Environmental Inequality and the Black-White Test Score Gap Anna Aizer Janet Currie Peter Simon Patrick Vivier Racial Disparities in Test Scores Large literature examining causes of the gap • Reviewed by Jencks and Phillips, eds (2008 and 2011) and Magnuson and Waldfogel, eds (2008) • Consider the following factors: • • • • • • • Family income School segregation Family structure Parenting practices Quality of educational inputs School segregation (Reber, 2010) Neighborhood segregation (Card and Rothstein, 2007) • Even considering all the above factors, substantial gaps remain New sources to consider? • Jencks and Phillips (2008) recommend looking at other, less traditional sources, to explain the persistence of the gap • Chay, Guryan and Mazumder (2009) alternative explanation: relative improvement in infant health of African Americans for cohorts born in the American south between 1963 and 1971. New Source: Environmental Inequality • Large literature documenting that African American and low income families’ greater exposure to environmental toxins (Brown, 1995; Brulle and Pellow, 2006; Ringquist, 2005; Mohai and Saha, 2006) • Cause of the disparities: • Intentional siting of hazards in low income/minority neighborhoods • Housing prices reflect environmental quality (Chay and Greenstone, 2005; Currie et al, 2014) • Disparities in regulatory response to an existing hazard Environmental Justice (EJ) • Inequity in the regulatory response to pollution by race/class led to new federal regulation in 1994: • “Preventing discrimination in the development, implementation and enforcement of environmental laws, regulations and policies” (EPA) • Research examining the extent to which the regulatory response has become less discriminatory is mixed • Sites in areas with a high share of black families or poor families still less likely to receive superfund designation (O’Neal, 2007) • Conditional on superfund designation, no longer appears to be any disparity in the duration of clean up (Burda and Harding, 2014) The Questions: • Can environmental inequality explain any of the racial gap in test scores? • African-Americans disproportionately exposed to pollutants • Conditional on exposure, may have fewer resources to counter negative effects (eg, nutrition) • Can environmental regulation reduce disparities in test scores? Focus on a Specific Pollutant: Lead • Long epidemiological literature linking lead exposure with diminished cognition • Lead has no biological value and is toxic. Mimics calcium in the body, interfering with renal, endocrine, cardiovascular and nervous systems. • In the brain, lead damages the prefrontal cerebral cortex, hippocampus and cerebellum (Finkelstein et al, 1998) • Nutrition affects rates of absorption • Children particularly sensitive (Lidsky and Schneider, 2003) • Greater exposure (inhalation and ingestion) • Conditional on exposure, lead more likely to be absorbed by children • Conditional on absorption, lead more likely to affect a developing nervous system Historical Sources of Lead • Gasoline: in the 1970s, a gallon of gasoline contained 4.0 grams of lead • Household Paint: contained high lead content (10-40% for some colors) prior to world war II • Post war, manufacturers drastically reduced the amount of lead contained in paint. Regulatory Response • 1970, US Surgeon General issued his first formal statement on lead poisoning, naming it a national health problem. • Since then, 2 major regulatory changes: • Eliminating lead from gas: 1985 0.5 grams/gallon, 1986 0.1 grams/gallon, 1996 zero • Eliminating lead from household paint: completely de-leaded in 1978 • Lead in the air has declined from 2.5 ug/m3 in 1980 to less than 0.5 by 2012 (EPA) • Nationally, preschool age children with lead levels in excess of 10 has declined from 8.6% in 1998 to 1.3% in 2004. Is Lead Still a Problem? • Lead remains a hazard due to residual lead found in the air, dust and soil. • 4.5 million households in the US are still exposed to high levels of lead and half a million preschool aged children have elevated blood lead levels (CDC, 2011) • Disadvantaged children disproportionately affected (1999-2002 NHANES) • Lead levels 50% higher for African American Children • Lead levels 30% higher for low income children • Elimination of elevated blood levels a goal in Healthy People 2020, the ten year national objectives for improving the health of all Americans • More recent focus on the danger of low lead levels. • CDC “lead levels of concern” which have declined over time from 60, to 10 to 5 and now they no longer use the term – any level above zero is a concern. Greater Exposure of African-Americans to Lead • Nationally, African Americans more likely to live in old (pre 1978) housing • 73% of African Americans vs. 63% of