Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 101 (2012) 181–186 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/pharmbiochembeh Assessment of the aversive effects of peripheral mu opioid receptor agonism in Fischer 344 and Lewis rats Catherine M. Davis, Jennifer L. Cobuzzi ⁎, Anthony L. Riley Psychopharmacology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, American University, Washington, D.C. 20016, United States a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 9 October 2011 Received in revised form 22 December 2011 Accepted 3 January 2012 Available online 11 January 2012 Keywords: F344 LEW Morphine Loperamide CTA Drug abuse a b s t r a c t The Fischer 344 (F344) and Lewis (LEW) inbred rat strains differ on a host of biochemical, neuroanatomical, immunological and behavioral endpoints. One behavioral difference of interest is their differential reactivity to the aversive effects of morphine as indexed by the conditioned taste aversion preparation (aversions acquired by F344 rats are significantly greater than those acquired by the LEW strain). This differential effect appears to be specific to opioids that work primarily on the mu opioid receptor. Given that morphine works systemically, it is unknown whether these differential effects in F344 and LEW animals are centrally or peripherally mediated. To address this issue, the present study investigated the ability of the peripherally acting mu preferring opioid agonist loperamide to induce differential taste aversions in F344 and LEW animals. Both F344 and LEW animals acquired dose-dependent taste aversions to the loperamide-associated solution with no difference between them. Additionally, control animals initially injected with vehicle during aversion training with loperamide and subsequently conditioned with morphine displayed the typical aversive profile to morphine (F344 > LEW). Although the basis for the present data is unknown, their relation to morphineinduced taste aversions and the role of the interaction of stimulus effects of drugs that produce differential abuse liability were discussed. © 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Previous research has demonstrated that the Fischer 344 (F344) and Lewis (LEW) inbred rat strains differ on a host of biochemical, neuroanatomical, immunological and behavioral endpoints. Initial assessments with these strains focused on their differential stress reactivity (Sternberg et al., 1992) and responsivity to immunological challenges (Sun et al., 1999). More recently, the F344 and LEW strains have been investigated relative to their differential reactivity to a variety of drugs of abuse (for a review of this issue, see Kosten and Ambrosio, 2002; Riley et al., 2009; Riley, 2011). Although the initial work with these animals assessed the rewarding effects of such drugs, subsequent investigations analyzed their differential responsivity to the drugs' aversive properties. In one of the first assessments of differences in the aversive effects of drugs in these strains, Lancellotti et al. (2001) reported that the F344 strain rapidly acquired a robust taste aversion to a morphine-associated solution. LEW rats, ⁎ Correspondence author at: Psychopharmacology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, American University, 4400 Mass. Ave., NW Washington, DC 20016, United States. Tel.: + 1 202 885 1721; fax: + 1 202 885 1081. E-mail address: jc9378a@american.edu (J.L. Cobuzzi). 0091-3057/$ – see front matter © 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2012.01.001 on the other hand, failed to acquire an aversion even at high doses and with repeated conditioning trials (for subsequent assessments, see Gomez-Serrano et al., 2009; for a review, see Riley, 2011). This strain difference in morphine-induced taste aversion learning was not likely due to a general difference in learning or in the processing of gustatory stimuli between the strains, in that LEW animals display stronger cocaine-induced taste aversions at low and intermediate doses than their F344 counterparts (Glowa et al., 1994), an effect opposite to that seen with morphine (Lancellotti et al., 2001). Instead, it is more likely that the differential acquisition of taste aversions in response to morphine administration in the F344 and LEW animals is a consequence of their differential sensitivity to its aversive effects (for similar assessments with other compounds, see Desko et al., 2011; Foynes and Riley, 2004; Glowa et al., 1994; Pescatore et al., 2005; Roma et al., 2006; Vishwanath et al., 2011). In an attempt to isolate the specific opioid receptor on which morphine might be working to induce its behavioral effects within the aversion preparation, Davis et al. (2009) examined the ability of three opiate agonists with differential binding profiles, e.g., SNC80 (relative selectivity for the delta opioid receptor subtype), (−)U50,488H (relative selectivity for the kappa opioid receptor subtype) and heroin (affinity for all three receptor subtypes with preference for the mu opioid receptor) to induce aversions in the F344 and LEW rat strains. Although all three drugs induced aversions, those induced by heroin were significantly stronger than those induced by 182 C.M. Davis et al. / Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 101 (2012) 181–186 SNC80 and (−)-U50,488H. Further, the only drug for which F344 and LEW animals differed was heroin, wherein F344 rats acquired significantly stronger aversions than the LEW strain, an effect consistent with the prior work with morphine (Lancellotti et al., 