Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 (2012) 2193–2205 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev Review Conditioned taste aversion and drugs of abuse: History and interpretation Andrey Verendeev ∗ , Anthony L. Riley Department of Psychology, American University, 4400 Massachusetts Avenue, NW, Washington, DC 20016, USA a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 14 March 2012 Received in revised form 3 August 2012 Accepted 9 August 2012 Keywords: Conditioned taste aversion Classical emetics Radiation Drugs of abuse Sickness Drug novelty Reward comparison Conditioned fear Self-administration a b s t r a c t Conditioned taste aversion (CTA) learning describes a phenomenon wherein an animal learns to avoid consumption of a particular taste or food following its pairing with an aversive stimulus. Although initially demonstrated with radiation and classical emetics, CTAs have also been shown with drugs of abuse. The ability of rewarding drugs to support CTA learning was described as paradoxical by many investigators, and a number of attempts have been made to resolve this paradox. The present review offers a historical perspective on the CTA literature with a particular focus on CTAs induced by self-administered drugs. Specifically, this review describes and summarizes several interpretations of CTA learning that offer possible mechanisms by which drugs of abuse support CTAs, including sickness, drug novelty, reward comparison and conditioned fear. It is concluded that the reported “paradox” is no paradox at all in that drugs of abuse are complex pharmacological compounds that produce multiple stimulus effects, not all of which are positive reinforcing. Finally, a possible role of drug aversion in drug self-administration is discussed. © 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Contents 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The role of sickness in conditioned taste aversion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Conditioned taste aversions and drugs of abuse . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The role of drug novelty in conditioned taste aversion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Conditioned taste aversion and the reward comparison hypothesis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The mechanism of drug-induced conditioned taste aversion: The role of conditioned fear . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Conditioned taste aversion and drugs of abuse: Paradox revisited . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Conclusion: The role of drug aversion in drug use and abuse . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1. Introduction Conditioned taste aversion (CTA) learning describes a phenomenon wherein an animal learns to avoid consumption of a particular taste or food following its pairing with an aversive stimulus. CTA was conceptually introduced in the 1950s and 1960s when John Garcia and his colleagues published a series of papers demonstrating the nature of CTA as a learning phenomenon (see Freeman and Riley, 2009 for a review of the history of CTA). In their initial experimental demonstration of CTA, Garcia et al. (1955) gave rats a pairing of a novel saccharin taste and exposure to gamma radiation, following which they were given a choice between saccharin and ∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 202 885 1720; fax: +1 202 885 1023. E-mail address: andrey.verendeev@gmail.com (A. Verendeev). 0149-7634/$ – see front matter © 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.004 2193 2194 2195 2196 2197 2199 2201 2201 2201 water in the absence of radiation. The authors reported that irradiated animals decreased their saccharin preference compared to their initial preference and to sham-irradiated controls (see Fig. 1; Garcia et al., 1955). In subsequent years, several aspects of CTA were elucidated that made it a very special (and specialized, see below) form of learning. For example, the initial report by Garcia et al. (1955) demonstrated the robust nature of CTA learning in that the decrease in saccharin preference persisted for at least 30 days of continuous saccharin/water exposure following the taste-radiation pairing (see Fig. 1). Additionally, acquisition of CTA required few pairings (and often only one; Garcia et al., 1955; Swank and Bernstein, 1994). For instance, in the 1955 report a single pairing of saccharin and radiation was enough for the rats to acquire a CTA and significantly suppress their saccharin preference. Moreover, CTA learning occurred with long inter-stimulus delays. In one paper, Garcia and 2194 A. Verendeev, A.L. Riley / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 (2012) 2193–2205 Fig. 1. Conditioned taste aversion: first experimental demonstration. Median saccharin preferences scores [(saccharin solution intake/total fluid intake) × 100] during extinction for groups initially given access to saccharin solution and concurrently exposed to gamma radiation (at 30 or 57 r). Redrawn from Garcia et al. (1955). his colleagues showed that aversions to saccharin were conditioned even if injections of apomorphine (aversive stimulus) were delayed for as long as 75 min following saccharin presentation (Garcia et al., 1966; see also McLaurin and Scarborough, 1963; Revusky, 1968). Finally, taste aversion learning was shown to be selective to gustatory stimuli. In one demonstration of this, rats selectively associated saccharin with irradiation-induced nausea but not with foot shock; conversely, rats selectively associated an audiovisual cue with the foot shock but not radiation (Garcia and Koelling, 1966). These demonstrations put CTA at odds with traditional learning theory that held that learning occurred with multiple conditioning trials, required short inter-stimulus intervals and was independent of the nature of CS and US (i.e., most CSs and USs serve equally well in conditioning) (Freeman and Riley, 2009). The explanation of these inconsistencies with the learning theory of the day came in the form of the interpretation of CTA as a specialized form of learning. As argued in the three early reviews of the field (Garcia and Ervin, 1968; Revusky and Garcia, 1970; Rozin and Kalat, 1971; see Freeman and Riley, 2009 for a review of these papers), the exceptional nature of taste aversion learning (i.e., onetrial learning, long-delay learning and selective associations) made sense in the light of the ecological context of the animal. Allowing for the normal time course of digestive function (i.e., some delay between ingestion of food and the effects of its consumption), the ability to learn over long delays prevents the repeated consumption of potentially poisonous foods. This “preparedness” also extended to the selective nature of CTA that facilitated associations between biologically relevant stimuli (i.e., taste and sickness) but retarded associations between biologically irrelevant stimuli (i.e., audiovisual cue and sickness). Moreover, the advantage of onetrial learning allowed the animal to quickly recognize and avoid potentially harmful substances. 2. The role of sickness in conditioned taste aversion As described, the initial demonstrations of CTA used radiation and other illness-inducing agents, such as lithium chloride (LiCl), apomorphine and cyclophosphamide, all of which share the ability to produce gastrointestinal distress. It is not surprising then that early studies suggested nausea, gastrointestinal illness and malaise (terms used interchangeably) or general “toxicosis”1 as the mechanism by which different treatments (both pharmacological and non-pharmacological) induced CTA (Garcia and Ervin, 1968; Garcia et al., 1955; Garcia and Koelling, 1966). According to this general view, the gastrointestinal distress produced by these agents served as a US with which a taste stimulus (CS) was paired resulting in a conditioned aversion to the taste and its avoidance upon subsequent exposure. This view fit well with the interpretation of CTA as a specialized form of learning, evolved through natural selection to allow organisms to avoid potentially dangerous (i.e., toxic) foods. Support for this general position came from a number of studies that demonstrated that lesions of the area postrema, a brain region responsible for monitoring blood-borne toxins, attenuated taste aversions produced by emetics and radiation (Berger et al., 1973; Curtis et al., 1994; Ossenkopp and Giugno, 1985, 1989; Rabin et al., 1983, 1984a,b; Ritter et al., 1980). Additional evidence came from the demonstration that antiemetic drugs attenuated CTA learning (Coil et al., 1978; Provenza et al., 1994; Racotta et al., 1997; Symonds and Hall, 2000; although see Goudie et al., 1982; Rabin and Hunt, 1983 for opposing results). A problem with this interpretation arose, however, when some known toxins were reported ineffective in inducing taste aversions (see Riley and Tuck, 1985 for a list of well-known toxins ineffective in producing CTA). For example, sodium cyanide (a rodenticide) failed to produce taste aversion at near lethal doses in rats (Nachman and Hartley, 1975). Failure to illicit CTA by cyanide poisoning was also reported by Ionescu and Buresova (1977; although see O’Connor and Matthews, 1997 for cyanideinduced CTA in possums). Similar results were reported for other toxins such as strychnine (Nachman and Hartley, 1975), malonate and gallamine (Ionescu and Buresova, 1977), as well as aluminum chloride, warfarin and others (Riley and Tuck, 1985). Additionally, Barker et al. (1977) showed that the degree of visible sickness did not correlate well with the strength of taste aversions that were produced by irradiation, LiCl or cyclophosphamide. Further, a number of antiemetic drugs, which are used to reduce gastrointestinal distress, have been demonstrated to be capable of inducing CTAs. Such ability, for example, has been reported for scopolamine (Berger, 1972). Other illness-reducing agents, such as tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), have been reported to induce taste aversions as well (Amit et al., 1977; Corcoran et al., 1974; Switzman et al., 1981). Moreover, pretreatment with either scopolamine or the antiemetic prochlorperazine did not attenuate aversions induced by LiCl, amphetamine or morphine (Goudie et al., 1982). As becomes evident from these observations, the ability of a treatment to serve as an effective US within the CTA preparation is not clearly dependent on its ability to produce sickness or toxicosis. As described above, non-toxic agents have been reported to induce taste aversions; moreover, some well-known toxins have been reported to fail to produce taste aversions. This led several authors to conclude that sickness or general toxicity is not a necessary condition to produce a CTA (Barker et al., 1977; Berger, 1972; Gamzu, 1977; Hunt and Amit, 1987), although it may be sufficient for some compounds. 1 It is important to note that toxicity was often not defined, and the term was used to mean a number of different things. Some researchers, for example, limited the use of the term “toxicity” to discussion of the effects produced by classical toxins such as LiCl and cyclophosphamide. For others, toxicity was defined in terms of the drugs’ effects on feeding and drinking behavior (i.e., reduction in both feeding and drinking). More frequently, however, the term was used to mean some form of sickness or gastrointestinal distress (see, for example, Boland, 1973; Dantzer, 1980; Gemberling et al., 1980; Lindberg et al., 1982 and others). A. Verendeev, A.L. Riley / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 (2012) 2193–2205 2195 paradigms can account for the fact that in one paradigm a drug seems aversive while in the other paradigm the same drug seems reinforcing. Thus it must be concluded that injections of abused drugs do not represent simple positive pharmacological stimuli; rather, drug injections must be viewed as compound stimuli with both positive and negative elements.” (Wise et al., 1976, p. 1274) Fig. 2. Conditioned taste aversion induced by a drug of abuse. Amount of saccharin consumed in different groups of rats injected with various doses of amphetamine. Redrawn from Table 1 of Cappell and LeBlanc (1971). 