AN ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS OF Craig A. Tinus for the degree of Master of Science in Fisheries Science presented on March 2, 1999. Title: Territorial Behavior in Juvenile Steelhead Trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss): How Redside Shiner (Richardsonius balteatus) Influence Intraspecific Interactions. Redacted for Privacy Abstract approved: Gordon H. Reeves Juvenile steelhead are known to associate with shiner groups, though they also compete for food. Steelhead form dominance hierarchies within cohorts and aggressively defend feeding territories against all other fish. This study focused on the differential effect of shiner competition on steelhead of different social standing. Survival of subordinate juvenile steelhead was significantly enhanced by the presence of redside shiner under laboratory conditions. A factorial experiment in 80 L tanks examined the relative effects of 0, 3, and 9 shiner at 15° and 20° C on the growth and survival of 3 juvenile steelhead per tank. No temperature effect was detected and there was no significant difference in steelhead growth though statistical power was low (n=5). The largest steelhead did not die in any treatment and no steelhead died in the presence of 9 shiner. In treatments where no shiner were present mortality in the smallest steelhead was 80% (p-value < 0.01). Aggressive interactions between steelhead allowed pathogens to colonize breaks in the skin of stressed fish resulting in death. In 6800 L recirculating stream channels with natural substrate, 10 steelhead were held either alone or with 20 shiner at 15° C. No steelhead died and their growth was not significantly different between treatments, but, in the absence of shiner fin damage was 16 times greater (p-value < 0.01) in the smallest three steelhead. If a shiner group was present the smallest steelhead appeared to shoal with shiner to avoid attack by dominant steelhead. Territorial Behavior in Juvenile Steelhead Trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss): How Redside Shiner (Richardsonius bahewus) Influence Intraspecific Interactions. by Craig A. Tinus A THESIS submitted to Oregon State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science Completed March 2, 1999 Commencement June 1999 Master of Science thesis of Craig A. Tinus presented on March 2, 1999 APPROVED: Redacted for Privacy Y M or Professor, representing Fisheries Science Redacted for Privacy ead of Department of Fish s and Wildlife Redacted for Privacy Dean of Graduat chool I understand that my thesis will become part of the permanent collection of Oregon State University libraries. My signature below authorizes release of my thesis to any reader upon request. Redacted for Privacy Craig A. Tinus, Author ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I thank the members of my committee: Gordon Reeves, Mark Hixon, Hiram Li, and Antonio Amandi, each of whom contributed significantly to the successful completion of this project. In addition I thank Craig Banner, Richard Holt, Robert Chitwood, Donald Stevens, Paul Murtaugh, Christian Zimmerman, Kitty Grizwold and Carol Seals. I also thank Mark Carr who is, for better or worse, the mentor who got me into this business. Finally, I thank Gay Lynn Timken, and Linda Carroll for helping to keep my head screwed on. TABLE OF CONTENTS Page INTRODUCTION 1 METHODS 6 Laboratory Tank Experiment Laboratory Stream-Channel Experiment RESULTS 8 11 15 Laboratory Tank Experiment 15 Steelhead Survival Steelhead Growth 15 Stream-Channel Experiments Growth Fin Damage 19 19 19 19 DISCUSSION 25 LITERATURE CITED 34 APPENDIX 39 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. Page Location of collection sites for both the juvenile steelhead and redside shiner 7 Set-up of individual 80L tanks used in laboratory experiments 9 Set-up of artificial stream channels 12 Proportional survival of the largest steelhead in each treatment 16 Proportional survival of the intermediate sized steelhead in each treatment 17 Proportional survival of the smallest steelhead in each treatment 18 Proportional change in wet weight of the largest individual steelhead in each treatment 20 Proportional change in wet weight of the intermediate size steelhead 21 Proportional change in wet weight of the smallest individual steelhead 22 Box plots representing steelhead growth in stream-channel experiments 23 Mean score and SE of fin tears in the smallest three steelhead in each of two treatments 24 Model illustrating that dominant juvenile steelhead have a strong negative effect on subordinate steelhead in the absence of other competitors for food 30 Model illustrating how redside shiner modify the strength and direction of the interaction between dominant and subordinate steelhead 30 LIST OF APPENDIX TABLES TABLE Page Data indicating the time to death and the general identification of the most common pathogens cultured on cytophaga agar from those steelhed which died in the laboratory tank experiments 40 B. Steelhead survival data for all laboratory tank trials 41 C. Growth data measured as the proportional change in wet weight of the smallest three steelhead in laboratory stream channel experiments 42 Accumulated fin damage data of the smallest three steelhead in the stream channel experiments 43 A. D. Territorial Behavior in Juvenile Steelhead Trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss): How Redside Shiner (Richardsonius balteatus) Affect Intraspecific Interactions INTRODUCTION Aggressive interactions between individuals may be considered a competitive interaction (Krebs and Davies, 1993). In general, competition occurs when a number of organisms of the same or different species utilize common resources which are in short supply. However, competition models are not always dependent on resource supply. If the lack of a certain resource prevents an increase in size or number of a given organism it is considered to be limiting. When one group or individual removes a common resource from the available supply it is termed exploitation competition. For competition to exist in this case the resource must be in short supply. When one group or individual harms another in the process of attaining resources it is termed interference competition. When interference competition is modeled there is no assumption that resources are limiting (Krebs, 1994). In this paper the important distinction is that shiner do not physically harm competitors when feeding (exploitation competition). Steelhead (Oncorhynchus mykiss) can and do cause physical harm when defending territories and competing for food (interference competition) Abbott and Dill, 1989. Because exploitation competition is difficult to observe directly in nature, the effects of competitive interactions are often inferred from indirect evidence such as a change in distribution of one group of fish when another group is present, or from direct observation of interference competition such as territorial exclusion. The real effects of competition are difficult to determine because, presumably, the