Proc. R. Soc. B (2006) 273, 2799–2808 doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.3583 Published online 12 July 2006 Review Tooth microstructure tracks the pace of human life-history evolution M. Christopher Dean* Department of Anatomy and Developmental Biology, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, UK A number of fundamental milestones define the pace at which animals develop, mature, reproduce and age. These include the length of gestation, the age at weaning and at sexual maturity, the number of offspring produced over a lifetime and the length of life itself. Because a time-scale for dental development can be retrieved from the internal structure of teeth and many of these life-history variables tend to be highly correlated, we can discover more than might be imagined about fossil primates and more, in particular, about fossil hominids and our own evolutionary history. Some insights into the evolutionary processes underlying changes in dental development are emerging from a better understanding of the mechanisms controlling enamel and dentine formation. Our own 18–20-year period of growth and development probably evolved quite recently after ca 17 million years of a more ape-like life-history profile. Keywords: life history; enamel; dentine; incremental markings; hominid; evolution 1. INTRODUCTION Some of the most informative things about our own evolutionary history have emerged from an understanding of how morphological adaptations relate to the ways hominoids lived their lives. Variation in life-history strategy is dependent on a broad set of biological rules that govern the balance between fertility and mortality rates. Some apply across many organisms, others are characteristic of particular phylogenetic lineages and some are specific to local ecological circumstances. At one end of the spectrum, for example among eusocial insects, there are large differences in lifespan between the well-protected queens that experience low mortality rates and the more exposed and vulnerable workers (Keller & Genoud 1997). Generally among animals when mortality rates are low, reproductive success can be maximized by postponing age at first reproduction for as long as possible and by spreading reproductive output into adulthood over as long a period as possible (Ashmole 1963; Charnov 1991, 1993; Harvey & Nee 1991; Stearns 1992). In an environment where adult mortality rates are high, the greater costs to the mother of early and more frequent reproduction are outweighed by the need for increased reproductive effort to maximize reproductive success (Harvey & Zammuto 1985; Promislow & Harvey 1990, 1991). At the other end of the spectrum, however, especially when studying more closely related groups of animals such as primates, there are additional influences on life-history variables to consider (Harvey & Clutton-Brock 1985; Charnov & Berrigan 1993). Life-history strategy is also about optimizing available energy resources over a lifetime and primates exploit diverse diets and habitats in very different ways (Kozlowski & Weiner 1997; Gurven & Walker 2006; *ucgacrd@ucl.ac.uk Received 2 March 2006 Accepted 5 May 2006 Dirks & Bowman in press). Among primates, orang-utans (Pongo pygmaeus) for example, are remarkable for their later age at weaning, later age at first reproduction, long interbirth intervals (6–9 years) and lower mortality rates compared with chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) (Hill et al. 2001; Wich et al. 2004). In the case of the orang-utan, food availability, home range and foraging strategy are thought to underlie these life-history traits as well as just differences in mortality rates (Wich et al. 2004). While on the one hand there is huge life-history diversity among primates, one life-history trait, the period of growth and development, appears to have undergone just three major grade shifts (Schultz 1940, 1969). Among lemurs, irrespective of body size (Schwartz et al. 2002), growth is fast. Both New and Old World monkeys, despite differences among and between them, have a much more prolonged period of growth than prosimian primates and major differences exist in the period of development between monkeys and apes (Harvey & Clutton-Brock 1985; Kelley 1997, 2002). Similarly, the modern human growth period is greatly prolonged with respect to that of apes. Long periods of relative stasis in the general pace of development seem, therefore, to characterize primate evolution. This begs questions about the evolutionary origins of the major shifts between monkey-like, ape-like and modern human-like patterns of development. A complex hierarchy of interrelated factors has contributed to the evolution of primate development, growth and lifespan. While much of this evolutionary history is unknowable, given only the fossil record of bones and teeth, teeth have to grow within whatever period of development is available. As such their growth is a reflection of the time determined by natural selection acting on key life-history variables (Godfrey et al. 2001; Dirks 2003). 