3. Conditioned taste aversions and drugs of abuse This challenge to a sickness interpretation of CTA received additional support when it was demonstrated that CTAs could also be induced by a variety of drugs of abuse, compounds taken for their rewarding effects and not generally characterized as being illness inducing. In one of the earlier reports on this, Cappell and LeBlanc (1971) gave rats a novel saccharin solution to drink followed by an injection of one of three doses of amphetamine (2, 4 or 8 mg/kg). On the test day, saccharin consumption was measured and it was reported that amphetamine significantly suppressed saccharin intake at all doses tested (i.e., amphetamine conditioned taste aversions; see Fig. 2). Today, the list of psychoactive substances capable of supporting taste aversion learning includes rewarding drugs of every major class: morphine (Bechara and van der Kooy, 1985; Riley et al., 1978; Verendeev and Riley, 2011; White et al., 1977), heroin (Davis et al., 2009; Grigson et al., 2000b; although see Switzman et al., 1981), cocaine (Booth et al., 1977; Ferrari et al., 1991; Glowa et al., 1994; Goudie et al., 1978), amphetamine (Booth et al., 1977; Cappell and LeBlanc, 1971; Carey and Goodall, 1974; Verendeev and Riley, 2011), nicotine (Etscorn et al., 1986; Iwamoto and Williamson, 1984; Kumar et al., 1983; Pescatore et al., 2005; Rinker et al., 2008), caffeine (Brockwell et al., 1991; Steigerwald et al., 1988; Vishwanath et al., 2011; White and Mason, 1985), ethanol (Cappell et al., 1973; Cunningham, 1979; Roma et al., 2006), and THC (Corcoran et al., 1974; Fischer and Vail, 1980; Kay, 1975; Parker and Gillies, 1995; see also Hunt and Amit, 1987 for a list of rewarding drugs capable of inducing CTAs). The finding that such aversion-inducing drugs are reliably taken by animals (i.e., establish and maintain operant self administration; see Weeks, 1962) and establish preferences for environments with which they are associated (i.e., support place preference conditioning; see Bardo and Bevins, 2000) was considered paradoxical by many (Gamzu, 1977; Goudie, 1979; Hunt and Amit, 1987; White et al., 1977). How can drugs that are rewarding in both rats and humans also produce avoidance of taste stimuli with which they were previously paired? One way out of this dilemma was to suggest that drugs of abuse possess multiple stimulus effects, both positive and negative. This interpretation, for example, was given in a study (Wise et al., 1976) wherein individual rats both selfadministered intravenous apomorphine and subsequently avoided a taste stimulus associated with the apomorphine injections. The authors note: “These data, then, demonstrate for the first time that the same drug injection can be both positive reinforcing and aversive. The demonstration of both properties in the same animals, in the same test session, rules out arguments that differences in The position that a drug produces both positive and negative effects was later supported by a series of studies that demonstrated these dual properties of cocaine in a runway model. Specifically, Ettenberg and Geist (1991, 1993) showed that although rats would learn to run down a straight alley for intravenous injections of cocaine, they would increase the running time to reach the goal box with repeated sessions. This escalation in time was a function of an increase in the frequency of retreats as the animals approached the goal box. This approach/avoidance conflict behavior was interpreted in terms of cocaine’s positive and negative effects, respectively, an effect consistent with the earlier work by Wise and his colleagues. If drugs of abuse possess both positive and negative properties, what is the nature of their negative (i.e., aversive) stimulus effects? Early investigators suggested that by virtue of the shared ability of both toxins and drugs of abuse to suppress consumption of solutions with which they were previously paired, drugs of abuse must possess some properties similar to those of LiCl and other classical toxins (see, for example, Cappell and LeBlanc, 1973, 1977; Elsmore and Fletcher, 1972; Lester et al., 1970; Riley and Zellner, 1978). In other words, drugs of abuse were deemed capable of producing some form of gastrointestinal distress. Subsequent work, however, failed to demonstrate this suggested ability of drugs of abuse to produce sickness. Berger (1972), for example, reported that amphetamine, scopolamine and chlorpromazine all produce CTAs without visible signs of sickness. This and other (e.g., Cappell and LeBlanc, 1977) studies led several investigators to conclude that sickness (i.e., gastrointestinal distress) is not a necessary condition and cannot account for the ability of drugs of abuse to produce CTAs (Gamzu, 1977; Goudie, 1979; Hunt and Amit, 1987). This interpretation, however, met with some criticism. The failure of drugs of abuse to produce visible signs of sickness while maintaining their ability to suppress saccharin consumption at the same dose may be interpreted in a number of ways. One way to do so was to suggest that the mechanism underlying drug-induced CTAs was different from that responsible for toxin-induced taste aversions. However, this argument, as Riley and Tuck pointed out, “rests upon the assumption that other behavioral measures are better indicators of a compound’s toxicity than the conditioned taste aversion design” (Riley and Tuck, 1985, p. 273). They suggested that another way to interpret these data is to view CTA as a more sensitive measure of drug toxicity. In support of this, they noted that a number of compounds are effective in producing CTAs at doses much smaller than generally required to produce any measurable effects in other assays of drug toxicity (e.g., food and water consumption; Riley and Tuck, 1985). The direct test of the sickness account of CTA was made by Parker and her colleagues in a series of assessments comparing taste reactivity responses elicited by classical toxins (such as LiCl) and reinforcing drugs (Parker, 1982, 1984, 1988, 1991, 1995; Parker et al., 1984). Developed by Grill and Norgren (1978), the taste reactivity (TR) test measures conditioned disgust using behavioral responses elicited by the intraoral infusion of a taste that has been previously paired with a nausea-inducing agent (Grill and Norgren, 1978; Parker, 1995; Parker et al., 2009). These conditioned disgust reactions, which presumably reflect the degree to which a taste has become unpalatable for the animal, include gaping, chin 2196 A. Verendeev, A.L. Riley / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 (2012) 2193–2205 Fig. 3. Conditioned taste aversion and taste reactivity to drugs of abuse and LiCl. CTA (panel A) and aversive reactions (panel B). Adapted from Parker (1995). rubbing and paw treading (Grill and Norgren, 1978), and have been documented using a number of classical toxins, such as LiCl, cyclophosphamide and apomorphine (Berridge et al., 1981; Parker, 1998; Parker and Brosseau, 1990), as well as naloxone-precipitated withdrawal (McDonald et al., 1997) and full body rotation (Cordick et al., 1999; Ossenkopp et al., 2003). Using the TR test, Parker and her colleagues compared the ability of LiCl and a number of rewarding drugs at different doses to elicit conditioned aversive TR responses. They showed that LiCl, but not drugs of abuse, produced active rejection responses and that this effect was not dependent on the magnitude of suppression of saccharin intake (with the only exception being high doses of nicotine; see Fig. 3; Parker, 1995). According to Parker, although both drugs of abuse and classical toxins decrease saccharin consumption, drugs of abuse, unlike classical toxins, fail to produce conditioned aversive reactions (although see Wheeler et al., 2008). This led her to conclude that sickness is not a necessary condition for the induction of taste aversions and that the term “conditioned taste aversion” was inappropriately applied to drug-induced suppression of saccharin intake. Instead, she suggested the term “conditioned taste avoidance” be used to describe such behavioral suppression. Such a term also differentiated conditioned nausea from the avoidance of the taste stimulus (see below; see also Berger, 1972; Goudie et al., 1982; Hunt and Amit, 1987). 4. The role of drug novelty in conditioned taste aversion If gastrointestinal disturbance (i.e., nausea) is not the mechanism mediating saccharin suppression induced by drugs of abuse, what is? One of the first non-sickness accounts of drug-induced CTA was presented by Gamzu (1977) in the drug-novelty hypothesis. According to this view, drug treatment disrupts the “internal milieu” of the animal and the pairing of the taste with this disruption results in the subsequent suppression of consumption of the drug-associated taste. This view recognizes the homeostatic state of the various physiological systems in the drug-naive animal and the ability of a drug treatment to disrupt these systems and result in a variety of internal stimuli which are quite novel to the animal subject (see Gamzu, 1977). The ability of drugs of abuse to induce CTA is then a function of their ability to disrupt these tightly regulated homeostatic states, which may be perceived as potentially dangerous by the animal (see also Kalat and Rozin, 1973). These novel states, it is important to note, are not necessarily aversive in the traditional (i.e., gastrointestinal disturbance à la Garcia) view of CTA (Gamzu, 1977). The drug-novelty hypothesis makes at least two predictions regarding the ability of rewarding drugs to suppress consumption of otherwise palatable taste stimuli. First, a drug treatment that induces a novel state for the animal should condition aversions to a taste stimulus with which it is paired. Second, exposure to the drug before a taste-drug pairing should render the drug relatively ineffective as an aversion-inducing agent. Regarding the first prediction, many drugs have been shown to induce aversions at doses that are behaviorally active in other assays (see Hunt and Amit, 1987). Presumably, these doses of drugs would induce a novel state in a drug-naive animal. It should be noted, however, that a number of behaviorally active drugs are ineffective in supporting taste aversion learning, e.g., strychnine (Nachman and Hartley, 1975), cyanide (Ionescu and Buresova, 1977, although see O’Connor and Matthews, 1997); malonate and gallamine (Ionescu and Buresova, 1977); aluminum chloride, warfarin and some others (Riley and Tuck, 1985). Although suggestive that a drug’s novel state may not be sufficient to induce an aversion, it should be noted that because a compound may be behaviorally active it may not necessarily have perceived stimulus effects. Until such compounds are tested for their discriminative effects, it remains unknown to what extent their inability to support taste aversion conditioning is evidence against the drug novelty hypothesis of taste aversion learning. The second prediction is largely supported by the existing drug preexposure literature in that drug exposure (also known as US preexposure) before a taste-drug pairing retards later acquisition of the aversion (see Hall, 2009), which in CTA takes the form of attenuated aversions (Braveman and Crane, 1977; Gamzu, 1977; see Riley and Simpson, 2001 for a review of the US preexposure effect in taste aversion conditioning). Although this effect has been repeatedly shown for a number of drugs (see Riley and Simpson, 2001) and is consistent with the drug-novelty hypothesis, several investigators have shown that the effects of US preexposure (i.e., attenuation of taste aversions) can be overridden with multiple conditioning trials. For example, Riley and Diamond (1998) exposed rats to cocaine via either intraperitoneal (i.p.) or subcutaneous (s.c.) injections administered daily or spaced every fourth day prior to repeated saccharin-cocaine pairings. They found that both preexposure treatments initially resulted in an attenuation of taste aversions. This attenuation, however, was no longer evident after repeated conditioning in the animals given s.c. injections. For the animals receiving i.p. injections, the attenuation was less pronounced than for subcutaneously-injected animals; however, this US preexposure effect, too, was eliminated with prolonged conditioning. Since, according to the drug-novelty hypothesis, pretreatment with the drug US should either eliminate or at least reduce the ability of the drug to suppress consumption of the taste CS later, the fact that chronic treatment with a drug results in conditioned suppression is not easily explained by the drug-novelty hypothesis. A. Verendeev, A.L. Riley / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 (2012) 2193–2205 The drug-novelty hypothesis was further advocated by Hunt and Amit (1987) in their review of the CTA literature under the name of “drug shyness”. The authors agreed with Gamzu in that it is the novelty of the drug state that mediates taste aversion learning. There was, however, one important aspect in which the drug-shyness hypothesis of Hunt and Amit was different from the drug-novelty position of Gamzu: specifically, Hunt and Amit proposed that it is the positive rewarding stimulus properties of drugs that provide the novel state that mediates taste aversion learning. In other words, they suggested a functional relationship between the positive rewarding and negative aversive effects of self-administered drugs and argued that rather than being separate and dichotomous, these effects have common discriminative characteristics. Whether a drug experience has positive reinforcing or aversive punishing effects depends on current environmental conditions as well as the previous learning history of the animal subject (Hunt and Amit, 1987). To support their position, they argued that pharmacological manipulations that block the reinforcing effects of drugs also attenuate their CTA-inducing properties (Sklar and Amit, 1977). For example, pimozide, a dopamine receptor antagonist, which had been shown to attenuate amphetamine- (Yokel and Wise, 1976) and cocaine- (De Wit and Wise, 1977) self-administration, was also shown to attenuate both amphetamine- (Grupp, 1977) and cocaine- (Hunt et al., 1985) induced CTAs. Although consistent with their position, there are several other studies that show that pharmacological manipulations differentially affect drug reward and aversion, which suggests that a drug’s rewarding and aversive effects can be dissociable at least under some conditions (see below). Moreover, Hunt and Amit cite evidence from the human drug literature that suggests that the initial drug experience is predominantly aversive rather than euphoric in nature (Jaffe and Martin, 1980; McAuffle, 1975; Tecce and Cole, 1974). This temporal dissociation further argues against a functional relationship between the rewarding and aversive effects of drugs. When one considers the growing literature that demonstrates that drug reward and aversion are dissociable, it becomes evident that any hypothesis that proposes a functional relationship between the rewarding and suppressive effects of drugs has to deal with this dissociation. Given the evidence for such dissociation, it becomes almost impossible to defend a view of a commonality of mechanism(s) underlying drug reward and aversion. We now turn to this growing body of evidence in light of another and quite recent hypothesis, namely reward comparison, that proposes a functional relationship between drug reward and aversion. 