2001). Although both heroin and morphine act non-specifically on all three receptor subtypes, the differential aversions with these drugs coupled with the failure of either the delta or kappa agonists to induce straindependent aversions support the position that morphine's aversive effects are likely mediated by its action at the mu opioid receptor (alone or in combination with other opioid receptor subtypes; see Davis et al., 2009) and suggest that differential action at this site mediates the observed strain differences with morphine. Although these results suggest that the mu opioid receptor is involved in the behavioral differences between the F344 and LEW strains in the acquisition of morphine-induced taste aversions, it is not known whether this effect is centrally or peripherally mediated, especially given that systemically administered morphine (and heroin) act at both sites (Davis et al., 2009). Interestingly, there is relatively little work investigating central vs. peripheral mediation of taste aversion learning, and for the drugs that have been investigated, the mechanism of action appears drug specific. For example, for THC (Amit et al., 1977) and nicotine (Kumar et al., 1983; Shoaib and Stolerman, 1995), taste aversions appear to be mediated centrally; for ethanol (Crankshaw et al., 2003), lithium chloride (LiCl; Smith, 1980), acetaldehyde (Brown et al., 1978), fenfluramine (Lorden et al., 1980), and fluoxetine (Lorden and Nunn, 1982), aversions appear to be mediated via peripheral mechanisms. Interestingly, the work with morphine in outbred rats is quite consistent on the peripheral mediation of such aversions. For example, morphine administered icv (Stapleton et al., 1979) and direct infusions of morphine into either the hippocampus or caudate fail to induce taste aversions (see Amit et al., 1977). In more direct assessments of central vs. peripheral mediation, Bechara et al. (1987) reported that methylnaltrexone, an opioid antagonist that does not cross the blood brain barrier, blocked the acquisition of both morphine-induced place and taste aversions when morphine was given intraperitoneally (for a related report, see also Martin et al., 1988). Morphine-induced taste aversions are also blocked by the destruction of peripheral opiate receptors with capsaicin. In a related report (see Bechara and van der Kooy, 1985), vagotomy blocked the acquisition of both place and taste aversions induced by systemic morphine, indicating that peripherally-located opioid receptors are a necessary component of the aversive response to morphine administration. If morphine's effects are mediated peripherally, it would be expected that peripherally acting, mu agonists would induce aversions. Further, it would be expected that if the differential effects of morphine in the F344 and LEW strains are mediated by these receptors, such drugs would differentially induce aversions in the two strains (F344 > LEW). These predictions were tested in the following experiment in which rats from the F344 and LEW strains were given a novel saccharin solution to drink and then injected with vehicle or various doses of the peripherally acting, mu agonist loperamide (Awouters et al., 1983; DeHaven-Hudkins et al., 1999; Giagnoni et al., 1982; Nozaki-Taguchi and Yaksh, 1999; Wüster and Herz, 1978). Following this treatment, animals injected with vehicle during conditioning were subsequently given saccharin followed by morphine to confirm the typical strain difference (where F344 subjects display greater morphine-induced aversions than the LEW strain). Given that drug use and abuse are thought to be a function of the balance between the rewarding and aversive effects of a compound (Riley, 2011; Wise et al., 1976), understanding the factors that impact both these properties may lead to a better understanding of the vulnerability to drug use and abuse. Genetic mediation of these affective properties is one such factor, and the F344 and LEW animals provide a useful model to investigate genetic contributions to the relative balance between the aversive and rewarding effects of drugs. 2. Material and methods 2.1. Subjects and apparatus Subjects were 134 experimentally naive LEW (n = 67) and F344 (n = 67) male rats (purchased from Harlan Sprague Dawley, Indianapolis). Subjects were run in two replicates (n = 33 per strain, Replicate 1; n = 34 per strain, Replicate 2), and all groups for each strain were represented in each replicate. Given that there were no differences between replicates in terms of the ability of loperamide [t (132) = 1.092,p = 0.277] or morphine [t(132) = 0.178,p = 0.859] to induce taste aversions, data from the two replicates were pooled for analysis and presentation. At the start of the experiment, animals were approximately 90 days of age and weighed approximately 300 g (LEW) and 250 g (F344). Animals were housed in individual wire-mesh cages and maintained on a 12:12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 0800 h) and at an ambient temperature of 23 °C for the duration of the experiment. Rat chow (Harlan Sprague–Dawley, Indianapolis, Indiana) was provided ad libitum. All fluids were presented in 50ml Nalgene tubes affixed to the front of the cages. Procedures recommended by the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (National Research Council, 1996) and the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at American University were followed at all times. 