Developmental Psychobiology Zachary E. Hurwitz Jennifer L. Cobuzzi Andrew P. Merluzzi Bradley Wetzell Anthony L. Riley Psychopharmacology Laboratory Department of Psychology American University 4400 Mass. Ave., NW, Washington, DC 20016 E-mail: zacharyehurwitz@gmail.com Prepubertal Fischer 344 Rats Display Stronger MorphineInduced Taste Avoidance Than Prepubertal Lewis Rats ABSTRACT: The present report asked if the previously reported differences in morphine-induced conditioned taste avoidance between adult F344 and LEW rats (F344 > LEW) are also evident in prepubescence (early adolescence). To assess this possibility, adult (Experiment 1) and prepubertal (Experiment 2) F344 and LEW rats were assessed for their ability to acquire morphine-induced taste avoidance (0, 3.2, 10, or 18 mg/kg) in a modified taste avoidance procedure. In each experiment, rats of both strains were given repeated pairings of saccharin and morphine followed by a final two-bottle avoidance test. Adult and prepubertal F344 subjects displayed a more rapid acquisition of the avoidance response as well as stronger suppression of consumption than their LEW counterparts. These data suggest the strains differ in their sensitivity to the aversive effects of morphine and that this differential sensitivity is evident early in development and is developmentally stable. The basis for these strain differences in morphine-induced avoidance was discussed. ß 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Dev Psychobiol Keywords: strain differences; prepubertal; early adolescence; adult; F344; LEW; morphine-induced taste avoidance; development INTRODUCTION Recent work on conditioned taste avoidance (CTA) learning between the inbred F344 and LEW rat strains has demonstrated strain differences in drug-induced taste avoidance that vary depending on the drug of abuse tested (for cocaine, see Davis & Riley, 2007; Glowa, Shaw, & Riley, 1994; for nicotine, see Pescatore, Glowa, & Riley, 2005; for ethanol, see Roma, Flint, Higley, & Riley, 2006). One compound for which such strain differences have been well characterized is morphine. Specifically, Lancellotti, Bayer, Glowa, Manuscript Received: 2 July 2013 Manuscript Accepted: 30 September 2013 The authors declared that they have no conflicts of interest. Correspondence to: Zachary E. Hurwitz Contract grant sponsor: Mellon Foundation Contract grant sponsor: American University Artists and Scholars Fellowship Article first published online in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com). DOI 10.1002/dev.21176 ß 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Houghtling, and Riley (2001) reported that the F344 strain displayed dose-dependent morphine-induced taste avoidance, while the LEW strain did not acquire avoidance at any dose tested, even after repeated conditioning trials (Davis, Cobuzzi, & Riley, 2012; see also Gomez-Serrano, Kearns, & Riley, 2009; for a review, see Riley, 2011). That the F344 animals show stronger avoidance is interesting in that they also selfadminister less morphine (Ambrosio, Goldberg, & Elmer, 1995; Garcı́a-Lecumberri et al., 2011; Martı́n et al., 1999, 2003) and form weaker place preferences (Davis, Roma, Dominguez, & Riley, 2007; Grakalic, Schindler, Baumann, Rice, & Riley, 2006) than the LEW strain, suggesting that morphine intake in these strains may be impacted by the relative balance of these affective properties. Although the differences between the F344 and LEW strains with respect to morphine-induced taste avoidance are consistently observed in adults (see above), it remains unknown if such effects are evident early in life and are developmentally stable or if they 2 Hurwitz et al. are a function of specific developmental histories that result in differential behavior in adulthood. Interestingly, strain differences have been reported to vary over development for a number of physiological and behavioral endpoints in a variety of rodent strains. For example, Wilking et al. (2012) reported that nicotine preference varied as a function of age and dose in the C3H/Ibg and C57bl/6j mouse strains. Specifically, there were no differences between the strains across various doses tested in late adolescence and adulthood. However, in early adolescence C3H/Ibg subjects were more sensitive than the C57bl/6j strain at 10 mg concentration while in middle adolescence C3H/Ibg subjects emitted less behavior than C57bl/6j mice at 20 and 30 mg concentrations (for other examples of developmental differences in strain comparisons, see Allam, 2012; Fairless et al., 2012; Farid, Martinez, Geyer, & Swerdlow, 2000; Moore, Forrest, & Boehm, 2013; Moore, Linsenbardt, Melón, & Boehm, 2011; Paylor, Baskall-Baldini, Yuva, & Wehner, 1996; Satinder, 1981; Sinaiko & Mirkin, 1974; Tonkiss, Shultz, & Galler, 1992; Vogl, Atchley, & Xu, 1994). Although age differences across a variety of strains have been well characterized, little has been examined with the F344 and LEW rat lines, especially studies examining the ontogenesis of strain differences in drug reactivity (see Gomez-Serrano, Sternberg, & Riley, 2002; Gomez-Serrano, Tonelli, Listwak, Sternberg, & Riley, 2001). In the only developmental assessment of behavioral differences between the two strains, Siviy, Love, DeCicco, Giordano, and Seifert (2003) reported that strain differences in play behavior (LEW > F344) were present and comparable at all developmental