5. Conditioned taste aversion and the reward comparison hypothesis Following the reports that intake of a gustatory stimulus is avoided when paired with either illness-inducing agents, such as LiCl or radiation, or a drug of abuse, it was reported that rats also avoid consumption of a saccharin CS when it is paired with a highly rewarding sucrose solution (Flaherty and Checke, 1982). This phenomenon came to be known as the anticipatory contrast effect and was interpreted as avoidance of a rewarding taste stimulus in anticipation of a more rewarding taste stimulus (for a review see Flaherty, 1996). Given the parallel between the sucrose-induced suppression of saccharin intake and the suppression induced by drugs of abuse (avoidance of taste CS; rewarding US), Grigson (1997) reinterpreted the phenomenon of conditioned taste aversions wherein rewarding drugs of abuse suppress intake of rewarding saccharin in terms of a mechanism akin to anticipatory 2197 contrast (i.e., reward comparison). That is, according to the reward comparison hypothesis, rats avoid consumption of the rewarding saccharin solution in anticipation of a more rewarding drug. As Grigson put it, “[r]ats decrease intake of a gustatory CS following taste-drug pairings because the rewarding properties of the gustatory stimulus pale in comparison to those of the impending drug of abuse” (Grigson, 1997, p. 134). This position was perhaps reinforced by the finding that drugs of abuse did not produce active rejection responses in taste reactivity tests (see above), whereas LiCl did (Parker, 1988, 1991, 1995; see Grigson, 1997; although see Wheeler et al., 2008). Thus, saccharin avoidance induced by drugs of abuse resembled more the suppression induced by a rewarding sucrose solution than that following treatment with LiCl. It is in this context that Grigson proposed the reward comparison hypothesis (Grigson, 1997). In the initial assessment of this hypothesis, Grigson (1997; Experiments 1 and 2B) argued that suppression induced by drugs of abuse should depend on the rewarding properties of the gustatory CS. That is, given that the hypothesis assumes that two rewarding stimuli are compared, only CSs demonstrated to be rewarding (albeit less than the drug US) should result in suppressed consumption. In an assessment of this prediction, Grigson reported that when saccharin (a rewarding taste stimulus) was paired with morphine or cocaine, consumption was subsequently suppressed. On the other hand, when a more neutral (i.e., less rewarding) NaCl solution was paired with either morphine or cocaine, consumption was not affected (see Figs. 1 and 2 of Grigson, 1997). Interestingly, both solutions resulted in suppressed intake when they were paired with LiCl, an effect consistent with the position that LiCl is aversive and this aversive property induced aversions to both tastes with which it was paired (see Fig. 4 of Grigson, 1997). In the same paper, Grigson assessed the effects of different concentrations of the saccharin CS on the suppressive effects of morphine (Grigson, 1997; Experiment 3). The logic for this assessment stemmed from work by Flaherty who reported that reward contrast increased with increased concentrations of saccharin (see Flaherty, 1996; see also Flaherty et al., 1994). Grigson predicted that if the suppression induced by drugs of abuse is due to reward comparison then the degree of suppression should also vary with the concentration of the saccharin solution. To test this, different groups of rats received multiple pairings of saccharin and morphine. The groups differed in the concentration of the saccharin CS. Consistent with the prediction, the suppressive effects of morphine increased with increasing concentrations of saccharin (see Fig. 5 of Grigson, 1997). Next, Grigson analyzed the effects of strain differences (LEW vs. F344 rats) in the suppressive effects of cocaine (Grigson and Freet, 2000; see also Glowa et al., 1994). Given that these two strains differ in their sensitivity to the rewarding effects of drugs, i.e., LEW rats display greater cocaine-induced place preferences (Guitart et al., 1992) and more readily acquire cocaine self-administration (Kosten et al., 1997) than F344 rats, the authors predicted that the suppressive effects of cocaine would be more pronounced in LEW than in F344 rats (i.e., the reward comparison would be greater in the LEW rats). Consistent with this prediction, cocaine suppressed saccharin consumption in LEW rats to a greater degree than in the F344 strain. Further support for the reward comparison hypothesis was provided by an assessment of the effects of bilateral lesions of the gustatory thalamus on morphine-induced suppression. These lesions had previously been reported to disrupt the development of anticipatory contrast following saccharin-sucrose pairings (Reilly and Pritchard, 1996) but had no impact on the avoidance of a saccharin solution previously paired with LiCl (Scalera et al., 1997). It was predicted that if the suppressive effects of morphine are 2198 A. Verendeev, A.L. Riley / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 (2012) 2193–2205 due to reward comparison (and not a function of a traditional CTA like that with LiCl), then the bilateral lesions of the gustatory thalamus should also disrupt morphine-induced suppression. As predicted, lesions of the gustatory thalamus disrupted morphineinduced suppression of saccharin consumption but had no impact on the LiCl-induced suppression of sweet-tasting alanine (Grigson et al., 2000a). Although supportive of the reward comparison hypothesis, the aforementioned work is not without criticism. According to Grigson (1997), if the reduction in intake induced by drugs of abuse is due to reward comparison then only a rewarding CS should result in suppression following the taste-drug pairing. However, a number of studies report that drugs of abuse support suppression of presumably neutral CSs. For example, a recent study by Lin et al. (2009) reported that rats suppressed consumption of a NaCl solution following its pairing with morphine. In that study, rats had access to a NaCl solution and were intraperitoneally injected with 15 mg/kg morphine. One group of rats then received excitotoxic lesions of the insular cortex, and the other group received sham lesions. Animals that received sham lesions and were treated with morphine suppressed their intake of NaCl relative to their sham-lesioned controls that were injected with saline. Similar NaCl aversions have been shown with d-amphetamine (Bielavska and Bures, 1994) and following second-order conditioning with morphine (Bevins et al., 1996). Additionally, Grigson argued that the ability of drugs of abuse, but not of LiCl, to induce taste aversions should vary with increased palatability of the CS (Grigson, 1997). In line with this prediction, Grigson demonstrated greater suppressive effects of morphine with increasing concentrations of the saccharin cue (see above). Although supportive of her model, there are several concerns with this analysis. For example, no assessment was concurrently made on the suppressive effects of LiCl and thus the prediction that the palatability of the CS is a central characteristic to reward comparison, but “irrelevant to CTA learning”, was not directly tested (Grigson, 1997). Furthermore, considerable data suggest that the suppressive effects of LiCl do increase with increasing concentrations of the saccharin cue. For example, as early as 1971 Garcia documented the relationship between degree of aversion and the concentration of saccharin solution. When the dose of LiCl was held constant, suppression of 0.1% saccharin solution was greater than suppression of 0.025% saccharin, and suppression of 0.35% and 0.7% solutions was greater than that of 0.1% saccharin (Garcia, 1971). Similar findings have been reported in other studies that paired different concentrations of saccharin with LiCl (Gilley and Franchina, 1985; Huang and Hsiao, 2008; Iraola and Alonso, 1995) or other classical toxins such as cyclophosphamide (Braun and Rosenthal, 1976; Dragoin, 1971) or radiation (Barker, 1976). These data suggest that the degree of suppression induced by both drugs of abuse and classical toxins may be a function of salience of the conditioned stimulus (e.g., saccharin), with higher concentrations more easily associable with the US. Finally, one should proceed cautiously in assuming that the increasing concentrations of saccharin equate with increasing palatability (i.e., rewarding value) of the saccharin solution. It may actually be the case that the reverse is true (see, for example, Dess, 1993). Unfortunately, no attempt was made by Grigson to demonstrate that increasing concentrations of saccharin resulted in increased preference (i.e., palatability) of the saccharin solution. This assumption, therefore, was left untested. Although the work with different strains and cocaine appears to be generally consistent with the reward comparison position in that the suppressive effects of cocaine (Glowa et al., 1994; Grigson and Freet, 2000), but not LiCl (Foynes and Riley, 2004) are greater in the presumably more drug-sensitive LEW rats, data from studies that use other rewarding drugs are not consistent with this position. For example, although LEW rats exhibit faster acquisition of morphine self-administration (Ambrosio et al., 1995; Martin et al., 1999, 2003; Sanchez-Cardoso et al., 2007; Suzuki et al., 1988b) and morphine-induced place preference (Guitart et al., 1992; but see Davis et al., 2007) than F344 rats, these strains actually reverse in their display of morphine-induced taste aversions (i.e., the F344 strain shows greater morphine-induced taste aversions than the LEW strain; Davis et al., 2009; Gomez-Serrano et al., 2009; Lancellotti et al., 2001). Similar findings have been reported for other drugs of abuse such as nicotine (Brower et al., 2002; Horan et al., 1997; Pescatore et al., 2005; but see Suzuki et al., 1999) and ethanol (Roma et al., 2006; Suzuki et al., 1988a; see Riley et al., 2009 for a review of strain differences in self-administration, conditioned place preference (CPP) and CTA in LEW and F344 rats). Moreover, even the strain effect with cocaine can be overridden by a higher dose (50 mg/kg) of the drug (LEW = F344; see Glowa et al., 1994). Lesion data are also not decisively supportive of the reward comparison hypothesis. For example, although lesions of the gustatory thalamus do disrupt morphine- (Grigson et al., 2000a) and cocaine(but not LiCl-) induced suppression of consumption, this disruptive effect is dose-dependent and is no longer evident when higher doses of cocaine are used as the US (Grigson et al., 2009). Moreover, a recent study by Geddes et al. (2008) found that whereas bilateral lesions of the insular gustatory cortex fully disrupted the suppressive effects of morphine (15 and 30 mg/kg, s.c.), these same lesions only attenuated (10 mg/kg, i.p.) or had no effect (20 mg/kg, i.p.) on cocaine-induced suppression. These lesions also had no impact on LiCl-induced suppression. Furthermore, it should be noted that the evidence from the lesion studies is indirect, as these results only provide evidence that the anatomical mediation of morphine- and cocaine-induced suppression is different from suppression induced by LiCl. For the data to be conclusively supportive, it should be demonstrated that lesions that block or attenuate suppression induced by drugs of abuse should also block or attenuate the rewarding effects of such drugs (as measured by place preference or self-administration). A recent study by Sellings et al. (2008) investigated the effects of 6-OHDA lesions of the nucleus accumbens core vs. medial shell on nicotine-induced CPP and CTA. Lesions of the medial shell, but not core, reduced nicotine-induced place preferences, whereas lesions of the core, but not medial shell, fully disrupted nicotine-induced taste aversions. In other words, the rewarding and aversive effects of nicotine were anatomically dissociable. In addition to the abovementioned criticisms, there are other data that are not easily reconcilable with the reward comparison hypothesis. For example, Simpson and Riley (2005) examined the effects of morphine preexposure on morphine-induced place preferences and taste aversions. Rats received subcutaneous injections of 5 mg/kg morphine every other day for a total of 5 injections. The rats were then trained in a combined CTA/CPP procedure wherein a single injection of morphine (1 or 5 mg/kg) was administered to produce both a CTA and a