2.2. Drugs and solutions Loperamide hydrochloride (Sigma) was dissolved in 10% dimethylsulfoxide (DMSO) and saline (5 mg/ml), and morphine sulfate (generously supplied by the National Institute on Drug Abuse) was dissolved in saline (5 mg/ml). All drug doses were expressed as the salt form and were administered subcutaneously (sc); doses used for loperamide were selected from dose ranges previously reported (see Medvedev et al., 1998; Shinoda et al., 2007). Sodium saccharin (Sigma) was prepared as a 1 g/l solution in tap water. 2.3. Procedure 2.3.1. Phase I: Habituation After 23 2/3-h water deprivation, rats were given 20-min access to water, beginning at 1000 h. This procedure was repeated daily until all rats were approaching and drinking from the tube within 2 s of its presentation. Once this criterion was reached, aversion conditioning began. 2.3.2. Phase II: Loperamide conditioning On Day 1 of this phase, animals received 20-min access to a novel saccharin solution during their daily fluid-access period. Immediately following saccharin access, rats within each strain were assigned to one of four groups such that consumption was comparable across groups and injected with one of four doses of loperamide. Specifically, subjects were injected with 0 mg/kg (vehicle; n = 16 per strain), 10 mg/kg (n = 17 per strain), 18 mg/kg (n = 17 per strain) or 32 mg/ kg (n = 17 per strain) of loperamide, yielding Groups F0, F10, F18, F32, L0, L10, L18 and L32. For each group, the letter denotes the strain of the animal and the number denotes the dose of loperamide administered. On Days 2–4 of this phase, all animals received 20-min access to water during the fluid-access period. No injections were given following this access. This cycle of one conditioning day followed by 3 water-recovery days was repeated for three full cycles. The fourth conditioning trial was treated as a one-bottle test of the aversion to saccharin. 2.3.3. Phase III: Morphine conditioning Following 3 water-recovery days, animals injected with vehicle during Phase II, i.e., Groups F0 and L0, were given 20-min access to C.M. Davis et al. / Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 101 (2012) 181–186 saccharin. Immediately following saccharin access, rats within each strain were assigned to one of two groups such that consumption was comparable between groups and injected with either vehicle or morphine (5 mg/kg), yielding Groups FV-V, FV-M, LV-V and LV-M (n's = 8 per group). For each group, the first two letters denote the strain of the animal and the treatment during Phase II; the third letter denotes the injection given following saccharin in Phase III. On the 3 days following morphine conditioning, all subjects were given 20min access to water. No injections followed these water-recovery sessions. This procedure of conditioning followed by 3 water-recovery days was repeated for four additional cycles. On the day following the last cycle of Phase III, all subjects were given 20-min access to saccharin in a one-bottle aversion test. 2.4. Statistical analysis An Independent Sample's t-test revealed that LEW and F344 rats differed in consumption on the initial saccharin exposure at the outset of Phase II [t(132) = 6.692, p b 0.001]. Consequently, each strain was evaluated independently. For each strain, differences in mean saccharin consumption during loperamide conditioning (Phase II) was analyzed using a 4 × 4 mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) with a between-subjects factor of Dose (0, 10, 18, 32 mg/kg) and a within-subjects factor of Trials (1–4). Given that saccharin consumption at the outset of Phase III did not differ between F344 and LEW animals [t(132) = 0.636, p = 0.526], saccharin consumption during morphine treatment (Phase III) was analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 5 mixed ANOVA with between-subjects factors of Strain (F344; LEW) and Drug (VEH; MOR) and a within-subjects factor of Trials (1–5). Where appropriate, one-way ANOVAs followed by Tukey's post-hoc tests were performed on each trial to analyze differences in saccharin consumption between groups; significance was assessed at α ≤ 0.05. 3. Results 3.1. Phase II: Loperamide conditioning F344: The Trial × Dose mixed ANOVA on mean saccharin consumption over conditioning (Phase II) revealed significant effects of Trial [F(3,189) = 163.219, p b 0.001] and Dose [F(3,63) = 97.907, p b 0.001] as well as a significant Trial × Dose interaction [F(9,189) = 32.968, p b 0.001]. In relation to this interaction, a subsequent oneway ANOVA revealed significant differences between doses of loperamide on Trials 2, 3 and 4 (all p's b 0.001). Tukey's post-hoc analysis indicated that on Trials 2, 3 and 4, F344 animals injected with 10, 18 and 32 mg/kg loperamide consumed significantly less saccharin than F344 animals injected with vehicle (p's b 0.001) and on Trials 2 and 3 F344 animals injected with 32 mg/kg loperamide consumed significantly less saccharin than F344 animals injected with 10 mg/ kg loperamide (p's b 0.05; see Fig. 1, Panel A). LEW: The Trial × Dose mixed ANOVA on mean saccharin consumption revealed significant main effects of Trial [F(3,189) = 217.021, p b 0.001] and Dose [F(3,63) = 833.65, p b 0.001] as well as a significant Trial × Dose [F(9,189) = 27.193, p b 0.001] interaction. In relation to this interaction, a subsequent one-way ANOVA revealed significant differences between doses of loperamide on Trials 2, 3 and 4 (all p's b 0.001). Tukey's post-hoc analysis indicated that on Trials 2, 3 and 4 LEW animals injected with 10, 18 and 32 mg/kg loperamide consumed significantly less saccharin than LEW animals injected with vehicle (p's b 0.001). Further, on Trial 3 LEW animals