periods assessed. Although suggestive that these phenotypic differences are highly heritable and stable, no developmental assessments have been made with the F344 and LEW strains in relation to their relative sensitivities to drugs of abuse yet alone their ability to acquire drug-induced taste avoidance. It is important to note in this context that prepubertal (early adolescent) and adult outbred rats differ dramatically in taste avoidance learning (Anderson, Agoglia, Morales, Varlinskaya, & Spear, 2012; Anderson, Varlinskaya, & Spear, 2010; Cobuzzi et al., 2013; Hurwitz, Merluzzi, & Riley, 2013; Infurna & Spear, 1979; Schramm-Sapyta et al., 2007; Schramm-Sapyta, Morris, & Kuhn, 2006; Shram, Funk, Li, & Lê, 2006; VetterO’Hagen, Varlinskaya, & Spear, 2009; Wilmouth & Spear, 2004) with prepubertal rats exhibiting significantly weaker taste avoidance relative to their adult counterparts. Such results suggest that prepubescent rats are less sensitive to the aversive effects of such drugs than adults, a differential sensitivity that may confer an increased susceptibility to the subsequent use and abuse of these Developmental Psychobiology compounds (see Doremus-Fitzwater, Varlinskaya, & Spear, 2010; Misanin, Anderson, & Hinderliter, 2009; Schramm-Sapyta et al., 2010; Spear, 2013). Given that differences with other strains have been shown to vary over development and that outbred prepubertal and adult rats differ significantly in druginduced avoidance across a range of drugs of abuse, it might be predicted that the patterns evident in F344 and LEW may differ across development. If prepubertal F344 and LEW rats do not differ in their avoidance patterns (and as such differ from the pattern displayed by adults), one might argue that some unique developmental history interacting with the unique genetic backgrounds of the two strains may mediate the results typically seen in adults. Conversely, if the strain differences observed in adults are also present in prepubescence, it would suggest that these differences in avoidance learning are highly heritable, substantiating the use of these strains as genetic models for drug sensitivity, use and abuse (Beitner-Johnson, Guitart, & Nestler, 1991; Guitart et al., 1993; Kosten et al., 1997). To address this issue, prepubescent rats of the F344 and LEW stains were given access to a novel saccharin solution and injected with varying doses of morphine (Experiment 2). The specific procedure employed (see below) was modified from the typical taste avoidance procedure to accommodate their developmental window and allow for assessment of avoidance learning under conditions that minimize the effects of fluid deprivation (and concomitant weight loss; see Hurwitz et al., 2013). This modification employs deprivation procedures not generally used in work with adults. Consequently, prior to this assessment, adult F344 and LEW subjects were tested under similar conditions to assure that the often-reported differences between the two strains in adult animals were evident under the modified procedure (Experiment 1). METHOD Subjects One hundred thirty-nine experimentally naı̈ve male F344 and LEW rats (Harlan Laboratories, Indianapolis, IN) arrived at the facility on PND 21, weighing approximately 40 g. Experiments 1 and 2 were each run in two replicates with subjects in all dose groups and both strains represented in each replicate. Food and water were available ad libitum unless stated otherwise. Procedures recommended by the National Research Council (1996) and the Committee on Guidelines for the Care and Use of Animals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research (2003) were followed at all times. The research was reviewed and approved by the American University IACUC. Developmental Psychobiology Apparatus Upon arrival to the animal colony, subjects in each strain were initially handled and then group housed (2– 4 rats per bin) in polycarbonate bins (23 cm 44 cm 21 cm). All subjects were maintained on a 12:12 light–dark cycle (lights on at 0800 hr) and at an ambient temperature of 23˚C. All conditioning and testing occurred during the light phase of the light–dark cycle at 0800 hr and in the same room in which the animals were housed. During adaptation and conditioning, animals were transferred to individual hanging wire-mesh test cages (24.3 cm 19 cm 18 cm) but were returned to their group-housed bins after conditioning trials (see details below). Drugs Morphine sulfate was generously supplied by the National Institute on Drug Abuse. For behavioral testing, morphine sulfate was dissolved in sterile isotonic saline (.9%) at a concentration of 5 mg/ml and was subsequently filtered through a .2 mm filter to remove any contaminants before being administered subcutaneously (SC) at a dose of 3.2, 10, or 18 mg/kg. Morphine administered at these doses and by this route has been reported to induce dose-dependent avoidance in outbred rats (Hurwitz et al., 2013) and in the F344 and LEW strains (Davis et al., 2012; Gomez-Serrano et al., 2009; Lancellotti et al., 2001). Sterile isotonic saline was also filtered before being administered to vehicle controls and was given equivolume to the highest dose of morphine administered (18 mg/kg). Volume of the injection was manipulated in favor of concentration given the influence concentration has on drug absorption and distribution. Morphine-Induced Taste Avoidance Experiment 