patterns 2 observed in nature over short time scales are the result of competitive interactions which have achieved stability (Connell 1980) Competition in freshwater stream fish may result in habitat displacement of a poor competitor and has thus been shown to influence the structure of some fish assemblages (Nilsson and Northcote 1981, Magnan and FitzGerald 1982). To an individual fish competition may come in two forms: interspecifc, meaning competition from other species, and intraspecific, competition from members of the same species. Intraspecific competition can be further divided into competition from different age groups, e.g. older, larger individuals from an earlier cohort, or from individuals from the same cohort, which may or may not be siblings. Competition within a cohort may be especially intense when physically similar individuals require similar limited resources (Keast, 1977). It is often assumed that competition significantly influences the distribution and density of fish such as juvenile salmonids (Crisp, 1993). Many researchers have examined interspecific competition for food and habitat by comparing growth and survival in single and mixed species groups (see Matthews 1998 for a review). Few studies have examined simultaneously the effects of intra- and interspecific competition (Fausch 1988), or have included environmental factors. Stream dwelling juvenile steelhead provide a good subject for such a study. Juvenile steelhead trout are aggressively territorial (Hutchison and Iwata 1997, Abbott and Dill 1985). As density of juvenile steelhead increases, subordinates in the dominance hierarchy are increasingly unable to defend feeding territories (Fausch and White 1986, McFadden 1969, Chapman 1962). The fate of these subordinate individuals is poorly understood. Several researchers examining juvenile salmonids have determined that subordinates unable to establish feeding territories are more likely to die from starvation, disease, or predation than territory holders (Crisp 1993, Li and Brocksen 1977). Alternatively, Metcalfe (1988) suggests some subordinate individuals have higher than expected survival because they do not expend energy in territorial defense. Individuals may emigrate or modify behavior to improve their ability to attain resources. Fish from many taxa have been shown to reduce the negative effects of competition by modifying behavioral responses (Ryer and 011a 1991, Endler 1988, Robertson et al. 1976). Ayu (Plecoglossus attivelis), a stream dwelling salmonid-like osmerid, switch from territorial defense to shoaling depending on several environmental conditions including food abundance (Kawanabe 1969). Typically, within-group competition (single species) has been thought of in terms of its negative effects on subordinate individuals. There are also well documented advantages to individuals living in groups and they may be equated to advantages enjoyed by individuals in mixed species groups (Shaw 1978, Pitcher 1986). Potential benefits to individuals in mixed species groups include enhanced feeding success and predator avoidance (Pitcher 1986). Matthews (1998) points out that (1) in streams, fish are not evenly distributed in the available area and (2) mixed-species groups are common. A patchy distribution pattern suggests a more complex array of costs and benefits to behavioral interactions than those suggested by simple competition models. Interactions between two organisms, whether of the same or 4 different species, have been generally considered in isolation. This approach may yield spurious results by exaggerating the effects of some interactions which would in nature be balanced or mitigated by other interactions, e.g. associating with food finders or omnivory. When three or more organisms are interacting directly the potential exists for a class of interactions termed "indirect effects" (see Menge 1995 for a review of indirect effects). For example, when two species compete for a common food resource and a third species preys preferentially on one of the competitors there is an indirect positive effect on the competitor not preyed upon. The adage "the enemy of my enemy is my friend" applies in such cases. In an examination of field data on birds, ants, and zooplankton, Stone and Roberts (1991) suggest many potentially "competitive" interactions which have been assumed to be negative may in fact produce many positive indirect effects. When they modeled community interaction webs, 20-40% of interspecific interactions needed to be positive for the system to be stable. Their models of community interaction webs are reportedly in good agreement with empirical data. Juvenile steelhead trout are sometimes found in close association with redside shiner (Richardsonius balteatus) shoals (Reeves 1984, Li personal communication, personal observation). Reeves et al. (1987) found that shiner and juvenile steelhead compete directly for food and space but that the outcome of this competition was moderated by water temperature. Steelhead were competitively dominant (displaced shiner from a focal point) at 12-15° C and shiner were dominant at 19-22° C. 5 Steelhead which were associated with shiner shoals were not defending territories while other steelhead in the same habitat were. The implications of this observed distribution pattern are that there may be multiple strategies for gaining food resources and that the presence of a shoaling species might encourage subordinates to switch from territory acquisition to shoaling behavior. The objective of this study was to test the effects of shiner density and temperature on the survival and growth of subordinate juvenile steelhead and to determine the mechanisms if significant differences were found. 6 METHODS Redside shiner and juvenile steelhead were captured by electroshocking in Steamboat Creek in the Umpqua River basin (Fig. 1). Fish were collected in September and October of 1995 and 1996 in stream sections where they were sympatric. Shiner and steelhead were sorted in the field and placed in dark 150-L tubs lined with black plastic bags. Transport water was taken directly from the stream and NaCI was added to produce a 1-3% salt solution. This helped to reduce osmotic stress during transport. Ice placed on top of the bags kept transport water below 15°C. Battery-operated aerators kept the transport water well oxygenated. Transport time was always less than five hours and no fish died during transport. Fish were initially placed in 600-L 0.91-m diameter x 0.91-m deep circular tanks in the Oregon Cooperative Fisheries Research Unit hatchery, Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR. Well water at 12.8° C flowed through each tank at 600-L hour-1 Each tank had six 0.3-m sections of 0.8-m sewer pipe cut into half circles and placed on the tank bottom to provide both overhead cover and visual isolation to reduce stress. Steelhead densities ranged from 25-50 fish per tank depending on fish size. Shiner were held at densities of up to 250 fish per tank. Fish were treated against external parasites with malachite green