Appetitive Reinforcement in Nonhuman Primates: Revisiting the Necessity and Implementation of Feeding Restriction for Behavioral Studies A.J. Kirsten, S.A. Davis, S.N. Von Huben, C.C. Lay, S.N. Katner, and M.A. Taffe Department of Neuropharmacology, The Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA Progressive Ratio 200% LasResp 500% 400% 300% 200% 100% Post +49 Post +47 Post +45 Post +43 Post +41 Post +39 Post +37 Post +35 Post +33 Post +31 Post +29 Post +27 Post +25 Post +23 Post +21 Post +19 Post +17 Post +15 Post +13 Post +11 Post +1 Post +49 Post +47 Post +45 Post +43 Post +41 Post +39 Post +37 Post +35 Post +33 Post +31 Post +29 Post +27 Post +25 Post +23 Post +21 Post +19 Post +17 Post +15 Post +13 Post +11 Post +9 RunRate 2500% 2000% 1500% 51 46 41 36 31 26 21 16 11 6 1 150% 130% 110% 51 46 41 36 31 26 21 16 11 6 90% 70% 50% 1 250% 230% 210% 190% 170% 150% 130% 110% 90% 70% 50% 51 46 41 36 31 26 21 16 11 457 455 454 6 51 46 41 36 31 Percent of NRC/NAS Estimate 51 46 41 36 31 21 16 11 26 26 390 • The bodyweight and metabolic requirement distributions for laboratory nonhuman primates are broad enough that population-based protocols (e.g., 85% of NRC food estimate, 85% of van Wagenen weight estimate) are inadequate. E.g., ~16% of individuals will be smaller than 85% of van Wagenen. • Normative growth means for male rhesus monkeys vary by as much as 1 SD and/or 15%. The larger sample Rawlins et al. (1984) and Saxton & Lotz (1990) datasets may provide improved estimates over van Wagenen & Catchpole (1956). 1000% 500% 0% Weeks of Study Post +49 Post +47 Post +45 Post +43 Post +39 Post +37 Post +35 Post +33 Post +31 Post +29 Post +27 Post +25 Post +23 Post +21 Post +19 Post +17 Post +15 Post +49 Post +47 Post +45 Post +43 Post +41 Post +39 Post +37 Post +35 Post +33 Post +31 Post +29 Post +27 Post +25 Post +23 Post +21 Post +19 Post +17 410 70-85% 70-85% 70-85% 70-85% 70-85% 85% 85% 85% 100% 100% 500 450 400 300 # Reinf -500% Post +15 44 54 64 74 84 Months of Age (range) The NIA 3-5 yr old groups begin the study much smaller than would be predicted by van Wagenen or Rawlins. Growth rates in both control and experimental groups are also reduced. 412 190% 170% • Food restriction is necessary to produce reliable behavior in nonhuman primates. Many papers provide insufficient methodological detail to evaluate claims of “normative feeding” or “minimal restriction”. In some cases, analysis of bodyweights/ages, calculation of “normal” intake from reinforcers earned (“animals earned 15% of normal ration in the test chamber”, etc) or personal communication suggests that significant restriction was in fact practiced. 3000% 0% Post +13 34 200 Baseline Taffe, 2004, Physiol Behav RunRate -50% 24 405 Discussion Chow (% of weekly NRC/NAS estimate) # Resp 50% Post +11 20 30 40 Months of Age (range) The NIA 0.6-1 yr old groups begin the study at weights approximating the van Wagenen or Rawlins distributions. Growth rates over 13 months are attenuated in both groups relative to either normative dataset. Chow 3500% 100% Post +9 10 413 0 Post +13 vW&C1956 IngramAdultContStart Control young adult (3-5 yr) male N=6 IngramAdultContEnd N=6 IngramAdultCRstart Exp. young adult(30%CR) 3-5yr male N=6 IngramAdultCRend N=6 Rawlins 1984 2 0 # Resp Percent of Baseline 4 2 Reinforcers Time to Last Response * 4000% 150% 6 0 0 Post +5 200% y = 16.504x(-0.8219) 0 Post +3 Post +1 8 § 0% Post +5 1 50% Post +3 2 100% Post +1 3 LasResp 0% Percent of Baseline 4 * 4 600% 150% 10 § § 700% # Reinf 12 5 Kilograms (range) 250% Percent of Baseline In Wisconsin NPRC study, adult male CR animals end up at a slightly reduced mean weight and ~64% of control animals. Bodyfat ranged from 4.8% to 24.5% in CR animals; 21.5-40% in controls. Gresl et al, 2001 Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab NIA Caloric Restriction Study vW&C1956 IngramJuvContStart Control juvenile (0.6-1yr) male N=6 IngramJuvContEnd N=6 IngramJuvCRstart Exp. juvenile(30%CR)0.6-1yr male N=6 IngramJuvCRend N=6 Rawlins 1984 3 Post +11 300 * * Post +7 250 * § Post +5 200 * 6 Post +5 150 6 * 9 Post +3 100 Months of Age (range) 6 * (g/day) Post +3 50 415 Middle/bottom panels: Baseline or improved performance generally requires 70%-85% restriction at a minimum. In many cases, performance tracks feeding changes instituted for non-performance reasons such as growth rate or body condition. Monkeys in training (N=7) Percent of Baseline 10 15 20 Kilograms NIA CR feeding is 55-60% of our NRC estimate for <5kg animals and 75-85% for >5kg animals. Ingram et al. J.Gerontol., 45: B148-B163, 1990 12 Monkeys well trained (N=5) 0 5 8 § Post +9 5 15 Post +9 10 0 0 0 PR performance is not maintained in animals who were welltrained at study initiation. Performance of animals in training is restored or slightly improved although no control group was included to assess PR improvement under prior restriction practices. 