1: Adults. Procedures were adapted from Hurwitz et al. (2013). Specifically, F344 (n ¼ 35) and LEW subjects (n ¼ 36) were brought into the laboratory on PND 21 and maintained on ad libitum food and water until PND 77 to permit the control of their developmental environment (housing conditions, handling, and light/dark cycle). Subjects were weighed and handled and had their water consumption measured from PND 77–83 at which point water adaptation began. On PND 84, subjects in each strain underwent full fluid deprivation at 0800 hr and on the following day (PND 85) they were removed from their group-housed bin at 0800 hr, weighed and placed into individual test cages where they were given 45-min access to tap water in graduated 50-ml Nalgene tubes beginning at 0830 hr. This specific procedure was used to minimize the Prepubertal Strain Differences in Morphine CTA 3 potential stress associated with more severe deprivation schedules, for example, extended 20-min daily access, in adolescent subjects (see Anderson et al., 2010; Hurwitz et al., 2013). Under this modified deprivation procedure, fluid consumption is generally lower, necessitating longer access to assure sufficient consumption. After 45 min, bottles were removed, consumption was recorded and subjects remained in the hanging cages for an additional 20 min before being returned to their group-housed bin and given ad libitum water until 0800 hr the following day (PND 86). This procedure was repeated one additional time to adapt animals to consuming fluid in the test cages prior to being given access to saccharin during conditioning. On the day prior to the first saccharin conditioning day, water access was again fully restricted before subjects underwent conditioning in the test cages. On this first conditioning trial (PND 89), subjects were weighed and handled (as previously described) and given 45-min access to a novel saccharin solution (1 g/L) in the test cages after which they remained for an additional 20 min. At this point, subjects in each strain (independent of their group-housed bin) were assigned to one of four groups such that saccharin intake was comparable among groups. Based on these group assignments, subjects in each strain were injected with either morphine (3.2, 10, or 18 mg/kg, SC) or vehicle and then returned to their group-housed bins and given ad libitum water until 0800 hr the following day. This procedure yielded Groups F0 (n ¼ 9), L0 (n ¼ 9), F3.2 (n ¼ 8), L3.2 (n ¼ 8), F10 (n ¼ 8), L10 (n ¼ 9), F18 (n ¼ 10), and L18 (n ¼ 10) where the letter refers to the strain of the subject and the number indicates the dose of morphine administered. On the next day, subjects in each bin had their fluid consumption restricted before the next saccharin conditioning trial. This 2-day procedure (saccharin-water recovery-full deprivation) was repeated four times. On the day after the fourth cycle (PND 97), subjects were transferred to test cages where two 50-ml Nalgene tubes (one containing tap water; the other containing the .1% saccharin solution) were affixed to the cage for 45 min and consumption of both solutions was recorded. Placement of the bottles was counterbalanced across all subjects to prevent positioning effects. After the 45 min, the bottles were removed, consumption was recorded and subjects were returned to their home cages where water was available ad libitum. Experiment 2: Prepubescents. The procedures described above were identical for the prepubescent rats with the following exceptions. F344 and LEW rats (n ¼ 34 per strain) were brought into the facility at PND 21 and were weighed and handled and had their water consumption measured until PND 29. Following 4 Hurwitz et al. Developmental Psychobiology (Replicate) 2 (Strain) 4 (Dose) 4 (Trial) mixedmodel ANOVA yielded significant effects of Trial [F (3, 156) ¼ 11.611], Strain [F (1, 55) ¼ 159.148], Dose [F (3, 55) ¼ 37.403] as well as significant Trial Replicate [F (3, 156) ¼ 6.950], Trial Strain [F (3, 156) ¼ 23.220], Trial Dose [F (9, 156) ¼ 22.869], Strain Dose [F (3, 55) ¼ 1.389], Replicate Strain [F (1, 55) ¼ 7.536] and Trial Strain Dose [F (9, 156) ¼ 6.098] interactions. Although on Trial 1 F344 subjects did not differ in saccharin consumption, on Trials 2–4 all drug-treated F344 subjects drank less saccharin than vehicle-treated controls with Group 18 drinking less saccharin than Group 3.2. On Trial 1, LEW subjects also did not differ in saccharin consumption. On Trials 2–4, Group 18 drank less than Group 0. On all trials, F344 subjects consumed significantly less saccharin than LEW subjects. Tukey’s HSD revealed that on Trial 1 Groups F0, F3.2, and F18 drank significantly less saccharin than Groups L0, L3.2, and L18, respectively, with no differences between Groups F10 and L10. On Trials 2–4, Groups F0 and L0 did not differ but Groups F3.2, F10, and F18 drank significantly less saccharin than Groups L3.2, L10, and L18, respectively (see Fig. 1). Over conditioning, Group F0 displayed a significant increase in saccharin intake from Trials 1 to 4 [t (8) ¼ 5.812]. Group F3.2 [t (7) ¼ 3.036] exhibited no significant changes in saccharin consumption, while Group F10 [t (7) ¼ 10.583] and Group F18 [t (7) ¼ 11.239] significantly decreased saccharin intake over trials. Group L0 [t (8) ¼ 3.440] exhibited a significant increase in saccharin consumption over trials, while Groups L3.2 [t (7) ¼ 1.843], L10 [t (8) ¼ .000], and L18 [t (9) ¼ 1.467] did not display any significant changes in saccharin intake from Trials 1 to 4. water adaptation (PND 29–32; see above), conditioning began. During conditioning (PND 33–40), subjects in each strain were given four saccharin-morphine (or vehicle) pairings until the two-bottle test, which was administered on PND 41. The group designations were similar to those in Experiment 1: Groups F0 (n ¼ 9), L0 (n ¼ 9), F3.2 (n ¼ 8), L3.2 (n ¼ 8), F10 (n ¼ 8), L10 (n ¼ 8), F18 (n ¼ 9), and L18 (n ¼ 9). Statistical Analysis Acquisition. A 2 (Replicate) 2 (Strain) 4 (Dose) 4 (Trial) mixed model ANOVA on saccharin consumption (ml) on the four conditioning trials was run for each experiment to determine differences between the strains as a function of dose and trial. One-way ANOVAs and Tukey’s HSD posthost tests were employed where merited by significant interactions to determine differences between the strains and doses across trials. To determine if there were differences in saccharin consumption between Conditioning Trials 1 and 4, Bonferroni-corrected t-tests were employed (a ¼ .0125) for each group. Two-Bottle Test. A 2 (Replicate) 2 (Strain) 4 (Dose) univariate ANOVA was run for each experiment on the percent saccharin consumed on the two-bottle test. One-way ANOVAs and Tukey’s HSD post hoc tests were used where merited to evaluate differences in the percent saccharin consumed between replicates and strains at the different dose groups. RESULTS Experiment 1: Adults Acquisition. Over conditioning, F344 subjects drank significantly less saccharin than LEW subjects, indicative of greater morphine-induced taste avoidance. The 2 A Two-Bottle Test. On the two-bottle test, the percent saccharin consumed by morphine-injected adult F344 B FIGURE 1 Mean (SEM) saccharin consumption (ml) by F344 (A) and LEW (B) adults during acquisition. #Significant differences between Group 3.2 and Group 18. Significant differences between Group 0 and Groups 3.2, 10 and 18. $Significant differences between Group 0 and Group 18. Prepubertal Strain Differences in Morphine CTA Developmental Psychobiology subjects was less than that of adult LEW subjects. The 2 (Replicate) 2 (Strain) 4 (Dose) univariate ANOVA on the percent saccharin consumed on the two-bottle test revealed significant effects of Dose [F (3, 55) ¼ 25.403] and Strain [F (1, 55) ¼ 9.508] as well as a significant Dose Strain interaction [F (3, 55) ¼ 9.674]. All drug-treated F344 subjects drank a significantly lower percentage of saccharin relative to vehicle-treated F344 subjects. Within-strain analyses of the LEW strain indicated that Group L18 drank a significantly lower percentage of saccharin relative to Group L0 with no other differences. Subsequent use of Tukey’s HSD revealed that Groups F3.2 and F10 drank a significantly lower percentage of saccharin than Groups L3.2 and L10, with no strain differences between Groups F0 and L0 and F18 and L18 (see Fig. 2). over conditioning. On all trials, F344 subjects consumed significantly less saccharin than LEW subjects. Tukey’s HSD revealed that on Trial 1, F0 and F3.2 drank significantly less saccharin than L0 and L3.2, respectively, with Groups F10 and F18 drinking comparable levels of saccharin relative to Groups L10 and L18. On Trial 2, Groups F0 and F3.2 did not differ from Groups L0 and L3.2, while Groups F10 and F18 drank significantly less saccharin than Groups L10 and L18. On Trials 3 and 4, Groups F0 and L0 did not differ, while all drug-treated F344 subjects consumed significantly less saccharin than their respective drugtreated LEW subjects (see Fig. 3). Over conditioning, Group F0 displayed a significant increase in saccharin intake from Trials 1 to 4 [t (8) ¼ 5.812]. Group F3.2 [t (7) ¼ 3.036] exhibited no significant changes in saccharin consumption, while Group F10 [t (7) ¼ 10.583] and Group F18 [t (7) ¼ 11.239] significantly decreased saccharin intake over trials. Group L0 [t (8) ¼ 3.440] exhibited a significant increase in saccharin consumption over trials, while Groups L3.2 [t (7) ¼ 1.843], L10 [t (8) ¼ .000], and L18 [t (9) ¼ 1.467] did not display any significant changes in saccharin intake from Trials 1 to 4. Experiment 2: Prepubescents Acquisition. Over conditioning, F344 subjects drank significantly less saccharin than LEW subjects, indicative of greater morphine-induced taste avoidance. The 2 (Replicate) 2 (Strain) 4 (Dose) 4 (Trial) mixedmodel ANOVA yielded significant effects of Trial [F (3, 156) ¼ 7.651], Strain [F (1, 52) ¼ 198.754], Dose [F (3, 52) ¼ 21.722], and Replicate [F (1, 52) ¼ 17.960] as well as significant Trial Replicate [F (3, 156) ¼ 12.594], Trial Strain [F (3, 156) ¼ 37.621], Trial Dose [F (9, 156) ¼ 14.885], Strain Dose [F (3, 52) ¼ 6.818], and Trial Strain Dose [F (9, 156) ¼ 6.098] interactions. Although F344 subjects did not differ in saccharin consumption on the initial exposure to saccharin, on Trial 2 Groups 10 and 18 drank significantly less than Group 0. On Trials 3 and 4, all drug-treated F344 subjects differed from vehicle-treated subjects. On Trial 4, Group 10 drank less than Group 3.2. LEW subjects did not differ in saccharin consumption at any point A 5 Two-Bottle Test. On the two-bottle test, the percent saccharin consumed by morphine-injected F344 subjects was less than that of LEW subjects. The 2 (Replicate) 2 (Strain) 4 (Dose) univariate ANOVA on the percent saccharin consumed revealed a significant 3-way interaction [F (3, 52) ¼ 3.349]. Subsequent 2 (Strain) 4 (Dose) univariate ANOVAs on each replicate revealed significant effects of Dose and Strain as well as a significant Dose Strain interaction (Replicate One: [F (3, 22) ¼ 19.348]; [F (1, 22) ¼ 121.513]; [F (3, 22) ¼ 5.120]; Replicate Two: [F (3, 30) ¼ 9.485]; [F (1, 30) ¼ 14.727]; [F (3, 30) ¼ 13.578]; respectively). Subsequent one-way B FIGURE 2 Mean (SEM) percent saccharin consumed by F344 (A) and LEW (B) adults. Significant differences between Group 0 and Groups 3.2, 10 and 18. Significant differences between Group 0 and Group 18. 