, 1982). A recent review by MunozSolomando, Kendall, & Whittington (2008) also suggests that cognitive-behavioural interventions can have beneficial effects delivered in absence of medication or as adjunct to continued routine medication for children with ADHD. Furthermore, current NICE guidelines retain their support for cognitive-behavioural interventions for children with ADHD; they consider group-based cognitive-behavioural interventions to be both effective with children with ADHD and cost efficient. In light of these discordant views, combined with the fact that many reviews are now dated, a reconsideration of the area is necessary. The parent’s role in supporting the child is crucial, yet literature highlights that parents of children with ADHD often struggle to manage their child’s problems, suffering from stress and exhaustion (Green, McGinnity, Meltzer, Ford, & Goodman, 2005). Further, recent legislation has highlighted the importance of involving parents in treatment options and the treatment process, making the parent’s perspective of particular relevance to current practice. As such, this review will focus on the parent’s perspective. 5 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Review Question From a parent’s perspective, is cognitive-behavioural treatment an effective intervention for children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder? Critical Review of Evidence Base Literature Search The current review was carried out in December 2013. PsychInfo, ERIC and Medline (Ovid) databases were searched using the search terms: cognitive-behavioural* or cognitive-behavioral* or cognitive behavioural* or cognitive behavioral* or CB* AND attention deficit hyperactivity disorder OR ADHD AND adolescen* OR child* OR youth* The use of both UK and US written English was used to ensure relevant studies were not overlooked. These search terms, in the abstract, gave a total of 1,054 results (531, 334, and 189 respectively for each database) some of which overlapped between databases. Of these 1,044 were rejected by title or abstract, ten remained to inspect in full. Five were excluded based on the inclusion criteria in Table 1; please see Appendix A and C for the specific reasons for exclusion, as well as a rationale for each inclusion criterion. Five studies remained from this process. Ancestral searches were carried out on these five studies to further ensure that relevant papers were not overlooked. Four studies were highlighted from the title and inspected in full. All four were excluded based on the inclusion criteria (please see Appendix D). The full literature search is illustrated in a flow diagram (see Appendix B). The five studies included in this review can be viewed in Table 2 and are summarised in Appendix E. 6 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Table 1 Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria 1. Intervention Inclusion Criteria Exclusion Criteria A cognitive-behavioural approach targeted at ADHD delivered to child. Not a cognitive-behavioural approach targeted at ADHD or delivered to child. Ability to isolate cognitivebehavioural treatment effect on dependent measures from other interventions that do not target ADHD or are not delivered to child. 2. Variable A variable measuring ADHD symptoms from the parent perspective Unable to isolate cognitivebehavioural treatment effect on dependent measures from other interventions that do not target ADHD or are not delivered to child. No variable measuring ADHD symptoms from the parent perspective 3. Participant Age 19 and under Over 19 4. Participant Diagnosis A diagnosis of ADHD, diagnosed by DSM-III-R – DSM 5 or European equivalent (e.g. ICD-10). No diagnosis of ADHD, diagnosed by DSM-III-R – DSM 5 or European equivalent (e.g. ICD-10). 5. Participant Other Can be on medication for ADHD On medication for other disorders No diagnosis of other considerable and/or distinct disorders Diagnosis of other considerable disorders. 6. Type of study Empirical study containing primary data. A review, meta-analyses, pilot studies or use secondary data. 7. Publication type Published in a peer reviewed journal. Unpublished work or grey literature such as personal websites or nonpeer reviewed journals. 8. Language Full article available in English Full article not available in English 9. Date Must be published from 1987 to December 2013 Published before 1987 or after December 2013 7 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Table 2 Studies Eligible for the Review Studies Included in the Review 1. Abdollahian, E. Mokhber, N., Balaghi, A., & Moharrari, F. (2013). The effectiveness of cognitive-behavioural play therapy on the symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children aged 7-9 years. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 5 (1), 41 – 46. 2. Fehlings, D. L., Roberts, W., Humphries, T., & Dawe, G. (1991). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Does cognitive behavioral therapy improve home behavior? Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 12 (4), 223 – 228. 3. Froelich, J., Doepfner, M., & Lehmkuhl, G. (2002). Effects of combined cognitive behavioural treatment with parent management training in ADHD. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 30, 111 – 115. 4. Horn, W. F., Ialongo, N., Greenberg, G., Packard, T., & Smith- Winberry, C. (1990). Additive effects of behavioral parent training and self-control therapy with attention deficit hyperactivity disordered children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 19 (2), 98 – 110. 5. Miranda, A., & Jesús Presentación, M. (2000). Efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy in the treatment of children with ADHD, with and without aggressiveness. Psychology in the Schools, 37 (2), 169 – 182. Weight of evidence Gough’s (2007) guidance and a coding protocol (adapted from Kratochwill, 2003 see appendix G) were used to assess the relevance of each paper to the review question. The articles were coded only for the measures that were relevant to answering the review question. The weighting given to each study included in this review is shown in Table 3. The outcomes examined in this review are child behavioural outcomes associated with ADHD, reported by parents. 8 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Table 3 Weight of Evidence (WoE) Study WoE A: Quality of Methodology WoE B: Relevance of Methodology WoE C: Relevance of Evidence to the Review Question WoE D: Overall Weight of Evidence Abdollahian et al. (2013) Medium Medium Medium Medium Fehlings et al. (1991) Medium High High High Froelich et al. (2002) Low Low Medium Low Horn et al. (1990) Low High Medium Medium Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) Low Low Low Low Participants The sample sizes ranged from 18 participants (Froelich, Doepfner, & Lehmkuhl, 2002) to 60 (Horn et al., 1990). However, due in part to portions of study samples having to be excluded (Horn et al., 1990; Miranda & Jesús Presentación, 2000); none of the studies included in this review achieved a ‘sufficiently large N’ (at alpha level .05 and a medium effect size). This suggests that the number of participants in each condition is not necessarily sufficient to yield enough statistical power for detecting effects of cognitive-behavioural treatment on symptoms of children with ADHD. Caution is therefore needed when drawing conclusions from study findings. But in light of the specialised population utilised in all the studies, it is not surprising that sample sizes did not reach above 30 on average. The ages of the participants ranged from 6 to 13 years old. Two of the studies did not state the gender of the participants (see Table 4), and of the remaining studies 81.7% of the sample were 9 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth males and 18.3% were females. This difference supports the documented disproportion of males with ADHD compared to females (Gaub & Carlson, 1997). All participants included in this review had a diagnosis of ADHD (diagnosed from the DSM-III-R or subsequent DSM manuals, or European equivalent). Types of ADHD were not controlled for in this review. Three studies contained a sample of children diagnosed with ADHD alone (in some studies absence of other disorders was made explicit, in others it was not), Abdollahian, Mokhber, Balaghi, & Moharrari’s (2013) sample contained only participants on medication for ADHD and Froelich et al.’s (2002) sample contained two participants with learning difficulties and eight on medication for ADHD. Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) included children with aggressiveness but this part of the sample was not looked at in this review. Children in this study were also receiving anger management therapy (AM) for their ADHD symptoms. As Table 4 shows, no trend was found between studies that contained a sample that included children with learning difficulties and children on medication, and those that did not. The WoE C took into account the sample characteristics; in order to achieve a high weighting the sample of interest had to be typically developing children with the appropriate diagnosis of ADHD who were not receiving additional interventions aimed at ADHD. Table 4 shows that two of the studies successfully adhered to this criterion. Fehlings, Roberts, Humphries, & Dawe (1991) had a highly relevant sample and obtained typically large effect sizes between pre and post-test phases (on significant findings). Horn et al. (1990) similarly had a relevant sample, but found only a medium effect size over time from the Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) parent questionnaire. 10 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Table 4 Key Features of the Review Studies Study Participants Research Design & Intervention Details Outcome Measuresª Post Intervention Effect Sizesᵇ Abdollahian et al. (2013) 7-9, ADHD (all on medication) Quasi-experimental with control/ CB play therapy Rutter rating of ADHD symptoms 0.8 Large Medium Fehlings et al. (1991) 7-13 (all m), ADHD diagnosis only RCT/ CBT WWAS assessing hyperactivity 0.98 Large High SCRS assessing impulsivity (n/s) 0.77 Medium BPC-AP assessing inattention (n/s) 0.65 Medium Yale Children’s Inventory assessing core ADHD symptoms u Yale Children’s Inventory assessing conduct problems u Home Situations Questionnaire for conflict at home u Froelich et al. (2002) 6-12 (17/18 m), ADHD diagnosis,( 2 learning difficulties, 8 medication) Pre- & Post- test (no control)/ CBT + focus on generalisation skills Individual Problem List to assess individually relevant problem behaviour Horn et al. (1990) Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) 7 – 11 ½ 9 (42m, 18f), ADHD diagnosis only 9 – 12 ADHD diagnosis, receiving AM as well RCT/ CB selfcontrol therapy Pre- & Post- test (no control)/ CB self-control (+ AM) 11 WoE D Low u Homework Problem Checklist u CBCL CB SC (Total Problems scale) 0.62 Medium CBCL CB SC (Externalising scale) 0.58 Medium CBCL CB SC (hyperactivity scale) (n/s) 0.17 Small Assessment of hyperactivity on DSMIII-R (non-aggressive ADHD) u Assessment of antisocial behaviour on EPC (non-aggressive ADHD) u Assessment of school problems on EPC (non- u Medium Low Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Study Participants Research Design & Intervention Details Outcome Measuresª Cara Southworth Post Intervention Effect Sizesᵇ WoE D aggressive AD Assessment of internalisation on EPC (n/s) u Assessment of psychopath disorders on EPC u Note.