CPP. It was found that preexposure to morphine attenuated taste aversions but enhanced place preferences within the same animals. That the same pharmacological manipulation affects drug reward and aversion in an opposing manner strongly argues that these drug effects are dissociable. Similar dissociation of rewarding and aversive effects of morphine has been reported by Martin et al. (1988) and Gaiardi et al. (1991). Moreover, separate assessments of drug preexposure on morphine’s rewarding and aversive properties have also shown differential effects, i.e., enhancement of its rewarding effects (Harris and Aston-Jones, 2003; He et al., 2004; Manzanedo et al., 2005) but attenuation of its aversive effects (Cappell et al., 1975; Dacanay and Riley, 1982; Domjan and Siegel, 1983; LeBlanc and Cappell, 1974; Riley et al., 1984). A. Verendeev, A.L. Riley / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 (2012) 2193–2205 Additional evidence for dissociation of rewarding and aversive effects of drugs of abuse comes from data that show that adolescent rats respond differently to the rewarding and aversive effects of drugs compared to adult rats. For example, adolescent rats, compared to adults, are more sensitive to the rewarding effects of nicotine but less sensitive to the aversive effects of the drug (Shram et al., 2006). Specifically, in this study nicotine produced dose-dependent place preferences in adolescent, but not adult, rats (see Fig. 1 of Shram et al., 2006); when paired with a novel saccharin solution, the same doses of nicotine produced dose-dependent saccharin suppression in adult, but not adolescent, rats (see Fig. 3 of Shram et al., 2006; see also Wilmouth and Spear, 2004). If saccharin suppression is dependent on the ability of drugs to produce reward, these two (i.e., reward and suppression) should co-vary. However, this study did not demonstrate such a relationship. Age-dependent differences in the sensitivity to the rewarding (Badanich et al., 2006; Brenhouse and Andersen, 2008; Brenhouse et al., 2008; Zakharova et al., 2009a,b) and aversive (Infurna and Spear, 1979; Philpot et al., 2003; Quinn et al., 2008; Schramm-Sapyta et al., 2006, 2007) effects have been documented for other drugs as well (although see Aberg et al., 2007; Adriani and Laviola, 2003; Balda et al., 2006 for a reversal in age sensitivity to drug reward in rats and mice; see Schramm-Sapyta et al., 2009 for a review on age-dependent differences in drug reward and aversion). Recently, we (Verendeev and Riley, 2011) directly tested the reward comparison model by examining the relationship between the ability of morphine and amphetamine to condition taste aversions and place preferences in individual subjects. According to the reward comparison hypothesis, there should be a direct relationship between the ability of drugs to produce place preferences and taste aversions, since, for Grigson, it is the rewarding effects that condition both place preference and suppression of intake. We examined this relationship using a combined CTA/CPP procedure with two different classes of drugs (i.e., opiates and psychostimulants) and at two different doses (5 and 10 mg/kg morphine and 3 and 5 mg/kg amphetamine). Specifically, rats were given a novel saccharin solution to drink, injected with a drug and placed in the CPP apparatus. We then examined their change from baseline in saccharin consumption and change from baseline in time spent on the drug-paired side (DPS) of the CPP apparatus. Two interesting findings emerged. First, individual subjects differed greatly in their sensitivity to the rewarding and aversive effects of both morphine and amphetamine. Individual subjects were sensitive to the rewarding effects of drugs but not their aversive effects or vice versa; or, individual subjects were sensitive to both or neither of these effects. Second, there was no relationship between the ability of either drug to produce a place preference and its ability to produce a taste aversion (see Fig. 4). Specifically, animals were divided into High and Low responders for all conditions and the relationships between their change in saccharin consumption and change in time spent on the DPS were analyzed. We found no significant relationship between the two in any of the conditions, except for one: animals given 5 mg/kg amphetamine that displayed strongest aversions were less likely to show an increase (or showed an actual decrease) in time spent on the DPS (i.e., a relationship opposite to what reward comparison would predict). Together, a host of behavioral (e.g., Schramm-Sapyta et al., 2009; Shram et al., 2006; Verendeev and Riley, 2011), anatomical (e.g., Sellings et al., 2008) and pharmacological (e.g., Simpson and Riley, 2005) data argue that the rewarding and aversive effects of drugs of abuse can be (and are likely) dissociated, leading to the conclusion that the rewarding effects of drugs do not mediate the suppression of intake in the conditioned taste aversion preparation. It is important to note, however, that the nature of such suppression remains to be determined (see below). 2199 Fig. 4. Lack of relationship between the rewarding and aversive effects of morphine. Correlational analysis of the relationship between change in time spent on the drugpaired side and change in saccharin consumption for animal subjects injected with 5 mg/kg morphine, s.c. Visual analysis reveals substantial individual variability in sensitivity to both the rewarding and aversive effects of morphine. Included in the graph are Pearson’s r, p value, as well as the line of best fit for the given relationship. See also Verendeev and Riley (2011). 6. The mechanism of drug-induced conditioned taste aversion: The role of conditioned fear If CTAs induced by drugs of abuse are not mediated by the drugs’ rewarding effects, as discussed above, what then is the nature of the suppression of consumption of taste stimuli produced by these psychoactive drugs? As previously mentioned, a number of possible mechanisms have been proposed, e.g., nausea (Elsmore and Fletcher, 1972; Lester et al., 1970; Riley and Zellner, 1978), novelty (Gamzu, 1977; Hunt and Amit, 1987), reward comparison (Grigson, 1997), but none has been without criticism. In this section, we would like to examine yet another hypothesis that has been recently summarized by Parker and her colleagues (Parker et al., 2009), according to which it is conditioned fear that mediates the drug-induced suppression of drinking. One important aspect of this view is that it acknowledges the tightly regulated homeostatic state of the animal (cf. Gamzu, 1977) and the ability of a drug treatment to disrupt this homeostasis. In animals such as the rat that are incapable of vomiting, any disruption in homeostasis following consumption must be perceived as dangerous. Any food associated with this disruption evokes a conditioned fear response which results in the avoidance of the food associated with the disruption in homeostasis. Interestingly, when a novel saccharin solution is paired with drugs of abuse in species capable of emesis (e.g., house musk shrew Suncus murinus), a different ingestive pattern emerges from that of a rat. For these animals, disruptions in homeostatsis do not necessarily evoke a fear response given that true toxins can be removed from the digestive track via vomiting. Consequently, disruptions in homeostasis that do not evoke vomiting do not induce fear and should not induce a taste aversion to foods associated with them. In a test of this prediction with shrews, Parker et al. (2002) paired a novel sucrose or saccharin solution with morphine and amphetamine at doses (20 mg/kg each) that also produced conditioned place preference in the same species. These doses of morphine (Simpson and Riley, 2005; White et al., 1977) and amphetamine (Cappell and LeBlanc, 1971; Wise et al., 1976) also exceeded those necessary to produce 2200 A. Verendeev, A.L. Riley / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 (2012) 2193–2205 conditioned suppression in the rat (see also Turenne et al., 1996; Verendeev and Riley, 2011). They reported that shrews developed a conditioned taste preference for the drug-paired flavors. In shrews, the ability to vomit allows the animal to be “less careful”. That is, toxicosis is dealt with by removing the toxic substance from the ingestive tract by vomiting. Drugs of abuse fail to induce conditioned avoidance of taste stimuli due to the lack of a fear response in these animals to such states. From this analysis, Parker et al. concluded that the ability of rewarding drugs to condition taste avoidance varies with the ability of the species to vomit (see also Davis et al., 1986). To further investigate the conditioned fear hypothesis, Parker examined whether rewarding drugs actually elicited conditioned fear in rats. In one assessment of this, Rana and Parker (2007) examined the ability of LiCl- and amphetamine-paired flavors to elicit fear in the fear-potentiated acoustic startle response (ASR). The ASR had been previously shown to be enhanced in the presence of shock-paired cues, but attenuated in the presence of cues that had been paired with sickness (see Parker et al., 2009). If the drug-paired flavor elicits conditioned fear, it would be expected to potentiate the ASR; however, if the drug-paired flavor elicits conditioned sickness, then it would be expected to attenuate the ASR. As predicted by the conditioned fear hypothesis, Rana and Parker found that intraoral infusion of amphetamine-paired saccharin potentiated the startle response (akin to shock-paired cue), but presentation of LiCl-paired saccharin attenuated the startle response, akin to other treatments that make rats sick (Rana and Parker, 2007). Furthermore, when nausea produced by LiCl was inhibited by pretreatment with the antiemetic ondansetron, LiCl-paired saccharin solution no longer blunted the ASR, but instead was found to elicit a response similar to the amphetamine-paired saccharin (i.e., potentiated startle response). Interestingly, other studies by Parker and her colleagues have shown that pretreatment with ondansetron interfered with LiCl-induced rejection reactions in the TR test, but had no effect on LiCl-induced suppression within the CTA preparation (Limebeer and Parker, 2000). This seems to suggest then that even in the absence of nausea the changes in affective homeostasis produced by LiCl treatment become selectively associated with the novel taste in the CTA preparation resulting in the avoidance of the taste stimulus (Parker et al., 2009). These findings not only lend further support for the conditioned fear interpretation of taste avoidance, but also provide an explanation for the failure of antiemetic drugs to block suppression of drinking induced by classical toxins (see, for example, Goudie et al., 1982). Moreover, the ability of antiemetic agents to produce taste avoidance on their own (see, for example, Amit et al., 1977; Berger, 1972; Corcoran et al., 1974; Switzman et al., 1981; see the section “The role of sickness in conditioned taste aversion” above) is also easily explained by the conditioned fear hypothesis, according to which drugs (including antiemetic drugs) that disrupt homeostatic state of the animal are capable of producing conditioned suppression of the taste with which they are paired. Taken together, these and other findings by Parker and her colleagues offer the following interpretation of the conditioned taste avoidance induced by psychoactive drugs. First, there is a qualitative difference in CTAs induced by rewarding drugs and CTAs produced by classical emetics (such as LiCl) and other illnessinducing treatments (as measured by the TR test; Parker, 1988, 1991, 1995; Parker et al., 2009). Second, drugs of abuse produce conditioned taste avoidance (i.e., suppression of drug-paired taste CS) but do not produce conditioned taste aversion (i.e., change in palatability of the taste stimulus; Parker, 1982, 1991, 1995; Parker et al., 2009). Third, drugs of abuse produce conditioned taste avoidance via conditioned fear, wherein a change in homeostasis following a drug treatment becomes readily associated with the flavor of the taste stimulus, and the taste is subsequently avoided because it comes to signal potential danger to the animal (Parker et al., 2009). Fourth, nausea is not necessary to condition taste avoidance for either drugs of abuse (which do not produce gastrointestinal sickness) or for LiCl (as discussed above, pretreatment with antiemetics fails to block LiCl-induced suppression of saccharin intake; Goudie et al., 1982; Limebeer and Parker, 2000), but might be sufficient to do so (as shown, for example, in case of rotationinduced suppression of a taste stimulus; Braun and McIntosh, 1973; Cordick et al., 1999; Green and Rachlin, 1973; Sakai and Yamamoto, 1997). Fifth, nausea is necessary to produce conditioned taste aversion (again, measured by rejection responses in the TR test; Parker, 1982, 1995, 1998; Parker et al., 2009). One interesting question that follows from this analysis is whether the conditioned fear evoked by disruption in homeostasis is similar in nature to fear induced by exteroceptive stimuli, such as foot shock. Although both preparations investigate conditioned fear, the two have not been systematically examined under the identical experimental conditions. Nevertheless, a recent study suggests that the conditioned fear induced by disruption in homeostasis may share