6 Hurwitz et al. Developmental Psychobiology A B FIGURE 3 Mean (SEM) saccharin consumption (ml) by F344 (A) and LEW (B) prepubescents during acquisition. #Significant differences between Group 0 and Groups 10 and 18. Significant differences between Group 0 and Groups 3.2, 10 and 18. ^Significant differences between Group 3.2 and Group 10. that robust differences have been observed in taste avoidance learning induced by a variety of drugs of abuse in outbred prepubertal and adult rats (see above), Experiment 2 assessed if the often-reported differences in morphine-induced avoidance learning in adult F344 and LEW rats are also evident in prepubescence. As described, prepubescent F344 rats acquired the avoidance at a faster rate and to a stronger degree than animals in the LEW strain, a pattern that paralleled the results of Experiment 1 with adults under the modified deprivation procedure and those previously reported with adults in these strains (Davis et al., 2012; GomezSerrano et al., 2009; Lancellotti et al., 2001). The present results demonstrate that differences in morphine-induced taste avoidance are evident as early as prepubescence and are developmentally stable, suggesting that these differences are highly heritable (for other developmental strain assessments, see Allam, 2012; Fairless et al., 2012; Farid et al., 2000; Moore et al., 2011, 2013; Paylor et al., 1996; Satinder, 1981; Sinaiko & Mirkin, 1974; Tonkiss et al., 1992; Vogl et al., 1994; Wilking et al., 2012). ANOVAs on the percent saccharin consumed in Replicate One indicated significant differences between groups [F (7, 29) ¼ 31.796]. Tukey’s HSD indicated that Groups F0 and L0 did not differ while all drugtreated F344 subjects drank a significantly lower percentage of saccharin than drug-treated LEW subjects. The one-way ANOVA on Replicate Two also indicated significant differences between groups [F (7, 37) ¼ 12.181]. Subsequent use of Tukey’s HSD on Replicate Two indicated Groups F0 and L0, F3.2 and L3.2, and F10 and L10 did not differ while F18 drank a significantly lower percentage of saccharin than Group L18. Given that for each replicate, the general effect was similar (i.e., F344 > LEW), data from each replicate were pooled for presentation (see Fig. 4). DISCUSSION Given that strain differences have been reported to vary over development for a number of rodent strains and A B FIGURE 4 Mean (SEM) percent saccharin consumed by F344 (A) and LEW (B) prepubescents. Significant differences between Group 0 and Groups 3.2, 10, and 18. Developmental Psychobiology Although strain differences are clearly evident in morphine-induced taste avoidance as early as prepubescence, the basis for these differences are unknown. One obvious possibility is that the two strains may differ in blood and/or brain levels of morphine. Interestingly, morphine plasma levels do not differ between adult F344 and LEW rats (Davis & Riley, 2007; Gosnell & Krahn, 1993; Lancellotti et al., 2001), although one report has demonstrated that F344 animals have higher brain morphine levels relative to LEW animals 30 min following morphine injection (Gosnell & Krahn, 1993). It remains unknown whether adolescents show a similar pattern of peripheral and/or central distribution and how that may relate to strain differences in avoidance learning. Given that the F344 strain displays greater stress reactivity then the LEW strain (Dhabhar, McEwen, & Spencer, 1993; Dhabhar, Miller, McEwen, & Spencer, 1995; Sternberg et al., 1992; for a review see Kosten & Ambrosio, 2002), the reported strain differences in morphine-induced taste avoidance could be due to potential stress associated with the specific procedures utilized in conditioning taste avoidance in the present experiments, for example, deprivation, handling, injection. Despite the differential stress reactivity in these strains (F344 > LEW), work assessing the effects of stress on the acquisition and expression of taste avoidance in outbred subjects is mixed, typically with the results indicating no direct relationship between stress and the degree of taste avoidance conditioned (Anderson, Hinderliter, & Misanin, 2006; Bourne, Calton, Gustavson, & Schachtman, 1992; Bowers, Amit, & Gringras, 1996; Misanin, Kaufhold, Paul, Hinderliter, & Anderson, 2006; Revusky & Reilly, 1989). It is important to note that the vast majority of this work has been done in adults, precluding any conclusions as to how stress might differentially impact prepubescent F344 and LEW subjects. Interestingly, in the one direct assessment of the effects of different stressors (isolation housing, restraint stress) on the acquisition of ethanolinduced taste avoidance in outbred prepubertal and adult rats stress had no significant effect on ethanol-induced taste avoidance in either age