ª n/s refers to a non-significant outcome; ᵇ ‘u’ is marked when effect size is unable to be calculated. The studies included in this review took place in five different countries: Iran, Canada, Germany, USA and Spain. As studies needed participants with a specific condition, they did not take measures to ensure participants came from a diverse range of socio-economic backgrounds, although some studies did report information on the socio-economic status of their participants. When this information was provided it indicated that the vast majority of participants were white and resident to the country of testing. In addition, the samples were small and gender balance was skewed. Generalisations to non-white ethnic populations and females are therefore limited. There is no clear reason why benefits of a cognitive-behavioural approach should be limited to white and male populations, however. Indeed, Abdollahian et al. (2013), Horn et al. (1990) and Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) were able to appropriately deliver cognitive-behavioural treatment to children of both sexes and of varying socio-economic levels and ethnicities. Finally, although two of the studies were carried out in English speaking countries (US and Canada (Toronto), none of the papers reviewed were done in the UK. Caution should be taken therefore before committing resources to implement cognitive-behavioural treatment to support children with ADHD in the UK. 12 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Research Design Fehlings et al. (1991) and Horn et al. (1990) employed a randomised controlled trial, both with active control groups (alternative treatments: supportive therapy and parent behavioural training respectively). Abdollahian et al. (2013) employed a quasiexperimental study design, and Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) and Froelich et al. (2002) employed a pre-test-post-test design, though only Abdollahian et al. (2013) set up a control group comparison. All but Abdollahian et al. (2013) and Froelich et al. (2002) had additional testing at a follow-up time (ranging from 2 to 8 months). Individual participants were the unit of randomisation in the randomised controlled trials, however for the other three studies participants were either matched (Abdollahian et al., 2013) and then allocated to a treatment group, or the allocation procedure was not made explicit: Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) gave little information as to how participants were allocated to each condition and did not have an appropriate control group, and Froelich et al. (2002) did not have a control group. The results of these studies are therefore more likely to be due, in part, to confounding extraneous variables. It is possible that effects observed were due to children being in an intervention group of any sort rather than being involved in cognitive-behavioural treatment. As a result, they obtained a ‘high’ in methodological quality or relevance, or a ‘high’ overall (WoE D). 13 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Intervention Table 5 Comparison of Cognitive-Behavioural Interventions Study Delivery/Length /Duration Procedure Target Behaviours Abdollahian et al. (2013) Therapist - group Cognitive-behavioural play sessions conducted with different thematic activities. Attention, working memory, maintenance, impulsivity, self-control and organisation. Direct instruction on CB strategies: Problem solving broken down to five step process. Home behaviour Predominantly included training in self-instructional, self-monitoring and problemsolving skills. With a focus on generalisation of skills learnt. Core symptoms of ADHD and conduct problems. Instruction in the self-control strategies adapted from Camp and Bash (1981), Kendall and Braswell (1985) and Meichenbaum (1977). Each child was taught a “Problem Solving Plan”. ADHD cardinal features (e.g. externalising and hyperactivity). Applied Kendall et al.’s “Stop and Think” program (1980) with small modifications. Improve concentration and reflection. Eight sessions twice weekly 45 min Fehlings et al. (1991) Therapist – individual & family Twelve sessions twice weekly & eight sessions at home every 2 weeks 60 min & 2 hours Froelich et al. (2002) Therapist - individual Six lessons one per week 60 min Horn et al. (1990) Therapist – group 12 sessions weekly 90 min Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) Therapist – group 22 session over 3 months 60 min The studies varied in regard to the cognitive-behavioural treatment they employed (see Table 5). Four of the studies however, implemented cognitive-behavioural treatment that instructed children in similar strategies. These included: problemsolving, self-instructional training, self-monitoring and self-control therapy. The studies used similar methods of training, such as modelling, role playing and guided practice (Fehlings et al., 1991; Horn et al., 1990; Miranda & Jesús Presentación, 2000). Several also incorporated problem-solving 14 of both academic and Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth interpersonal problems. Froelich et al. (2002) also reported discussing individually relevant academic or social problems. Home assignments and token reinforcement systems were used in all four of the studies mentioned. Parents were taught about CBT in Fehlings et al.’s (1991) and Froelich et al.’s (2002) studies. Finally, particular efforts to facilitate generalisation of the new skills taught were made by Froelich et al. (2002). Froelich et al. achieved significant findings across all measures and the authors felt that emphasis on generalisation of newly acquired skills played a key part in achieving this. Abdollahian et al. (2013), in contrast to the other studies, implemented cognitive-behavioural play therapy. Sessions conducted varied in thematic activities, but the target behaviours were similar, including attention, impulsivity and self-control. Horn et al. (1990) and Fehlings et al. (1991) reported steps to ensure faithfulness to the intervention. It was noted that all therapists were extensively trained, treatment manuals were developed, supervision was provided on a weekly basis and all treatment sessions were audiotaped. In the other three studies there was missing information about intervention fidelity. The interventions were carried out by therapists in all five of the studies. Further details were provided only by two of the studies: Horn et al. (1990) reported that treatment groups were co-led by two advanced level graduate students carrying out clinical psychology training, and eight therapists then provided the treatment in pairs; Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) reported that the interventions were carried out by therapists with PhDs in psychology who were experts in the ‘Stop and Think’ programme. Taken alongside missing integrity to interventions information, concern is warranted over the consistency of intervention delivery. All the above highlights that there is a lack of conformity between interventions across the five studies within this review; as a 15 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth result it is difficult to establish specific aspects of the cognitive-behavioural treatment that could be considered particularly effective and care is required when drawing conclusions. Outcome Measures All of the studies included in this review used questionnaires to gather information from parents. Every study used different questionnaires to measure ADHD symptoms and related behaviour however. Froelich et al. (2002) used an abbreviated German version of the Yale Children’s Inventory to measure parents rating of core ADHD symptoms. Recent studies have found the reliability coefficient for this scale to be high (.94 for internal consistency for the total score). These authors also calculated high reliability coefficients for the other rating scales used in their studies (>.90). Abdollahian et al. (2013) reported that the Rutter Parental Questionnaire used in their study has been found to have an internal validity and reliability of .74 (Rutter, 1976). Finally, Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) reported an alpha coefficient of .88 and a test-retest correlation of .79. However, Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) neglected to provide information about reliability and validity on the second measure they used (adapted DSM-III-R), and the remaining two studies neglected to mention it entirely. As a result, reported effects on these measures should be interpreted with caution. However, many of the measures used in the remaining two studies that neglected to provide information about reliability and validity are widely used, for example the CBCL and the Self-Control Rating Scale (SCRS), and reports of their reliability and validity elsewhere are satisfactory (Barkley, 1990; Chen, Faraone, Biederman, & Tsuang, 1994; Kendall & Wilcox, 1979). Although interparent interrater reliability was reported to be good in the Werry Weiss Activity Scale (Barkley, 1988), no information 16 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth regarding the test-retest reliability or internal consistency was located and no information was found for the Behaviour Problem Checklist-Attention Problem Subscale (BPC-AP). Further, all measures included in this study were parent reports via rating scales. Parental information was the only outcome of interest in this review, but it should be considered that the lack of multi-method data collection in the studies that employed only one questionnaire can lead to a single dimension conceptualisation of outcomes. Findings In all five of the studies, the effectiveness of cognitive-behavioural treatment on ADHD symptoms in children with ADHD, as measured by parents, was determined by the interactive effect between groups (intervention vs. control) and/or over time (pre vs. post intervention) (refer to Appendix E). Significant interactions were found in all studies, but not across all interactions and measures (See Table 4 for post intervention interactions). All five papers found significant interactions, on some or all measures, over time in the experimental group: Abdollahian et al. (2013) and Fehlings et al. (1991) with large effect sizes, and Horn et al. (1990) with medium effect sizes. Fehlings et al. (1991), Horn et al. (1990) and Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) also reported significant differences at follow-up times. Nonsignificant findings were found over time in the control groups in two out of the three studies that had appropriate control groups. Horn et al. (1990) did find a significant difference in the control group over time, but this is likely due to the active ‘alternative’ treatment in this control group (parental behavioural training). Table 6 aims to break down the outcomes looked at by the five studies into four categories: total (ADHD) problems, inattentiveness, hyperactivity and impulsiveness (& conduct). These categories were developed to reflect the outcomes measured in 17 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth the five studies included in this review and the three specific behaviours commonly used as behavioural components of ADHD. As can be seen in the table, the studies that reported total problems as their outcomes (Abdollahian et al., 2013; Froelich et al., 2002; Horn et al., 1990) had medium to large effect sizes, where effect sizes could be calculated. Unfortunately, although many outcomes were reported, only a selection of effect sizes could be calculated. Fehlings et al. (1991) found a significant interaction between cognitive-behavioural treatment and hyperactivity over time with a large effect size. Finally, Horn et al. (1990) reported a significant interaction between externalisation and cognitive-behavioural treatment over time but with only a medium effect size. 18 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Table 6 Comparison of ADHD Behavioural Symptoms Outcome measure Significanceª Effect sizeᵇ (post intervention) WoE D Abdollahian et al. (2013) ADHD symptoms s 0.80 Medium Froelich et al. (2002) Core ADHD symptoms s u Low Horn et al. (1990) Total Problems s 0.62 Medium n/s 0.65 High Study Total behaviours Specific to one of the three core ADHD behaviours: Inattentiveness Fehlings et al. (1991) Inattention Hyperactivity Fehlings et al. (1991) Hyperactivity s 0.98 High Horn et al. (1990) Hyperactivity n/s 0.17 Medium Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) Hyperactivity s u Low n/s 0.77 High Impulsiveness (& conduct) Fehlings et al. (1991) Impulsivity Froelich et al. (2002) Conduct Problems s u Low Froelich et al. (2002) Conflict at home s u Low Froelich et al. (2002) Homework Problems s u Low Froelich et al. (2002) Individually relevant problem behaviour s u Low Horn et al. (1990) Externalising s 0.58 Medium Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) Anti-social behaviour s u Low Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) School Problems s u Low Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) Internalisation n/s u Low Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) Psychopath Disorder s u Low Note.ª n/s refers to a non-significant outcome, s refers to a significant outcome; ᵇ ‘u’ is marked when effect size was unable to be calculated. 