and against bacterial infection with Spectrogram, a furan-based antibiotic. Both species were initially fed live three week old crickets, about 30 per tank, and frozen krill estimated to equal 2.5% of the biomass in each tank twice daily until all fish were feeding, generally 3-5 days. 7 Figure 1. Location of collection sites for both the juvenile steelhead and redside shiner. The trout and shiner were collected in habitats where they occur together. Steelhead Trout and Redside Shiner Collection Sites Steamboat Creek, Oregon A Collection Sites 12 Kilometers 8 They were then fed only frozen krill (Euphausia pacifica) in the same amount as before twice daily. A photoperiod of 10 hours light and 14 hours dark was maintained with full spectrum lights under timer control. Fish were held under these conditions for at least two weeks before the experimental trials. Laboratory Tank Experiment The experiments were conducted in tanks at the Oregon State Salmon Disease Laboratory in Corvallis, Oregon. The ten 80-L oval shaped tanks were 0.7-m x 0.35-m x 0.35-m deep. A single 25-L min.-' submersible pump created water flow within each tank. One air stone in each tank maintained adequate dissolved oxygen. Clean water was added to each tank at 0.5 L min:', resulting in a complete replacement of tank water every 160 minutes. Desired water temperatures were maintained with immersion heaters. The primary light source was natural light from overhead windows. One 21 x 9 x 6 cm terra cotta brick and one 15 x 15 cm tile together were used to create overhead cover and an eddy. These features were designed to mimic the "pocket water" areas trout defend as feeding territories in natural streams. Two brick and tile structures were in each tank (Fig. 2). Frozen krill equaling 2.5% of the total wet weight of fish was placed midmorning and mid afternoon in a 0.019 m PVC pipe that had its outlet just forward of the water pump in each tank. Uneaten food was removed from each tank by a filter in the intake of the water pump 9 The six experimental treatments of this 3 x 2 factorial design were 3 juvenile steelhead alone, 3 steelhead with 3 shiner, 3 steelhead with 9 shiner at either 15 or 20°C. A ratio of one juvenile steelhead to three shiner was observed in the stream at the point and time of capture. The experimental density ratios of 1:1 and 1:3 were chosen to test for a shiner density effect. Figure 2. Set-up of individual 80L tanks used in laboratory experiments. A submersible pump (A) maintained a clockwise flow. A slurry of food mixed with water was introduced through a pipe (B) just in front of the pump. Two structures made of two bricks and one tile (C) provided cover with a good opportunity to capture food particles and a submersible heater (D) maintained the temperature in 20° trials. 10 Each tank was randomly assigned a treatment allocation with the constraint that a single tank did not have the same treatment twice in a row. The trial period was 21 days and there were 5 replicates. The order in which the tanks had fish placed in them was randomized with the constraint that steelhead placed in a tank were of similar size. Fish were anesthetized in buffered methanotricansulfate (MS222) before being weighed and measured. Fish were measured to the nearest 1.0 mm, weighed to the nearest 0.1 g, and inspected for signs of disease. Fish were assigned an identification number so individuals could be tracked throughout the experiment. Individuals were identified by a unique combination of body marks, size, weight, and tank allocation. Shiner ranged from 65-100 mm fork length and 3-20 g wet weight. Steelhead ranged from 90-153 mm and 8.6-41.4 g; ranges similar to those found in the field. Larger steelhead that began to smolt were not used in the analysis because smolts behave differently from juveniles. Fish that died or became moribund during the course of the experiment were removed and identified. Dead and dying fish were not weighed to estimate growth because of tissue wasting from disease, physical damage, and osmotic imbalance. These fish were identified by length, body marks, and reference to daily notes that described the health of each fish. All fish were removed within 8 hours of death and necropsied. Suspected pathogens were isolated, identified, and roughly quantified. When a known pathogen was found on a fish in nearly pure culture, it was considered the cause of death. 11 At the end of the 21-day test period, all surviving fish were removed from the tanks and killed in MS222. Each fish was weighed, measured, identified, and necropsied. Growth, general condition, and pathogen load were recorded for each fish. Mortality and growth were compared in steelhead for all treatments using a Fisher's Exact Test. Because there was no variation in the largest size category a logistic regression could not be used to analyze the binary data. A Fisher's Exact Test is a two by two matrix which compares the actual distribution of data to a random distribution. Laboratory Stream-Channel Experiment A second experiment was conducted in two laboratory stream channels at the USDA Forest Service, Pacific Northwest Research Station, Corvallis, OR. Each oval channel was 4.3 m x 4.9 m on the sides, 0.8 m wide, and 0 61 m high. Channels were divided into two equal size sections creating four experimental cells by attaching 4 mm Vexar mesh in a Plexiglas frame to the walls of the channel (Fig. 3). The paddle wheels, pump intake, and exhaust were similarly isolated in another section of each channel (see Reeves et al. 1983). Inner walls were clear Plexiglas. Opaque curtains surround the channels on both sides, limiting outside light and disturbance, and allowing an observer to be unnoticed by the fish. Substrate was pea gravel (6-12 mm) in each pool and gravel (20-50 mm) mixed with cobble (100-500 mm) in the riffles. Each section had identical structure. Two shallow areas 35 cm deep, and two areas, 50 cm deep were in each section. Channels were filled with deionized water and allowed to circulate for 24-48 hours before fish were added. Water was continuously pumped through sand filters. 12 Figure 3. Set-up of artificial stream channels. Shown is one of two identical stream channels. Paddle wheel unit I iffle Downstream screen and trap .W....