2799 q 2006 The Royal Society 2800 M. C. Dean Review Figure 1. Calcospheritic mineralization of root dentine is shown on the left with fluorescent labels seen with confocal microscopy (fieldwidth 130 microns), in the middle with SEM of a mineralizing root dentine front (fieldwidth 70 microns) and on the right in a ground section of a w1.6 million year old fossil hominid root (fieldwidth 125 microns). 2. ENAMEL AND DENTINE FORMATION The developmental processes that control the rates of enamel and dentine formation ultimately hold the key to a better understanding of how the timing of dental development comes to track evolutionary change in lifehistory timing. Enamel and dentine, while fundamentally different tissues, are both extremely hard and never remodel. Dentine is secreted by odontoblasts. These differentiate from the mesenchymal neural crest cells of the dental papilla or future pulp through a long process involving reciprocal inductive signals between oral ectoderm and peripheral dental papilla cells (Lesot et al. 2001; Thesleff et al. 2001). Odontoblasts secrete a proteoglycanrich organic matrix, part of whose function is to delay mineralization for several days until collagen fibre growth and orientation within the matrix is complete ( Jones & Boyde 1984; Linde 1984, 1985; Boyde 1990), and then to induce hydroxyapatite formation at the predentine– dentine boundary (Irving 1963; Veis et al. 1977; Wuthier 1984). Several growth factors, including Insulin-like Growth Factor (IGF-1, IGF 11), Fibroblast Growth Factor (FGF-1) and members of the Transforming Growth Factor beta family (TGF-bs and Bone Morphogenetic Protein, BMP-2) may act synergistically to induce terminal odontoblast differentiation and stimulate the synthesis of dentine matrix formation. Importantly, some of these (e.g. TGF-b1) remain sequestered and potentially active throughout life within mineralized dentine (Tziafas et al. 2000; Sloan et al. 2000; DenBesten et al. 2001). Enamel matrix is secreted by ameloblasts that differentiate from the inner enamel epithelium of the tooth. Unlike dentine, enamel contains no collagen and is even harder. IGF-1 and IGF-11 are known to increase rates of enamel matrix secretion and are associated with increased expression of genes for the enamel matrix proteins involved in mineralization, such as enamelin and amelogenin (Catón et al. 2005). The onset of enamel matrix secretion is delayed with respect to dentine matrix Proc. R. Soc. B (2006) secretion by at least 30 hours, as continuing inductive epithelial–mesenchymal signalling occurs between these tissues (Gaunt 1967), but then mineralization, unlike dentine, begins almost immediately (Rosser et al. 1967). 3. CIRCADIAN INCREMENTAL MARKINGS IN TEETH It is the incremental record of tooth growth that provides a time-scale for reconstructing life history. Circadian rhythms are almost universal among living organisms and the mineralizing systems of teeth, shells and corals are no exception. Fossil coral sequences preserve a record of the slowing of the Earth’s rotation over 350 million years (Wells 1963; Scrutton 1964) and fossil teeth contain a record of the daily secretory rhythms of the cells that formed them (Dean 1987). The experimental evidence for circadian incremental markings in enamel rests largely on a number of labelling experiments, where either sodium fluoride or certain fluorescent markers have been administered at known intervals while enamel was still forming (Schour & Poncher 1937; Okada 1943; Bromage 1991; Smith 2006). There is also evidence for both shorter- (intradian) and longer-period increments in enamel and dentine (Shinoda 1984; Dean 1987, 1995; Boyde 1989; Ohtsuka & Shinoda 1995; FitzGerald 1998; Smith 2006). No rhythms are known for growth factor synthesis in tooth germs, but circadian rhythms clearly exist in predentine matrix synthesis, dentine mineralization, odontoblast cell process morphology (Boyde 1990) and during tooth eruption (Risinger & Proffitt 1996). Both mineralization and eruption rhythms have been shown to be maximal at the end of the light (or active) period and to be linked, respectively, with peak adrenal cortex activity and the late evening secretion of growth hormone and thyroid hormone (Miani & Miani 1971; Risinger & Proffitt 1996; Craddock & Youngson 2004). All of this is likely to be controlled by the suprachiasmatic nucleus, and temporary Review M. C. Dean 2801 cementum cementoblasts dentine fully differentiated odontoblasts cells of surrounding dental follicle developing pulp epithelial root sheath developing alveolar bone Figure 2. The rate at which odontoblasts differentiate determines the rate of root extension. The original angle of alignment of the fully differentiated odontoblasts to the root surface (the cementum) can be reconstructed (black arrows) from the orientation of the incremental markings in roots to the root surface. In ground sections of Proconsul heseloni tooth roots, the incremental lines in a deciduous second molar root (the middle image) are near parallel in their orientation to the root surface (extension