19 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Conclusions and Recommendations Conclusions Although grounded in theory, cognitive-behavioural treatment has shown mixed effectiveness for children with ADHD (Abikoff, 1987; Abikoff, 1991). Cognitivebehavioural interventions continue to be recommended by the NICE, however. This systematic review aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of cognitive-behavioural treatments on the behaviours of children with ADHD from the perspective of the parent. Two of the studies achieved overall weightings of medium quality, with one receiving high (Fehlings et al., 1991) and two receiving low (Miranda & Jesús Presentación, 2000; Froelich et al., 2002). Fehlings et al. received a high due to both the relevance of the methodology and to the review question. Many of the specific weightings within studies were medium, but Miranda and Jesús Presentación received a low on all three weightings. Further, Froelich et al. (2002) received a low for both methodological relevance and methodological quality, and Horn et al. (1990) for methodological quality. All the studies included in this review reported that cognitive-behavioural intervention had some positive effect on ADHD behaviours from the parent’s point of view. When weight of evidence rating is taken into account, measures assessing overall ADHD problem behaviours appeared to find the most convincing results, with medium to large effect sizes. Fehlings et al.’s highly controlled study found a large effect on hyperactivity, and other effects were calculated, including externalisation (medium). It should be noted that only a few effect sizes were able to be calculated in this review, which places restrictions on possible conclusions and recommendations. 20 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth This review indicates that parents report that cognitive-behavioural treatment has a positive impact on the ADHD related behaviours of their children. However, in light of the limitations noted, conclusions should be interpreted carefully. The limited methodological quality and relevance of the studies included in this review and the mixed findings of the studies with sound evidence suggest that evidence is limited. Recommendations Based on the findings of this review the recommendations are as follows: 1. Although many of the studies used controlled group experimental designs or took some appropriate steps to ensure some methodological quality, research is needed into the effectiveness of cognitive-behavioural treatment on pupils with ADHD using robust and tested research methods such as group experimental designs. 2. Four out of the five studies were conducted over 10 years ago. Current research is needed to be able to generalise findings to the current population. 3. All of the studies were conducted outside of the UK, which creates limitations for generalisability. Future research is needed on using cognitive-behavioural interventions with children with ADHD in the UK. 4. Cognitive-behavioural interventions can be considered an effective treatment for children with ADHD from the parent’s point of view. It should therefore continue to be a recommended treatment by NICE guidelines and by educational psychologists, especially when considering NICE’s conclusion that it is cost effective. However, considering the mixed reliability of the effects reported and the varying impact on specific ADHD behaviours other treatments should be considered: cognitive-behavioural treatment does not necessarily produce the largest reductions in ADHD behaviours. 21 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth 5. Although excluded in this review, in light of the high frequency of comorbidity of ADHD with other disorders, future studies should explore cognitivebehavioural treatment effects on children with ADHD as well as other distinct disorders. It would be of interest to explore if differing diagnoses effects the impact of cognitive-behavioural treatment, for example by comparing children with ADHD alone and children with ADHD and an anxiety disorder comparison. 22 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth References Abdollahian, E. Mokhber, N., Balaghi, A., & Moharrari, F. (2013). The effectiveness of cognitive-behavioural play therapy on the symptoms of attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder in children aged 7-9 years. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 5(1), 41 – 46. Abikoff, H. (1987). An evaluation of cognitive behaviour therapy for hyperactive children. In B.B. Lahey & A.E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology (pp. 171-216). New York: Plenum. Abikoff, H. (1991). Cognitive training in ADHD children: Less to it than meets the eye. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24, 205-209. Abikoff, H., Ganeles, D., Reiter, G., Blum, C., Foley, C., & Klein, R. G. (1988). Cognitive training in academically deficient ADHD boys receiving stimulant medication. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 411-432. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations for thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman/Times Books/Henry Holt & Co. Barkley, R. A. (1988). Attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity. In E.J. Mash & L.J. 23 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Terdal (Eds.), Behavioral assessment of childhood disorders (pp. 66-104). New York: Guildford. Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment. New York: Guildford. Barkley, R. A. (2006). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford. Beck, A. T. (1964). Thinking and depression II: Theory and therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 10, 561 – 571. Beck, A. T. (2005). The current state of cognitive therapy: A 40-year retrospective. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 953-959. Butcher, J. N., Mineka, S., & Hooley, J. M. (2010). Abnormal Psychology. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Cameron, M. L., & Robinson, V.M.J. (1980). Effects of cognitive training on academic and on-task behavior of hyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 8, 405-419. Chen, W. J., Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., & Tsuang, M. T. (1994). Diagnostic accuracy of the Child Behavior Checklist scales for attention-deficit hyperactivty disorder: A reciever-operating characteristic analysis. Journal of 24 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62(5), 1017-1025. Fehlings, D. L., Roberts, W., Humphries, T., & Dawe, G. (1991). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Does cognitive behavioral therapy improve home behavior? Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 12(4), 223 – 228. Flanagan, R., & Miller, J. A. (2010). Specialty competencies in school psychology. New York: Oxford University Press. Froelich, J., Doepfner, M., & Lehmkuhl, G. (2002). Effects of combined cognitive behavioural treatment with parent management training in ADHD. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 30, 111 – 115. Gaub, M., & Carlson, C. L. (1997). Gender differences in ADHD: A meta-analysis and critical review. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(8), 1036 -1045. Gittelman, R., & Kanner, A. (1986). Psychopharmacology. In H. Quay & J. Werry (Eds.), Psychopathological disorders of childhood (pp. 455-495). New York: Guildford. Gough, D. (2007). Weight of evidence: A framework for the appraisal of the quality and relevance of evidence. In J. Furlong & A. Oancea (Eds.), Research Papers in Education, 22(2), 213-228. 25 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Gray, K. M., Riggs, P. D., Min, S.J., Mikulich-Gilbertson, S. K., Bandyopadhyay, D., & Winhusen, T. (2011). Cigarette and cannabis use trajectories among adolescents in treatment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and substance use disorders. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 117(2-3), 242–247. Green, H., McGinnity, A., Meltzer, H., Ford, T., & Goodman, R. (2005). Mental Health of Children and Young People in Great Britain, 2004. A survey carried out by the Office for National Statistics on behalf of the Department of Health and the Scottish Executive. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan. Hechtman, L., Weiss, G., & Perlman, T. (1984). Young adult outcome of hyperactive children who received long-term stimulant treatment. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 23, 261- 269. Hinshaw, S. P., & Melnick, S. (1992). Self-management therapies and attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder: Reinforced self-evaluation and anger control interventions. Behavior Modification, 16(2), 253-273. Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., Mcgee, G., Odom, S., & Wolery, M. (2005). The use of single-subject research to identify evidence-based practice in special education. Exceptional Children, 71, 165-179. Horn, W. F., Ialongo, N., Greenberg, G., Packard, T., & Smith- Winberry, C. (1990). 26 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Additive effects of behavioral parent training and self-control therapy with attention deficit hyperactivity disordered children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 19(2), 98 – 110. Jensen, P.S., Mrazek, D., Knapp, P. K., Steinberg, L., Pfeffer, C., Schowalter, J., & Shapiro, T. (1997). Evolution and revolution in child psychiatry: ADHD as a disorder of adaptation. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(12), 1672-1681. Kendall, P.C., & Braswell, L. (1982). Cognitive-behavioral self-control therapy for children: A comprehensive analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 672-689. Kendall, P.C., & Braswell, L. (1985). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for impulsive children. New York: Guildford. Kendall, P.C., Padever, W., & Zupan, B. (1980). Developing self-control in children: A manual of cognitive-behavioral strategies. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota. Kendall, P. C., & Wilcox, L. E. (1979). Self-control in children: Development of a rating scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47(6), 1020-1029. Kratochwill, T. R., & Steele Shernoff, E. (2004). Evidence-Based Practice: Promoting 27 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Evidence-Based Interventions in School Psychology. School Psychology Review, 33, 34-48. Langberg, J. M., Froehlich, T. E., Loren, R. E., Martin, J. E., & Epstein, J. N. (2008). Assessing children with ADHD in primary care settings. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 8, 627 – 641. Levine, E. S., & Anshel, D. J. (2011). “Nothing Works!” A case study using cognitivebehavioral interventions to engage parents, educators, and children in the management of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychology in the Schools, 48(3), 297 – 306. Miranda, A., & Jesús Presentación, M. (2000). Efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy in the treatment of children with ADHD, with and without aggressiveness. Psychology in the Schools, 37(2), 169 – 182. Muñoz-Solomando, A., Kendall, T., & Whittington, C. J. (2008). Cognitive behavioural therapy for children and adolescents. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 21(4), 332–7. NICE (2009). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Diagnosis and management of ADHD in children young people and adults. London, National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence. 28 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Rutter, M. (1976). A children’s behavior questionnaire for completion by teacher. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 8, 1-11. Springer, J. F., Phillips, J. L., Phillips, L., Cannady, L. P., & Kerst-Harris, E. (1992). CODA: A creative therapy program for children in families affected by abuse of alcohol or drugs. Journal of Community Psychology, 75, 55-74. Tamm, L., Trello-Rishel, K., Riggs, P., Nakonezny, P. A., Acosta, M., Bailey, G., & Winhusen, T. (2013). Predictors of treatment response in adolescents with comorbid substance use disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 44(2), 224–30. Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2008). Sources of self-efficacy in school: Critical review of the literature and future directions. Review of Educational Research, 78(4), 751–796. 29 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Appendices Appendix A: Rationale for Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria 1. Inclusion of papers that maintain fidelity to a cognitive-behavioural approach and that also target attention deficit hyperactivity disorder ensures that the review question is answered accurately. Ability to isolate cognitivebehavioural treatment impact on dependent measures from other interventions that do not target ADHD and/or are not delivered to child or young person helps to control for extraneous variables. Due to the nature of ADHD treatment it was not always possible to isolate cognitive-behavioural treatment from other interventions that target ADHD that are delivered to the child or young person, such as ADHD medication. This was however taken into account and affected the weighting of evidence accordingly. 