group (Anderson et al., 2010). It is possible that differences in taste, learning and memory processing mediate the differences between the F344 and LEW strains. These are unlikely, however, given that the reported differences between the two strains are highly dependent upon the specific drug examined. For example, although F344 rats display greater morphine-induced taste avoidance than LEW rats, the difference is reversed for cocaine (i.e., LEW > F344; Davis & Riley, 2007; Glowa et al., 1994). For other compounds such as LiCl (Foynes & Riley, 2004), there are no strain differences. The most parsimonious Prepubertal Strain Differences in Morphine CTA 7 explanation for the present results is that morphine is differentially aversive in these strains. What accounts for this differential sensitivity, however, remains unknown (for a discussion of the nature of avoidance learning, see Verendeev and Riley, 2012), although when c-fos activity in the brainstem is examined in the two strains, cellular activity in avoidance-related areas (Grabus, Glowa, & Riley, 2004) are differentially activated following morphine and in ways that parallel the ability of morphine to induce aversions in the two strains, that is, morphine which induces greater avoidance in the F344 rat induces significantly greater activity in these areas than that seen in the LEW strain. Such assessments are limited to a few drugs and in adults, so it is again unclear if these results generalize to other ages. The present data are suggestive of highly heritable behavioral differences between the two strains; however, they do not argue that such differences cannot be impacted by a host of environmental challenges. In fact, a number of behavioral differences between the strains are impacted by stress (Grakalic et al., 2006; Siviy et al., 2003), diurnal cycle (Gomez-Serrano et al., 2009), and maternal rearing (Gomez-Serrano et al., 2001, 2002; Gomez-Serrano & Riley, 2006; Riley, 2011; Roma, Davis, & Riley, 2007; Siviy et al., 2003). Further, it is unknown to what degree prenatal experience impacts the behavioral differences reported here. What is clear is that the differences in morphineinduced taste avoidance learning in these two strains are evident early in life and are developmentally stable. Further assessments are needed to determine the point at which these behavioral differences become evident and the basis for this stability. NOTES This work was supported in part by grants from the Mellon Foundation to ALR and ZEH and the American University Artists and Scholars Fellowship to APM. REFERENCES Allam, A. A. (2012). The development of sensorimotor reflexes in albino mice; albino rats and black-hooded rats. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 30 (7), 545–553. Ambrosio, E., Goldberg, S., & Elmer, G. (1995). Behavior genetic investigation of the relationship between spontaneous locomotor activity and the acquisition of morphine self-administration behavior. Behavioural Pharmacology, 6 (3), 229–237. 8 Hurwitz et al. Anderson, M. J., Hinderliter, C. F., & Misanin, J. R. (2006). The effects of chronic water deprivation on metabolic rate and long-trace taste-aversion conditioning in rats. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 85(3), 199–205. Anderson, R. I., Agoglia, A. E., Morales, M., Varlinskaya, E. I., & Spear, L. P. (2012). Stress, kappa manipulations, and aversive effects of ethanol in adolescent and adult male rats. Neuroscience, 249, 214–222. Anderson, R. I., Varlinskaya, E. I., & Spear, L. P. (2010). Ethanol-induced conditioned taste aversion in male Sprague Dawley rats: Impact of age and stress. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 34(12), 2106– 2115. Beitner-Johnson, D., Guitart, X., & Nestler, E. J. (1991). Dopaminergic brain reward regions of Lewis and Fischer rats display different levels of tyrosine hydroxylase and other morphine-and cocaine-regulated phosphoproteins. Brain Research, 561(1), 147–150. Bourne, M. J., Calton, J. L., Gustavson, K. K., & Schachtman, T. R. (1992). Effects of acute swim stress on LiClinduced conditioned taste aversions. Physiology & Behavior, 51(6), 1227–1234. Bowers, W., Amit, Z., & Gringras, M. (1996). Timedependent exacerbation of amphetamine-induced taste aversions following exposure to footshock. Psychopharmacology, 125(1), 43–49. Cobuzzi, J. L., Siletti, K. A., Hurwitz, Z. E., Wetzell, B., Baumann, M. H., & Riley, A. L. (2013). Age differences in () 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA)induced conditioned taste aversions and monoaminergic levels. Developmental Psychobiology, DOI: 10.1002/ dev.21132 Davis, C. M., Cobuzzi, J. L., & Riley, A. L. (2012). Assessment of the aversive effects of peripheral mu opioid receptor agonism in Fischer 344 and Lewis rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 101(2), 181–186. Davis, C. M., & Riley, A. L. (2007). The effects of cocaine preexposure on cocaine-induced taste aversion learning in Fischer and Lewis rat strains. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 87(1), 198–202. Davis, C. M., Roma, P. G., Dominguez, J. M., & Riley, A. L. (2007). Morphine-induced place conditioning in Fischer and Lewis rats: Acquisition and dose-response in a fully biased procedure. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 86(3), 516–523. Dhabhar, F. S., McEwen, B. S., & Spencer, R. L. (1993). Stress response, adrenal steroid receptor levels and corticosteroid-binding globulin levels—A comparison between Sprague-Dawley, Fischer 344 and Lewis rats. Brain Research, 616(1), 89–98. Dhabhar, F. S., Miller, A. H., McEwen, B. S., & Spencer, R. L. (1995). Effects of stress on immune cell distribution. Dynamics and hormonal mechanisms. The Journal of Immunology, 154(10), 5511–5527. Doremus-Fitzwater, T. L., Varlinskaya, E. I., & Spear, L. P. (2010). Motivational systems in adolescence: Possible implications for age differences in substance abuse and Developmental Psychobiology other risk-taking behaviors. Brain and Cognition, 72(1), 114–123. Fairless, A. H., Katz, J. M., Vijayvargiya, N., Dow, H. C., Kreibich, A. S., Berrettini, W. H., … Brodkin, E. S. (2012). Development of home cage social behaviors in BALB/cJ vs. C57BL/6J mice. Behavioural Brain Research, 237, 338–347. Farid, M., Martinez, Z. A., Geyer, M. A., & Swerdlow, N. R. (2000). Regulation of sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex by serotonin 2A receptors: Ontogeny and strain differences. Neuropsychopharmacology, 23(6), 623–632. Foynes, M., & Riley, A. (2004). Lithium-chloride-induced conditioned taste aversions in the Lewis and Fischer 344 rat strains. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 79, 303–308. Garcı́a-Lecumberri, C., Torres, I., Martı́n, S., Crespo, J. A., Miguéns, M., Nicanor, C., … Ambrosio, E. (2011). Strain differences in the dose–response relationship for morphine self-administration and impulsive choice between Lewis and Fischer 344 rats. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 25 (6), 783–791. Glowa, J. R., Shaw, A. E., & Riley, A. L. (1994). Cocaineinduced conditioned taste aversions: Comparisons between effects in LEW/N and F344/N rat strains. Psychopharmacology, 114(2), 229–232. Gomez-Serrano, M., & Riley, A. L. (2006). The effects of cross-fostering on morphine-induced conditioned taste aversions in Fischer and Lewis rats (in preparation). Gomez-Serrano, M., Tonelli, L., Listwak, S., Sternberg, E., & Riley, A. L. (2001). Effects of cross fostering on openfield behavior, acoustic startle, lipopolysaccharide-induced corticosterone release, and body weight in Lewis and Fischer rats. Behavior Genetics, 31(5), 427–436. Gomez-Serrano, M. A., Kearns, D. N., & Riley, A. L. (2009). The effects of light cycle phase on morphine-induced conditioned taste aversions in the Lewis, Fischer and Sprague-Dawley rat strains. Behavioural Brain Research, 196(1), 116–122. Gomez-Serrano, M. A., Sternberg, E. M., & Riley, A. L. (2002). Maternal behavior in F344/N and LEW/N rats: Effects on carrageenan-induced inflammatory reactivity and body weight. Physiology & Behavior, 75(4), 493–505. Gosnell, B. A., & Krahn, D. D. (1993). Morphine-induced feeding: A comparison of the Lewis and Fischer 344 inbred rat strains. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 44(4), 919–924. Grabus, S. D., Glowa, J. R., & Riley, A. L. (2004). Morphineand cocaine-induced c-Fos levels in Lewis and Fischer rat strains. Brain Research, 998(1), 20–28. Grakalic, I., Schindler, C. W., Baumann, M. H., Rice, K. C., & Riley, A. L. (2006). Effects of stress modulation on morphine-induced conditioned place preferences and plasma corticosterone levels in Fischer, Lewis, and Sprague– Dawley rat strains. Psychopharmacology, 189(3), 277– 286. Guitart, X., Kogan, J. H., Berhow, M., Terwilliger, R. Z., Aghajanian, G. K., & Nestler, E. J. (1993). Lewis and Fischer rat strains display differences in biochemical, Developmental Psychobiology electrophysiological and behavioral parameters: Studies in the nucleus accumbens and locus coeruleus of drug naive and morphine-treated animals. Brain Research, 611(1), 7– 17. Hurwitz, Z. E., Merluzzi, A. P., & Riley, A. L. (2013). Age-dependent differences in morphine-induced taste aversions. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(4), 415– 428. Infurna, R. N., & Spear, L. P. (1979). Developmental changes in amphetamine-induced taste aversions. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 11(1), 31–35. Kosten, T. A., & Ambrosio, E. (2002). HPA axis function and drug addictive behaviors: Insights from studies with Lewis and Fischer 344 inbred rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 27(1), 35–69. Kosten, T. A., Miserendino, M. J., Haile, C. N., DeCaprio, J. L., Jatlow, P. I., & Nestler, E. J. (1997). Acquisition and maintenance of intravenous cocaine self-administration in Lewis and Fischer inbred rat strains. Brain Research, 778 (2), 418–429. Lancellotti, D., Bayer, B. M., Glowa, J. R., Houghtling, R. A., & Riley, A. L. (2001). Morphine-induced conditioned taste aversions in the LEW/N and F344/N rat strains. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 68(3), 603– 610. Martı́n, S., Lyupina, Y., Crespo, J. A., González, B., Garcı́aLecumberri, C., & Ambrosio, E. (2003). Genetic differences in NMDA and D1 receptor levels, and operant responding for food and morphine in Lewis and Fischer 344 rats. Brain Research, 973(2), 205–213. Martı́n, S., Manzanares, J., Corchero, J., Garcı́a-Lecumberri, C., Crespo, J. A., Fuentes, J. A., & Ambrosio, E. (1999). Differential basal proenkephalin gene expression in dorsal striatum and nucleus accumbens, and vulnerability to morphine self-administration in Fischer 344 and Lewis rats. Brain Research, 821(2), 350–355. Misanin, J. R., Anderson, M. J., & Hinderliter, C. F. (2009). Conditioned taste aversion across the life span from prenascence to senescence. In S. Reilly & T. R. Schachtman (Eds.), Conditioned taste aversion: Behavioral and neural processes (pp. 281). New York, New York: Oxford University Press. Misanin, J. R., Kaufhold, S. E., Paul, R. L., Hinderliter, C. F., & Anderson, M. J. (2006). A time contraction effect of acute tail-pinch stress on the associative learning of rats. Behavioural Processes, 71(1), 16–20. Moore, E. M., Forrest, R., & Boehm, S. (2013). Genotype modulates age-related alterations in sensitivity to the aversive effects of ethanol: An eight inbred strain analysis of conditioned taste aversion. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 12(1), 70–77. Moore, E. M., Linsenbardt, D. N., Melón, L. C., & Boehm, S. L. (2011). Ontogenetic differences in adolescent and adult C57BL/6J and DBA/2J mice: Anxiety-like, locomotor, and consummatory behaviors. Developmental Psychobiology, 53(2), 141–156. Paylor, R., Baskall-Baldini, L., Yuva, L., & Wehner, J. M. (1996). Developmental differences in place-learning perfor- Prepubertal Strain Differences in Morphine CTA 9 mance between C57BL/6 and DBA/2 mice parallel the ontogeny of hippocampal protein kinase C. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110(6), 1415–1425. Pescatore, K. A., Glowa, J. R., & Riley, A. L. (2005). Strain differences in the acquisition of nicotine-induced conditioned taste aversion. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 82(4), 751–757. Revusky, S., & Reilly, S. (1989). Attenuation of conditioned taste aversions by external stressors. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 33(1), 219–226. Riley, A. L. (2011). The paradox of drug taking: The role of the aversive effects of drugs. Physiology & Behavior, 103 (1), 69–78. Roma, P. G., Davis, C. M., & Riley, A. L. (2007). Effects of cross-fostering on cocaine-induced conditioned taste aversions in Fischer and Lewis rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 49(2), 172–179. Roma, P. G., Flint, W. W., Higley, J. D., & Riley, A. L. (2006). Assessment of the aversive and rewarding effects of alcohol in Fischer and Lewis rats. Psychopharmacology, 189(2), 187–199. Satinder, K. P. (1981). Ontogeny and interdependence of genetically selected behaviors in rats: Avoidance response and open field. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 95(1), 175–187. Schramm-Sapyta, N. L., Cha, Y. M., Chaudhry, S., Wilson, W. A., Swartzwelder, H. S., & Kuhn, C. M. (2007). Differential anxiogenic, aversive, and locomotor effects of THC in adolescent and adult rats. Psychopharmacology, 191(4), 867–877. Schramm-Sapyta, N. L., DiFeliceantonio, A. G., Foscue, E., Glowacz, S., Haseeb, N., Wang, N., … Kuhn, C. M. (2010). Aversive effects of ethanol in adolescent versus adult rats: Potential causes and implication for future drinking. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 34(12), 2061–2069. Schramm-Sapyta, N. L., Morris, R. W., & Kuhn, C. M. (2006). Adolescent rats are protected from the conditioned aversive properties of cocaine and lithium chloride. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 84(2), 344– 352. Shram, M. J., Funk, D., Li, Z., & Lê, A. D. (2006). Periadolescent and adult rats respond differently in tests measuring the rewarding and aversive effects of nicotine. Psychopharmacology, 186(2), 201–208. Sinaiko, A., & Mirkin, B. L. (1974). Ontogenesis of the renin-angiotensin system in spontaneously hypertensive and normal Wistar rats. Circulation Research, 34(5), 693– 696. Siviy, S. M., Love, N. J., DeCicco, B. M., Giordano, S. B., & Seifert, T. L. (2003). The relative playfulness of juvenile Lewis and Fischer-344 rats. Physiology & Behavior, 80 (2), 385–394. Spear, L. P. (2013). Adolescent neurodevelopment. Journal of Adolescent Health, 52(2), S7–S13. Sternberg, E. M., Glowa, J. R., Smith, M. A., Cologero, A. E., Listwak, S. J., Aksentijevich, S., … Gold, P. W. (1992). Corticotropin releasing hormone related behavioral 10 Hurwitz et al. and neuroendocrine responses to stress in Lewis and Fischer rats. Brain Research, 570(1), 54–60. Tonkiss, J., Shultz, P., & Galler, J. R. (1992). Long-Evans and Sprague-Dawley rats differ in their spatial navigation performance during ontogeny and at maturity. Developmental Psychobiology, 25(8), 567–579. Verendeev, A., & Riley, A. (2012). Conditioned taste aversion and drugs of abuse: History and interpretation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(10), 2193–2205. Vetter-O’Hagen, C., Varlinskaya, E., & Spear, L. (2009). Sex differences in ethanol intake and sensitivity to aversive effects during adolescence and adulthood. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 44(6), 547–544. Developmental Psychobiology Vogl, C., Atchley, W. R., & Xu, S. (1994). The ontogeny of morphological differences in the mandible in two inbred strains of mice. Journal of Craniofacial Genetics and Developmental Biology, 14(2), 97–110. Wilking, J. A., Hesterberg, K. G., Nguyen, V. H., Cyboron, A. P., Hua, A. Y., & Stitzel, J. A. (2012). Comparison of nicotine oral consumption and baseline anxiety measures in adolescent and adult C57BL/6J and C3H/Ibg mice. Behavioural Brain Research, 233(2), 280–287. Wilmouth, C. E., & Spear, L. P. (2004). Adolescent and adult rats’ aversion to flavors previously paired with nicotine. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021(1), 462–464.