rate ca 18 mmK1), while in a permanent premolar root (image on far right) they form a greater angle to the root surface (extension rate ca 8 mmK1). In both cases, the origin of the arrow begins 200 mm (ca 80 days) deep to the root surface. or permanent obliteration of this nucleus results in the loss of a circadian rhythm during dentine formation (Ohtuka-Isoya et al. 2001). However, it is highly likely that there are one or more as yet unidentified intracellular clocks operating during enamel and dentine formation of the kind that regulate intradian, circadian and developmental timing (e.g. Smith et al. 1987; Moore 1999; Adoutte 2000). The circadian mineralizing lines in dentine are distinguished by their characteristic appearance that is described as calcospheritic (see figure 1). Small spheres of mineralizing dentine increase in size until they eventually coalesce with each other. It is the mineralizing increments in dentine that are especially well preserved in fossils (Boyde 1990; Furseth-Klinge et al. 2005). Okada (1943) summarized a series of elegant experiments that together suggest that the mechanism underlying the appearance of mineralizing lines in dentine is the daily change in acid–base balance that accompanies alternating periods of activity and rest (see also Lutz & Rhoads 1977; Boyde 1979). Ultimately, this is manifest as regions of differing refractive index that appear lighter or darker in transmitted light microscopy, and as light or dark bands under back-scattered scanning electron microscopy that reflect regular shifts density of mineral laid down (Boyde 1979, 1989). 4. LIFE-HISTORY VARIABLES AND THE ERUPTION OF MOLAR TEETH Permanent molar teeth develop in sequence between birth and adulthood. In particular, their emergence into the mouth is in each case a developmental milestone that can be used as a proxy to determine the pace of life history in fossil primates. Modern human permanent molars (M1, M2, M3) emerge roughly 6 years apart at ages 6, 12 and 18 years. Modern chimpanzee permanent molars emerge roughly at 3.5, 6.5 and 10.5 years and those of macaque monkeys at roughly 1.3, 3.5 and 5.5 years of age (Smith et al. 1994). Gingival emergence of M1 correlates best with several life-history traits (Smith 1989, 1991, 1992; Kelley 2002, 2004). For example, completion of 90% of brain growth occurs at M1 emergence in all primates Proc. R. Soc. B (2006) (Ashton & Spence 1958; Smith 1989; Allman & Hasenstaub 1999) and skeletal maturity correlates broadly with M3 emergence (Demisch & Wartmann 1956; Watts 1990; Trinkaus & Tompkins 1990). Thus, many of the crucial questions about the pace of life history in fossil hominoids depend on being able to reconstruct the timing of gingival emergence in molar teeth. 5. HOW TOOTH ROOTS GROW IN LENGTH A clearer understanding of the comparative processes involved in root growth is allowing us to recover more information about the timing of dental development in living and fossil hominoids. It is the cells of the internal layer of the epithelial root sheath that induce the peripheral cells of the dental papilla to differentiate into odontoblasts and go on to form tooth roots (figure 2). Initially, preodontoblast cells of the dental papilla align themselves along the basal lamina that separates them from the epithelial root sheath. A gradient of differentiation then begins that may initially be regulated by signals from the epithelium and subsequently by a lateral relay mechanism between odontoblasts (Lesot et al. 2001; Thesleff et al. 2001). This process ends with final polarization of the root odontoblasts, predentine secretion and mineralization (Thomas 1995). The rates of odontoblast differentiation and rates of dentine formation can still be determined from longitudinal ground sections of tooth roots (figure 2). Daily incremental markings in the root indicate that mineralization rates begin slowly close to the root surface and that for at least the first 200 mm of dentine formation, the gradient of increasing rate is comparable in both living and fossil hominoids (Dean 1993, 1998). Based on the average spacing of daily mineralizing lines, the first 200 mm of root dentine takes ca 80 days to form. Longer-period accentuated incremental markings in the root reflect the former position and orientation of the odontoblast cell sheet at any one time during root formation. The angle these longer-period lines make to the root surface reflects the rate of differentiation of new odontoblasts during root development (Dean & Wood 1981). For example, in 2802 M. C. Dean Review 16 root extension rate (microns/day) 14 12 10 8 6 4 Gorilla/Pongo Homo 2 Pan 0 200 500 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000 6000 7000 8000 9000 10 000 mesiobuccal root length (microns) Figure 3. Root extension rates for the first 10 mm of mesiobuccal root length in hominoid M1s show the gradual rise and fall typical of permanent molar teeth. The different extension rates in each taxon reflect both overall tooth height and the amount of time available to grow the root. 80 days, a wave of