injected with 32 mg/kg loperamide consumed significantly less saccharin than LEW animals injected with 10 or 18 mg/kg loperamide (p's b 0.05) and on Trial 4 LEW animals injected with 32 mg/kg loperamide consumed significantly less saccharin than LEW animals injected with 10 mg/kg loperamide (pb 0.05; see Fig. 1, Panel B). A Strain × Dose univariate ANOVA on saccharin consumption during the one-bottle aversion test (Trial 4) of Phase II revealed a significant main effect of Dose [F(3,126) = 269.794, p b 0.001] and no other significant effects or interactions. Tukey's post-hoc analysis revealed that animals injected with 10, 18 and 32 mg/kg loperamide consumed significantly less saccharin than vehicle-injected animals (p'sb 0.001) and animals injected with 32 mg/kg loperamide consumed significantly less saccharin than animals injected with 10 mg/kg loperamide (pb 0.01). There was no significant effect of Strain (alone or in combination with Dose). 3.2. Phase III: Morphine conditioning The Strain × Drug × Trial mixed ANOVA on mean saccharin consumption over morphine conditioning (Phase III) revealed significant main effects of Trial [F(4,112) = 8.618, p b 0.001], Strain [F(1,28) = 27.816, p b 0.001] and Drug [F(1,28) = 94.208, p b 0.001], as well as significant Trial × Strain [F(4,112) = 10.311, p b 0.001], Trial× Drug [F (4,112) = 51.493, p b 0.001], Strain × Drug [F(1,28) = 32.35, p b 0.001] and Trial× Strain × Drug [F(4,112)= 18.689, p b 0.001] interactions. In relation to this final interaction, a subsequent one-way ANOVA indicated significant group differences on Trials 2–5 (all p's b 0.001). Tukey's post hoc analysis revealed that on Trials 2–5 F344 animals injected with morphine consumed significantly less saccharin than F344 animals injected with vehicle (all p's b 0.001; see Fig. 2, Panel A), indicative of the formation of a robust taste aversion to morphine in F344 animals. LEW animals injected with morphine consumed significantly less saccharin than LEW animals injected with vehicle only on Trials 4 and 5 (both p's b 0.05; see Fig. 2, Panel B). On Trials 2–5, F344 animals injected with morphine consumed significantly less saccharin than comparably treated LEW animals (all p's b 0.005), F344 Mean Saccharin Consumption (ml) 15 LEW A 15 * 183 * B * * 10 * * 10 # 5 V 10 18 32 # 0 1 2 3 Trial 4 5 # ^ 0 1 2 3 4 Trial Fig. 1. Mean (± SEM) saccharin consumption throughout Phase II: CTA Conditioning, by strain. Panel A: (F344) *F344 animals injected with 32, 18 and 10 mg/kg loperamide consumed significantly less saccharin than F344 animals injected with vehicle. #F344 animals injected with 32 mg/kg loperamide consumed significantly less saccharin than F344 animals injected with 10 mg/kg loperamide. Panel B: (LEW) *LEW animals injected with 32, 18 and 10 mg/kg loperamide consumed significantly less saccharin than LEW animals injected with vehicle. #LEW animals injected with 32 mg/kg loperamide consumed significantly less saccharin than LEW animals injected with 10 or 18 mg/kg loperamide. ^LEW animals injected with 32 mg/kg loperamide consumed significantly less saccharin than LEW animals injected with 10 mg/kg loperamide. 184 C.M. Davis et al. / Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 101 (2012) 181–186 Mean Saccharin Consumption (ml) 15 A 10 * F344 * LEW * * 15 B * * 4 5 10 # # # 5 5 # V-V V-M 0 1 2 3 4 5 Trial 0 1 2 3 Trial Fig. 2. Mean (± SEM) saccharin consumption throughout Phase III: Morphine Conditioning, by strain. Panel A: F344 animals injected with vehicle in Phase II and undergoing morphine conditioning. *F344 animals injected with morphine consumed significantly less saccharin than F344 animals injected with vehicle. #F344 animals injected with morphine consumed significantly less saccharin than LEW animals injected with morphine (see Panel B). Panel B: LEW animals injected with vehicle in Phase II and undergoing morphine conditioning. *LEW animals injected with morphine consumed significantly less saccharin than LEW animals injected with vehicle. indicative of strain differences in the acquisition of the morphineinduced aversion. Vehicle-injected animals did not differ in saccharin consumption during this phase (all p's > 0.05). 4. Discussion The F344 and LEW inbred rat strains display differential acquisition and expression of taste aversions in response to morphine (and heroin) administration (Davis et al., 2009; Lancellotti et al., 2001). It is unknown whether this differential response is mediated centrally or peripherally given that systemic morphine (and heroin) works at both sites (Davis et al., 2009). To test a possible peripheral mediation of aversions in the two strains, the present experiment examined the ability of the peripherally acting, mu preferring agonist loperamide to produce aversions in F344 and LEW rats. In this assessment, F344 and LEW animals developed comparable dose-dependent aversions to the loperamide-associated solution, i.e., loperamide induced aversions that were not strain dependent. These results are in marked contrast to those with systemically-acting opioid agonists, e.g., morphine, where the F344 strain displays greater aversions than the LEW strain. Interestingly, animals initially injected with vehicle during loperamide conditioning and subsequently conditioned with morphine displayed a profile similar to that typically reported with morphine. As described, F344 animals displayed a robust taste aversion relative to LEW animals and their own vehicle-controls, where LEW animals displayed a weak and late-onset taste aversion to the morphineassociated solution. The