--­ Riffle --"'" Cooling - heating Flow 4.9 m Pool 6.4-mm plexiglass side Pool Downstream screen and trap Riffle Riffle /I 6.4-mm plywood side 4.3 m 13 A plexiglas paddle wheel in each channel maintained water flow comparable with the areas in the stream where the fish were collected. Ultraviolet sterilizers provided additional breakdown of metabolic waste as well as limiting pathogens in the water column. Water temperature was maintained at 15° C. The photoperiod was maintained as a 15-hour daylight period, including a two hour brightening and two hour dimming phase that simulated a twilight period. Krill was delivered through a 25-mm PVC pipe that zigzagged from a head box down the length of each channel section. The pipe was perforated variously so as to deliver an equal portion of food to each riffle-pool section (Reeves et al. 1983). Daily rations (5% of the total wet biomass in each section) were fed in two daily feeding periods, one mid morning, the other mid afternoon. The krill was frozen in blocks of ice so particles would be delivered to the system slowly as the ice melted. Each feeding period lasted about 1 hour to reduce scramble competition which may have been an important experimental artifact. Fish were taken from holding tanks and randomly assigned to a channel section. The four sections were randomly assigned a treatment with the constraint that no section received the same treatment twice in a row. In this experiment only the presence of shiner with steelhead varied. This arrangement provided two replicates per trial. All fish were weighed and measured as in the first experiment, and their unique marks identified individual fish. Any fin damage was noted for each fish. Ten steelhead and twenty shiner were added to two sections. Ten steelhead alone were 14 added to the remaining two sections. The experiment was repeated to provide additional replication. Shiner were introduced first and 12 hours later the steelhead were introduced. Trials ran for ten days and no fish died during the trials. After ten days, all fish were removed from the channels by bubbling CO2 through the water and killed in MS222. All fish were weighed, measured and identified. Fin tears, which indicate aggressive interaction between steelhead, were assessed (Moutou, McCarthy, and Houlhan 1998). Minor fin tears tended to be short while larger tears tended to run the length of the fin. Fin tears less than 30% of the fin length were given a score of 0.5. Tears greater than 30% of the total fin length were scored as 1.0. The choice of a 30% cutoff was arbitrary. The total fin damage score for each fish was then recorded. Although ten steelhead were in each section, several of the largest individuals began to smolt and could not be used in the analysis. To adjust for this problem, a decision was made to focus the analysis on the three smallest steelhead in each section, which was reasonable because the results from the first experiment indicated the smallest individuals suffered the most from aggressive interactions with other steelhead. Differences in growth and fin damage between the three smallest steelhead with shiner were compared with those held without shiner by ANOVA. 15 RESULTS Laboratory Tank Experiment Steelhead Survival Water temperature did not significantly affect steelhead survival or growth within or between size categories (Fishers Exact Test, p-value >> 0.05). However, there was a trend in the average time to death (ANOVA p-value = 0.13). At 20° C the mean time to death was 10.0 days (SE +/- 2.45, n=9). At 15° mean time to death was 13.6 days (+/-1.87, n=10). These data are shown in Appendix A. There was a significant effect of shiner presence (Fishers Exact Test p-value <0.01). The largest trout, considered dominant in a social hierarchy, had 100% survival regardless of the number of shiner present (Fig. 4). The second largest trout was affected by the presence of shiner (Fig. 5). When shiner were absent at 15° C, only 20% (SE +/- 0.2) of intermediate sized trout survived, at 20° C, 60% (+/- 0.24). When three shiner were present at 15° C (shiner and trout at equal density) 40% (+/- 0.24) of intermediate sized trout survived, at 20° C, 80% (+/- 0.2) survived. When nine shiner were present survival of intermediate sized trout was 100% at both temperatures. The smallest trout were most affected by the presence of shiner (Fig. 6). When shiner were absent at 15° C and 20° C only 20% (+/- 0.2) of the smallest trout survived. In treatments where three shiner were present survival of the smallest trout was 100% at 15° and 80% (+/- 0.2) at 20° C. When nine shiner were present survival of the smallest trout was 100%. These data are shown in Appendix B. 16 Figure 4. Proportional survival of the largest individual steelhead in each treatment. The x-axis represents the number of shiner present in each tank. The y-axis represents steelhead survival with standard error of the mean. The dark bars are treatments at 15°C and the light bars are treatments at 20°C. 1.2 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 0 15° Li..;zzzl 20° 3 Number of Shiner Present 9 17 Figure 5. Proportional survival of the intermediate sized steelhead in each treatment. 1.2 co cl) co 10 io 0.8 E teu­ c 0 E 0.6 0.4 Tu. 02 0 0. 2 0.0 3 15° Lad 20° Number of Shiner Present 9 18 Figure 6. Proportional survival of the smallest steelhead in each treatment. 1.2 1:3 cv 1.0 a) 0.8 To "5 0.6 ._ z 0.4 To t 0 o 0.2 c,2 0.0 9 EM 15° lam:3 20° Number of Shiner Present 19 Steelhead Growth Growth rates between different size categories were not significantly different (ANOVA p-value = 0.34) (figs. 7,8,9). There was no significant within treatment temperature effect (p-value > 0.05). Water temperature effects may be compared by examining the adjacent light and dark bars in (Figs. 7,8,9). Stream Channel Experiments Growth There were no significant differences in growth of the smallest steelhead between treatments (p-value = 0 53, n=4) (fig. 10). The mean proportional change in wet weight of the smallest three steelhead in the absence of shiner was 3.6% (SE +/­ 0.036, n=4). In the presence of shiner the smallest three steelhead increased by 0.67% (SE +/- 0.04, n=4) The data are shown in Appendix C. Fin Damage There was no mortality of steelhead trout during the experiments. However, the smallest three steelhead accumulated sixteen times more fin damage from attacks by larger steelhead in treatments with no shiner present (p-value < 0.01). In treatments where shiner were present the smallest three steelhead accumulated a mean score of 0.17 (SE 0.11) fin tears in the caudal and dorsal fins. When shiner where not present the smallest three steelhead accumulated a mean score of 2.67 (0.57) tears in the caudal and dorsal fins (Fig 11). The data are shown in Appendix D. 20 Figure 7. Proportional change in wet weight of the largest individual steelhead in each treatment. The x-axis represents the number of shiner present in each tank. The y-axis is the mean proportional change in wet weight with standard error of the mean. The dark bars are treatments at 15° C and the light bars are those treatments at 20° C. .c3) 0.25 -cs cu -c 0.20 a) 0.15 cn cu _c 0 0.10 0 0 0 0 a_ 0.05 (1) 0.00 0 MI 15° 7779 20° 3 Number of Shiner Present 9 21 Figure 8. Proportional change in wet weight of the intermediate size steelhead. 0 15° 20° 3 Number of Shiner Present 9 22 Figure 9. Proportional change in wet weight of the smallest individual steelhead. 0.20 0.15 0.10 0.05 0.00 -0.05 0 15° 20° 3 Number of Shiner Present 9 23 Figure 10.. Box plots representing steelhead growth in stream channel experiments. The boxes indicate the Intraquartile Range (IQR) with the median observation represented by a line. The whiskers are those observations which fall within 1.5 x IQR, and the dots are observations (outliers) which fall beyond 1.5 x IQR. The median response is very close to zero both in treatments with a group of shiner present and in those treatments where shiner were not present. The mean proportional increase in weight in treatments without shiner was 0.036 (SE 0.036), and the mean increase in steelhead weight in those treatments with shiner present was 0.017 (SE 0.041) n=4. 