differentiation proceeding at 10 mmK1 will extend 800 mm down the root, whereas a differentiation rate of just 4 mmK1 will extend only 320 mm. The rate of odontoblast differentiation determines the rate of root elongation, and this relationship has been formally described and referred to as the root extension rate (Shellis 1984). Calculating the changing root extension rates from ground sections of fossil primate tooth roots allows us to compare their growth with living primates. A number of studies present data for rates of root extension in humans. Stack (1967) measured the increase in length of deciduous incisor tooth roots, which appear to grow linearly at ca 19 mmK1. This is likely to be as fast as human tooth roots ever grow. Root growth in permanent teeth is, however, characteristically nonlinear and more complex. Human permanent tooth roots can take up to 7 years to complete, and all the evidence suggest that rates of molar root extension begin slowly, reach a peak and then reduce in rate again as the apex of the root closes (figure 3). Gleiser & Hunt (1955) estimated initial rates of root extension to be ca 4 mmK1 for first permanent molars. Feasby (1981) and Inoue & Suzuki (1992) all present data for other permanent tooth roots that initiate with comparably slow rates, but then rise to maximal rates of between ca 8 and 18 mmK1 (Gleiser & Hunt 1955; Takeshima et al. 2004). There is a consensus that eruption rates come to exceed rates of root growth as a tooth approaches occlusion (Gleiser & Hunt 1955; Schumaker & El Hadary 1960; Ando et al. 1965; Darling & Levers 1976; Feasby 1981; Inoue & Suzuki 1992; Takeshima et al. 2004). The spurt in root growth and the eruption surge are far from equal in magnitude, and eruption rates can exceed root growth by as much as 4.6 mm yrK1 in humans (Feasby 1981), and can to some extent be predicted from the height of the jaws (Takeshima et al. 2004). It is clear that root extension and tooth eruption are independent processes and that only root length at the time of gingival emergence is a useful comparator for Proc. R. Soc. B (2006) mm 25 20 15 10 5 0 mesiobuccal root length Pan Gorilla / Pongo Homo sapiens crown height total tooth height Figure 4. Measurements from radiographs (Dean & Wood 1981, 2003) and directly from some museum specimens indicate that total M1 mesial tooth height, from the cusps tips to the root apex, is made up of predictable proportions of mesial crown height and of mesiobuccal root length. Among fossil primates, knowledge of crown height alone can be used to predict how much root would have been formed at gingival emergence. gauging differences in developmental timing. Total tooth height at gingival emergence is made up of similar proportions of root and crown height in great apes and modern humans (figure 4). Hence, the proportions in living taxa allow us to calculate the likely amount of root formed at gingival emergence in fossil primates on the basis of measurements of crown height. 6. LIFE HISTORY AND THE EARLIEST APES Many Miocene apes were a morphological mosaic of characters that we recognize today as specific to either modern apes or monkeys. Great apes have a prolonged life-history profile compared with Old World monkeys. They have reduced rates of mortality, longer gestation Review periods, mature more slowly and have and much later ages at first reproduction (Harvey & Clutton-Brock 1985; Kelley 1997, 2002). Kelley (1997) suggested that besides studying their morphological attributes, M1 emergence times in Miocene fossil apes might, in addition, identify the first evidence of a shift to a modern ape-like life-history profile. Age at first permanent molar emergence has been calculated for several fossil apes now, using the various daily and longer-period increments preserved in fossil enamel. These include Sivapithecus parvada (Kelley 1997, 2002), a 10 million year old ape from the Siwaliks of Pakistan, Afropithecus turkanensis (Kelley 2002; Kelley & Smith 2003), a 17 million year old ape from Moruorot in northern Kenya, Dryopithecus laietanus from Can Llobateres, a 9.5 million year old site in Spain (Kelley et al. 2001) and two species of Proconsul, dated to 17.5–17.9 million years ago, Proconsul heseloni and Proconsul nyanzae from Rusinga Island, Kenya. The consensus is that Sivapthecus, Afropithecus and Dryopithecus had mean age of M1 emergence times that place them firmly within the known range for modern chimpanzees (Kelley 2004). Harrison (1987) has argued that the various species of Proconsul were stem catarrhines. Owing to the potential phylogenetic implications that a modern Old World monkey-like or modern great ape-like life-history profile might therefore have, an age for M1 emergence in Proconsul has been viewed as more controversial that in other Early Miocene apes. Proconsul heseloni and P. nyanzae were both arboreal quadrupeds, with long narrow trunks that showed few signs of any forelimb suspensory behaviour, but possessed frontal sinuses and a larger than expected cranial capacity for their body size (Walker 1997). Kelley (1997), on