15 Post +7 NRC/NAS estimate NRC/NAS M-F for 85% NRC/NAS M-F for 70% Rawlins 1984 Post +7 100 GreslCREnd 2001 N=12 Post +1 Kilograms (range) 150 Kilograms (range) Chow (g) 200 GreslCRStart 2001 N=15 vW&C1956 20 250 GreslContEnd 2001 N=13 FR 1,2,3...8, 10,12...24, 28,32... 10 minute session Terminates 3 minutes after last response. 10 § Post +7 300 GreslContStart 2001 N=15 250% 230% 210% Top panels: Sustained 70% restriction levels in the older, larger animals suggests the NRC estimate is too high for this age range. Minutes 25 50 18 WNPRC Caloric Restriction study 350 Ingram90 Ingram90 M-F for 85% Ingram90 M-F for 70% Progressive Ratio Task Reinforcers Acquired NIA Caloric Restriction Study 389 320 325 329 333 Weeks of Study Post +41 Operant Food Restriction vs. Caloric Restriction for Aging 250% 230% 210% 190% 170% 150% 130% 110% 90% 70% 50% 1 1 Weeks of Study 230% 210% 190% 170% 150% 130% 110% 90% 70% 50% 51 PostFeed +48 PostFeed +46 PostFeed +44 PostFeed +42 PostFeed +40 PostFeed +38 PostFeed +36 PostFeed +34 PostFeed +32 PostFeed +30 PostFeed +28 PostFeed +26 PostFeed +24 PostFeed +22 PostFeed +20 PostFeed +18 PostFeed +16 PostFeed +14 PostFeed +12 PostFeed +10 PostFeed +4 PostFeed +6 PreFeed PostFeed +46 PostFeed +44 PostFeed +42 PostFeed +40 PostFeed +38 PostFeed +36 PostFeed +34 PostFeed +32 PostFeed +30 PostFeed +28 PostFeed +26 PostFeed +24 PostFeed +22 PreFeed The effect of any feeding protocol on growth is best determined by reference to within-institute populations rather than published datasets. PostFeed +8 80% 75% 457 455 454 46 90% 250% 41 85% 100% 36 95% 110% 31 105% 120% 390 26 115% 130% 410 21 125% 140% 412 21 135% 405 16 150% 413 11 145% PostFeed +2 Week 290 Week 279 Week 268 Week 257 Week 246 Week 235 Week 224 Week 213 Week 202 Week 191 Week 180 Week 169 Week 158 Week 147 Week 136 Week 125 Published “normative” datasets can differ by about 1 SD and/or 85% for the ~2-5 year old age range. Week 114 Week 70 0 160% Percent of Baseline 2 155% 415 16 Saxton and Lotz, 1990; J Med Primatol [N=33 Naval Aerospace Med Research, Pensicola FL] 4 170% PostFeed +20 100 Monkeys in training (N=7) 165% PostFeed +18 80 Week 103 60 6 Week 92 40 8 Week 81 20 Unrestricted(sd) Restricted(sd) Rawlins84(sd) PostFeed +16 Rawlins, et al., 1984 Monkeys well trained (N=5) PostFeed +14 Saxton & Lotz, 1990 Growth: by Claimed Birthdate 250% 230% 210% 190% 170% 150% 130% 110% 90% 70% 50% 11 Taffe, 2004, Physiol Behav 50% 6 (% of weekly NRC/NAS estimate) 6 Chow (g/day) 6 Percent of Baseline (Latency) 70-85% 70-85% 70-85% 70-85% 70-85% 85% 85% 85% 100% 100% 500 450 60% 400 80% 190% 170% 150% 130% 110% 90% 70% 1 100% 389 320 325 329 333 1 120% § § Restriction Level 250% 230% 210% § § Responding for a highly preferred reinforcer in the BMS task is negatively affected by net increases in feeding. Week 290 Week 279 Week 268 Week 257 Week 246 Week 235 Week 224 Week 213 Week 202 Week 191 Week 180 Week 291 Week 281 Week 271 Week 261 Week 251 Week 241 Week 231 0 Percent of Baseline van W agenen & Catchpole, 1956 0 Male rhesus monkeys (M. mulatta), 2.5-8 yrs Nutrient Requirements of Nonhuman Primates: Second Revised Edition (2003), National Research Council of The National Academy of Sciences, The National Academies Press, Washington, D.C. 0 5 * * * Chow PostFeed +12 10 0 The descriptions of the feeding restriction protocols employed in published monkey studies are minimal and misleading. where y is bisc/kg/day and x is kilograms bodyweight 10 PostFeed +10 4 The power of an appetitive reward to reinforce behavior is significantly increased by making the subject in question hungry. In training to concurrently perform behavioral tests assaying different cognitive domains. Prior restriction practices based on performance and laboratory “rules of thumb”. Re-conceptualization of food restriction based on data provided in “Nutrient Requirements of Nonhuman Primates” (2003); Tables 2-2,2-3. 