0.3 0.2 0.1 0.0 0.1 -0.2 0.3 -0.4 No Shiner Present Shiner Present 24 Figure 11. Mean score and SE of fin tears in the smallest three steelhead in each of two treatments. Where no shiner were present the mean number of fin tears was 2.67, SE = 0.57. Where a shiner group was present only 0.17, SE = 0.11, fin tears accumulated, n=4. 4 3 2 0 no shiner present shiner present 25 DISCUSSION The presence of redside shiner had a mediating effect on interactions among steelhead. In the tank experiments, where steelhead densities were 0.0375 fish/liter, the smallest steelhead died when shiner were not present. Steelhead densities in the stream channels were 0.0024 fish/liter and the length of the trials where half as long as those in the tanks. In the stream channel experiments no steelhead died, but the smallest steelhead suffered extensive fin damage. The fin damage indicated these steelhead received injury in aggressive interactions and would likely have died from the effects of stress and disease if the experiments ran longer (Schreck 1992, Holt et al. 1975). In these experiments dominance was inferred from fish size. I considered the smallest steelhead to be subordinate in a dominance hierarchy though some workers have suggested that other factors such as "fierceness" and not strictly size determine social standing (Keeley and McPhail 1998, Titus and Mosegaard 1991, Huntingford et al. 1990). Even if size were not the correct indicator of social position, the experiments showed that more steelhead were able to live within a given area in the presence of an equal or greater number of shiner than were able to live without shiner. The fates of subordinate individuals in territorial fishes such as salmonids are varied and poorly understood. Several researchers have suggested that subordinates unable to establish feeding territories die from starvation, disease, or predation (LeCren 1965, Crisp 1993, Frazer 1968, Li and Brocksen 1977). However, Metcalfe (1986) in a reexamination of Li and Brocksen (1977) found that subordinates may have higher 26 than expected survival suggesting the consequences of different behavioral strategies may be more complex than previously thought. Fish mortality is due both to biotic factors (e.g. predation, and competition for food), and to abiotic factors (e.g. temperature stress and debris torrents). Biotic factors tend to be "deterministic" meaning that, for example, good competitors for food are more likely to survive than poor competitors if food is scarce. In contrast abiotic factors are stochastic and an individual's survival is governed by a probability distribution. Which kind of factor is most important to fish survival at a particular period is not well understood. Elliott (1994) states that in brown trout (Salmo trutta) only territory holders survive the "critical period" and that downstream migrants have poor survival. Elliott (1994) operationally defines the "critical period" as a developmental stage when density-dependent forces, such as competition, are acting to regulate the population. In summer, though temperature stress can be significant, the frequency of physical disturbance is low in streams in the Pacific Northwest. In winter it is much higher. Power (1992) found the importance of biological factors are increasingly important with decreasing frequency of environmental stress. Titus (1991) found little evidence for strong density-dependent mortality in brown trout and suggests abiotic factors are most important. If "critical periods" as defined above are in fact important for juvenile steelhead they would occur during the summer months in their first and second years of life before going to sea. If, however, abiotic factors are more important to juvenile steelhead survival, much of the early mortality would be expected during the first winter when the frequency of disturbance is high. Food 27 resources are low in winter, but because water temperatures are low metabolic requirements are greatly reduced in fish. Agonistic behavior between juvenile steelhead may lead to increased mortality of subordinate individuals (Abbott and Dill 1985). If this assumption is true the following options remain to a subordinate individual trying to secure food resources. A subordinate juvenile steelhead must take shelter and starve, leave an area which is known to contain food and other resources but is occupied by other steelhead, continue to challenge a more dominant individual for food, or modify its behavior such that it can remain in the area and not suffer from constant attacks. One way of doing this is to adopt the feeding behavior of another group of fish which successfully feed in a desirable area. Redside shiner exhibit a large repertoire of behaviors which allow them to feed successfully in habitats occupied by steelhead as well other species under a wide range of environmental conditions (Reeves, 1984). One example is that shiner have been shown to increase group density as a response to stress (Lindsey and Northcote 1963) Ryer and 011a (1991) show that subordinate chum salmon (Oncorhynchus keta) form groups to defeat a despotic dominant. Similarly, shiner will feed in tighter groups as a way of competing with territory holding juvenile steelhead. In this study the shiner spent most of their time in slower water, but during periods when food was abundant shiner moved into focal points bringing them into direct competition with steelhead which had already established territories. When only steelhead were present subordinates continually tried to move into these focal points suffering physical damage from attack. When shiner were present the subordinate 28 steelhead moved into the focal point within the shiner group. There was a "swarm effect" when a dominant's territory was invaded simultaneously by many intruders. When shiner were present the subordinate steelhead were seen repeatedly taking refuge in a group of shiner when chased by a larger steelhead. The dominant steelhead broke off the chase when the smaller steelhead were close to a shiner group, possibly because it could not track its target within a group of similar sized fish. When shiner were absent the subordinates were driven far from a focal point area, often after being bitten repeatedly. The "decision" to associate closely with a group of shiner represents a balance of costs and benefits. The cost to subordinate steelhead from association with a group of shiner is additional competition for food. The benefit is a reduction in stress and physical damage which would result if subordinates continually challenged dominants. Juvenile steelhead can become very aggressive with other fish, particularly other salmonids (Harvey and Nakamoto 1996, Everest and Chapman 1972). Under certain conditions