the basis of cranial capacity estimates, cautiously proposed that M1 emergence in the smaller siamangsized 9–12 kg P. heseloni may have been close to 20.6 months and interpreted this as being later into development than might be expected in an Early Miocene catarrhine. This age for M1 emergence can now be matched using data for enamel and dentine growth combined with data for M1 crown height in P. heseloni. Histological data for M1 crown formation times suggest that this took between 1 and 1.2 years (Beynon et al. 1998), but estimates for M1 root extension rates and of how much root might have been formed at gingival emergence have been harder to make (Walker & Shipman 2005). Smith et al. (2006) concluded that the method described by Shellis (1984) for calculating extension rates and used by Beynon et al. (1998) is prone to error when used on teeth with short, rapid periods of development. Reanalysis of the P. heseloni M1 roots reveals average extension rates of 9–10 mmK1 over 7 mm of root formation. Two M1 crowns are each ca 4 mm tall (Beynon et al. 1998). Assuming crown height to be the same proportion of total tooth height as it is in modern chimpanzees (62–68%), then root length formed at gingival emergence (figure 4) will be 32–38% of total tooth height or 47–61% of crown height. It follows, therefore, that between 1.88 and 2.45 mm of root is likely to have been formed at M1 emergence in P. heseloni. At an average of 9.5 mmK1, this translates as 198–258 days of root formation in P. heseloni. Even adding the shortest enamel formation time estimate of 1 year to this (Beynon et al. 1998) gives a likely age at M1 emergence as Proc. R. Soc. B (2006) M. C. Dean 2803 18.5–20.4 months, a remarkably close match with Kelley’s previous estimate of 20.6 months (Kelley 1997). Even though the small body size of P. heseloni makes it awkward to make direct comparisons with living apes (especially with so little data for living gibbons and siamangs; Dirks 1998), this age for M1 emergence suggests a more prolonged life history than would be expected for an Early Miocene catarrhine of the same body mass (Kelley 1997) and certainly contrasts with the faster enamel and dentine formation rates of the first known fossil Old World monkey, Victoriapithecus macinnesi (Dean & Leakey 2004). The larger small chimpanzee-sized Proconsul nyanzae upper M1 crown analysed by Beynon et al. (1998) is 6 mm tall and took 2 years to form its enamel in the crown. Only 540–690 microns of the root are preserved either buccally or lingually in this M1. It now seems likely, by the same argument, that root length at gingival emergence would have been 47–61% of crown height and therefore, that 2.83–3.66 mm of root would have been formed at gingival emergence. Extension rates in this root average 6.25 microns/day, which compares well with those known for modern chimpanzees. Similar calculations for the sum of crown and root formation time give an age for M1 emergence at between 3.26 and 3.6 years, which is well within the age range known for M1 gingival emergence in modern chimpanzees. All these data for Miocene apes suggest that ca 18 million years ago in the hominoid lineage there was a prolonged life-history profile that broadly matched that of living great apes. It is important to recognize, however, that within this profile a fast–slow dental/life-history continuum remains even today that reflects both the apportionment of available energy resources and population mortality rates (Godfrey et al. 2001; Dirks 2003; Kelley 2004; Nargolwalla et al. 2005; Dirks & Bowman in press). How this might have evolved is a more complex issue, perhaps as an adaptive prolongation of a shorter lifehistory profile in an as yet unidentified catarrhine ancestor, or as a simple extension of a prolonged lifehistory profile in a small bodied ancestor that simply kept pace with increasing body size (Kelley & Smith 2003). 7. WHEN AND WHY DID OUR MODERN LIFE-HISTORY PATTERN EVOLVE? Modern humans have big brains, walk on two feet and have small teeth and jaws. The earliest bipedal fossil hominids from Chad and Kenya are dated at between 6 and 7 million years ago, but for at least the first 4 million years of hominid evolution there is little evidence for an increase in brain size. Brain size and body proportions seem to have changed significantly with the emergence of the genus Homo at about 2 million years and changes in brain size are closely linked with many life-history traits (Smith 1989; Allman & Hasenstaub 1999). Initially, it was thought, almost by definition, that the earliest hominids would show evidence of a human-like life-history profile, but it is now generally accepted this cannot be substantiated (Bromage & Dean 1985; Smith 1986). There are several lines of evidence that allow us to track the evolution of life-history traits among fossil hominids. Various incremental markings in enamel have been used to estimate an age at M1 emergence, as have their relatively small cranial capacities, and both approaches conclude 2804 M. C. Dean enamel thickness (microns) (a) 2500 2250 