2. The study must measure ADHD symptoms from the parent perspective as this is the focus of the review. 3. Participants must not be over 19 as when this review was carried out the SEN Code of Practice stated that a child with special educational needs or disabilities could be supported for their whole school career, therefore up until the age of 19. 4. A diagnosis of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), diagnosed by DSM-III-R – DSM 5 or European equivalent (e.g. ICD-10) was required as it was in the DSM-III-R that Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder was refined and the ADHD label introduced. The ADHD label has only remained stable since this point. This ensures that the participants included within the review have undergone the same diagnosis process and have been measured against the same criteria. 30 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth 5. Participants on medication for ADHD were accepted within this review as it is a very common occurrence that children with ADHD are treated with some form of medication (e.g. methylphenidate based medication). Further, the Nice Guidelines states that although medication should not be considered as the first-line of treatment it should be considered for those with more severe symptoms (NICE 2009). However, participants on medication for other disorders and participants diagnosed with other distinct disorders were excluded to help control for extraneous variables. 6. The study must have primary data in order to analyse effect sizes of the outcomes and to ensure original synthesise of study findings. This review is investigating impact and so descriptive guides will not be relevant. 7. The publication must be a peer reviewed journal as it should be assessed against a set of criteria measuring its quality. There is less chance of fictitious results or large reporting errors when the study has been reviewed. 8. The study must be available in English as the reviewer cannot read other languages adequately. 9. The study must be published before January 2014 as this was the date of the database search and must be published after 1987 because this was the year the DSM-III-R was introduced. See Criteria 5 for further details about the DSM-III-R. 31 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Appendix B: Flow Diagram of full literature search Papers identified from search terms (PsychInfo, ERIC, and Medline) (n – 1,054) Excluded based on: Title and/or Abstract (n – 1,044) Full articles assessed (n - 10) Excluded based on: Inclusion Criteria (Criteria 1, 2 and 6) (n - 5) Five studies remained from database search (n - 5) Ancestral searches conducted on 5 remaining studies from database search (n – 193) Excluded based on: Title (n - 189) Full articles assessed (from ancestral search) (n - 4) All Excluded based on: Inclusion Criteria (Criteria: 1, 6, 7, and 8) (n - 4) Five studies obtained from database search included in synthesis (n - 5) 32 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Appendix C: Studies excluded from systematic database search Reference Muñoz-Solomando, A., Kendall, T., & Whittington, C. J. (2008). Cognitive behavioural therapy for children and adolescents. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 21(4), 332–7. Reason for excluding Criteria 6: Is not an empirical study containing primary data. Bloomquist, M. L., August, G. J., & Ostrander, R. (1991). Effects of a school-based cognitive-behavioral intervention for ADHD children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 19(5), 591–605. Criteria 2: Does not contain a variable measuring ADHD symptoms from the parent perspective Hinshaw, S. P., & Melnick, S. (1992). Self-management therapies and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: reinforced self-evaluation and anger control interventions. Behavior Modification, 16(2), 253–273. Criteria 6: Is not an empirical study containing primary data. Mirnasab, M. M. (2011). Case report effects of selfmonitoring technique on inattentive behaviors of students with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, 5, 84–86. Criteria 2: Does not contain a variable measuring ADHD symptoms from the parent perspective Levine, E. S., & Anshel, D. J. (2011). “Nothing Works!” A case study using cognitive-behavioral interventions to engage parents, educators, and children in the management of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychology in the Schools, 48(3), 297 – 306. Criteria 1: Unable to isolate CB treatment effect from other interventions not delivered to child. Appendix D: Studies excluded from ancestral search Reference Abikoff, H. (1991). Cognitive training in ADHD children: Less to it than meets the eye. Journal Learning Disabilities, 24, 205-209. Blinn, E, L. (2000). Efficacy of play therapy on problem behaviour behaviors of a child with attention deficit hyperactivity disorders. Retrieved for the World Wide Web. http://www.Altavista.com Reason for excluding Criteria 6: Is not an empirical study containing primary data Criteria 7: Unpublished work or grey literature such as personal websites or non-peer reviewed journals Hansen, S., Miessler, K., Ovens, R. (2000). Kids together: A Criteria 1: Not cognitivebehavioural treatment group play therapy model for children with ADHD symptomology. Journal of Child and Adolescent Group Therapy, 10, 191-21.1 Janatian, S., Nouri, A., Shafti, A., Molvai H., & Samavatyan, H. (2008). Effectiveness of play therapy on the bases of cognition behavioural approach of symptoms of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) among primary school male students aged 9-11. Journal of Research in Behavioural Sciences, 6 (2), 110-112. 33 Criteria 8: Full article not available in English Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Appendix E: Summary of Studies Study and Aims Sample Design Intervention Relevant Measures Results in relation to relevant measures/sample Abdollahian et al. (2013) N = 30 Quasi-experimental study. Setting: Mashhad, Iran Parents asked to fill in Rutter Parental Questionnaire. (only parent measures reported here) Aimed to study the efficacy of cognitivebehavioural play therapy on the symptoms of ADHD in children aged 79 years. Fehlings et al. (1991). To determine CBT’s effectiveness in improving home behavior in children with ADHD. Age: 7-9 (66.7% of EG and 73.3% of CG being under age of 8) Identification: Consecutive referrals to the child and adolescent psychiatric clinic of the Ebne-Sina university hospital during a specified 4month period + then a diagnosis of ADHD made by two board certified child and adolescent psychiatrists. All participants were on medication N = 26 (all m) (1 excluded from data analyses) Age: 7-13 Identification: Diagnosis of ADHD (DSM-III-R), a rating of 15+ on parent Conners 10 item scale, 150+ on self- 15 children assigned to EG and 15 to CG. Matched in confounding variables (birth order, parental occupations &educ. level, school marks) Parents and teachers blind to group allocation. CB treatment: cognitivebehavioural play therapy sessions, conducted with different thematic activities: don’t drop it; throw the balls; touch it; be careful and throw the rings; note and remember; me and my shadow; work for a longer time; tell me act..tell me; running inside a maze and talking. Delivery: Therapist Pre-tests and post-tests obtained for both groups. EG: Eight 45-min sessions of play therapy on a twice-weekly basis (in gps of 5) CG: Eight shame play therapy sessions (carried out in same room for same time & therapeutic conditions matched). Target behaviours: aimed to improve sustained attention, working memory, maintenance, impulsivity, self-control and organisation in children with ADHD. Randomised controlled trial. Setting: Toronto, Ontario, Canada 13 children assigned to CBT EG and 13 to CG (supportive therapy). CB treatment: received direct instruction on CB strategies, reinforced by token contingency reward system. Problem solving broken down to five step process (1) defining the problem (2) setting a goal (3) generating workable problem-solving strategies (4) choosing a Pre-tests and post-tests and 5month post-tests obtained for both groups. EG: 12 60-min individual 34 The Rutter Parental Questionnaire: comprises of 18 items, including some related to the health and habits of children. Rutter reported internal validity and reliability of .74 for the questionnaire. The correlation between the responses of the fathers and mothers was .64, and the internal consistency of the test was .79 (Rutter 1976). Parent measures: Self Control Rating Scale (SCRS) (a measure of impulsivity); Revised Behavior Problem Checklist-Attention Problem Subscale (BPC-AP subscale) (a measure of inattention) and the Modified Werry Weiss Activity Scale (a measure of the child’s activity level in The results of the paired t test for the scores of both the pre- and post-test questionnaire for parents showed that: there was a significant different in the experimental group (p=.0007, effect size = 0.8); hyperactivity scores decreased after play therapy treatment, indicating that this stimulus had a reducing effect. No significant difference pre- and post- was found in the control group (p=.116). A t test also showed that the ADHD scores for the experimental group were significantly lower than those for the control group (p=.000). One-two way ANOVA with post hoc t tests was computed for each dependent variable. Werry Weiss Activity Scale revealed a statistically significant treatment effect favouring CBT (p<.03). A post hoc t test revealed that there was a significant difference for the pre- and post- treatment comparison (p<.020 and the pre and 5month post comparison (p<.01). Paired t test assessing differences in CG did not reach significance for pre and post or pre and 5month. Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Study and Aims Cara Southworth Sample Design Intervention Relevant Measures control rating scale, score of 85+ on WISC-R. Excluded if on stimulant medication or if met criteria for conduct disorder or other major psychiatric disorders. sessions, between child and behavioral therapist, twice weekly, and 8 2hour sessions once every 2 weeks with the family in their home. CG: received same amount of exposure to therapist and tasks. solution (5) evaluating the outcome with self-reinforcement. Child taught these using modelling, role playing, selfinstructional training, cue cards, homework assignments and behavioural techniques. Problems included academic and then interpersonal ones, with focus on home behavior. Parents taught about CBT and how to encourage it. the home). Results in relation to relevant measures/sample No sig. differences for SCRS and BPC-AP EG. Delivery: Therapist Target Behaviours: home behaviour Froelich et al. (2002) To demonstrate the effectiveness of cognitive behavioural treatment (CBT) with a focus on academic skills and conduct problems (and to increase parents’ educational skills in managing aggressive and oppositional behaviour in a subsequent parent training N= 18 (17m, 1f) 16 cognitive functioning normal, 2 with LD. 8 on ADHD medication. Age: 6-12 Identification: Referred from the Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Outpatient unit of the University Clinic Cologne. Diagnosis corresponded to Hyperkinetic Conduct disorder (ADHD Europe) (ICD-10 F 90.1). Pre-, post-test study design without a control group. 4 weeks baseline, 6 weeks CBT (+GEN) (6 weeks PMT – not part of this review) Treatment effects measured at end of each week via rating scales. CB treatment incorporated six one-hour lessons for each pupil. One per week. Setting: Cologne, Germany CB treatment: Predominantly included training in selfinstructional, self-monitoring and problem-solving skills. Tasks included attractive games e.g. puzzles, picture sequences. Transfer to self-instructional problem-solving strategies on homework or school problems to behavioural problems of inattention and impulsivity was initiated. Social-conflict situations occurring at home or school were introduced. Individually relevant academic or social problems were discussed during the sessions and homework assignments given to facilitate generalisation. Children received a phone call a week to support treatment 35 Parents rated core symptoms of ADHD on an abbreviated German version (15 items) of the Yale Children’s inventory. Auth. calculated a total internal consistency for the total score of .94. Cronbach’s Alpha for each symptom subscale ranged between .80-.88. Conflict situations at home were measured in the Home Situations Questionnaire, adapted German version (16 items, 9-point scale). Internal consistency calculated alpha > 90). Homework problems assessed via modified Homework Problem Checklist (Alpha >.90) and Individual Multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA) with one within subject factor were performed. No statistical differences at baseline. During CBT (+ GEN) phase ADHD core symptoms and symptoms related to conduct disorder significantly decreased according to parent ratings (p<.001 core symptoms; p<.0001 conduct problems). Plus, the following were also significant different to baseline after CB treatment: individually relevant behaviour problems (p<.004), conflict situations at home (p<.001) and homework problems (p<.001). Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Study and Aims Sample Design (PMT). Cara Southworth Intervention Relevant Measures generalization. Parents and teachers received regular written hand-outs of treatment content and continuously asked to prompt and to reinforce appropriate behaviour of the child at home or school. Problem List to measure effectiveness of treatment on individually relevant problem behaviour. Results in relation to relevant measures/sample Delivery: Therapist Target Behaviours: core symptoms of ADHD and conduct problems. Horn et al. (1990) To assess the separate and combined effects of behavioural parent training and cognitivebehavioural self-control therapy with 42 elementary school age children diagnosed as having ADHD. N= 42 (34m, 8f) + 18 non-ADHD for CG (8m, 10f). Age: 7 years, 0 months -11 years, 6 months. Identification: Referred to a university-based psychological clinic for treatment of chronic inattentiveness, impulsivity, and over activity. Diagnosis of ADHD from DSM-III-R. Additional inclusion criteria: not receiving medication for ADHD, absence of psychoses and intellectual difficulties. The Randomised controlled trial. Participants randomly assigned to PT, CB SC, or PT + SC, and the nonADHD control group. 14 families in each group. CB SC and PT (and nonADHD CG) compared in study and this review. Dependent measures were administered at three points: pre-test, post-test, and 8-month follow-up. CB SC EG: treatment (therapy group split into two groups of 7 children) groups met for 12 weekly- 90-min sessions. Non-ADHD CG: no details. PT group: Parents received training which Setting: US CB Treatment: CB self-control therapy consisted of instruction in the self-control strategies adapted from Camp and Bash (1981), Kendall and Braswell (1985) and Meichenbaum (1977). Each child was taught a “Problem Solving Plan” that incorporated following steps: Am I having a problem? Take a deep breath and think “calm and relax”; What is my problem?; How many solutions can I think of?; How good is each solution?; Pick the best solution and try it: How did my solution work?. Deep muscle relaxation, in conjunction with imaginal rehearsal, was incorporated into the problem-solving training. Training was comprised of didactic presentations, modelling, guided practice and role plays. Role play incorporated both academic and 36 Parents asked to complete The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Total Problems, Externalising and Hyperactivity scales). The Treatment x Period MANOVAs carried out on parent reports did not yield significant Treatment x Period interactions. Significant period effects were found for the MANOVAs performed on parent reports (p<.001). Subsequent Univariate repeatedmeasures ANOVAs yielded significant pretest-to-post-test period effects for each of the parent CBCL report scales. Pre-test to post-test: CBCL total problems (p<.001), CBCL Externalizing (p<.001), CBCL Hyperactivity (p<.001). Pre-test to follow up: CBCL total problems (p<.01), CBCL Externalizing (p<.01), CBCL Hyperactivity (p<.01). Within-group t tests from pre-test to posttest and from pre-test to follow-up for the parent outcome variables were then carried out on treatment conditions. Parent reports in CB SC group resulted in significant results in all except CBCL hyperactivity pre-test to post-test and pre-test to followup. CBCL total: pre-test to post-test (p<.01); pre-test to follow-up (p<.01) CBCL Externalizing: pre-test to post-test (p<.01); pre-test to follow-up (p<.001). Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Study and Aims Cara Southworth Sample Design Intervention Relevant Measures Peabody Vocabulary test used to screen for participant’s intellectual status. focused on teaching parents to apply social learning principles to the management of their child’s behaviour. interpersonal problems. Token reinforcement system was used for behavioural control of the children in group sessions. Results in relation to relevant measures/sample However, PT also achieved significant results across the scales, including hyperactivity. CB SC was compared with non-ADHD to see if ADHD subjects were within the normal limits at post-test or follow-up. In parent reports ADHD remained significantly different from non-ADHD subjects at posttest and follow-up. Delivery: Therapist Target behaviours: ADHD cardinal features (e.g. externalising and hyperactivity). CB SC final sessions were videotaped to obtain a measure of how well each of the children had learned the six steps of the Problem-solving plan. Analysis indicated nearly perfect performance of the six steps. Miranda and Jesús Presentación (2000) N= 32 total (16 nonaggressive, 16 aggressive sample excluded) To show the efficacy of cognitivebehavioral selfcontrol therapy of children with ADHD (with and without aggressiveness ). Age: 9 – 12 Identification: Diagnosed by the school psychologists as having chronic problems of inattention, impulsivity, and over activity. Teachers interviewed to corroborate. Then had to meet this criteria: score 15 + on Conners rating Scale, score 16 or higher on DSM-III- Pre-, post-test (+follow up) study design without a control group. Participants divided into two groups 16 with and 16 without aggressiveness. Participants spilt into 4 groups: ADHD with CB SC, ADHD with CB SC + AM, (Excluded: ADHD&aggressiveness with CB SC, ADHD&aggressiveness with CB SC + AM.) Dependent measures taken at three points: pretreatment, post-treatment and 2-months follow-up. CB SC EG: 22 sessions over 3 months, on a Setting: Castellón, Spain CB treatment: Applied Kendall et al.’s “Stop and Think” program (1980) with small modifications. Includes selfinstruction, modelling, and behavioural contingencies, to help solve various kinds of problems. Children trained to use self-instructional strategies through the process of problem solving in order to consider possible course of action, to reflect on potential outcomes and to make decisions about options. The problem solving process was broken down into 5 statements: Problem definition, problem approach, focusing attention, selecting an answer, self-evaluation/selfreinforcement. The program looked at interpersonal problem- 37 Parents were given: A hyperactivity questionnaire adapted from the DSM-III-R and the Scale of Behavioural Problems (EPC). The Hyperactivity questionnaire: 14 items corresponding to 14 criteria described in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (APA, 1987) for the diagnosis of ADHD. Items reflect the behaviours observable in inattention, impulsivity and over activity. The EPC: gives information on the child’s character and actions in different environments for the parent’s point of view. This MANOVA carried out using information obtained from the parents of the two groups with eight non-aggressive ADHD children showed significant differences at the three times of testing: DSM-III-R (p≤.001); Antisocial behaviour (p≤.13); School problems (p≤.004); and psychopathological disorders p≤.033). The scores in all these variables significantly diminished in the post-test and maintenance phases, compared to the scores obtained in the pre-test phase. No differences found either between the treatment groups or in the analysis of between-group and intragroup interaction. It was not possible to separate CB SC & CB SC + AM for significant effects over time. Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Study and Aims Cara Southworth Sample Design Intervention Relevant Measures R, 1+ persistence of symptoms, IQ score 80+ of WISC, absence of psychosis or any gross neurological, sensory or motor impairment. Iowa Scale for Teachers screened for aggressiveness. twice-weekly basis and lastly approx. one-hour each, with four groups in the morning and four in the afternoon. Sessions took place outside of school hours. solving, looked at hypothetical problems and later real-life problems. Programme used token system, social- and selfreinforcement. Response cost also used. Generalisation encouraged. scale has an alpha coefficient of .88 and a testretest correlation of .79 in various subscales. It also has adequate level of construct and criterial validity. Delivery: Therapist. Target behaviours: improve concentration and reflection. 38 Results in relation to relevant measures/sample Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Appendix F: Weight of Evidence Weight of Evidence A. Considers the reliability and validity of measures (A.1), any control measures used (A.2), and the intervention measures (A.3). In order to gain ‘High’ weighting for methodological quality the study must have an average weighting of 3. In order to gain ‘Medium’ weighting for methodological quality the study must have an average weighting of 2. In order to gain ‘Low’ weighting for methodological quality the study must have an average weighting of 1. Weight of Evidence B. Methodological Relevance considers whether the research design was suitable for evaluating the effectiveness of using cognitive-behavioural treatment to improve ADHD symptoms (B.1). In order to gain ‘High’ weighting for methodological relevance the study must be randomized with a control group. In order to gain ‘Medium’ weighting for methodological relevance the study may be a non-randomised group design with some control measures. In order to gain ‘Low’ weighting for methodological relevance the study may be a non-randomised group design with no control measures. Weight of Evidence C. Relevance to the review question considers whether the design was suitable for evaluating the effectiveness of using cognitive-behavioural treatment to improve ADHD symptoms for children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (C.1). In order to gain ‘High’ weighting for relevance the study must use cognitivebehavioural treatment to target specific behaviours associated with ADHD. 39 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Must measure the target behaviour before and after intervention (follow-up if possible). The sample of interest must be children with an appropriate diagnosis of ADHD, comparison group also with a diagnosis of ADHD. Sample must be typically developing children with the exception of the ADHD diagnosis, and must not be receiving additional interventions aimed at ADHD (e.g. medication/anger management). ADHD symptoms must be measured by the parent. In order to gain ‘Medium’ weighting for relevance the study must use cognitive-behavioural treatment to target specific behaviours associated with ADHD. Must measure the target behaviour before and after intervention (follow-up if possible). The sample of interest must be children with an appropriate diagnosis of ADHD. Sample can include children with other disorders in addition to the diagnosis of ADHD, and can be receiving medication for ADHD. ADHD symptoms must be measured by the parent. In order to gain ‘Low’ weighting for relevance the study must use cognitivebehavioural treatment to target specific behaviours associated with ADHD. Must measure the target behaviour before and after intervention (follow-up if possible). The sample of interest must be children with an appropriate diagnosis of ADHD. Sample can include children receiving medication for ADHD or additional non-medication interventions for ADHD. ADHD symptoms must be measured by the parent. Weight of Evidence D. Calculated as an average of the weight of evidence A, B and C. 40 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Effect Sizes. The effect sizes are listed at the end of the coding protocols. Effect sizes have either been quoted directly from the studies or they have been calculated. If calculated, the effect size was calculated using means, standard deviations and ns with either the following formula – (1) Cohen’s d from means and standard deviations: (2) Hedge’s g from means, standard deviations and ns: Due to the small samples in the studies included in this review, Hedge’s g was used where possible as it incorporates an adjustment which removes the bias of Cohen’s d. The effect size rating (small, medium or large) is listed in Table 7, the rationale for these ratings can be found in Cohen (1988). Table 7 Effect size interpretation Type of effect size Small Cohen’s d/Hedge’s g 0.20 Medium 0.50 41 Large 0.80 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Appendix G: Coding Protocol A coding protocol adapted from Kratochwill (2003) and Gough (2007) was used to evaluate the studies in this review. Full Reference: Abdollahian, E. Mokhber, N., Balaghi, A., & Moharrari, F. (2013). The effectiveness of cognitive-behavioural play therapy on the symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children aged 7-9 years. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 5(1), 41 – 46. Weight of Evidence A A.1. Quality of Measures Check Reliability and Validity Multi-method Multi-source (as only interested in parent) n/a (Relevant) Outcomes 1: The Rutter Parental Questionnaire Reliability - Rutter reported internal validity and reliability of .74 for the questionnaire. The correlation between the responses of the fathers and mothers was .64, and the internal consistency of the test was .79 (Rutter 1976) Validity - above Outcome 2: Reliability = Validity = Overall Rating for measurement (3) Strong evidence: case for validity, reliability above 0.85 (in majority or primary outcomes), multiple methods and multiple sources (2) Promising evidence: reliability above 0.70, multiple methods or multiple sources (1) Weak evidence: reliability above 0.50, multiple methods or multiple sources – Judgement: Reliability reported is high, but limitations elsewhere. (0) None of the above A.2 Quality of Control Measure Check Control Measure Type of comparison group Typical intervention Attention placebo Intervention element placebo (intervention minus active ingredient linked to change) – Sham CB play therapy sessions Alternative intervention Pharmacotherapy No intervention Wait list/delayed intervention 42 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Minimal contact Unable to identify type of comparison Overall judgement of type of comparison group Very low (little basis) Low (guess) Moderate (weak inference) High (strong inference) Very high (explicitly stated) Unable to identify comparison group Counterbalancing of change agent (e.g. teacher or therapist) By change agent Statistical Other Not reported/none – not reported, but control group attended same room where play therapy took place for same amount of time, who administered sessions is unknown. Group equivalence established Random assignment Posthoc matched set Statistical matching Post hoc test for group equivalence Equivalent mortality Low attrition (less than 20% for post) Low attrition (less than 30% for follow up) Intent to intervene analysis carried out (include findings): Overall Rating for Control Measure (3) Strong evidence: at least one active comparison group, group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), counterbalancing, low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (2) Promising evidence: at least one ‘no intervention’ comparison group. Plus at least two of: group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), or counterbalancing or low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (1) Weak evidence: at least one comparison group. Plus at least one of: group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), or counterbalancing or low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (0) no efforts made to control A.3 Quality of Intervention Group Experimental Design Primary outcomes in appropriate unit of analysis Primary outcomes familywise/experimenter wise error rate controlled when applicable Primary outcomes have sufficiently large N At least 75% of primary outcomes significant 50-74% of primary outcomes significant 25-49% of primary outcomes significant Less than 25% of primary outcomes significant Overall Rating for Intervention (3) Strong evidence: Significant outcomes for at least 75%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. (2) Promising evidence: Significant outcomes for 50-74%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. – Judgement: over 50-74% but not sufficiently large N (1) Weak evidence: Significant outcomes for 24-49%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. (0) No/Limited evidence: Less than 25% significant outcomes and none of the above Overall Rating for WoE A (1+2+2) /3 = 1.66 43 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low Weight of Evidence B B.1 Design Check Single Case Design Check Group Experimental Design Type of Design: Within –Series: Simple phase change Non-randomised design Within – Series: Complex phase Non-randomised block design (between change participants) Between – Series: Comparing two Non-randomised block design (within interventions participants) Between – Series: Comparing Non-randomised hierarchical design interventions with no interventions Combined – Series: Multiple baseline Optional coding for quasi-experimental across participants design Combined – Series: Multiple baseline across behaviours Combined – Series: Multiple baseline across settings Combined – Series: Multiple probe Mixed single participant design (tick two of the above) Mixed single participant and group design (tick one of the above and one in the right hand column) Other (specify): If randomisation used: Completely randomised Randomised block design (between participants e.g. matched classrooms) Randomised block design (within participants) Randomised hierarchical design (nested treatments) Units of randomisation: Individual Classroom School Other (specify): Overall confidence of judgement on how participants were assigned: Very low (little basis) Low (guess) Moderate (weak inference) High (strong inference) Very high (explicitly stated) N/A Unknown/unable to code Overall Rating for WoE B (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low 44 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Weight of Evidence C C.1 Sample N Consecutive referrals to the child and adolescent psychiatric clinic of the Ebne-Sina university hospital during a specified 4-month period + then a diagnosis of ADHD made by two board certified child and adolescent psychiatrists 30 Sample size at start of study Current exposure to alternative intervention Prior exposure to alternative intervention No historical exposure to alternative intervention Single Case Design Participant N additional information: 15 15 P1 P2 P3 Overall Rating for WoE C (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low Group Experimental Design Intervention Group size Control Group size Attrition (drop out) Weight of Evidence D Overall Rating for WoE D (2+2+2) /3 = 2 (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low Effect sizes Relevant Outcomes Outcome 1. Parent questionnaire (Rutter) to assess ADHD symptoms in intervention group (pre-and post-test). Type of effect size Effect size Comment Cohen’s d 0.80 Explicitly stated. Unable to convert to Hedge’s g. Outcome 2. Parent questionnaire (Rutter) intervention vs. control group posttest. Unable to calculate effect size 45 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Full Reference: Fehlings, D. L., Roberts, W., Humphries, T., & Dawe, G. (1991). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Does cognitive behavioral therapy improve home behavior? Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 12(4), 223 – 228. Weight of Evidence A A.1 Quality of Measures Check Reliability and Validity Multi-method Multi-source (parents only) (Relevant) Outcomes 1: Self Control Rating Scale (SCRS) Reliability Validity (Relevant) Outcome 2: Revised Behaviour Problem Checklist-Attention Problem Subscale (BPC-AP) Reliability = Validity = (Relevant) Outcome 3: Modified Werry Weiss Activity Scale Reliability = Validity = Overall Rating for measurement (3) Strong evidence: case for validity, reliability above 0.85 (in majority or primary outcomes), multiple methods and multiple sources (2) Promising evidence: reliability above 0.70, multiple methods or multiple sources (1) Weak evidence: reliability above 0.50, multiple methods or multiple sources Judgement: References show reliability of methods used but no statement of this in study or with the study’s population. Multiple methods. (0) None of the above n/a A.2 Quality of Control Measure Check Control Measure Type of comparison group Typical intervention Attention placebo Intervention element placebo (intervention minus active ingredient linked to change) Alternative intervention – Supportive Therapy Pharmacotherapy No intervention Wait list/delayed intervention Minimal contact Unable to identify type of comparison Overall judgement of type of comparison group Very low (little basis) Low (guess) Moderate (weak inference) High (strong inference) Very high (explicitly stated) Unable to identify comparison group Counterbalancing of change agent (e.g. teacher or therapist) 46 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth By change agent – Both groups received equal amount of exposure to same therapist and tasks. Statistical Other Not reported/none Group equivalence established Random assignment Posthoc matched set Statistical matching Post hoc test for group equivalence Equivalent mortality Low attrition (less than 20% for post) Low attrition (less than 30% for follow up) Intent to intervene analysis carried out (include findings): Overall Rating for Control Measure (3) Strong evidence: at least one active comparison group, group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), counterbalancing, low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (2) Promising evidence: at least one ‘no intervention’ comparison group. Plus at least two of: group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), or counterbalancing or low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (1) Weak evidence: at least one comparison group. Plus at least one of: group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), or counterbalancing or low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (0) no efforts made to control A.3 Quality of Intervention Group Experimental Design Primary outcomes in appropriate unit of analysis Primary outcomes familywise/experimenter wise error rate controlled when applicable Primary outcomes have sufficiently large N At least 75% of primary outcomes significant 50-74% of primary outcomes significant 25-49% of primary outcomes significant Less than 25% of primary outcomes significant Overall Rating for Intervention (3) Strong evidence: Significant outcomes for at least 75%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. (2) Promising evidence: Significant outcomes for 50-74%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. (1) Weak evidence: Significant outcomes for 24-49%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. – Judgement: no familywise control, but appropriate unit of analysis and 33.3% primary outcome significance. (0) No/Limited evidence: Less than 25% significant outcomes and none of the above Overall Rating for WoE A (1+3+1) /3 = 1.66 (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low 47 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Weight of Evidence B B.1 Design Check Single Case Design Check Group Experimental Design Type of Design: Within –Series: Simple phase change Non-randomised design Within – Series: Complex phase Non-randomised block design (between change participants) Between – Series: Comparing two Non-randomised block design (within interventions participants) Between – Series: Comparing Non-randomised hierarchical design interventions with no interventions Combined – Series: Multiple baseline Optional coding for quasi-experimental across participants design Combined – Series: Multiple baseline across behaviours Combined – Series: Multiple baseline across settings Combined – Series: Multiple probe Mixed single participant design (tick two of the above) Mixed single participant and group design (tick one of the above and one in the right hand column) Other (specify): If randomisation used: Completely randomised Randomised block design (between participants e.g. matched classrooms) Randomised block design (within participants) Randomised hierarchical design (nested treatments) Units of randomisation: Individual Classroom School Other (specify): Overall confidence of judgement on how participants were assigned: Very low (little basis) Low (guess) Moderate (weak inference) High (strong inference) Very high (explicitly stated) N/A Unknown/unable to code Overall Rating for WoE B (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low 48 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Weight of Evidence C C.1 Sample N Diagnosis of ADHD (DSM-III-R), a rating of 15+ on parent Conners 10 item scale, 150+ on selfcontrol rating scale, score of 85+ on WISC-R. Excluded if on stimulant medication or if met criteria for conduct disorder or other major psychiatric disorders. 