2 PostFeed +8 12 2 Introduction Methods 15 PostFeed +6 6 Rawlins et al., 1984; J Med Primatol [N=41, 44, 36, 25, 21, 18 for yrs 1-6 Cayo Santiago, free ranging, 230 g/chow per day per capita plus forage] Describe effects of food restriction on the growth of developing animals 20 4 PostFeed +4 14 Kirk, 1972; Lab Anim Sci [N=19 USAF, Brooks AFB] Establish restriction protocols on a more quantitative framework 25 PostFeed +2 8 kilograms Kilograms 10 van Wagenen and Catchpole 1956; Am J Phys Anthropol [N=17, 12, 10, 9, 7, 6 at ends of years 1-6 Yale] Evaluate the need for food restriction. 35 140% 12 Months of Age Objectives: Week 221 Week 211 Week 201 Week 191 Week 181 Week 171 Week 161 14 Week 151 100 Week 141 80 0 40 6 Week 169 0 Week 131 60 5 Week 121 40 10 Week 111 20 15 4 15 raisins in Lexan holeboard Latency to extract raisins Bimanual strategy universally used 45 30 Week 158 Kirk, 1972 0 20 2 van W agenen & Catchpole, 1956 0 6 Week 101 2 25 8 Week 147 4 30 Week 136 6 35 8 10 Week 125 kilograms Kilograms 8 40 vanWagenen(15%) Normative Feeding (sd) Rawlins84 MonkeyN Week 114 10 10 45 Number of monkeys Food Restricted (sd) vanWagenen (sd) Rawlins84 (sd) Monkey'N' 50 Week 103 12 12 Week 92 12 50 Week 81 14 Week 70 14 BMS Performance 160% TSRI Normative Feeding: by Claimed Birthdate kilograms Male Rhesus Monkey Growth Individual Differences Bimanual Motor Skill Task 300 TSRI Food Restricted: by Claimed Birthdate Number of monkeys Early experimental psychologists made broad use of knowledge that is undoubtedly as old as animal domestication, i.e., that the power of appetitive reinforcement is enhanced by restricting the subjects‘ access to food. This has led to the nearly universal practice of restricting common laboratory rodent and avian subjects to 85% of free feeding weight for operant experiments. Appetitive operant procedures in nonhuman primates vary more widely, in part because of the time required for such animals to reach mature weight and greater individual variability in body size compared with inbred laboratory species. In addition, many nonhuman primates will grow obese under true ad libitum feeding. Therefore, food restriction protocols for monkeys tend to be highly individualized and conducted on the basis of laboratory experience within a given model. The present study was undertaken to determine to what extent short term ad libitum food consumption in rhesus macaques would impair performance on an established neuropsychological testing battery. A second part of the study was to formalize food restriction parameters to determine what degree of restriction was required to produce consistent behavioral performance. Results show clearly that behavioral performance on a range of tasks is detrimentally affected by short term ad libitum chow feeding, even when the reinforcer is highly preferred or the tasks are well trained. Furthermore, it is shown that maintenance of weekly chow intake in the range of 70%-85% of National Research Council recommendations for metabolizable energy is necessary for consistent behavioral responding. Finally, a review of published normative datasets for male rhesus monkey growth indicates that the most frequently referenced dataset derived from VanWagenen and Catchpole (Am J Phys Anthropol 1956; 19:245-273) may be optimistic. Implications for food restriction protocols in a context of current monkey availability and increased veterinarian scrutiny are discussed. Supported by DA13390, MH62261 and MH616192. Reference to the limited amount of published data regarding rhesus monkey growth rates is unlikely to provide a complete solution for generating consistent levels of food restriction. Bimanual Motor Skill Comparison with Growth Under Operant Food Restriction Percent of Baseline Abstract 200 “Normative” Growth of Male Rhesus Monkeys: We suggest that authors, reviewers and editors work to ensure that methodological descriptions of appetitive operant procedures in nonhuman primate manuscripts meet the basic scientific requirement of providing detail sufficient for replication.