2000 1750 1500 1250 1000 750 500 250 0 African apes Homo sapiens C. torquatus enamel thickness (microns) (b) 2500 2250 2000 1750 1500 1250 1000 750 500 250 0 Review African apes Homo sapiens Early Homo Australopiths 100 200 300 400 500 600 enamel formation time (days) 700 Figure 5. (a) Rates of occlusal enamel formation are slowest in modern humans, intermediate in African great apes and fastest in the sooty mangabey (Cercocebus torquatus), irrespective of how thick enamel eventually grows. These distinct trajectories appear to reflect the general grade shifts in maturational rate among primates. (b) None of the early australopithecines or fossils attributed to early Homo (H. habilis and H. erectus) have slow occlusal enamel formation trajectories resembling those in modern humans (Dean et al. 2001). these ages fall within the range known for living great apes (Bromage & Dean 1985; Smith 1986, 1989). However, within this generally ape-like picture, we should expect as much variation among early hominids as we see today among living apes and there may be hints of this in the fossil record. Despite similar infant diets, great ape deciduous tooth wear patterns vary with early or late first intake of supplementary food during the weaning period and seem also to reflect their longer or shorter inter-birth intervals (Aiello et al. 1991). Clearly, early hominids had diverse diets and this is reflected in the thinner or thicker enamel (both deciduous and permanent) of some australopithecines. Australopithecines at around 3.5 million years (e.g. LH-2, Australopithecus afarensis) show much less deciduous tooth wear at chronological age ca 3.5 years than later ones at ca 2 million years (e.g. Taung, Australopithecus africanus). But some juveniles that are chronologically younger (ca 2.5 years) show even greater degrees of dentine exposure (e.g. KNM-ER 1477, Paranthropus boisei ) despite having thicker enamel to counter this. It is difficult to attribute all of these differences in deciduous tooth wear to their infant diets and equally likely there were contrasting life-history strategies among australopithecines that mirrored both the availability and quality of food and their evolutionary history (Aiello et al. 1991). In some late surviving australopithecines, heavy Proc. R. Soc. B (2006) deciduous tooth wear early in life may have been linked to early first intake of supplementary food, early weaning and shorter inter-birth intervals. Significant adaptive changes in reproductive strategy and life history may have kept up with pressure to survive over millions of years. After 2 million years, only the ‘robust’ australopithecines (P. boisei from East Africa and P. robustus from South Africa) remain in the fossil record and these were extinct by 1 million years ago. Cumulative trajectories describing enamel growth over time in monkeys, apes and humans (figure 5a,b) appear to show evidence of the major grade shifts in the pace of general somatic growth (Dean et al. 2001). All early hominid enamel growth trajectories fall among the African great apes and have shorter enamel crown formation times compared with those in modern humans (Beynon & Wood 1987; Dean et al. 2001; Reid & Dean 2005; Lacruz et al. in press). This alone suggests that dental development as a whole took less time than in modern humans. M1 emergence times for Homo erectus have been predicted from average cranial capacity estimates (Smith 1992; Smith & Tompkins 1995) and are a close match for those derived from dental histology. The combined evidence from both the enamel and the root extension rates in a single H. erectus specimen from Java (S7-37) indicates that M1 emergence times were close to 4.5 years rather than 6 years in modern humans and that M2 emergence times were nearer to 8 years than 12 years (Dean et al. 2001). On the one hand, this is the first evidence for a significant change from the broadly ape-like life-history profile for ca 17 million years. On the other hand, it seems that even at this relatively late stage of human evolution there was still on average a faster life-history profile than in modern humans today, but one that had slowed in parallel with a significant increase in brain size. The link between brain size, tooth development and a slower life-history profile is a clear one but a curious one (Smith 1989; Allman & Hasenstaub 1999; Godfrey et al. 2001). Kelley (2004) has argued that the whole changing life-history profile during primate evolution has influenced brain growth and that it is this that has ultimately facilitated cognitive evolution. Longer periods of embryonic development have extended many phases of human brain growth, including glial cell mitosis, dendritic growth, synapogenesis, dendritic pruning and myelination (McKinney 2002). Just three or four extra days of embryonic development between E40 and E43 result in three or four more rounds of founder cell mitosis in the brain, which is enough to account for the 10-fold larger human cortex than of a macaque monkey (Rakic 1995; Hill & Walsh 2005). Brain size as well as brain