dominant steelhead can have an important negative effect on subordinate steelhead (figure 12) In this example the negative effect may be seen in a reduction in growth and survival of less dominant steelhead. The effects come in two forms: exploitation competition, the monopolization of resources, and interference competition, the physical damage incurred by the subordinates from attacks by the dominant steelhead. This physical damage allows pathogens to colonize breaks in the skin (Holt et al. 1975). Small steelhead turned darker in color after being repeatedly attacked indicating stress hormones were elevated (Iger et al. 1995). Elevated stress 29 hormones lead to immune system depression (Mau le et al. 1989). Though redside shiner are thought to be significant competitors for food with juvenile salmonids in some systems (Crossman and Larkin 1959), shiner can have positive effects on subordinate juvenile steelhead. Where a shiner group and dominant steelhead compete equally, subordinate steelhead, by their association with a shiner group can successfully compete and remain in areas with high food abundance. A subordinate steelhead may function as a member of a shiner group and gain the benefits of the shiner's behavioral response to a dominant's defense of a territory. When a group of shiner approach an area which is being defended by dominant steelhead, the subordinate steelhead enjoy a net benefit even though the shiner are using food resources because they can remain in a profitable feeding territory and do not suffer physical abuse. Figure 13 illustrates how shiner can have a strong negative effect on dominant steelhead by defeating their ability to monopolize resources. There is a much weaker negative effect on shiner from the energetic cost of competing with dominant steelhead. As shown in figure 12, dominant steelhead have strong negative effect on subordinant steelhead from both exploitation and interference competition. There is a much weaker negative effect on dominant steelhead from the cost of territorial defense. When shiner are present there is a strong positive indirect effect from shiner to subordinant steelhead. In addition there are weak negative effects direct effects. These effects are the cost of exploitation competition when steelhead and shiner shoal together. 30 Figure 12. Model illustrating that dominant juvenile steelhead have a strong negative effect on subordinate steelhead in the absence of other competitors for food. The relative strength of the interaction effect is indicated by the width of the line in the model. Dominant Steelhead ( ) ( ) Subordinate Steelhead Figure 13. Model illustrating how redside shiner modify the strength and direction of the interaction between dominant and subordinate steelhead (-) Dominant Steelhead (-) Shiner ( -) Subordinate steelhead (-) 31 Water temperature was expected to alter both shiner and steelhead behavior. The tank experiments did not show a temperature effect on either growth or survival, though 15° C and 20°C are within the range typically encountered by these fish in nature and may not greatly affect behavioral responses. In this experiment survival was not a sensitive enough measure to detect a temperature effect. To detect a temperature effect would probably require a more sensitive measure such as growth. Growth data were recorded but sample sizes were very small and differences between treatments may have been obscured by high within treatment variation. To improve precision growth should be measured in dry weight. In addition, food should be delivered more evenly and for a longer period of time. This helps to avoid a scramble competition which may cause fish behavior different from that seen in nature (Chapman 1962). Scramble competition should tend to break down territory boundaries and periodic delivery of food should encourage boundary maintenance. That territories were maintained by steelhead in these experiments encourages the assumption that steelhead behavior in these experiments was similar to that seen in nature. In streams drifting food particles are most abundant during crepuscular periods, though food is likely to be present throughout the day (Waters 1968, Rader 1995) Food abundance has been shown to reduce territory size in steelhead (Slaney and Northcote 1974, Fausch 1984), but how food distribution in streams effects dominance hierarchies in juvenile steelhead is poorly understood. One way of providing a natural food distribution would be to conduct the trials in a natural stream. However, when fish are confined in a small area in a natural setting the experimental artifacts become the same as in the laboratory. To 32 address this problem the scale must increase. As the scale of the experiment increases, tracking individual fish becomes more difficult. The tank experiments were used to efficiently quantify which of multiple factors were important in influencing steelhead growth and survival. This was a necessary first step in determining which factors were important. The laboratory stream channels allowed direct observation of fish which provided insight into the mechanisms influencing fish behavior. Natural substrate was used allowing for greater visual isolation and fish densities were similar to those seen in nature. Laboratory experiments are generally "hard-edged" where even at densities similar to those in nature fish do not have the option to move back and forth from marginal habitats. If individuals either cannot or do not leave a distinct area they are referred to as "hard­ edged" habitats. These laboratory experiments are "hard- edged ". If an individual can and will move in and out of a habitat it requires, the habitat is "soft- edged ". The geometry of hard-edged and soft-edged territories may have important consequences for how and how many animals persist in a given area (Stamps et al. 1987). A territory surrounded by other territories is qualitatively different than one which borders marginal, undefended habitats. One reason is the rate of territory invasion by conspecifics is likely to be different, another is that soft edge habitats may have different predation pressures. There is evidence for both kinds of habitats in nature but stream dwelling fish like juvenile steelhead probably live in soft-edged territories with convoluted geometry. Even though steelhead in nature have more options than in a laboratory environment, the pattern of steelhead response to shiner in these 33 experiments was so strong that similar behavioral patterns are likely to exist in nature. An experimental manipulation of shiner and steelhead density in natural streams on a large scale would be an important next step to further study this aspect of fish behavior. The long term effects of subordinate steelhead associating with shiner are unknown. Whether or not subordinate steelhead which change their behavior to better compete with dominants have greater fitness or even survive to smolt is unknown. However, juvenile steelhead with lower growth rates than larger individuals within the same cohort may smolt at different times and diverge in their later life history strategies. Pre-smolt growth poorly predicts ocean growth rates in steelhead (Johnson et al. 1997). There is some evidence that higher presmolt growth rates lead to later onset of smolting (Ward et al. 1989). It is possible that if a subordinate steelhead survives through its first winter it will be a dominant the following summer and smolt earlier. To better understand the long-term effects of juvenile behavior it will be necessary to track individual fish through all its life history stages. Since survival of steelhead from emergence to spawning age adults is probably less than one percent (Unpublished data on Fish Creek OR, USFS-PNW ALI Corvallis) the resources required to track a sufficient number of fish would be prodigious. Additionally, wild steelhead stocks in the lower 48 states have declined steadily since the turn of the century (Cone and Ridlington 1996) so future experiments may require the use of hatchery steelhead for experimentation. 