differences reported here with morphine are unlikely a function of differential latent inhibition in the two strains, given that they parallel those seen in animals without such a history (Lancellotti et al., 2001). It is important to note, however, that latent inhibition has not been examined in these two strains and any role that it might have in animals with familiarization to the taste CS is not known (although the highly correlated prepulse inhibition response does not differ between the two strains under baseline conditions; see Varty and Geyer, 1998). Given that loperamide is a peripherally-acting opioid with limited access to the CNS (Awouters et al., 1983; DeHaven-Hudkins et al., 1999; Giagnoni et al., 1982; Nozaki-Taguchi and Yaksh, 1999; Wüster and Herz, 1978), aversions induced by loperamide are likely mediated by peripherally-located opioid receptors. The fact that loperamide can induce such aversions by its action at these peripheral sites is consistent with other assessments of opiate-induced aversions in outbred animals in which peripheral sites of action have been implicated. For example, differential manipulations that result in the destruction of peripherally located opiate receptors (Bechara and van der Kooy, 1985; Bechara et al., 1987) result in the loss of morphine-induced place and taste aversions. Additionally, the administration of opiate antagonists, such as methylnaltrexone that do not readily cross the blood brain barrier, block morphine-induced place and taste aversions (Bechara et al., 1987). Further, administration of morphine into the hippocampus and caudate putamen (Amit et al., 1977) or via an intracerebroventricular (icv) route (Hunt et al., 1983) fails to induce taste aversions (though see Liu and Grigson, 2005 for evidence of aversions induced by icv DAMGO). What remains to be explained is what the present results mean in relation to the reported differences between the F344 and LEW strains in morphine-induced taste aversions (Lancellotti et al., 2001; see also Davis et al., 2009). As noted, morphine-induced aversions are significantly greater in the F344 strain relative to LEW rats (see also present results with morphine). Given that peripherally-acting loperamide induces aversions that are not strain-dependent suggests one of two possibilities in relation to the effects with morphine. First, morphine may act on different subtypes (and/or varieties) of peripheral opiate receptors (Pasternak, 2004; Wolozin and Pasternak, 1981) than does loperamide and these different sites of peripheral action may mediate the differential effects produced by the two drugs (i.e., strain differences with morphine; no strain differences with loperamide). A second possibility is that morphine may be acting centrally to produce its strain-dependent effects. In relation to the possible differences between morphine and loperamide in their peripheral activity, Shannon and Lutz (2002) reported that 10 times more naloxone (a mu-preferring opioid antagonist) was required to antagonize the analgesic effects of loperamide than morphine when both compounds were administered subcutaneously. This suggests that loperamide and morphine work on separate subsets of opiate receptors in the peripheral nervous system or that they possess differential binding affinity to their preferred receptor type that might lead to different aversive profiles. Others argue, however, that loperamide and morphine work on the same receptor subtype (Awouters et al., 1983; Mackerer et al., 1976; Manara and Bianchetti, 1985), so it is unknown to what degree, if any, differential binding in the periphery mediates the reported differences in aversion learning in response to morphine and loperamide. Further, if it were the case that these compounds work on different opiate receptors to produce their effects, it would also have to be demonstrated that LEW and F344 rats differ in binding to morphine at such peripheral sites to account for the differential behavioral effects with morphine. In the absence of direct assessments matching the parameters under which the behavioral differences have been reported, the possibility of such differential peripheral action between the F344 and LEW rats remains speculative. An alternative explanation for the differential mediation of aversions in the F344 and LEW strains with morphine is associated with its central activity following systemic administration. In this context, morphine may be acting centrally on some subset of receptors, e.g., kappa opioid receptors, to induce taste aversions and the strains differ in kappa opioid receptor activation. Although possible, Davis et al. (2009) have recently reported that when the kappa agonist C.M. Davis et al. / Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 101 (2012) 181–186 U50,488H was used to induce taste aversions, aversions were acquired in both the F344 and LEW strains, but there was no strain difference. Another possibility involving central mediation concerns morphine's rewarding effects which are generally associated with its central action (Bechara and van der Kooy, 1985; Koob and Nestler, 1997). It is possible that morphine's central activity modulates any aversive effects produced by its peripheral action. Accordingly, this position argues that the peripheral aversive effects produced by morphine are similar in the LEW and F344 strain but the central, rewarding effects differ. Given that the LEW strain has been reported to display greater opiate-induced conditioned place preferences and more robust opiate self-administration (Ambrosio