complexity is, therefore, time dependent as is dental development and simply yet another part of the life-history package (Allman & Hasenstaub 1999). Brain size might not then have been driven solely by selection for cognitive ability. The sequence of events may have begun with a shift in life-history strategy and growth of a bigger more complex brain, which then increased the likelihood of selection for increased cognitive ability. And in the end, increased cognitive ability is bound to contribute towards a further reduction in adult mortality rates (Kelley 2004). What then of Neanderthals whose brains were bigger than modern humans, but whose life-history profile is described either as at the lower limit of the modern human range on the basis of shorter enamel formation times Review (Dean et al. 1986; Stringer et al. 1990; Tompkins 1996; Guatelli-Steinberg et al. 2005) and patterns of molar tooth wear (Wolpoff 1979; Caspari & Lee 2004) or as being distinct and 15% shorter than modern humans (Ramirez Rozzi & Bermudez de Castro 2004)? Clearly, the late surviving Neanderthals were under considerable demographic pressure from both sudden colder, drier climatic conditions and from incoming biologically and behaviourally modern human populations (Mellars 2006). Neanderthals are likely to have experienced high levels of infant and adult mortality. It makes no biological sense for any primate to take 18–20 years to grow up only to die as a young adult (Trinkaus & Tompkins 1990), but then again the Neanderthals did not survive. Understanding the demise of the Neanderthals may ultimately come from a better understanding of our own survival. At present, it is simply not clear when and where a prolonged modern human-like growth period evolved, nor, as with large brains, if this arose independently in Neanderthals and modern humans (Dean 2000). The evidence from life-history theory suggests that a reduction in adult mortality rates is likely to have played a big role, but providing evidence for this in the fossil record is fraught with difficulty. Caspari & Lee (2004) calculated the ratio of older to younger adults in successive time periods through the hominid fossil record. They concluded that while there was a significant and successive increase in longevity between all the groups they examined, adult survivorship increased most dramatically among the modern humans of the Early Upper Palaeolithic in Europe compared with Neanderthals (see also Trinkaus & Tompkins 1990). A prolonged life-history profile and a long period of dependency are costly, and it may be that modern humans have evolved mechanisms for offsetting these costs through increased rates of reproduction. Slow human growth between weaning and puberty reduces the energetic demands on parents with multiple dependents (Gurven & Walker 2006). Delayed maturation, slower growth, shorter inter-birth intervals, increased fertility and longevity are all associated with early cessation of reproduction in modern humans and the menopause (Wich et al. 2004). These observations have implications for the importance of investment by older generations in their children’s offspring as a selective advantage in ensuring their survival and thereby contributing to population increase (Hawkes et al. 1998; O’Connell et al. 1999). Effectively, we may have quite recently, perhaps in parallel with our prolonged life-history profile, evolved mechanisms for escaping some of the constraints imposed by extremely slow maturation (Allman & Hasenstaub 1999). 8. CONCLUSIONS Primates are ecologically diverse and achieve a balance between fertility and mortality rates by optimizing energy resources in many different ways. Major shifts in lifehistory profile seem to have been rare during the last 20 million years of primate evolution, but even today there is considerable variation on the broad ape-like life-history theme as there was always likely to have been during the early stages of human evolution. It seems our own extended life-history profile may have evolved quite recently and primarily in response to low adult mortality rates. However, this may now be linked to novel ways of Proc. R. Soc. B (2006) M. C. Dean 2805 investing more time and energy in our young, but at the same time increasing reproductive rates by reducing interbirth intervals. The future prospects for retrieving more about primate life histories from fossil teeth are promising. Moreover, the better we come to understand the processes involved in controlling rates of enamel and dentine formation, the closer we get to defining the mechanisms that underlie evolutionary changes in developing primate dentitions. I am grateful for the helpful comments of three referees. I thank The Leverhulme Trust and The Royal Society for grants awarded to me for research on comparative dental development. I am grateful to the National Museums of Kenya for access to fossil primate material in their care. REFERENCES Adoutte, A. 2000 Small but mighty timekeepers. Nature 408, 37–38. (doi:10.1038/35040669) Aiello, L. C., Montgomery, C. & Dean, M. C. 1991 The natural history of deciduous tooth attrition in hominoids. J. Hum. Evol. 21, 397–412. (doi:10.1016/00472484(91)90114-B) Allman, J. & Hasenstaub, A. 1999 Brains, maturation time and parenting. Neurobiol. Aging 20, 447–454. (doi:10. 