34 LITERATURE CITED Abbott, J. C., and L. M. Dill, 1985. Patterns of aggressive attack in juvenile steelhead trout ( Salmo gairdneri). Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 42(11): 1702-1706. Abbott, J. C., and L. M. Dill, 1988. The relative growth of dominant and subordinate juvenile steelhead trout ( Salmo gairdneri) fed equal rations. Behaviour. 108(1-2): 104-113. Chapman, D. W., 1962. Aggressive behavior in juvenile coho salmon as a cause of emigration. J. Fish. Res. Board Can. 19: 1047-1080. Cone, J. and S. Ridlington, 1996. The northwest salmon crisis: a documentary history, p.11. Oregon State University Press, Corvallis. Connell, J. H., 1980. Diversity and the coevolution of competitors, or the ghost of competition past. Oikos 35: 131-138. Crisp, D. T., 1993. Population densities of juvenile trout (Salmo Plata) in five upland streams and their effects upon growth, survival and dispersal. J. Appl. Ecol. 30: 759-771. Crossman, E. J., and P. A. Larkin. 1959. Yearling liberations and change of food as effecting rainbow trout yield in Paul Lake, British Columbia. Trans. Am. Fish. Soc. 88(1959): 36 -44. Elliott, J. M. 1994. Quantitative ecology and the brown trout, p. 201. Oxford series in ecology and evolution, Oxford University Press, Oxford. Endler, J. A. 1988. Sexual selection and predation risk in guppies. Nature 332: 593­ 594. Everest, F. H., and D. W. Chapman. 1972. Habitat selection and spatial interaction by juvenile chinook salmon and steelhead trout in two Idaho streams. J. Fish. Res. Board Can. 29:91-100. Fausch, K. D. 1984. Profitable stream positions for salmonids: relating specific growth rate to net energy gain. Can. J. Zool. 62: 441-445. Fausch, K. D. 1988. Tests of competition between native and introduced salmonids in streams: what have we learned? Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci., 45: 2238-2246. 35 Fausch, K. D., and R. J. White. 1986. Competition among juveniles of coho salmon, brook trout, and brown trout in a laboratory stream, and implications for Great Lakes tributaries. Trans. Am. Fish. Soc., 115, 363-381. Frazer, F.J. 1969. Population density effects on survival and growth of juvenile coho salmon and steelhead trout in experimental stream-channels, p.253-268. In T.G. Northcote [ed] Symposium on Salmon and Trout in Streams. University of British Columbia Press, Vancouver, B.C. Harvey, B. C., and R. J. Nakamoto. 1996. Habitat-dependent interactions between two size-classes of juvenile steelhead in a small stream. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 54: 27-31. Holt, R. A., J. E. Sanders, J. L. Zinn, J. L. Fryer, and K. S. Pitcher. 1975. Relation of water temperature to Flexibacter columnaris infection in steelhead trout (Salmo gairdneri), Coho (Oncorhynchus kisutch) and Chinook (0. tshawytscha) salmon. J. Fish. Res. Board Can. 32: 1553-1559. Huntingford, F. A., N. B. Metcalfe, J. E. Thorpe, W. D. Graham, and C. E. Adams. 1990. Social dominance and body size in Atlantic salmon parr, Salmo salar L. J. Fish Biol. 36: 877-881. Hutchinson M. J., and M. Iwata. 1997. A comparative analysis of aggression in migratory and non-migratory salmonids. Envir. Biol. Fish., 50: 209-215. Iger, Y., Balm, P. H. M., Jenner, H. A. and Wendelaar Bonga, S. E. 1995. Cortisol induces stress-related changes in the skin of rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). Gen. Comp. Endocr. 97: 188-198 Johnsson, J. I., J. Blackburn, W. C. Clarke and R. E. Withler. 1997. Does presmolt growth rate in steelhead trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) and coho salmon (Oncorynchus kisutch) predict growth rate in sea water? Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 54:430-433. Kawanabe, H. 1969. The significance of social structure in production of the "Ayu" Plecoglossus altivelis, p. 243-252. In T. G. Northcote [ed] Symposium on Salmon and Trout in Streams. University of British Columbia Press, Vancouver, B.C. Keast, A. 1977. Mechanisms expanding niche width and minimizing intraspecific competition in two centrarchid fishes. Evol. Biol., 10: 333-395. Keeley, E. R., and J. D. McPhail. 1998. Food abundance, intruder pressure and body size as determinants of territory size in juvenile steelhead trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). Behaviour 135: 65-82. 36 Krebs, C. J. 1994. Ecology: The experimental analysis of distribution and abundance, Fourth Edition. Harper Collins College Publishers. Krebs, J. R. and N. B. Davies. 1993. An introduction to behavioral ecology, Third Edition. Blackwell Science Ltd. LeCren, E. D. 1965. Some factors regulating the size of populations of freshwater fish. Mitteilungen der Internationalen Vereinigung fur theoretische and angewandte Limnologie 13: 88-115. Li, H. W., and R. W. Brocksen. 1977. Approaches to the analysis of energetic costs of intraspecific competition for space by rainbow trout (Salmo gairdneri). J. Fish. Biol. 11: 329-341. Lindsey, C. C., and T. G. Northcote. 1963. Life history of redside shiners, Richardsonius balteatus, with particular reference to movements in and out of Sixteen Mile Lake streams. J. Fish. Res. Board Can. 20(4): 1001-1030. Magnan, P. and G. J. FitzGerald. 1982. Resource partitioning between brook trout Salvelinus fontinalis Mitchill and creek chub Semotilus atromaculatus Mitchill in selected oligotrophic lakes of southern Quebec. Can. J. Zool., 60: 1612­ 1617. Maule, A. G., Schreck, C. B. and Sharpe, C. 1989. Stress alters immune function and disease resistance in Chinook salmon (Oncorhynchus tshawytscha). J. Endocrin. 120:135-142. Matthews, W. J. 1998. Patters in freshwater fish ecology. Chapman and Hall. McFadden, J. T. 1969. Dynamics and regulation of salmonid populations in streams. pp. 313-329. In T. G. Northcote [ed] Symposium on Salmon and Trout in Streams. University of British Columbia Press, Vancouver, B.C. Menge, B. A. 1995. Indirect effects in marine rocky intertidal interaction webs: patterns and importance. Ecol. Monog. 