Stories don’t get better with age: Maturation and life experience do not drive narrative structure in emerging languages Narrative coherence, or story structure, becomes more complex in the course of development in children acquiring spoken [1,2], and sign languages [3]. Is narrative development driven primarily by linguistic factors or by an increase in children’s cognitive abilities? These factors are difficult to disentangle in typically developing children. However, we can distinguish these contributions by studying homesigners, deaf individuals who do not receive conventional sign or spoken linguistic input, but nevertheless develop gesture systems to communicate [4]. Little prior research has investigated narrative development in homesigners beyond very early childhood, though some emerging abilities are documented in two adolescent homesigners [5]. Are these abilities realized by adulthood? Do they depend on linguistic input or interaction within a linguistic community? The present study represents a first step toward characterizing narrative abilities in an emerging language by examining the number of events reported by participants. We analyzed narratives from child and adult homesigners in Nicaragua, and compared them to narratives produced by signers of three consecutive cohorts of Nicaraguan Sign Language (NSL) users, thereby capturing four stages of NSL’s increasing complexity. We also analyzed narratives from signers of two established sign languages [6,7] (Table 1 provides participant characteristics). Participants in all groups individually watched an animated cartoon (“Canary Row”) and then narrated its story. We determined, for one one-minute segment (“Bellhop”), the total number of events reported by each participant, using a comprehensive list expanded from McNeill’s original 55 events [8]. Child and adult homesigners produced significantly fewer events than the signers of NSL (MannWhitney U=69, two-tailed, p=.0024) and the established sign languages (Mann-Whitney U=32.5, two-tailed, p=.0251) (Table 2). NSL signers did not differ from the signers of the established sign languages (Mann-Whitney U=47, two-tailed, p=.3271). Three possibilities could explain these differences: (1) Homesigners might not have linguistic or communicative devices to express events; (2) they may not understand which information is required to produce a good narrative; or (3) they may not adequately encode or retrieve the events in the stimulus events. Previous work shows that homesigners do have such linguistic devices [9,10,11]; no research thus far has investigated homesigners’ memory abilities. Simple maturation cannot explain the observed patterns: Adult homesigners reported fewer events than did Cohort 3 signers, who were half their age. These results are therefore consistent with the notion that homesigners lack the principles governing narrative structure and the understanding of which information is relevant for a listener. Despite many years of productive social interaction, an ability to observe and understand events, and apparently sufficient lexical and gestural resources, homesigners were nevertheless unable to successfully describe the individual events of the story. These findings accord well with those of Morford [5]. More fine-grained measures of story goodness that characterize sub-episodes and characters’ goals/motivations (ongoing projects) are likely to reveal a clearer picture of the effects of emerging language in both homesigners and signers in Nicaragua. The present results suggest that a language model and/or interaction in a linguistic community is required for the development of mature narrative structure. Word count: 500 Table 1. Participant characteristics Child Homesigners Participant 1 Participant 2 Adult participants in Nicaragua Homesigners (n=4) Cohort 1 (n=4) Cohort 2 (n=4) Cohort 3 (n=4) Year tested Age at testing 2008 2008 2009 2011 8 8 9 11 Year of Entry into the Deaf Community n/a 1977-1980 1985-1990 1994 Established sign languages Mean age of exposure to sign language Mean age at testing Nederlandse Gebarentaal (Dutch Sign Language, NGT, n=3) birth 33.5 American Sign Language (ASL, n=3) 4.7 months 38.7 Mean age of exposure to NSL n/a 3.83 3.24 4.28 Mean age at testing 25.80 26.33 15.61 11.78 Table 2. Number of events reported by group and participant Minimal linguistic input, no communication partners who communicate primarily using their hands Adult Cohort 1 Homesigners NSL Participant 1 at age 8 16* 15 25 Participant 1 at age 9 13* 17 27 Participant 1 at age 11 16 22 32 Participant 2 age 11 16 29 41 Group Mean 15.5 20.75 31.25 *these data points were not included in the statistical analyses Child Homesigners Participation in linguistic community Cohort 2 NSL 36 36 36 37 36.25 Cohort 3 NSL 28 32 33 33 31.5 NGT ASL 23 28 44 27 29 33 31.7 29.7 References [1] Berman, R. A. & D. I. Slobin. (1994). Relating Events in Narrative: A Crosslinguistic Developmental Study. Hillsdale: Erlbaum. [2] Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985). Language and cognitive processes from a developmental perspective. Lang Cogn Proc, 1(1), 61- 85. [3] Morgan, G. & B. Woll. (2003). The development of reference switching encoded through body classifiers in BSL. In K. Emmorey (ed.), Perspectives on classifier constructions in sign languages, pp. 297-310. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [4] Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). The resilience of language. New York: Psychology Press. [5] Morford, J. P. (1994). How to hunt an iguana: The gestured narratives of non-signing deaf children. In Bos, H. & Schermer, T. (Eds.), Sign Language Research 1994: Proceedings of the Fourth European Congress on Sign Language Research in Munich, pp. 99-115. Hamburg: Signum Press. [6] Crasborn, O., I. Zwitserlood & J. Ros. (2008). Het Corpus NGT. A digital open access corpus of movies and annotations of Sign Language of the Netherlands. Centre for Language Studies, Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen. URL: http://hdl.handle.net/hdl:1839/00-0000-0000-0004-DF8E-6. [7] Crasborn, O. & I. Zwitserlood (2008). The Corpus NGT: an online corpus for professionals and laymen. In Construction and Exploitation of Sign Language Corpora. 3rd Workshop on the Representation and Processing of Sign Languages, O. Crasborn, T. Hanke, E. Efthimiou, I. Zwitserlood & E. Thoutenhoofd, eds. ELDA, Parijs. pp 44-49. [8] McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press. [9] Coppola, M., & Newport, E. L. (2005). Grammatical subjects in home sign: Abstract linguistic structure in adult primary gesture systems without linguistic input. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(52), 19249-19253. [10] Coppola, M., and W. C. So. (2005). Abstract and Object-Anchored Deixis: Pointing and spatial layout in adult homesign systems in Nicaragua. In Proceedings of the Boston University Conference on Language Development, 29: 144-155. A. Brugos, M. R. Clark-Cotton, and S. Ha, eds. Boston: Cascadilla Press. [11] Coppola, M. and A. Senghas. (2010). Deixis in an emerging sign language. In Sign Languages: A Cambridge Language Survey, 543-569. D. Brentari, ed. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.