whites • Racial disparities greater than income disparities • Racial differences greater when we limit to within region/area Black White Hispanic <=100% FPL >=200% FPL RI Share in Housing Built Pre 1978 Pre 1945 0.83 0.52 0.74 0.37 0.83 0.53 0.81 0.43 0.76 0.4 African American Families Concentrated in Historic, Urban Centers Providence Rest of RI Share housing built pre 1978 Share housing built pre 1945 0.81 0.49 0.68 0.27 Share black population living in: Share white population living in: 0.86 0.51 0.14 0.49 Share population <100% FPL living in: Share population >100% FPL lining in: 0.77 0.55 0.23 0.45 Share population <100% FPL & black living in: Share population <100% FPL & white living in: 0.89 0.6 0.11 0.4 Calculations from US census IPUMS data for 1990, 2000 and 2010 Sample includes all children <=10 years old Structure of This Paper: • Document disparities in lead levels by race and income • Examine a policy aimed at reducing lead levels among RI children • Disproportionate declines in the lead levels of African American & the poor • Link declining disparities in lead levels with declining disparities in test scores • Establish a causal relationship between lead levels and future cognitive achievement • Identification issues Data • Data on child blood lead levels (BLLs) in first 72 months of life (RIDOH) linked with third through eight grade test scores (RIDE) • • • • On average, 4.7 (median=4) BLLs per child Date of test, location of child, method, level NECAP test scores for reading and math in grades 3-8, free-lunch status, IEP Linked with vital statistics data: maternal race, ethnicity, education, marital status, birth weight, gender, month prenatal care initiated, birth order • Covers birth cohorts 1997-2004 in the state. • In RI, 80% of all children screened at least once by 36 months • In RI, 15-20% in private school as of 2010 • Final sample of 57,000 linked children Part I: Documenting Disparities in Lead Levels & Trends Appendix Table 1: Within Tract Correlations Between Lead Levels and Child and Family Characteristics Avg lead level Married at Birth Maternal age at birth Mother African-American Maternal education in years Mother has at least one risk factor Always free/reduced lunch male Birth order Birth weight/100 Gestation in Weeks Month prenatal care initiated Observations R-squared -0.244 [0.0214] -0.0280 [0.00174] 0.328 [0.0325] -0.0526 [0.00371] 0.172 [0.0223] 0.359 [0.0267] 0.169 [0.0160] 0.207 [0.00870] -0.0147 [0.00180] 0.0389 [0.00510] 0.0865 [0.00730] 53,994 0.206 Part II: Why the disproportionate decline in lead levels for some groups? • Policy by the DOH and the HRC to reduce lead levels in RI children • DOH: Required all landlords of homes in which a child had an elevated lead level to obtain a “Lead Free” certificate. • State AG to file suit against non-compliant landlords • HRC: Required all landlords to obtain “Lead Safe” certificates to rent their homes • • • • Provided training and information to landlords about lead abatement Low-interest loans for abatement: section 8, family day care No resources for enforcement, only the threat of litigation Target neighborhoods with historically high lead burden – contracted with community based organizations in the 4 “core cities”, the oldest cities in the state with greatest share of disadvantaged children • 35% of children in the core cities poor vs. 9.4% rest of the state • 36% of children in the core cities African American vs. 2% rest of state • 73% of African American children and 46% of the poor live in the core cities • In 1997, 331 certificates issued; by 2010, 41,000 had been issued. Did Certificates Work? Leadi= α1certificates/old homesnt + α2 Xic + α3Xim + α4 Xntn + γn + γt + µi • Xic : BW, birth order, gestation, gender, year of birth, free lunch status • Xim: Maternal characteristics: race, ethnicity, years of schooling, marital status at birth, month prenatal care initiated, maternal age at birth • Xntn : share poor, median income, share African American, share Hispanic, share housing build pre-war, share housing built post 1978, ln(population) • Year of birth and tract FE Table 1: First Stage - Predicting a Child's Average Lead Level DOH certificates at birth/old housing units (1) (2) -0.0878 -0.184 [0.0152] [0.0285] (DOH certificates at birth/old housing units) Squared (3) (4) 0.000502 [0.000125] HRC certificates at birth/old housing units -0.113 [0.0147] Loans at birth/old housing units -0.00707 [0.00280] Observations 57,303 57,303 57,303 57,303 R-squared 0.204 0.204 0.205 0.204 F Statistic (excluded instruments) 33.18 17.19 59.00 12.25 Table 3: Certificates and Lead Levels: Heterogeneity by Child Characteristic DOH certificates at birth/old housing units (DOH Certificates at birth/old housing)*Black (1) (2) (3) (4) -0.0785 [0.0156] -0.0637 [0.0232] -0.0372 [0.0252] -0.129 [0.0394] -0.0836 [0.0174] (DOH Certificates at birth/old housing)*Free Lunch -0.0630 [0.0250] (DOH Certificates at birth/old housing)*Maternal Education 0.00347 [0.00308] (DOH Certificates at birth/old housing)*Male -0.00817 [0.0162] Observations 57,303 57,303 57,303 57,303 R-squared 0.205 0.204 0.204 0.204 Can lead certificates explain disproportionate decline in lead among black children? • More certificates in neighborhoods with a larger share black • Within neighborhoods, black children’s lead levels disproportionately affected by certificate availability • Of the 2.3 point decline in average lead levels among African Americans for 1998-2004 birth cohorts, 21% is explained by the rise in certificates Part III: Does Lead Exposure Reduce Cognitive Achievement? • Review the existing work • Discuss identification issues & strategy • Present IV estimates based on certificates as an IV • Examine impact of lead exposure on • • • • Reading and math scores Distributional impacts Test scores over time (3rd through 8th grade) Heterogeneous effects by child characteristic Existing Research on Lead and Child Outcomes • Long epidemiological literature linking lead and cognitive and noncognitive outcomes • Known for centuries that high levels of lead are associated with worse cognitive outcomes • More recent focus on low levels of lead • Individual-level analysis: • linked preschool lead levels with test scores and behavioral outcomes including ADHD and hyperactivity (Chardramouli et al, 2009; Canfield et al, 2003; Nigg et al, 2010, and Wasserman, 1997). • McLaine et al (2013) links preschool lead levels with kindergarten reading readiness among Providence children. Two Challenges to Estimating • Endogneity of lead exposure: omitted variable bias/confounding • Bellinger (2008) “It has long been recognized that due consideration must be given to the possibility that any observed association between lead exposure and neurodevelopment is, in part, an artifact of residual confounding…Indeed, adjusting for SES and related covariates can result in reductions of 50% of more in the magnitude of lead’s regression coefficient.” Measurement Error in Blood Lead Levels (BLLs) 1. Individual blood lead levels (BLLs) measured with considerable error • “The ratio of imprecision to measurement value, particularly at BLLs <10ug/dL, is relatively high.” (CDC, 2007) • The CDC allows measurement error up to 4 ug/dl or 10%, whichever is higher. 2. Infrequent testing & short half life of lead in blood • We have on average 4 measures for each child for the first 72 months of life • Lead half life in blood is 36 days • Remains in tissue much longer Cohort Level Analysis – identification more straight forward, but ecological fallacy • Lead and Cognition • Ferrie, Rolf and Troesken (2012) – variation from lead piping, examining impact on intelligence tests of WWII enlistees. • Effects only for low SES children. • Lead and Crime • Masters et al(1998); Reyes (2007); Nevin (2000 and 2007); Mielke and Zahran (2012); Nilsson (2014) and Reyes (2014) • Dramatic declines in lead levels (from the de-leading of gasoline) linked with the dramatic declines in crime rates Lead, SES and Cognitive Outcomes IV Strategy • Use certificates in one’s tract at birth to identify impact of lead on future cognitive achievement • Exclusion restriction: neighborhoods that see more growth in the number of certificates not otherwise changing in ways that would independently affect child cognitive outcomes Table 4: Lead and Reading Test Scores (Avg) Avg lead level (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) OLS Tract FE Tract FE Tract FE IV Mom FE Mom FE IV -1.029 -0.274 -1.931 -0.105 -3.197 [0.0188] [0.0190] [0.837] [0.0413] [1.609] 57,303 57,310 57,303 Randomly Drawn Lead Level -0.0792 [0.0142] Observations 60,582 57,310 57,310 R-squared 0.048 0.138 0.136 0.062 Table 5: Lead Levels and Third Grade Reading Proficiency (1) (2) Substantially below Proficient <30 (3) (4) Proficient >=40 (5) (6) with Distinction >56 Panel A: Reading Tract FE IV Tract FE IV Tract FE IV Avg lead level 0.00541 0.0790 -0.00858 -0.176 -0.00281 0.0205 [0.000537] [0.0261] [0.000795] [0.0450] [0.000729] [0.0308] Observations 53,978 53,972 53,978 53,972 53,978 53,972 R-squared 0.033 Average of dependent variable 0.067 21% 0.058 71% 10% Table 5: Lead Levels and Third Grade Math Proficiency (1) (2) Substantially below Proficient <30 (3) (4) Proficient >=40 (5) (6) with Distinction >56 Panel B: Math Proficiency Tract FE IV Tract FE IV Tract FE IV Avg lead level 0.00484 0.0644 -0.00479 -0.0316 -0.00176 0.00381 [0.000619] [0.0279] [0.000844] [0.0355] [0.000591] [0.0246] Observations 54,035 54,029 54,035 54,029 54,035 54,029 R-squared 0.032 Average