26 Sample size at start of study Current exposure to alternative intervention Prior exposure to alternative intervention No historical exposure to alternative intervention Single Case Design Participant N additional information: 13 13 2 P1 P2 P3 Group Experimental Design Intervention Group size Control Group size Attrition (drop out) – 1 from supportive therapy group, 1 family from supportive therapy group at 5-month follow-up. Overall Rating for WoE C (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low Weight of Evidence D Overall Rating for WoE D (2+3+3)/3 = 2.66 (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low Effect Sizes Relevant Outcomes Type of effect size Effect size Cohen’s d(Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 0.77 Outcome 1b. SCRS CBT pre-tofollow-up Cohen’s d(Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 1.47 Outcome 1c. SCRS CBT vs. Control post-test Cohen’s d(Adjusted to Hedge’s g) -0.27 Outcome 1d. SCRS CBT vs. Control follow-up Cohen’s d(Adjusted to Hedge’s g) -0.28 Outcome 2a. WWAS CBT pre-topost-test Cohen’s d (Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 0.98 Outcome 1a. SCRS CBT pre-to-posttest 49 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Relevant Outcomes Cara Southworth Type of effect size Effect size Outcome 2b. WWAS CBT Pre-tofollow-up Cohen’s d (Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 1.10 Outcome 2c. WWAS CBT vs. Control post-test Cohen’s d(Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 0.72 Outcome 2d. WWAS CBT vs. Control follow-up Cohen’s d(Adjusted to Hedge’s g) -0.49 Outcome 3a. BPCAP CBT pre-topost-test Cohen’s d(Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 0.65 Outcome 3b. BPCAP CBT pre-tofollow-up Cohen’s d(Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 1.00 Outcome 3c. BPCAP CBT vs. Control post-test. Cohen’s d(Adjusted to Hedge’s g) -0.48 Cohen’s d(Adjusted to Hedge’s g) -0.27 Outcome 3d. BPCAP CBT vs. Control follow-up Full Reference: Froelich, J., Doepfner, M., & Lehmkuhl, G. (2002). Effects of combined cognitive behavioural treatment with parent management training in ADHD. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 30, 111 – 115. Weight of Evidence A A.1 Quality of Measures Check Reliability and Validity Multi-method – Parent questionnaires: Adapted Yale Children’s Inventory, Home Situations Questionnaire, Homework Problem Checklist & Individual Problem Checklist Multi-source n/a Outcomes 1: Adapted Yale Children’s Inventory Reliability - Auth. calculated a total internal consistency for the total score of .94. Cronbach’s Alpha for each symptom subscale ranged between .80-.88. Validity – Outcome 2: Home Situations Questionnaire Reliability = Internal consistency calculated alpha > 90 Validity = Outcome 3: Homework Problem Checklist Reliability = Alpha >.90 50 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Validity = Overall Rating for measurement (3) Strong evidence: case for validity, reliability above 0.85 (in majority or primary outcomes), multiple methods and multiple sources (2) Promising evidence: reliability above 0.70, multiple methods or multiple sources (1) Weak evidence: reliability above 0.50, multiple methods or multiple sources (0) None of the above A.2 Quality of Control Measure Check Control Measure – no control group Type of comparison group Typical intervention Attention placebo Intervention element placebo (intervention minus active ingredient linked to change) Alternative intervention Pharmacotherapy No intervention Wait list/delayed intervention Minimal contact Unable to identify type of comparison Overall judgement of type of comparison group Very low (little basis) Low (guess) Moderate (weak inference) High (strong inference) Very high (explicitly stated) Unable to identify comparison group Counterbalancing of change agent (e.g. teacher or therapist) By change agent Statistical Other Not reported/none Group equivalence established Random assignment Posthoc matched set Statistical matching Post hoc test for group equivalence Equivalent mortality - unknown Low attrition (less than 20% for post) Low attrition (less than 30% for follow up) Intent to intervene analysis carried out (include findings): Overall Rating for Control Measure (3) Strong evidence: at least one active comparison group, group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), counterbalancing, low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (2) Promising evidence: at least one ‘no intervention’ comparison group. Plus at least two of: group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), or counterbalancing or low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (1) Weak evidence: at least one comparison group. Plus at least one of: group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), or counterbalancing or low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (0) no efforts made to control 51 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth A.3 Quality of Intervention Group Experimental Design – Primary outcomes in appropriate unit of analysis Primary outcomes familywise/experimenter wise error rate controlled when applicable Primary outcomes have sufficiently large N At least 75% of primary outcomes significant 50-74% of primary outcomes significant 25-49% of primary outcomes significant Less than 25% of primary outcomes significant Overall Rating for Intervention (3) Strong evidence: Significant outcomes for at least 75%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. (2) Promising evidence: Significant outcomes for 50-74%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. – Judgement due to lack of sufficient N but good significant outcomes. (1) Weak evidence: Significant outcomes for 24-49%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. (0) No/Limited evidence: Less than 25% significant outcomes and none of the above Overall Rating for WoE A (2+0+2)/3 = 1.33 (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low Weight of Evidence B B.1 Design Check Single Subject Design Check Group Experimental Design Type of Design: Within –Series: Simple phase change Non-randomised design Within – Series: Complex phase Non-randomised block design (between change participants) Between – Series: Comparing two Non-randomised block design (within interventions participants) Between – Series: Comparing Non-randomised hierarchical design interventions with no interventions Combined – Series: Multiple baseline Optional coding for quasi-experimental across participants design A pre-test- post-test design – no control Combined – Series: Multiple baseline group across behaviours Combined – Series: Multiple baseline across settings Combined – Series: Multiple probe Mixed single participant design (tick two of the above) Mixed single participant and group design (tick one of the above and one in the right hand column) Other (specify): If randomisation used: Completely randomised Randomised block design (between participants e.g. matched classrooms) Randomised block design (within participants) Randomised hierarchical design (nested treatments) 52 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Units of randomisation: Individual Classroom School Other (specify): Overall confidence of judgement on how participants were assigned: Very low (little basis) Low (guess) Moderate (weak inference) High (strong inference) Very high (explicitly stated) N/A Unknown/unable to code Overall Rating for WoE B (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low Weight of Evidence C C.1 Sample N Referred from the Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Outpatient unit of the University Clinic Cologne. Diagnosis corresponded to Hyperkinetic Conduct disorder (ADHD Europe) (ICD-10 F 90.1). 16 cognitive functioning normal, 2 with LD. 8 on ADHD medication. 18 8 Sample size at start of study Current exposure to alternative intervention - medication Prior exposure to alternative intervention No historical exposure to alternative intervention Single Case Design Participant N Group Experimental Design additional information: 18 All go through baseline then CBT . 2 LD Attrition (drop out) Overall Rating for WoE C (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low Weight of Evidence D Overall Rating for WoE D (1+1+2)/3 = 1.33 (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low 53 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Effect Sizes Relevant Outcomes Type of effect size Effect size Outcome 1. ADHD core symptoms pre-topost-test Comment Unable to compute effect size Outcome 2. Conduct Problems pre-to-posttest Unable to compute effect size Outcome 3. Home Situations pre-to-posttest Unable to compute effect size Outcome 4. Individual Problem List pre-topost-test Unable to compute effect size Outcome 5. Homework Problem Checklist preto-post-test Unable to compute effect size Full Reference: Horn, W. F., Ialongo, N., Greenberg, G., Packard, T., & SmithWinberry, C. (1990). Additive effects of behavioral parent training and self-control therapy with attention deficit hyperactivity disordered children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 19(2), 98 – 110. Weight of Evidence A A.1 Quality of Measures Check Reliability and Validity Multi-method – Multi-source – n/a Outcomes 1: (Parent) – The Child Behaviour Checklist Reliability Validity – Outcome 2: Reliability = Validity = Outcome 3: Reliability = Validity = Overall Rating for measurement (3) Strong evidence: case for validity, reliability above 0.85 (in majority or primary outcomes), multiple methods and multiple sources (2) Promising evidence: reliability above 0.70, multiple methods or multiple sources (1) Weak evidence: reliability above 0.50, multiple methods or multiple sources 54 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth (0) None of the above – Judgement: Only one measure for parents and no reliability or validity noted A.2 Quality of Control Measure Check Control Measure – several comparison groups Type of comparison group Typical intervention Attention placebo Intervention element placebo (intervention minus active ingredient linked to change) Alternative intervention - PT Pharmacotherapy No intervention Wait list/delayed intervention Minimal contact Unable to identify type of comparison – Non-ADHD control group – no details Overall judgement of type of comparison group Very low (little basis) Low (guess) Moderate (weak inference) High (strong inference) Very high (explicitly stated) Unable to identify comparison group Counterbalancing of change agent (e.g. teacher or therapist) By change agent Statistical Other Not reported/none Group equivalence established Random assignment Posthoc matched set Statistical matching Post hoc test for group equivalence Equivalent mortality Low attrition (less than 20% for post) Low attrition (less than 30% for follow up) Intent to intervene analysis carried out (include findings): Overall Rating for Control Measure (3) Strong evidence: at least one active comparison group, group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), counterbalancing, low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (2) Promising evidence: at least one ‘no intervention’ comparison group. Plus at least two of: group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), or counterbalancing or low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (1) Weak evidence: at least one comparison group. Plus at least one of: group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), or counterbalancing or low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (0) no efforts made to control 55 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth A.3 Quality of Intervention Group Experimental Design Primary outcomes in appropriate unit of analysis Primary outcomes familywise/experimenter wise error rate controlled when applicable Primary outcomes have sufficiently large N At least 75% of primary outcomes significant 50-74% of primary outcomes significant 25-49% of primary outcomes significant Less than 25% of primary outcomes significant Overall Rating for Intervention (3) Strong evidence: Significant outcomes for at least 75%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. (2) Promising evidence: Significant outcomes for 50-74%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. (1) Weak evidence: Significant outcomes for 