some neurobiological substrate with the conditioned fear induced by exteroceptive stimuli. Rana and Parker (2008) reported that lesions of the basolateral amygdala (BLA) attenuated LiCl-induced suppression of saccharin consumption (i.e., reduced conditioned fear) in rats, an effect consistent with other studies showing the importance of BLA in the establishment of conditioned fear (Killcross et al., 1997; Selden et al., 1991). Interestingly, lesions of BLA did not affect LiCl-induced conditioned disgust reactions, which further supported the position that conditioned suppression of drinking is mediated by conditioned fear but not conditioned disgust (see above). Although the conditioned fear account of taste aversion learning is well supported (see above), there are several issues that remain to be further addressed. For example, if fear is involved in the conditioning of taste aversions, it might be expected that foot shock, which readily conditions fear in the rat (see Maren et al., 1997; Maren and Fanselow, 1996), would in turn induce taste aversions. Although there is evidence that shock can be effective in such a preparation (Braveman, 1977; Delamater and Treit, 1988; Dess et al., 1988a,b; Hankins et al., 1976; Klunder and O’Boyle, 1979; Krane and Wagner, 1975; Miller et al., 1989; Nakajima, 2004; Revusky and Reilly, 1989), others have failed to find any evidence of its ability to induce aversions (Domjan and Wilson, 1972; Garcia and Koelling, 1966; Garcia et al., 1970; Green et al., 1972; Lasiter and Braun, 1981; Miller and Domjan, 1981). Interestingly, in several of the papers that have reported shock-induced taste aversions, if the same shock is given prior to or during taste aversion conditioning when a taste is paired with radiation or a general emetic such as LiCl or apomorphine, the taste aversions are weakened (see Dess et al., 1988b; Revusky and Reilly, 1989; Braveman, 1977; though see Lasiter and Braun, 1981). Further, taste-illness and taste-shock associations can be dissociated by the patterns of aversions generated (Klunder and O’Boyle, 1979; Miller et al., 1989; Rusiniak et al., 1982), the various delays that can be supported by each (Krane and Wagner, 1975) as well as the effects of a pharmacological challenge (Delamater and Treit, 1988). Further, although the conditioned fear hypothesis provides one possible mechanism for the conditioned taste avoidance induced by drugs of abuse – namely, disruption of homeostasis and the subsequent fear associated with it – it does not address the specific nature of the disruption. What exactly is disrupted by the drug injection? The answer may depend on the specific drug, e.g., anxiogenesis (cocaine; Ettenberg and Geist, 1991, 1993), hypothermia (ethanol; Cunningham et al., 1988; Cunningham and Niehus, 1989) and is likely affected by a number of factors, such as the route of administration (Busse et al., 2005; Ferrari et al., 1991; A. Verendeev, A.L. Riley / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 (2012) 2193–2205 Mayer and Parker, 1993), dose (Jensen et al., 1990; Nathan and Vogel, 1975; Parker, 1991; Riley et al., 1978), temporal parameters (Freeman and Riley, 2005), sex (Cailhol and Mormede, 2002; Randall-Thompson and Riley, 2003; van Haaren and Hughes, 1990), specific neurochemical systems affected (Goudie et al., 1975; Serafine and Riley, 2012; Sklar and Amit, 1977; see also Hunt and Amit, 1987), centrally- vs. peripherally-mediated action (Bechara et al., 1987; Davis et al., 2012; Freeman et al., 2005; Rabin and Hunt, 1989), genetics (Glowa et al., 1994; Grabus et al., 2004; Lancellotti et al., 2001; Roma et al., 2006), environmental conditions (GomezSerrano et al., 2009; Roma et al., 2007; Roma et al., 2008) and other factors. 7. Conditioned taste aversion and drugs of abuse: Paradox revisited As discussed above, a number of authors have noted the seeming paradox whereby drugs of abuse that are freely self-administered by both humans and animals (presumably due to their rewarding effects) also suppress the consumption of food and taste stimuli with which they are paired (Gamzu, 1977; Goudie, 1979; Hunt and Amit, 1987; White et al., 1977). This might indeed be viewed as paradoxical if drugs of abuse are considered in the sense of being simple pharmacological agents with positive rewarding effects only. This description of psychoactive drugs, however, may be too narrow in that drugs of abuse may possess multiple stimulus effects, not all of which are positive reinforcing (Koob and Le Moal, 2006; Lynch and Carroll, 2001; Stolerman, 1992). When one considers the multiplicity of a drug’s stimulus effects, the ability of a drug of abuse to be self administered and to condition a place preference and produce taste avoidance at the same time ceases to be a paradox. As envisioned early on by Wise et al. (1976), drugs of abuse are complex pharmacological compounds with both positive and negative effects (see also Riley et al., 2009). Although assessments of drug reward and aversion are usually done in separate experiments and often in separate studies and under different experimental conditions, a number of studies have examined these effects at the same time and in the same animals. For example, White et al. (1977) examined the ability of morphine to produce reward (as measured by runway speed to obtain food located in a goalbox) and aversion (measured by consumption of the food) at the same time. They found that the same morphine injections both increased the running speed to reach the goal box and reduced the amount of food consumed. Moreover, Wise et al. (1976) reported that rats avoided saccharin associated with either experimenter-administered amphetamine or self-administered apomorphine. We (Verendeev and Riley, 2011) reported that rats both preferred a distinctive environment (i.e., formed a CPP) and avoided a saccharin taste (i.e., formed a CTA) that have been paired with the same injection of either morphine or amphetamine. These and other (Cunningham, 1979; Reicher and Holman, 1977; Roma et al., 2006; Simpson and Riley, 2005; Ettenberg and Geist, 1991, 1993) studies suggest that both positive rewarding and negative aversive effects can be demonstrated under the same experimental conditions. However, although drug reward and drug aversion can be produced by the same drug injection, these effects are most likely dissociated and independent of each other (Verendeev and Riley, 2011; see above). The presence of both positive rewarding and negative aversive effects and their apparent dissociation from each other suggest that both of these effects should be examined in any attempt to model drug use and addiction. In conclusion of this review on conditioned taste aversion, we would like to discuss a possible role of drug aversion in drug taking behavior. 2201 8. Conclusion: The role of drug aversion in drug use and abuse Drugs of abuse clearly possess rewarding properties, the understanding of which is important in the on-going attempt to model their abuse potential (see Riley et al., 2009). However, instead of being viewed as simple pharmacological stimuli, drugs of abuse should be recognized as complex pharmacological compounds with multiple stimulus effects, some of which are positive and others that are aversive (see above). It is important to note, however, that the drugs’ aversive effects have either been largely ignored in attempts to model drug use and abuse or given minimal attention. For example, Meyer and Quenzer (2005) review a number of models of drug abuse and dependence, including the physical dependence model, the positive reinforcement model, opponent process model and incentive-sensitization model, none of which mentions a drug’s acute aversive effects. According to Meyer and Quenzer, however, a comprehensive model of drug use and dependence should, in addition to the positive reinforcing effects of drugs, include the aversive effects of drugs as well. Other factors involved in development and maintenance of compulsive drug use include the discriminative subjective effects of drugs, the stimuli conditioned to drug effects, and other risk and protective factors (see Meyer and Quenzer, 2005, pp. 207–212). One of the few attempts to assess the role of the aversive effects of a drug and its self-administration was recently summarized by Cunningham and his colleagues in their analysis of the genetic influences on ethanol-induced taste aversions and ethanol intake (Cunningham et al., 2009). Using 15 different inbred mouse strains, the authors examined their sensitivity to the aversive effects of 2 g/kg ethanol (see also Broadbent et al., 2002) and then correlated these differences with 10% ethanol intake reported for these strains in an earlier study (Belknap et al., 1993). A significant correlation was reported between the sensitivity to the aversive effects of ethanol and ethanol consumption, such that strains of mice more sensitive to the aversive effects of the drug (i.e., greater taste aversion) showed less ethanol self-administration (i.e., less ethanol intake) and vice versa (see also Cannon et al., 1994 and Risinger and Cunningham, 1998; for a related analysis with the F344 and LEW rat strains, see Riley et al., 2009; Riley, 2011). It becomes evident then that to better understand drug vulnerability in both animal and human subjects, one has to understand the ability of drugs to produce both effects and the interplay between the two. Indeed, it has been suggested by several authors that drug taking behavior may be a function of the relative balance between drug reward and aversion (Gaiardi et al., 1991; Riley and Simpson, 2001; Stolerman and D’Mello, 1981; see also Riley et al., 2009). According to this view, drug taking is a function of the balance between reward and aversion, with the drug’s aversive effects serving as a limiting factor. If so, successful modeling of drug taking behavior (both problem and non-problem use) should take into account the relative input of both rewarding and aversive effects of drugs, as well as different factors that affect these two properties. References Aberg, M., Wade, D., Wall, E., Izenwasser, S., 2007. Effect of MDMA (ecstasy) on activity and cocaine conditioned place preference in adult and adolescent rats. Neurotoxicology and Teratology 29, 37–46. Adriani, W., Laviola, G., 2003. Elevated levels of impulsivity and reduced place conditioning with d-amphetamine: two behavioral features of adolescence in mice. Behavioral Neuroscience 117, 695–703. Ambrosio, E., Goldberg, S.R., Elmer, G.I., 1995. Behavior genetic investigation of the relationship between spontaneous locomotor activity and the acquisition of morphine self-administration behavior. Behavioural Pharmacology 6, 229–237. Amit, Z., Levitan, D.E., Brown, Z.W., Rogan, F., 1977. Possible involvement of central factors in the mediation of conditioned taste aversion. Neuropharmacology 16, 121–124. 2202 A. Verendeev, A.L. Riley / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 (2012) 2193–2205 Badanich, K.A., Adler, K.J., Kirstein, C.L., 2006. Adolescents differ from adults in cocaine conditioned place preference and cocaine-induced dopamine in the nucleus accumbens septi. European Journal of Pharmacology 550, 95–106. Balda, M.A., Anderson, K.L., Itzhak, Y., 2006. Adolescent and adult responsiveness to the incentive value of cocaine reward in mice: role of neuronal nitric oxide synthase (nNOS) gene. Neuropharmacology 51, 341–349. Bardo, M.T., Bevins, R.A., 2000. Conditioned place preference: what does it add to our preclinical understanding of drug reward. Psychopharmacology 153, 31–43. Barker, L.M., 1976. CS duration, amount, and concentration effects in conditioning taste aversions. Learning and Motivation 7, 265–273. Barker, L.M., Smith, J.C., Suarez, E.M., 1977. Sickness and the backward conditioning of taste aversions. In: Barker, L.M., Best, M., Domjan, M. (Eds.), Learning Mechanisms in Food Selection. Baylor University Press, Waco, TX, pp. 533–553. Bechara, A., van der Kooy, D., 1985. Opposite motivational effects of endogenous opioids in brain and periphery. Nature 314, 533–534. Bechara, A., Zito, K.A., Vanderkooy, D., 1987. Peripheral receptors mediate the aversive-conditioning effects of morphine in the rat. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 28, 219–225. Belknap, J.K., Crabbe, J.C., Young, E.R., 1993. Voluntary consumption of ethanol in 15 inbred mouse strains. Psychopharmacology 112, 503–510. Berger, B.D., 1972. Conditioning of food aversions by injections of psychoactive drugs. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 81, 21–26. Berger, B.D., Wise, C.D., Stein, L., 1973. Area postrema damage and bait shyness. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 82, 475–479. Berridge, K., Grill, H.J., Norgren, R., 1981. Relation of consummatory responses and preabsorptive insulin release to palatability and learned taste aversions. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 95, 363–382. Bevins, R.A., Delzer, T.A., Bardo, M.T., 1996. Second-order conditioning detects unexpressed morphine-induced salt aversion. Animal Learning and Behavior 24, 221–229. Bielavska, E., Bures, J., 1994. Universality of parabrachial mediation of conditioned taste aversion. Behavioural Brain Research 60, 35–42. Boland, F.J., 1973. Saccharin aversions induced by lithium chloride toxicosis in a backward conditioning paradigm. Animal Learning and Behavior 1, 3–4. Booth, D.A., Pilcher, C.W., D’Mello, G.D., Stolerman, I.P., 1977. Comparative potencies of amphetamine, fenfluramine and related compounds in taste aversion experiments in rats. British Journal of Pharmacology 61, 669–677. Braun, J.J., McIntosh, H.J., 1973. Learned taste aversions induced by rotational stimulation. Physiological Psychology 1, 301–304. Braun, J.J., Rosenthal, B., 1976. Relative salience of saccharin and quinine in longdelay taste aversion learning. Behavioral Biology 16, 341–352. Braveman, N.S., 1977. What studies on preexposure to pharmacological agents tell us about the nature of the aversion-inducing agent. In: Barker, L.M., Best, M.R., Domjan, M. (Eds.), Learning Mechanisms in Food Selection. Baylor University Press, Waco, TX, pp. 511–530. Braveman, N.S., Crane, J., 1977. Amount consumed and the formation of conditioned taste aversions. Behavioral Biology 21, 470–477. Brenhouse, H.C., Andersen, S.L., 2008. Delayed extinction and stronger reinstatement of cocaine conditioned place preference in adolescent rats, compared to adults. Behavioral Neuroscience 122, 460–465. Brenhouse, H.C., Sonntag, K.C., Andersen, S.L., 2008. Transient D1 dopamine receptor expression on prefrontal cortex projection neurons: relationship to enhanced motivational salience of drug cues in adolescence. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 28, 2375–2382. Broadbent, J., Muccino, K.J., Cunningham, C.L., 2002. Ethanol-induced conditioned taste aversion in 15 inbred mouse strains. Behavioral Neuroscience 116, 138–148. Brockwell, N.T., Eikelboom, R., Beninger, R.J., 1991. Caffeine-induced place and taste conditioning: production of dose-dependent preference and aversion. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 38, 513–517. Brower, V.G., Fu, Y., Matta, S.G., Sharp, B.M., 2002. Rat strain differences in nicotine self-administration using an unlimited access paradigm. Brain Research 930, 12–20. Busse, G.D., Freeman, K.B., Riley, A.L., 2005. The interaction of sex and route of drug administration in cocaine-induced conditioned taste aversions. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 81, 814–820. Cailhol, S., Mormede, P., 2002. Conditioned taste aversion and alcohol drinking: strain and gender differences. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 63, 91–99. Cannon, D.S., Leeka, J.K., Block, A.K., 1994. Ethanol self-administration patterns and taste aversion learning across inbred rat strains. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 47, 795–802. Cappell, H., LeBlanc, A.E., 1971. Conditioned aversion to saccharin by single administrations of mescaline and d-amphetamine. Psychopharmacologia 22, 352–356. Cappell, H., LeBlanc, A.E., 1973. Punishment of saccharin drinking by amphetamine in rats and its reversal by chlordiazepoxide. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 85, 97–104. Cappell, H., LeBlanc, A.E., 1977. Parametric investigations of the effects of prior exposure to amphetamine and morphine on conditioned gustatory aversion. Psychopharmacology 51, 265–271. Cappell, H., LeBlanc, A.E., Endrenyi, L., 1973. Aversive conditioning by psychoactive drugs: effects of morphine, alcohol and chlordiazepoxide. Psychopharmacologia 29, 239–246. Cappell, H., LeBlanc, A.E., Herling, S., 1975. Modification of the punishing effects of psychoactive drugs in rats by previous drug experience. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 89, 347–356. Carey, R.J., Goodall, E.B., 1974. Amphetamine-induced taste aversion: a comparison of d- versus l-amphetamine. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 2, 325–330. Coil, J.D., Hankins, W.G., Jenden, D.J., Garcia, J., 1978. The attenuation of a specific cue-to-consequence association by antiemetic agents. Psychopharmacology 56, 21–25. Corcoran, M.E., Bolotow, I., Amit, Z., McCaughran Jr., J.A., 1974. Conditioned taste aversions produced by active and inactive cannabinoids. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 2, 725–728. Cordick, N., Parker, L.A., Ossenkopp, K.P., 1999. Rotation-induced conditioned rejection in the taste reactivity test. Neuroreport 10, 1557–1559. Cunningham, C.L., 1979. Flavor and location aversions produced by ethanol. Behavioral and Neural Biology 27, 362–367. Cunningham, C.L., Gremel, C.M., Groblewski, P.A., 2009. Genetic influences on conditioned taste aversion. In: Reilly, S., Schachtman, T.R. (Eds.), Conditioned Taste Aversion: Behavioral and Neural Processes. Oxford University Place, New York, pp. 387–421. Cunningham, C.L., Hawks, D.M., Niehus, D.R., 1988. Role of hypothermia in ethanolinduced conditioned taste aversion. Psychopharmacology 95, 318–322. Cunningham, C.L., Niehus, D.R., 1989. Effect of ingestion-contingent hypothermia on ethanol self-administration. Alcohol 6, 377–380. Curtis, K.S., Sved, A.F., Verbalis, J.G., Stricker, E.M., 1994. Lithium chloride-induced anorexia, but not conditioned taste aversions, in rats with area postrema lesions. Brain Research 663, 30–37. Dacanay, R.J., Riley, A.L., 1982. The UCS preexposure effect in taste aversion learning: tolerance and blocking are drug specific. Animal Learning and Behavior 10, 91–96. Dantzer, R., 1980. Conditioned taste aversion as an index of lead toxicity. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 13, 133–135. Davis, C.J., Harding, R.K., Leslie, R.A., Andrews, P.L.R., 1986. The organization of vomiting as a protective reflex. In: Davis, C.J., Lake-Bakaar, G.V., Grahame-Smith, D.G. (Eds.), Nausea and Vomiting: Mechanisms and Treatment. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp. 65–75. Davis, C.M., Cobuzzi, J.L., Riley, A.L., 2012. Assessment of the aversive effects of peripheral mu opioid receptor agonism in Fischer 344 and Lewis rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 101, 181–186. Davis, C.M., Rice, K.C., Riley, A.L., 2009. Opiate-agonist induced taste aversion learning in the Fischer 344 and Lewis inbred rat strains: evidence for differential mu opioid receptor activation. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 93, 397–405. Davis, C.M., Roma, P.G., Dominguez, J.M., Riley, A.L., 2007. Morphine-induced place conditioning in Fischer and Lewis rats: acquisition and dose–response in a fully biased procedure. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 86, 516–523. De Wit, H., Wise, R.A., 1977. Blockade of cocaine reinforcement in rats with the dopamine receptor blocker pimozide, but not with the noradrenergic blockers phentolamine or phenoxybenzamine. Canadian Journal of Psychology 31, 195–203. Delamater, A.R., Treit, D., 1988. Chlordiazepoxide attenuates shock-based and enhances licl-based fluid aversions. Learning and Motivation 19, 221–238. Dess, N.K., 1993. Saccharin’s aversive taste in rats: evidence and implications. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 17, 359–372. Dess, N.K., Chapman, C.D., Minor, T.R., 1988a. Inescapable shock increases finickiness about drinking quinine-adulterated water in rats. Learning and Motivation 19, 408–424. Dess, N.K., Raizer, J., Chapman, C.D., Garcia, J., 1988b. Stressors in the learned helplessness paradigm – effects on body-weight and conditioned taste-aversion in rats. Physiology and Behavior 44, 483–490. Domjan, M., Siegel, S., 1983. Attenuation of the aversive and analgesic effects of morphine by repeated administration – different mechanisms. Physiological Psychology 11, 155–158. Domjan, M., Wilson, N.E., 1972. Specificity of cue to consequence in aversion learning in the rat. Psychonomic Science 26, 143–145. Dragoin, W.B., 1971. Conditioning and extinction of taste aversions with variations in intensity of the CS and UCS in two strains of rats. Psychonomic Science 22, 303–305. Elsmore, T.F., Fletcher, G.V., 1972. Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol: aversive effects in rat at high doses. Science 175, 911–912. Etscorn, F., Moore, G.A., Hagen, L.S., Caton, T.M., Sanders, D.L., 1986. Saccharin aversions in hamsters as a result of nicotine injections. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 24, 567–570. Ettenberg, A., Geist, T.D., 1991. Animal model for investigating the anxiogenic effects of self-administered cocaine. Psychopharmacology 103, 455–461. Ettenberg, A., Geist, T.D., 1993. Qualitative and quantitative differences in the operant runway behavior of rats working for cocaine and heroin reinforcement. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 44, 191–198. Ferrari, C.M., O’Connor, D.A., Riley, A.L., 1991. Cocaine-induced taste aversions: effect of route of administration. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 38, 267–271. Fischer, G.J., Vail, B.J., 1980. Preexposure to delta-9-THC blocks THC-induced conditioned taste aversion in rats. Behavioral and Neural Biology 30, 191–196. Flaherty, C.F., 1996. Incentive Relativity. Cambridge University Press, New York. Flaherty, C.F., Checke, S., 1982. Anticipation of incentive gain. Animal Learning and Behavior 10, 177–182. Flaherty, C.E., Turovsky, J., Krauss, K.L., 1994. Relative hedonic value modulates anticipatory contrast. Physiology and Behavior 55, 1047–1054. A. Verendeev, A.L. Riley / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 (2012) 2193–2205 Foynes, M.M., Riley, A.L., 2004. Lithium-chloride-induced conditioned taste aversions in the Lewis and Fischer 344 rat strains. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 79, 303–308. Freeman, K.B., Konaklieva, M.I., Riley, A.L., 2005. Assessment of the contributions of Na+ channel inhibition and general peripheral action in cocaine-induced conditioned taste aversion. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 80, 281–288. Freeman, K.B., Riley, A.L., 2005. Cocaine-induced conditioned taste avoidance over extended conditioned stimulus-unconditioned stimulus intervals. Behavioural Pharmacology 16, 591–595. Freeman, K.B., Riley, A.L., 2009. The origins of conditioned taste aversion learning: a historical analysis. In: Reilly, S., Schachtman, T.R. (Eds.), Conditioned Taste Aversion: Behavioral and Neural Processes. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 9–33. Gaiardi, M., Bartoletti, M., Bacchi, A., Gubellini, C., Costa, M., Babbini, M., 1991. Role of repeated exposure to morphine in determining its affective properties: place and taste conditioning studies in rats. Psychopharmacology 103, 183–186. Gamzu, E., 1977. The multifaceted nature of taste-aversion inducing agents: Is there a single common factor? In: Barker, L.M., Best, M.R., Domjan, M. (Eds.), Learning Mechanisms in Food Selection. Baylor University Press, Waco, TX, pp. 477–509. Garcia, J., 1971. The faddy rat and us. New Scientist and Science Journal 25, 4–55. Garcia, J., Ervin, F.R., 1968. Gustatory-visceral and telereceptor-cutaneous conditioning: adaptation in internal and external milieus. Communications in Behavioral Biology Part A, 389–415. Garcia, J., Ervin, F.R., Koelling, R.A., 1966. Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement. Psychonomic Science 5, 121–122. Garcia, J., Kimeldorf, D.J., Koelling, R.A., 1955. Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. Science 122, 157–158. Garcia, J., Koelling, R.A., 1966. Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science 4, 123–124. Garcia, J., Kovner, R., Green, K.F., 1970. Cue properties vs palatability of flavors in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science 20, 313–314. Geddes, R.I., Han, L., Baldwin, A.E., Norgren, R., Grigson, P.S., 2008. Gustatory insular cortex lesions disrupt drug-induced, but not lithium chloride-induced, suppression of conditioned stimulus intake. Behavioral Neuroscience 122, 1038–1050. Gemberling, G.A., Domjan, M., Amsel, A., 1980. Aversion learning in 5-day-old rats: taste-toxicosis and texture-shock associations. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 94, 734–745. Gilley, D.W., Franchina, J.J., 1985. Effects of preexposure flavor concentration on conditioned aversion and neophobia. Behavioral and Neural Biology 44, 503–508. Glowa, J.R., Shaw, A.E., Riley, A.L., 1994. Cocaine-induced conditioned taste aversions: comparisons between effects in LEW/N and F344/N rat strains. Psychopharmacology 114, 229–232. Gomez-Serrano, M.A., Kearns, D.N., Riley, A.L., 2009. The effects of light cycle phase on morphine-induced conditioned taste aversions in the Lewis, Fischer and Sprague-Dawley rat strains. Behavioural Brain Research 196, 116–122. Goudie, A.J., 1979. Aversive stimulus properties of drugs. Neuropharmacology 18, 971–979. Goudie, A.J., Dickins, D.W., Thornton, E.W., 1978. Cocaine-induced conditioned taste aversions in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 8, 757–761. Goudie, A.J., Stolerman, I.P., Demellweek, C., D’Mello, G.D., 1982. Does conditioned nausea mediate drug-induced conditioned taste aversion. Psychopharmacology 78, 277–281. Goudie, A.J., Thornton, E.W., Wheatley, J., 1975. Attenuation by alphamethyltyrosine of amphetamine induced conditioned taste aversion in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 45, 119–123. Grabus, S.D., Glowa, J.R., Riley, A.L., 2004. Morphine- and cocaine-induced c-Fos levels in Lewis and Fischer rat strains. Brain Research 998, 20–28. Green, L., Bouzas, A., Rachlin, H., 1972. Test of an electric-shock analog to illnessinduced aversion. Behavioral Biology 7, 513–518. Green, L., Rachlin, H., 1973. The effect of rotation on the learning of taste aversions. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1, 137–138. Grigson, P.S., 1997. Conditioned taste aversions and drugs of abuse: a reinterpretation. Behavioral Neuroscience 111, 129–136. Grigson, P.S., Freet, C.S., 2000. The suppressive effects of sucrose and cocaine, but not lithium chloride, are greater in Lewis than in Fischer rats: evidence for the reward comparison hypothesis. Behavioral Neuroscience 114, 353–363. Grigson, P.S., Lyuboslavsky, P., Tanase, D., 2000a. Bilateral lesions of the gustatory thalamus disrupt morphine- but not LiCl-induced intake suppression in rats: evidence against the conditioned taste aversion hypothesis. Brain Research 858, 327–337. Grigson, P.S., Twining, R.C., Carelli, R.M., 2000b. Heroin-induced suppression of saccharin intake in water-deprived and water-replete rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 66, 603–608. Grigson, P.S., Twining, R.C., Freet, C.S., Wheeler, R.A., Geddes, R.I., 2009. Drug-induced suppression of conditioned stimulus intake: reward, aversion, and addiction. In: Reilly, S., Schachtman, T.R. (Eds.), Conditioned Taste Aversion: Behavioral and Neural Processes. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 74–91. Grill, H.J., Norgren, R., 1978. The taste reactivity test. I. Mimetic responses to gustatory stimuli in neurologically normal rats. Brain Research 143, 263–279. Grupp, L.A., 1977. Effects of pimozide on the acquisition, maintenance, and extinction of an amphetamine-induced taste aversion. Psychopharmacology 53, 235–242. Guitart, X., Beitner-Johnson, D., Marby, D.W., Kosten, T.A., Nestler, E.J., 1992. Fischer and Lewis rat strains differ in basal levels of neurofilament proteins and their regulation by chronic morphine in the mesolimbic dopamine system. Synapse 12, 242–253. 2203 Hall, G., 2009. Preexposure to the unconditioned stimulus in nausea-based aversion learning. In: Reilly, S., Schachtman, T.R. (Eds.), Conditioned Taste Aversion: Behavioral and Neural Processes. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 58–73. Hankins, W.G., Rusiniak, K.W., Garcia, J., 1976. Dissociation of odor and taste in shock-avoidance learning. Behavioral Biology 18, 345–358. Harris, G.C., Aston-Jones, G., 2003. Enhanced morphine preference following prolonged abstinence: association with increased Fos expression in the extended amygdala. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 292–299. He, S., Li, N., Grasing, K., 2004. Long-term opiate effects on amphetamine-induced dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens core and conditioned place preference. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 77, 327–335. Horan, B., Smith, M., Gardner, E.L., Lepore, M., Ashby, C.R., 1997. (−)-nicotine produces conditioned place preference in Lewis, but not Fischer 344 rats. Synapse 26, 93–94. Huang, A.C., Hsiao, S., 2008. Re-examination of amphetamine-induced conditioned suppression of tastant intake in rats: the task-dependent drug effects hypothesis. Behavioral Neuroscience 122, 1207–1216. Hunt, T., Amit, Z., 1987. Conditioned taste aversion induced by self-administered drugs: paradox revisited. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 11, 107–130. Hunt, T., Switzman, L., Amit, Z., 1985. Involvement of dopamine in the aversive stimulus properties of cocaine in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 22, 945–948. Infurna, R.N., Spear, L.P., 1979. Developmental changes in amphetamine-induced taste aversions. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 11, 31–35. Ionescu, E., Buresova, O., 1977. Failure to elicit conditioned taste aversion by severe poisoning. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 6, 251–254. Iraola, J.A., Alonso, G., 1995. The influence of flavored solution concentration on the poisoned-partner effect. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 63, 149–154. Iwamoto, E.T., Williamson, E.C., 1984. Nicotine-induced taste aversion: characterization and preexposure effects in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 21, 527–532. Jaffe, J.H., Martin, W.R., 1980. Opioid analgesics and antagonists. In: Gilman, A.G., Goodman, L.S., Gilman, A. (Eds.), Goodman and Gilman’s the Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics. , 6th ed. Macmillan, New York. Jensen, R.A., Gilbert, D.G., Meliska, C.J., Landrum, T.A., Szary, A.B., 1990. Characterization of a dose-response curve for nicotine-induced conditioned taste aversion in rats: relationship to elevation of plasma beta-endorphin concentration. Behavioral and Neural Biology 53, 428–440. Kalat, J.W., Rozin, P., 1973. Learned safety as a mechanism in long-delay tasteaversion learning in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 83, 198–207. Kay, E.J., 1975. Aversive effects of repeated injections of THC in rats. Psychological Reports 37, 1051–1054. Killcross, S., Robbins, T.W., Everitt, B.J., 1997. Different types of fear-conditioned behaviour mediated by separate nuclei within amygdala. Nature 388, 377–380. Klunder, C.S., O’Boyle, M., 1979. Suppression of predatory behaviors in laboratory mice following lithium chloride injections or electric shock. Animal Learning and Behavior 7, 13–16. Koob, G.F., Le Moal, M., 2006. Neurobiology of Addiction. Elsevier/Academic Press, Amsterdam, Boston. Kosten, T.A., Miserendino, M.J., Haile, C.N., DeCaprio, J.L., Jatlow, P.I., Nestler, E.J., 1997. Acquisition and maintenance of intravenous cocaine self-administration in Lewis and Fischer inbred rat strains. Brain Research 778, 418–429. Krane, R.V., Wagner, A.R., 1975. Taste aversion learning with a delayed shock US: implications for the generality of the laws of learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 88, 882–889. Kumar, R., Pratt, J.A., Stolerman, I.P., 1983. Characteristics of conditioned taste aversion produced by nicotine in rats. British Journal of Pharmacology 79, 245–253. Lancellotti, D., Bayer, B.M., Glowa, J.R., Houghtling, R.A., Riley, A.L., 2001. Morphineinduced conditioned taste aversions in the LEW/N and F344/N rat strains. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 68, 603–610. Lasiter, P.S., Braun, J.J., 1981. Shock facilitation of taste aversion learning. Behavioral and Neural Biology 32, 277–281. LeBlanc, A.E., Cappell, H., 1974. Attenuation of punishing effects of morphine and amphetamine by chronic prior treatment. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 87, 691–698. Lester, D., Nachman, M., Le Magnen, J., 1970. Aversive conditioning by ethanol in the rat. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol 31, 578–586. Limebeer, C.L., Parker, L.A., 2000. The antiemetic drug ondansetron interferes with lithium-induced conditioned rejection reactions, but not lithium-induced taste avoidance in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behavior Processes 26, 371–384. Lin, J.Y., Roman, C., Reilly, S., 2009. Morphine-induced suppression of conditioned stimulus intake: effects of stimulus type and insular cortex lesions. Brain Research 1292, 52–60. Lindberg, M.A., Beggs, A.L., Chezik, D.D., Ray, D., 1982. Flavor-toxicosis associations: tests of three hypotheses of long delay learning. Physiology and Behavior 29, 439–442. Lynch, W.J., Carroll, M.E., 2001. Regulation of drug intake. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 9, 131–143. Manzanedo, C., Aguilar, M.A., Rodriguez-Arias, M., Minarro, J., 2005. Sensitization to the rewarding effects of morphine depends on dopamine. Neuroreport 16, 201–205. 2204 A. Verendeev, A.L. Riley / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 (2012) 2193–2205 Maren, S., Aharonov, G., Fanselow, M.S., 1997. Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Behavioural Brain Research 88, 261–274. Maren, S., Fanselow, M.S., 1996. The amygdala and fear conditioning: has the nut been cracked. Neuron 16, 237–240. Martin, G.M., Bechara, A., van der Kooy, D., 1988. Morphine preexposure attenuates the aversive properties of opiates without preexposure to the aversive properties. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 30, 687–692. Martin, S., Lyupina, Y., Crespo, J.A., Gonzalez, B., Garcia-Lecumberri, C., Ambrosio, E., 2003. Genetic differences in NMDA and D1 receptor levels, and operant responding for food and morphine in Lewis and Fischer 344 rats. Brain Research 973, 205–213. Martin, S., Manzanares, J., Corchero, J., Garcia-Lecumberri, C., Crespo, J.A., Fuentes, J.A., et al., 1999. Differential basal proenkephalin gene expression in dorsal striatum and nucleus accumbens, and vulnerability to morphine selfadministration in Fischer 344 and Lewis rats. Brain Research 821, 350–355. Mayer, L.A., Parker, L.A., 1993. Rewarding and aversive properties of Ip and Sc cocaine – assessment by place and taste conditioning. Psychopharmacology 112, 189–194. McAuffle, W.E., 1975. A second look at first effects: the subjective effects of opiates on non-addicts. Journal of Drug Issues 5, 369–399. McDonald, R.V., Parker, L.A., Siegel, S., 1997. Conditioned sucrose aversions produced by naloxone-precipitated withdrawal from acutely administered morphine. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 58, 1003–1008. McLaurin, W.A., Scarborough, B.B., 1963. Extension of the interstimulus interval in saccharin avoidance conditioning. Radiation Research 20, 317–324. Meyer, J.S., Quenzer, L.F., 2005. Psychopharmacology: Drugs, the Brain, and Behavior. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, Mass. Miller, J.S., Hunt, P.S., Spear, N.E., 1989. Age-dependent facilitation of taste-footshock conditioning by prior exposure to the training context. Behavioral and Neural Biology 52, 123–130. Miller, V., Domjan, M., 1981. Specificity of cue to consequence in aversion learning in the rat: control for US-induced differential orientations. Animal Learning and Behavior 9, 339–345. Nachman, M., Hartley, P.L., 1975. Role of illness in producing learned taste aversions in rats: a comparison of several rodenticides. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 89, 1010–1018. Nakajima, S., 2004. Conditioned ethanol aversion in rats induced by voluntary wheel running, forced swimming, and electric shock: an implication for aversion therapy of alcoholism. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science: The Official Journal of the Pavlovian Society 39, 95–104. Nathan, B.A., Vogel, J.R., 1975. Taste aversions induced by d-amphetamine: doseresponse relationship. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 6, 287–288. O’Connor, C.E., Matthews, L.R., 1997. Duration of cyanide-induced conditioned food aversions in possums. Physiology and Behavior 62, 931–933. Ossenkopp, K.P., Giugno, L., 1985. Taste aversions conditioned with multiple exposures to gamma radiation: abolition by area postrema lesions in rats. Brain Research 346, 1–7. Ossenkopp, K.P., Giugno, L., 1989. Gamma radiation-induced conditioned taste aversions in rats: a comparison of the protective effects of area postrema lesions with differing doses of radiation. Physiology and Behavior 46, 747–750. Ossenkopp, K.P., Parker, L.A., Limebeer, C.L., Burton, P., Fudge, M.A., Cross-Mellor, S.K., 2003. Vestibular lesions selectively abolish body rotation-induced, but not lithium-induced, conditioned taste aversions (oral rejection responses) in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience 117, 105–112. Parker, L.A., 1982. Nonconsummatory and consummatorty behavioral CRs elicited by lithium- and amphetamine-paired flavors. Learning and Motivation 13, 281–303. Parker, L.A., 1984. Behavioral conditioned responses across multiple conditioning/testing trials elicited by lithium- and amphetamine-paired flavors. Behavioral and Neural Biology 41, 190–199. Parker, L.A., 1988. Positively reinforcing drugs may produce a different kind of CTA than drugs which are not positively reinforcing. Learning and Motivation 19, 207–220. Parker, L.A., 1991. Taste reactivity responses elicited by reinforcing drugs: a doseresponse analysis. Behavioral Neuroscience 105, 955–964. Parker, L.A., 1995. Rewarding drugs produce taste avoidance, but not taste aversion. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 19, 143–157. Parker, L.A., 1998. Emetic drugs produce conditioned rejection reactions in the taste reactivity test. Journal of Psychophysiology 12, 3–13. Parker, L.A., Brosseau, L., 1990. Apomorphine-induced flavor-drug associations: a dose–response analysis by the taste reactivity test and the conditioned taste avoidance test. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 35, 583–587. Parker, L.A., Corrick, M.L., Limebeer, C.L., Kwiatkowska, M., 2002. Amphetamine and morphine produce a conditioned taste and place preference in the house musk shrew (Suncus murinus). Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behavior Processes 28, 75–82. Parker, L.A., Gillies, T., 1995. THC-induced place and taste aversions in Lewis and Sprague-Dawley rats. Behavioral Neuroscience 109, 71–78. Parker, L.A., Hills, K., Jensen, K., 1984. Behavioral Crs elicited by a lithium-paired or an amphetamine-paired contextual test chamber. Animal Learning and Behavior 12, 307–315. Parker, L.A., Limebeer, C.L., Rana, S.A., 2009. Conditioned disgust, but not conditioned taste avoidance, may reflect conditioned nausea in rats. In: Reilly, S., Schachtman, T.R. (Eds.), Conditioned Taste Aversion: Behavioral and Neural Processes. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 92–113. Pescatore, K.A., Glowa, J.R., Riley, A.L., 2005. Strain differences in the acquisition of nicotine-induced conditioned taste aversion. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 82, 751–757. Philpot, R.M., Badanich, K.A., Kirstein, C.L., 2003. Place conditioning: age-related changes in the rewarding and aversive effects of alcohol. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research 27, 593–599. Provenza, F.D., Ortega-Reyes, L., Scott, C.B., Lynch, J.J., Burritt, E.A., 1994. Antiemetic drugs attenuate food aversions in sheep. Journal of Animal Science 72, 1989–1994. Quinn, H.R., Matsumoto, I., Callaghan, P.D., Long, L.E., Arnold, J.C., Gunasekaran, N., et al., 2008. Adolescent rats find repeated Delta(9)-THC less aversive than adult rats but display greater residual cognitive deficits and changes in hippocampal protein expression following exposure. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 1113–1126. Rabin, B.M., Hunt, W.A., 1983. Effects of antiemetics on the acquisition and recall of radiation- and lithium chloride-induced conditioned taste aversions. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 18, 629–635. Rabin, B.M., Hunt, W.A., 1989. Interaction of haloperidol and area postrema lesions in the disruption of amphetamine-induced conditioned taste-aversion learning in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 33, 847–851. Rabin, B.M., Hunt, W.A., Lee, J., 1983. Attenuation of radiation- and drug-induced conditioned taste aversions following area postrema lesions in the rat. Radiation Research 93, 388–394. Rabin, B.M., Hunt, W.A., Lee, J., 1984a. Effects of dose and of partial body ionizing radiation on taste aversion learning in rats with lesions of the area postrema. Physiology and Behavior 32, 119–122. Rabin, B.M., Hunt, W.A., Lee, J., 1984b. Recall of a previously acquired conditioned taste aversion in rats following lesions of the area postrema. Physiology and Behavior 32, 503–506. Racotta, I.S., Arzuffi, R., Garduno, I., Racotta, R., 1997. Antiemetic inhibits conditioned taste aversion, but not the hypophagia induced by epinephrine. Physiology and Behavior 62, 1189–1191. Rana, S.A., Parker, L.A., 2007. Effect of prior exposure to a lithium- and an amphetamine-paired flavor on the acoustic startle response in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behavior Processes 33, 172–184. Rana, S.A., Parker, L.A., 2008. Differential effects of neurotoxin-induced lesions of the basolateral amygdala and central nucleus of the amygdala on lithium-induced conditioned disgust reactions and conditioned taste avoidance. Behavioural Brain Research 189, 284–297. Randall-Thompson, J.F., Riley, A.L., 2003. Morphine-induced conditioned taste aversions: assessment of sexual dimorphism. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 76, 373–381. Reicher, M.A., Holman, E.W., 1977. Location preference and flavor aversion reinforced by amphetamine in rats. Animal Learning and Behavior 5, 343–346. Reilly, S., Pritchard, T.C., 1996. Gustatory thalamus lesions in the rat: I. Innate taste preferences and aversions. Behavioral Neuroscience 110, 737–745. Revusky, S., 1968. Aversion to sucrose produced by contingent x-irradiation: temporal and dosage parameters. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 65, 17–22. Revusky, S., Garcia, J., 1970. Learned associations over long delays. In: Bower, G., Spence, J. (Eds.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. Academic Press, New York, pp. 1–84. Revusky, S., Reilly, S., 1989. Attenuation of conditioned taste aversions by external stressors. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 33, 219–226. Riley, A.L., 2011. The paradox of drug taking: the role of the aversive effects of drugs. Physiology and Behavior 103, 69–78. Riley, A.L., Dacanay, R.J., Mastropaolo, J.P., 1984. The effect of morphine preexposure on the acquisition of morphine-induced taste-aversions – a nonassociative effect. Animal Learning and Behavior 12, 157–162. Riley, A.L., Davis, C.M., Roma, P.G., 2009. Strain differences in taste aversion learning: implications for animal models of drug abuse. In: Reilly, S., Schachtman, T.R. (Eds.), Conditioned Taste Aversion: Behavioral and Neural Processes. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 226–261. Riley, A.L., Diamond, H.F., 1998. The effects of cocaine preexposure on the acquisition of cocaine-induced taste aversions. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 60, 739–745. Riley, A.L., Jacobs, W.J., LoLordo, V.M., 1978. Morphine-induced taste aversions: a consideration of parameters. Physiological Psychology 6, 96–100. Riley, A.L., Simpson, G.R., 2001. The attenuating effects of drug preexposure on taste aversion conditioning: generality, experimental parameters, underlying mechanisms and implications for drug use and abuse. In: Mowrer, R.R., Klein, S.B. (Eds.), Contemporary Learning Theory. , 2 ed. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp. 505–559. Riley, A.L., Tuck, D.L., 1985. Conditioned taste aversions: a behavioral index of toxicity. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 443, 272–292. Riley, A.L., Zellner, D.A., 1978. Methylphenidate-induced conditioned taste aversions: an index of toxicity. Physiological Psychology 6, 354–358. Rinker, J.A., Busse, G.D., Riley, A.L., 2008. An assessment of sex differences in nicotineinduced conditioned taste aversions. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 88, 427–431. Risinger, F.O., Cunningham, C.L., 1998. Ethanol-induced conditioned taste aversion in BXD recombinant inbred mice. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research 22, 1234–1244. A. Verendeev, A.L. Riley / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 (2012) 2193–2205 Ritter, S., McGlone, J.J., Kelley, K.W., 1980. Absence of lithium-induced taste aversion after area postrema lesion. Brain Research 201, 501–506. Roma, P.G., Davis, C.M., Riley, A.L., 2007. Effects of cross-fostering on cocaine-induced conditioned taste aversions in Fischer and Lewis rats. Developmental Psychobiology 49, 172–179. Roma, P.G., Flint, W.W., Higley, J.D., Riley, A.L., 2006. Assessment of the aversive and rewarding effects of alcohol in Fischer and Lewis rats. Psychopharmacology 189, 187–199. Roma, P.G., Rinker, J.A., Serafine, K.M., Chen, S.A., Barr, C.S., Cheng, K., et al., 2008. Genetic and early environmental contributions to alcohol’s aversive and physiological effects. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 91, 134–139. Rozin, P., Kalat, J.W., 1971. Specific hungers and poison avoidance as adaptive specializations of learning. Psychological Review 78, 459–486. Rusiniak, K.W., Palmerino, C.C., Rice, A.G., Forthman, D.L., Garcia, J., 1982. Flavorillness aversions: potentiation of odor by taste with toxin but not shock in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 96, 527–539. Sakai, N., Yamamoto, T., 1997. Conditioned taste aversion and c-fos expression in the rat brainstem after administration of various USs. Neuroreport 8, 2215–2220. Sanchez-Cardoso, P., Higuera-Matas, A., Martin, S., del Olmo, N., Miguens, M., GarciaLecumberri, C., et al., 2007. Modulation of the endogenous opioid system after morphine self-administration and during its extinction: a study in Lewis and Fischer 344 rats. Neuropharmacology 52, 931–948. Scalera, G., Grigson, P.S., Norgren, R., 1997. Gustatory functions, sodium appetite, and conditioned taste aversion survive excitotoxic lesions of the thalamic taste area. Behavioral Neuroscience 111, 633–645. Schramm-Sapyta, N.L., Cha, Y.M., Chaudhry, S., Wilson, W.A., Swartzwelder, H.S., Kuhn, C.M., 2007. Differential anxiogenic, aversive, and locomotor effects of THC in adolescent and adult rats. Psychopharmacology 191, 867–877. Schramm-Sapyta, N.L., Morris, R.W., Kuhn, C.M., 2006. Adolescent rats are protected from the conditioned aversive properties of cocaine and lithium chloride. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 84, 344–352. Schramm-Sapyta, N.L., Walker, Q.D., Caster, J.M., Levin, E.D., Kuhn, C.M., 2009. Are adolescents more vulnerable to drug addiction than adults? Evidence from animal models. Psychopharmacology 206, 1–21. Selden, N.R., Everitt, B.J., Jarrard, L.E., Robbins, T.W., 1991. Complementary roles for the amygdala and hippocampus in aversive conditioning to explicit and contextual cues. Neuroscience 42, 335–350. Sellings, L.H., Baharnouri, G., McQuade, L.E., Clarke, P.B., 2008. Rewarding and aversive effects of nicotine are segregated within the nucleus accumbens. The European Journal of Neuroscience 28, 342–352. Serafine, K.M., Riley, A.L., 2012. Cocaine-induced conditioned taste aversions: role of monoamine reuptake inhibition. In: Hall, F.S. (Ed.), Serotonin: Biosynthesis, Regulation and Health Implications. NOVA Science Publishers, Inc, Hauppauge, NY. Shram, M.J., Funk, D., Li, Z., Le, A.D., 2006. Periadolescent and adult rats respond differently in tests measuring the rewarding and aversive effects of nicotine. Psychopharmacology 186, 201–208. Simpson, G.R., Riley, A.L., 2005. Morphine preexposure facilitates morphine place preference and attenuates morphine taste aversion. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 80, 471–479. Sklar, L.S., Amit, Z., 1977. Manipulations of catecholamine systems block the conditioned taste aversion induced by self-administered drugs. Neuropharmacology 16, 649–655. Steigerwald, E.S., Rusiniak, K.W., Eckel, D.L., O’Regan, M.H., 1988. Aversive conditioning properties of caffeine in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 31, 579–584. Stolerman, I.P., 1992. Drugs of abuse: behavioural principles, methods and terms. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences 13, 170–176. 2205 Stolerman, I.P., D’Mello, G.D., 1981. Oral self-administration and the relevance of conditioned taste aversions. In: Thompson, T., Dews, P.B., McKim, W.A. (Eds.), Advances in Behavioral Pharmacology. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 169–214. Suzuki, T., George, F.R., Meisch, R.A., 1988a. Differential establishment and maintenance of oral ethanol reinforced behavior in Lewis and Fischer 344 inbred rat strains. The Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 245, 164–170. Suzuki, T., Ise, Y., Maeda, J., Misawa, M., 1999. Mecamylamine-precipitated nicotinewithdrawal aversion in Lewis and Fischer 344 inbred rat strains. European Journal of Pharmacology 369, 159–162. Suzuki, T., Otani, K., Koike, Y., Misawa, M., 1988b. Genetic differences in preferences for morphine and codeine in Lewis and Fischer 344 inbred rat strains. Japanese Journal of Pharmacology 47, 425–431. Swank, M.W., Bernstein, I.L., 1994. c-Fos induction in response to a conditioned stimulus after single trial taste aversion learning. Brain Research 636, 202–208. Switzman, L., Fishman, B., Amit, Z., 1981. Pre-exposure effects of morphine, diazepam and delta 9-THC on the formation of conditioned taste aversions. Psychopharmacology 74, 149–157. Symonds, M., Hall, G., 2000. Contextual conditioning with an illness US is attenuated by the antiemetic ondansetron. Psychobiology 28, 260–266. Tecce, J.J., Cole, J.O., 1974. Amphetamine effects in man: paradoxical drowsiness and lowered electrical brain acitivity (CNV). Science 185, 451–453. Turenne, S.D., Miles, C., Parker, L.A., Siegel, S., 1996. Individual differences in reactivity to the rewarding/aversive properties of drugs: assessment by taste and place conditioning. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 53, 511–516. van Haaren, F., Hughes, C.E., 1990. Cocaine-induced conditioned taste-aversions in male and female Wistar rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 37, 693–696. Verendeev, A., Riley, A.L., 2011. Relationship between the rewarding and aversive effects of morphine and amphetamine in individual subjects. Learning and Behavior 39, 399–408. Vishwanath, J.M., Desko, A.G., Riley, A.L., 2011. Caffeine-induced taste aversions in Lewis and Fischer rat strains: differential sensitivity to the aversive effects of drugs. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 100, 66–72. Weeks, J.R., 1962. Experimental morphine addiction: method for automatic intravenous injections in unrestrained rats. Science 138, 143–144. Wheeler, R.A., Twining, R.C., Jones, J.L., Slater, J.M., Grigson, P.S., Carelli, R.M., 2008. Behavioral and electrophysiological indices of negative affect predict cocaine self-administration. Neuron 57, 774–785. White, B.C., Mason, F.D., 1985. Caffeine-induced taste aversion and mimetic responses. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 23, 515–518. White, N., Sklar, L., Amit, Z., 1977. The reinforcing action of morphine and its paradoxical side effect. Psychopharmacology 52, 63–66. Wilmouth, C.E., Spear, L.P., 2004. Adolescent and adult rats’ aversion to flavors previously paired with nicotine. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1021, 462–464. Wise, R.A., Yokel, R.A., DeWit, H., 1976. Both positive reinforcement and conditioned aversion from amphetamine and from apomorphine in rats. Science 191, 1273–1275. Yokel, R.A., Wise, R.A., 1976. Attenuation of intravenous amphetamine reinforcement by central dopamine blockade in rats. Psychopharmacology 48, 311–318. Zakharova, E., Leoni, G., Kichko, I., Izenwasser, S., 2009a. Differential effects of methamphetamine and cocaine on conditioned place preference and locomotor activity in adult and adolescent male rats. Behavioural Brain Research 198, 45–50. Zakharova, E., Wade, D., Izenwasser, S., 2009b. Sensitivity to cocaine conditioned reward depends on sex and age. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 92, 131–134.