of dependent variable 0.066 12% 0.034 60% 14% Table 6: Lead Levels and Reading Proficiency Over Time (1) (2) Third Grade (3) (4) Eighth Grade FE FE IV FE FE IV -0.00792 -0.0710 -0.00782 -0.120 [0.00135] [0.0626] [0.00111] [0.0648] Observations 16,511 16,511 17,850 17,850 R-squared 0.069 Avg lead level 0.070 2 sets of robustness checks • Alternative measures of lead • Alternative sets of Instruments Table 7: Different Measures of Lead and Reading Test Scores (Avg) Average level - censor at 25 (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) FE IV FE IV FE IV FE IV -0.278 -1.925 -0.252 -1.038 -0.140 -1.285 -0.309 -3.631 [0.0192] [0.833] Lead Area Under the Curve (AUC) [0.0147] [0.432] Max lead level [0.0107] [0.573] Geometric Mean of Lead [0.0207] [1.990] Observations 57,310 R-squared 0.138 57,303 57,309 0.140 57,302 57,310 0.138 57,303 56,278 0.140 56,271 Table 8: Second Stage Estimates of Lead and Average Reading Scores from Alternative IVs (1) Avg lead level Observations (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) -1.825 -1.736 -1.931 -1.812 -1.939 -1.717 -1.277 -1.770 -1.892 -1.807 [0.775] [0.752] [0.837] [0.816] [0.679] [0.622] [0.553] [0.594] [0.666] [0.611] 57,303 57,303 57,303 57,303 57,303 57,303 57,303 57,303 57,303 57,303 First Stage Instruments: DOH Certificates at birth/1000*Original (1997) Lead Level DOH Certificates at age 5/1000*Original (1997) Lead Level DOH certificates at birth/old housing units DOH certificates at age 5/old housing units HRC certificates at birth/1000*Original (1997) Lead Level HRC certificates at birth/Old housing units HRC certificates at age 5/Old housing units Total certificates at birth/1000*Original (1997) Lead Level Total certificates at birth/old housing units X X X X X X X X X X X X X X Additional robustness exercises: • Including more controls: • race*year FE • free lunch* year FE • Lead level in 1997 (in tract)*year • Propensity score estimates • Limit to tracts with predicted high numbers of certificates (by 2004) Summary (thus far) • Racial disparities in lead levels and test scores • Lead has a negative and causal impact of cognitive achievement that does not fade over time and is not heterogeneous • Policy in 1997 disproportionately lowered the lead levels of African American children • The declines did not simply reflect declines more generally by income • Important spatial component • African American children concentrated in urban areas with higher initial lead levels that received more “treatment” over this time period • Did the narrowing of the racial gap in lead levels result in a reduction in the racial gap in test scores? Trends in Lead Over Time Two exercises: • Calculate what share of the reduction in the black-white test score gap was due to the reduction in lead levels based on the causal estimate of the impact of lead on educational outcomes • Note: assume the impact of lead on test scores not vary by race • Examine directly how the gap in lead levels relates to the gap in test scores Trends in disparities in test scores • Racial gap in test scores fell from 9.7 for those born in 1998 to 6.3 for those born in 2004 (from 70% to 45% of a standard deviation). • For lead, the gap fell from 2.2 to 0.9 over this same period • Based on the range of causal estimates, the decline in the gap in lead levels explains 37-76% of the decline in the test score gap. • Income gap in test scores and lead both fell,but by smaller amounts: • Income test score gap fell from 9.3 to 8.4 (67% to 60% of a std deviation) • Income lead level gap fell from 1.83 to 0.99 Table 9: Disparities in Lead and Disparities in Test Scores - Tract and County Level (1) (2) (3) (4) White-Black Reading Scores Tract Level Disparities County Level Disparities OLS IV OLS IV -0.746 -2.101 -1.422 -1.321 [0.127] [1.487] [0.822] [2.179] Observations 1,736 1,695 40 40 R-squared 0.146 0.077 0.853 0.853 White-Black Lead Scores Each observation is a tract (county) year. Also included are county (tract) and year of birth fixed effects. All regressions weighted by tract (county) population Note that the White-Black lead score difference is negative, generally, since white lead levels are lower than black lead levels. Conclusions • RI implemented a policy that targeted and disproportionately reduced the lead levels of African American and low income children • The resulting declines in racial disparities in lead exposure can explain a substantial share of the recent decline in racial test score disparities. • Eliminating the black-white test score gap single most effective way to reduce racial economic inequality (Jencks and Phillips, 2011). • If we continue to target environmental regulation at children at greatest risk, then declines in disparities in future economic outcomes may follow.