24-49%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. (0) No/Limited evidence: Less than 25% significant outcomes and none of the above Overall Rating for WoE A (0+2+2)/3 = 1.33 (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low Weight of Evidence B B.1 Design Check Single Case Design Check Group Experimental Design Type of Design: Within –Series: Simple phase change Non-randomised design Within – Series: Complex phase Non-randomised block design (between change participants) Between – Series: Comparing two Non-randomised block design (within interventions participants) Between – Series: Comparing Non-randomised hierarchical design interventions with no interventions Combined – Series: Multiple baseline Optional coding for quasi-experimental across participants design Combined – Series: Multiple baseline across behaviours Combined – Series: Multiple baseline across settings Combined – Series: Multiple probe Mixed single participant design (tick two of the above) Mixed single participant and group design (tick one of the above and one in the right hand column) Other (specify): If randomisation used: Completely randomised Randomised block design (between participants e.g. matched classrooms) Randomised block design (within participants) Randomised hierarchical design (nested treatments) Units of randomisation: Individual 56 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Classroom School Other (specify): Overall confidence of judgement on how participants were assigned: Very low (little basis) Low (guess) Moderate (weak inference) High (strong inference) Very high (explicitly stated) N/A Unknown/unable to code Overall Rating for WoE B (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low Weight of Evidence C C.1 Sample N Referred to a university-based psychological clinic for treatment of chronic inattentiveness, impulsivity, and over activity. Diagnosis of ADHD from DSM-III-R. Additional inclusion criteria: not receiving medication for ADHD, absence of psychoses and intellectual difficulties. The Peabody Vocabulary test used to screen for participant’s intellectual status. Control group non-ADHD children. 60 Sample size at start of study Current exposure to alternative intervention Prior exposure to alternative intervention No historical exposure to alternative intervention Single Case Design Participant N additional information: 14 14 (14 18 P1 P2 P3 Overall Rating for WoE C (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low Weight of Evidence D Overall Rating for WoE D (1+3+2) /3 = 2 (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low 57 Group Experimental Design CB SC PT PT + SC) – excluded Control Attrition (drop out) Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Effect Sizes Relevant Outcomes Type of effect size Effect size Outcome 1a. CBCL (Total problems) CB SC pre-to-post-test Cohen’s d (Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 0.62 Outcome 1b. CBCL (Total Problems) CB SC pre-to-follow- up Cohen’s d (Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 0.65 Cohen’s d (Adjusted to Hedge’s g) -0.3 Cohen’s d (Adjusted to Hedge’s g) -0.36 Cohen’s d (Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 0.58 Cohen’s d (Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 0.87 Cohen’s d (Adjusted to Hedge’s g) -0.16 Cohen’s d (Adjusted to Hedge’s g) -0.22 Outcome 3a. CBCL (Hyperactivity) CB SC pre-to-post-test Cohen’s d (Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 0.17 Outcome 3b. CBCL (Hyperactivity) CB SC pre-to-follow-up Cohen’s d (Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 0.18 Outcome 3c. CBCL (Hyperactivity) CB SC vs. PT Control post-test Cohen’s d (Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 0.00 Outcome 3d. CBCL (Hyperactivity) CB SC vs. PT control follow-up Cohen’s d (Adjusted to Hedge’s g) 0.10 Outcome 1c. CBCL (Total Problems) CB SC vs. PT Control posttest Outcome 1d. CBCL (Total Problems) CB SC vs. PT Control follow-up Outcome 2a. CBCL (Externalizing) CB SC pre-to-post-test Outcome 2b. CBCL (Externalizing) CB SC pre-to-follow-up Outcome 2c. CBCL (Externalizing) CB SC vs. PT Control post-test Outcome 2d. CBCL (Externalizing) CB SC vs. PT Control followup 58 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Full Reference: Miranda, A., & Jesús Presentación, M. (2000). Efficacy of cognitivebehavioral therapy in the treatment of children with ADHD, with and without aggressiveness. Psychology in the Schools, 37(2), 169 – 182. Weight of Evidence A A.1 Quality of Measures Check Reliability and Validity Multi-method – Different rating scales (parent: DSM-III-R; EPC) Multi-source Outcomes 1: DSM-III-R adapted questionnaire Reliability Validity – Outcome 2: The Scale of Behavioural Problems (EPC) Reliability = This scale has an alpha coefficient of .88 and a test-retest correlation of .79 in n/a various subscales. Validity = It also has adequate level of construct and criterial validity. Outcome 3: Reliability = Validity = Overall Rating for measurement (3) Strong evidence: case for validity, reliability above 0.85 (in majority or primary outcomes), multiple methods and multiple sources (2) Promising evidence: reliability above 0.70, multiple methods or multiple sources (1) Weak evidence: reliability above 0.50, multiple methods or multiple sources – Judgment: multiple methods, reliability not calculated for this study population. (0) None of the above A.2 Quality of Control Measure Check Control Measure – Type of comparison group Typical intervention Attention placebo Intervention element placebo (intervention minus active ingredient linked to change) Alternative intervention Pharmacotherapy No intervention Wait list/delayed intervention Minimal contact Unable to identify type of comparison Overall judgement of type of comparison group Very low (little basis) Low (guess) Moderate (weak inference) High (strong inference) Very high (explicitly stated) Unable to identify comparison group Counterbalancing of change agent (e.g. teacher or therapist) By change agent Statistical Other 59 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Not reported/none Group equivalence established Random assignment Posthoc matched set Statistical matching Post hoc test for group equivalence Equivalent mortality Low attrition (less than 20% for post) Low attrition (less than 30% for follow up) Intent to intervene analysis carried out (include findings): Overall Rating for Control Measure (3) Strong evidence: at least one active comparison group, group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), counterbalancing, low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (2) Promising evidence: at least one ‘no intervention’ comparison group. Plus at least two of: group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), or counterbalancing or low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (1) Weak evidence: at least one comparison group. Plus at least one of: group equivalence must be established (preferably random assignment), or counterbalancing or low attrition at post and follow up (if applicable) (0) no efforts made to control A.3 Quality of Intervention Group Experimental Design Primary outcomes in appropriate unit of analysis Primary outcomes familywise/experimenter wise error rate controlled when applicable Primary outcomes have sufficiently large N At least 75% of primary outcomes significant 50-74% of primary outcomes significant 25-49% of primary outcomes significant Less than 25% of primary outcomes significant Overall Rating for Intervention (3) Strong evidence: Significant outcomes for at least 75%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. (2) Promising evidence: Significant outcomes for 50-74%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. – Judgement – appropriate significance and familywise error rate controlled.. but not sufficiently large N. (1) Weak evidence: Significant outcomes for 24-49%. Plus appropriateness of analysis, familywise/experimenter wise error controlled and sufficiently large N. (0) No/Limited evidence: Less than 25% significant outcomes and none of the above Overall Rating for WoE A (0+1+2)/3 = 1 (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low 60 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Weight of Evidence B B.1 Design Check Single Case Design Check Group Experimental Design Type of Design: Within –Series: Simple phase change Non-randomised design Within – Series: Complex phase Non-randomised block design (between change participants) Between – Series: Comparing two Non-randomised block design (within interventions participants) Between – Series: Comparing Non-randomised hierarchical design interventions with no interventions Combined – Series: Multiple baseline Optional coding for quasi-experimental across participants design A pretest – posttest (+ follow-up) design – no Combined – Series: Multiple baseline control group. across behaviours Combined – Series: Multiple baseline across settings Combined – Series: Multiple probe Mixed single participant design (tick two of the above) Mixed single participant and group design (tick one of the above and one in the right hand column) Other (specify): If randomisation used: Completely randomised Randomised block design (between participants e.g. matched classrooms) Randomised block design (within participants) Randomised hierarchical design (nested treatments) Units of randomisation: Individual Classroom School Other (specify): Overall confidence of judgement on how participants were assigned: Very low (little basis) Low (guess) Moderate (weak inference) High (strong inference) Very high (explicitly stated) N/A Unknown/unable to code Overall Rating for WoE B (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low 61 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Weight of Evidence C C.1 Sample N Diagnosed by the school psychologists as having chronic problems of inattention, impulsivity, and over activity. Teachers interviewed to corroborate. Then had to meet this criteria: score 15 + on Conners rating Scale, score 16 or higher on DSM-III-R, 1+ persistence of symptoms, IQ score 80+ of WISC, absence of psychosis or any gross neurological, sensory or motor impairment. Iowa Scale for Teachers screened for aggressiveness. 32 Sample size at start of study Current exposure to alternative intervention – only reported significance data for whole non-aggressive group & therefore some receiving AM as well as CB SC. Unable to isolate only CB SC – so sample also receiving AM. Prior exposure to alternative intervention No historical exposure to alternative intervention Single Case Design Participant N Group Experimental Design additional information: P1 8 ADHD + CB SC P2 8 ADHD + CB SC & AM P3 (8 ADHD aggression + CB SC) (8 ADHD aggression + CB SC & AM) Attrition (drop out) Overall Rating for WoE C (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low Weight of Evidence D Overall Rating for WoE D (1+1+1) /3 = 1 (3) High (2) Medium (1) Low Effect Sizes Relevant Outcomes Type of effect size Effect size Comment Outcome 1a. DSM-IIIR (Hyperactivity) preto-post-test Unable to calculate effect size Outcome 1b. DSM-IIIR (Hyperactivity) preto-follow-up Unable to calculate effect size Outcome 2a. EPC (Antisocial Behaviour) pre-to-post-test Unable to calculate effect size Outcome 2b. EPC (Antisocial Behaviour) pre-to-follow-up Unable to calculate effect size Outcome 3a. EPC (Internalisation) pretest-to-post-test Unable to calculate effect size 62 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Relevant Outcomes Type of effect size Effect size Cara Southworth Comment Outcome 3b. EPC (Internalisation) poreto-follow-up Unable to calculate effect size Outcome 4a. EPC (School Problems) preto-post-test Unable to calculate effect size Outcome 4b. EPC (School Problems) preto-follow-up Unable to calculate effect size Outcome 5a. EPC (Psychopath disorders) pre-to-post-test Unable to calculate effect size Outcome 5b. EPC (Psychopath disorders) pre-to-follow-up Unable to calculate effect size 63 Doctorate in Educational and Child Psychology Cara Southworth Appendices References Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gough, D. (2007). Weight of evidence: A framework for the appraisal of the quality and relevance of evidence. In J. Furlong & A. Oancea (Eds.) Research Papers in Education, 22(2), 213-228. Kratochwill, T. R., & Steele Shernoff, E. (2004). Evidence-Based Practice: Promoting Evidence-Based Interventions in School Psychology. School Psychology Review, 33, 34-48. NICE (2009). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Diagnosis and management of ADHD in children young people and adults. London, National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence. 64