65(1): 21-74. Metcalfe, N. B. 1986. Intraspecific variation in competitive ability and food intake in salmonids: consequences for energy budgets and growth rates. J. Fish Biol. 28: 525-531. Moutou, K. A., I. D. McCarthy, and D. F. Houlihan. 1998. The effect of ration level and social rank on the development of fin damage in juvenile rainbow trout. J. Fish. Biol. 52: 756-770. 37 Nilsson, N. A., and T. G. Northcote. 1981. Rainbow trout Salmo gairdneri and cutthroat trout S. clarki interactions in coastal British Columbia lakes. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci., 38: 1228-1246. Pitcher, T. J. 1986. Functions of shoaling in teleosts, In The Behavior of Teleost Fishes (ed. J. T. Pitcher). John Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD, pp. 294-337. Power, M. E. 1992. Habitat heterogeneity and the functional significance of fish in river food webs. Ecology 73: 1675-1688. Rader, R. B. 1995. A functional classification of the drift: traits that influence invertebrate availability to salmonids. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 54: 1211-1234. Reeves, G. H., 1984. Interaction and behavior of the redside shiner (Richardsonius balteatus) and the steelhead trout (Salmo gairdneri) in western Oregon: the influence of water temperature. Ph. D. dissertation, Oregon Sate University, 109p. Reeves, G. H., F. H. Everest, and C. E. McLemore. 1983. A recirculating stream aquarium for ecological studies. USDA For. Serv. Res. Note PNW-403. Pacific Northwest Forest and Range Experimental Station, Portland, OR. 8 p. Reeves, G. H., F. H. Everest, and J. D. Hall. 1987. Interactions between the redside shiner (Richardsonius balteatus) and the steelhead trout (Salmo gairdneri) in western Oregon: the effect of water temperature. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 44: 1603-1613. Robertson, D. R., H. P. A. Sweatman, E. A. Fletcher, and M. G. Clelland. 1976. Schooling as a mechanism for circumventing the territoriality of competitors. Ecology 57: 1208-1220. Ryer, C. H., and B. L. 011a. 1991. Agonistic behavior in a schooling fish: form, function and ontogeny. Environ. Biol. Fish. 31: 355-363. Schreck, C. B. 1992. Physiological, behavioral, and performance indicators of stress. American Fisheries Symposium 8: 29-37. Shaw, E. 1978. Schooling fishes. Amer. Sci., 66: 166-175. Slaney, P. A., and T. G. Northcote. 1974. Effects of prey abundance on density and territorial behaviour of young rainbow trout (Salmo gairdneri) in laboratory stream channels. J. Fish. Res. Board Can. 31: 1201-1209. 38 Stamps, J. A., M. Buechner, and V. V. Krishnan. 1987. The effects of habitat geometry on territorial defense costs: intruder pressure in bounded habitats. Amer. Zool. 27: 307-325. Stone, L., and A. Roberts. 1991. Conditions for a species to gain advantage from the presence of competitors. Ecology, 72: 1964-1972. Titus, R. G. 1991. Population regulation in migratory brown trout (Salmo trutta). Ph.D. thesis, Uppsala University, Uppsala. Titus, R. G., and H. Mosegaard. 1991. Selection for growth potential among migratory brown trout (Salmo trutta) fry competing for territories: evidence from otoliths. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci., 48: 19-27. Ward, B. R., and P. A. Slaney. 1988. Life history and smolt-to-adult survival of Keogh River steelhead trout (salmo gairdneri) and the relationship to smolt size. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci., 45: 1110-1122. Ward, B. R., P. A. Slaney, A. R. Facchin, and R. W. Land. 1989. Size-biased survival in steelhead trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss): back-calculated lengths from adults' scales compared to migrating smolts at the Keogh River, British Columbia. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 46: 1853-1858. Waters, T. F. 1968. Invertebrate drift-ecology and significance to stream fishes, p. 121-134. In T.G. Northcote [ed] Symposium on Salmon and Trout in Streams. H. R. MacMillan lectures in fisheries. University of British Columbia Press, Vancouver B. C. 39 Appendix 40 Table A. Data indicating the time in days to death and the general identification of the most common pathogens cultured on cytophaga agar from those steelhead which died in the laboratory tank experiments. In the treatment column the first number indicates the temperature and the second number indicates the number of shiner present in that tank. °C Number of Shiner Present 15 0 15 0 20 20 20 20 20 0 Water Temperature 15 15 20 20 0 0 Most Abundant Pathogen Isolated From Culture Flexibacter psychrophilus Flexibacter columnaris F. columnaris Saprolegnia spp. F. psychrophilus F. columnaris Unidentified lesions Unidentified lesions F. columnaris F. columnaris F. columnaris F. columnaris F. psychrophilus F. psychrophilus Unknown lesions Unknown lesion F. columnaris 3 F columnaris 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 15 15 0 15 3 20 3 Time to Death in Days 21 15 7 14 12 4 2 10 11 4 5 17 7 20 21 6 8 21 203Fmcht:ofhil_ 15 15 15 41 Table B. Steelhead survival data for all laboratory tank trials. The trial number is the order each set of trials was run, the second number is the identifying mark on each individual tank. The number of shiner present (0, 3,9) is the number of shiner in each tank, each with three steelhead. The remaining columns indicate whether or not an individual fish survived. Trial Tank ID Temperature °C 15° Number of Shiner Present 0 9 20° 1 2 7 8 15° 9 1 13 20° 2 2 2 2 15° 15° 3 20° 2 4 15° 2 2 2 7 14 15 20° 0 0 9 9 9 0 0 9 2 16 3 1 3 2 15° 15° 3 3 20° 9 9 9 3 4 15° 3 3 5 20° 3 6 15° 3 7 20° 0 0 0 3 1 1 1 15° 20° 20° 9 3 8 15° 3 13 20° 3 3 15° 0 3 14 15 16 20° 20° 3 3 3 2 15° 15° 5 20° 3 14 16 15° 3 20° 3 4 4 4 4 4 1 9 3 Largest Steelhead Intermediate Steelhead Smallest Steelhead Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Died Survived Survived Died Survived Survived Survived Survived Died Died Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Died Died Survived Died Died Died Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Died Survived Died Survived Survived Died Survived Survived Died Survived Died Died Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Died Died Survived Survived Survived Died Survived Survived Survived Survived Survived Died Died 42 Table C. Growth data measured as the proportional change in wet weight of the smallest three steelhead in laboratoy stream channel experiments. In this table the column headers indicate the number of shiner present in each trial. There is no indication that the presence of shiner affected steelhead growth. No Shiner Present 20 % 20 Shiner Present 19.8 % 1 12.7 16.2 1 20.9 4.8 6.2 23.5 1 10 1 0 Trial 1 1 2 -12.9 2 -4 2 - 3.9 - 2.7 -16.7 2 2 4 3 4.8 -28 - 1.4 -10.8 -15.6 - 4.3 0 43 Table D Accumulated fin damage data of the smallest three steelhead in the stream channel experiments. The method for scoring is explained in the method section. The column headers indicate the number of shiner present in each experimental cell. Trial No Shiner Present 20 Shiner Present 1 1 1 5 0 0 1 5 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 0 6 2 4 3 1 1 1 3 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 0