et al., 1995; Martin et al., 1999; Sánchez-Cardoso et al., 2007), these animals are generally assumed to display greater sensitivity to the opiate's rewarding effects. This greater rewarding effect may be impacting morphine's perceived aversiveness, reducing its ability to induce taste aversions. Given that loperamide acts peripherally due to either its restriction by the blood–brain barrier (DeHavenHudkins et al., 1999; Giagnoni et al., 1982; Nozaki-Taguchi and Yaksh, 1999; Stahl et al., 1977) or its accumulation in peripheral tissues and subsequent rapid metabolism (Awouters et al., 1983; Wüster and Herz, 1978), its ability to activate central receptors is minimal, if at all, and aversions are comparable for the two strains. Although this interaction of central and peripheral activity is possible, in the absence of any direct test of central opiate modulation of peripheral aversions, this, too, must remain speculative (see also Liu and Grigson, 2005). The position suggests that drugs have multiple stimulus effects that may interact in such a way to impact their perceived effect which in turn may impact use and abuse vulnerability (Riley, 2011; Stolerman and D'Mello, 1981; Verendeev and Riley, 2011; Wise et al., 1976). Independent of the mechanism underlying the differential expression of morphine-induced taste aversions in F344 and LEW animals (F344 > LEW), it is important to note that there are other compounds for which the strains display differential aversions. Specifically, F344 animals display greater nicotine- and ethanol-induced aversions than LEW rats (Pescatore et al., 2005; Roma et al., 2006), while LEW animals display stronger cocaine- and caffeine-induced aversions than F344 rats (Glowa et al., 1994; Vishwanath et al., 2011). Given that these compounds come from very different drug classes and that the direction of the differences in the acquisition of taste aversions in the two strains is drug dependent, it is unlikely that any common neurochemical or neuroanatomical pathway mediates the aversions or the differences between the two strains. Although the basis for the differences remains unknown, further examination of other compounds in their ability to induce aversions in the two strains may provide a more thorough characterization of the basis for the reported strain differences. The focus of the present work was to identify the central or peripheral mediation of the aversive effects of morphine in the F344 and LEW strains. This emphasis makes the assumption that the avoidance of tastes previously paired with morphine (or any drug) is a function of the drug's aversive effects. It should be noted, however, that there are other interpretations of this behavioral avoidance (for a history of aversion learning, see Freeman and Riley, 2009). One particular position argues that the avoidance of drug-associated tastes does not reflect anything about the aversive effects of the drug at all, but instead reflects the drug's rewarding properties. This position, the reward comparison hypothesis, argues that the reduction in the intake of the drug-paired taste is a function of the acquired associated between the taste and the rewarding effects of the drug such that when presented with the taste on subsequent exposures the rat anticipates the drug. Given that the taste pales in comparison to the drug, it is avoided (Grigson, 1997; Grigson et al., 2009). This hypothesis predicts that there should be a direct relationship between the ability of a drug to suppress consumption and produce their 185 rewarding effects, given that avoidance is a function of the drug's rewarding effects. Interestingly, data in support of this position is limited and a number of investigations have demonstrated that the rewarding and suppressive effects of drugs are dissociable (see Gaiardi et al., 1991; Martin et al., 1988; Simpson and Riley, 2005). Most recently, Verendeev and Riley (2011) performed a direct test of the reward comparison hypothesis by investigating individual subject differences in sensitivity to the rewarding and aversive effects of both morphine and amphetamine. Specifically, they ran a concurrent CTA/CPP preparation in which animals were given access to a novel saccharin solution, injected with morphine (or amphetamine) and then placed in one compartment of a place preference chamber. Under these conditions, taste aversions and place preferences were acquired, but there was no relationship between the two, i.e., individual animals that displayed strong taste aversions were just as likely to show weak or strong place preferences, and vice versa (see also Turenne et al., 1996). Taken together, these data provide evidence that the subjective effects produced by drugs of abuse can in fact be dissociated and the rewarding effects of drugs are not likely mediating the reduced consumption of the taste in the conditioned taste aversion preparation. The present assessment is the first of its kind to address the issue of whether the strain difference observed in morphine-induced taste aversions might be mediated via central or peripheral mechanisms. This assessment was made by examining if the peripherally acting mu agonist loperamide differentially induced aversions in the two strains. As reported, the F344 and LEW rat strains do not differ in their acquisition of aversions induced by the peripherally acting, mu agonist loperamide, despite the fact that they display the typical aversive profile (F344 > LEW) in response to the systemic opioid morphine. Although the basis for the differential reactivity of these strains to morphine is not known, these results suggest a central mediation of the strain difference in morphine-induced aversions, possibly involving the drug's rewarding effects. Acknowledgments This research was supported by a grant from the Mellon Foundation to A.L.R. The Mellon Foundation had no further role in the study design, data collection, analysis and interpretation, the writing of the report, or the decision to submit the manuscript for publication. References Ambrosio E, Goldberg SR, Elmer GI. Behavior genetic investigation of the relationship between spontaneous locomotor activity and the acquisition of morphine selfadministration behavior. Behav Pharmacol 1995;6:229–37. Amit Z, Levitan DE, Brown ZW, Rogan F. Possible involvement of central factors in the mediation of conditionted taste aversion. Neuropharmacology 1977;16:121–4. Awouters F, Niemegeers CJE, Janssen PAJ. Pharmacology of antidiarrheal drugs. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol 1983;23:279–301. Bechara A, van der Kooy D. Opposite motivational effects of endogenous opioids in brain and periphery. Nature 1985;314:533–4. Bechara A, Zito KA, van der Kooy D. Peripheral receptors mediate the aversive conditioning effects of morphine in the rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1987;28:219–55. Brown ZW, Amit Z, Smith B, Rockman GE. Differential effects on conditioned taste aversion learning with peripherally and centrally administered acetaldehyde. Neurophamacology 1978;17:931–5. Crankshaw DL, Briggs JE, Olszewski PK, Shi Q, Grace MK, Billington CJ, et al. Effects of intracerevroventricular ethanol on ingestive behavior and induction of c-Fos immunoreactivity in selected brain regions. Physiol Behav 2003;79:113–20. giardi. Davis CM, Rice KC, Riley AL. Opiate-agonist induced taste aversion learning in the Fischer 344 and Lewis rat strains: evidence for differential mu opioid receptor activation. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2009;93:397–405. DeHaven-Hudkins DL, Cortes Burgos L, Cassel JA, Daubert JD, DeHaven RN, Mansson E, et al. Loperamide (ADL 2–1294), an opioid antihyperalgesic agent with peripheral selectivity. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1999;289:494–502. Desko AG, Cobuzzi JL, Riley AL. Naloxone-induced conditioned taste aversions in opiate-naïve Lewis and Fischer 344 rat strains. Drug Alcohol Depend 2011. doi: 10.1016/j/drugalcdep.2011.09.016. Foynes MM, Riley AL. Lithium-chloride-induced conditioned taste aversions in the Lewis and Fischer 344 rat strains. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2004;79:303–8. 186 C.M. Davis et al. / Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 101 (2012) 181–186 Freeman KB, Riley AL. The origins of conditioned taste aversion learning: a historical analysis. In: Reilly S, Schachtman TR, editors. Conditioned taste aversion: behavioral and neural processes. New York: Oxford University Press; 2009. p. 9-33. Gaiardi M, Bartoletti M, Bacchi A, Gubellini C, Costa M, Babbini M. Role of repeated exposure to morphine in determining its affective properties: place and taste conditioning studies in rats. Psychopharmacology 1991;103:183–6. Giagnoni G, Parolaro D, Sala M, Marabini L, Senini R, Gori E. Increase of plasma corticosterone induced by loperamide in rats. Eur J Pharmacol 1982;79:101–4. Glowa JR, Shaw AE, Riley AL. Cocaine-induced conditioned taste aversions: comparisons between effects in LEW/N and F344/N rat strains. Psychopharmacology 1994;114:229–32. Gomez-Serrano MA, Kearns DN, Riley AL. The effects of light cycle phase on morphineinduced conditioned taste aversions in the Lewis, Fischer and Sprague–Dawley rat strains. Behav Brain Res 2009;196:116–22. Grigson PS. Conditioned taste aversions and drugs of abuse: a reinterpretation. Behav Neurosci 1997;111:129–36. Grigson PS, Twining RC, Freet CS, Wheeler RA, Geddes RI. Drug-induced suppression of conditioned stimulus intake: reward, aversion, and addiction. In: Reilly S, Schachtman TR, editors. Conditioned taste aversion: behavioral and neural processes. New York: Oxford University Press; 2009. p. 74–91. Hunt T, Amit Z, Switzman L, Sinyor D. An aversive naloxone–morphine interaction in rats. Neurosci Lett 1983;35:311–5. Koob GF, Nestler EJ. The neurobiology of drug addiction. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1997;9:482–97. Kosten TA, Ambrosio E. HPA axis function and drug addictive behaviors: insights from studies with Lewis and Fischer 344 inbred rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2002;27:35–69. Kumar R, Pratt JA, Stolerman IP. Characteristics of conditioned taste aversion produced by nicotine in rats. Br J Pharmacol 1983;79:245–53. Lancellotti D, Bayer BM, Glowa JR, Houghtling RA, Riley AL. Morphine-induced conditioned taste aversions in the LEW/N and F344/N rat strains. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2001;68:603–10. Liu C, Grigson PS. Mu opioid receptor agonist DAMGO-induced suppression of saccharin intake in Lewis and Fischer rats. Brain Res 2005;1064:155–60. Lorden JF, Nunn WB. Effects of central and peripheral pretreatment with fluoxetine in gustatory conditioning. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1982;17:435–43. Lorden JF, Callahan M, Dawson Jr R. Depletion of central catecholamines alters amphetamine- and fenfluramine-induced taste aversions in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1980;94:99-114. Mackerer CR, Clay GA, Dajani EZ. Loperamide binding to opiate receptor sites of brain and myenteric plexus. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1976;199:131–40. Manara L, Bianchetti A. The central and peripheral influences of opioids on gastrointestinal propulsion. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol 1985;25:249–73. Martin GM, Bechara A, van der Kooy D. Morphine preexposure attenuates the aversive properties of opiates without preexposure to the aversive properties. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1988;30:687–92. Martin S, Manzanares J, Corchero J, Garcia-Lecumberri C, Crespo JA, Fuentes JA, et al. Differential basal proenkephalin gene expression in dorsal striatum and nucleus accumbens, and vulnerability to morphine self-administration in Fischer 344 and Lewis rats. Brain Res 1999;821:350–5. Medvedev IO, Dravolina OA, Bespalov AY. Effects of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonists on discriminitave stimulus effects of naloxone in morphine-dependent rats using the y-maze drug discrimination paradigm. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1998;286:1260–8. National Research Council. Guidelines for the care and use of mammals in neuroscience and behavioral research. Washington, DC: National Academy; 1996. p. 1-125. Nozaki-Taguchi N, Yaksh TL. Characterization of the antihyperalgesic action of a novel peripheral mu-opioid receptor agonist — loperamide. Anesthesiology 1999;90: 225–34. Pasternak GW. Multiple opiate receptors: déjà vu all over again. Neuropharmacology 2004;47:312–23. Pescatore KA, Glowa JR, Riley AL. Strain differences in the acquisition of nicotineinduced conditioned taste aversion. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2005;82:751–7. Riley AL. The paradox of drug taking: the role of the aversive effects of drugs. Physiol Behav 2011;103:69–78. Riley AL, Davis CM, Roma PG. Strain differences in taste aversion learning: implication for animal models of drug abuse. In: Reilly S, Schachtman TR, editors. Conditioned taste aversion: behavioral and neural processes. New York: Oxford University Press; 2009. p. 226–61. Roma PG, Flint WW, Higley JD, Riley AL. Assessment of the aversive and rewarding effects of alcohol in Fischer and Lewis rats. Psychopharmacology 2006;189:187–99. Sánchez-Cardoso P, Higuera-Matas A, Martin S, del Olmo N, Miguens M, Garcia-Lecumberri C, et al. Modulation of the endogenous opioid system after morphine selfadministration and during its extinction: a study in Lewis and Fischer 344 rats. Neuropharmacology 2007;52:931–48. Shannon HE, Lutz EA. Comparison of the peripheral and central effects of the opioid agonists loperamide and morphine in the formalin test in rats. Neuropharmacology 2002;42:253–61. Shinoda K, Hruby VJ, Porreca F. Antihyperalgesic effects of loperamide in a model of rat neuropathic pain are mediated by peripheral [delta]-opioid receptors. Neurosci Lett 2007;411:143–6. Shoaib M, Stolerman IP. Conditioned taste aversions in rats after intracerebral administration of nicotine. Behav Pharmacol 1995;6:375–85. Simpson GR, Riley AL. Morphine preexposure facilitates morphine place preference and attenuates morphine taste aversion. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2005;80: 471–9. Smith DF. Central and peripheral effects of lithium on conditioned taste aversions in rats. Psychopharmacology 1980;68:315–7. Stahl KD, van Bever W, Janssen P, Simon EJ. Receptor affinity and pharmacological potency of a series of narcotic analgesic, anti-diarrheal and neuroleptic drugs. Eur J Pharmacol 1977;46:199–205. Stapleton JM, Lind MD, Merriman VJ, Bozarth MA, Reid LD. Affective consequences and subsequent effects on morphine self-administration of D-ala2-methionine enkephalin. Physiol Psychol 1979;7:146–52. Sternberg EM, Glowa JR, Smith MA, Calogero AE, Listwak SJ, Aksentijevich S, et al. Corticotropin releasing hormone related behavioral and neuroendocrine responses to stress in Lewis and Fischer rats. Brain Res 1992;570:54–60. Stolerman IP, D'Mello GD. Oral self-administration and the relevance of conditioned taste aversions. In: Thompson T, Dews PB, McKim WA, editors. Advances in behavioral pharmacology. New York: Academic Press; 1981. p. 169–214. Sun D, Whitaker JN, Wilson DB. Regulatory T cells in experimental allergic encephalomyelitis. III. Comparison of disease resistance in Lewis and Fischer 344 rats. Eur J Immunol 1999;29:1101–6. Turenne SD, Miles C, Parker LA, Siegel S. Individual differences in reactivity to the rewarding/aversive properties of drugs: assessment by taste and place conditioning. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1996;53:511–6. Varty GB, Geyer MA. Effects of isolation rearing on startle reactivity, habituation, and prepulse inhibition in male Lewis, Sprague–Dawley, and Fischer 344 rats. Behav Neurosci 1998;112:1450–7. Verendeev A, Riley AL. Relationship between the rewarding and aversive effects of morphine and amphetamine in individual subjects. Learn Behav 2011. doi: 10.3758/s13420-011-0035-5. Vishwanath JM, Desko AG, Riley AL. Caffeine-induced taste aversions in Lewis and Fischer rat strains: differential sensitivity to the aversive effects of drugs. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2011;100:66–72. Wise RA, Yokel RA, DeWitt H. Both positive reinforcement and conditioned aversion from amphetamine and from apomorphine in rats. Science 1976;191:1273–5. Wolozin BL, Pasternak GW. Classification of multiple morphine and enkephalin binding sites in the central nervous system. Proc Natl Acad Sci 1981;78:6181–5. Wüster M, Herz A. Opiate agonist action of antidiarrheal agents in vitro and in vivo — findings in support for selective action. Naunyn-Schmeideberg's Arch Pharmacol 1978;301:187–94.