1016/S0197-4580(99)00076-7) Ando, S., Aizawa, K., Nakashima, T., Shinbo, K., Sanka, Y., Kiyokawa, K. & Oshima, S. 1965 Studies on the consecutive survey of succedaneous and permanent dentition in the Japanese children. J. Nihon Univ. Sch. Dent. 7, 141–181. Ashmole, N. P. 1963 The regulation of numbers of tropical oceanic birds. Ibis 103b, 458–473. Ashton, E. H. & Spence, T. F. 1958 Age changes in the cranial capacity and foramen magnum of hominoids. Proc. Zool. Soc. Lond. 130, 169–181. Beynon, A. D. & Wood, B. A. 1987 Patterns and rates of molar crown formation times in East African fossil hominids. Nature 326, 493–496. (doi:10.1038/326493a0) Beynon, A. D., Dean, M. C., Leakey, M. G., Reid, D. J. & Walker, A. 1998 Comparative dental development and microstructure of Proconsul teeth from Rusinga Island, Kenya. J. Hum. Evol. 35, 163–209. (doi:10.1006/jhev. 1998.0230) Boyde, A. 1979 Carbonate concentration, crystal centres, core dissolution, caries, cross striation, circadian rhythms and compositional contrast in the SEM. J. Dent. Res. 58b, 981–983. Boyde, A. 1989 Enamel. In Handbook of microscopic anatomy, vol. 2. Teeth (ed. A. Oksche & L. Vollrath), pp. 309–473. Berlin, Germany: Springer. Boyde, A. 1990 Developmental interpretations of dental microstructure. In Primate life history and evolution: monographs in primatology, vol. 14 (ed. C. Jean De Rousseau), pp. 229–267. New York, NY: Wiley-Liss. Bromage, T. G. 1991 Enamel incremental periodicity in the pig-tailed macaque; a polychrome fluorescent labelling study of dental hard tissues. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 86, 205–214. (doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330860209) Bromage, T. G. & Dean, M. C. 1985 Re-evaluation of the age at death on immature fossil hominids. Nature 317, 525–527. (doi:10.1038/317525a0) Caspari, R. & Lee, S.-H. 2004 Older age becomes common late in human evolution. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 10 895–10 900. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0402857101) Catón, J., Bringas, P. & Zeichner-David, M. 2005 IGFs increase enamel formation by inducing expression of enamel mineralizing specific genes. Arch. Oral Biol. 50, 123–129. (doi:10.1016/j.archoralbio.2004.11.012) 2806 M. C. Dean Review Charnov, E. L. R. 1991 Evolution of life history variation among female mammals. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 88, 1134–1137. (doi:10.1073/pnas.88.4.1134) Charnov, E. L. R. 1993 Life-history invariants. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Charnov, E. L. & Berrigan, D. 1993 Why do female primates have such long lifespans and so few babies? Or life in the slow lane. Evol. Anthropol. 1, 191–194. (doi:10.1002/evan. 1360010604) Craddock, H. L. & Youngson, C. C. 2004 Eruptive movement—the current state of knowledge. Br. Dent. J. 197, 385–391. (doi:10.1038/sj.bdj.4811712) Darling, A. I. & Levers, B. G. H. 1976 The pattern of eruption. Arch. Oral Biol. 20, 89–96. (doi:10.1016/00039969(75)90159-4) Dean, M. C. 1987 Growth layers and incremental markings in hard tissues: a review of the literature and some preliminary observations about enamel structure in Paranthropus boisei. J. Hum. Evol. 16, 157–172. (doi:10. 1016/0047-2484(87)90074-1) Dean, M. C. 1993 Daily rates of dentine formation in macaque tooth roots. Int. J. Osteoarchaeol. 3, 199–206. (doi:10.1002/oa.1390030306) Dean, M. C. 1995 The nature and periodicity of incremental lines in primate dentine and their relationship to periradicular bands in OH 16 (Homo habilis). In Structure, function and evolution of teeth. Ninth Int. Symp. on Dental Morphology (ed. J. Moggi-Cecchi), pp. 239–265. Florence, Italy: International Institute for the Study of Man. Dean, M. C. 1998 Comparative observations on the spacing of short-period (von Ebner’s) lines in dentine. Arch. Oral Biol. 43, 1009–1021. (doi:10.1016/S0003-9969(98)00069-7) Dean, M. C. 2000 Progress in understanding hominoid dental development. J. Anat. 197, 77–101. (doi:10.1046/ j.1469-7580.2000.19710077.x) Dean, M. C. & Leakey, M. G. 2004 Enamel and dentine development and the life history profile of Victoriapithecus macinnesi from Maboko Island, Kenya. Ann. Anat. 186, 405–412. Dean, M. C. & Wood, B. A. 1981 Developing pongid dentition and its use for ageing individual crania in comparative cross-sectional growth studies. Folia Primatol. 36, 111–127. Dean, M. C. & Wood, B. A. 2003 A digital radiographic atlas of the great apes skull and dentition. In Digital archives of human paleobiology 3. Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche (ed. L. Bondioli & R. Macchiarelli). Milano, Italy: ADS Solutions. ISBN 88-87563-02-0. Dean, M. C., Stringer, C. B. & Bromage, T. G. 1986 Age at death of the Neanderthal child from Devils Tower, Gibraltar and the implications for studies of general growth and development in Neanderthals. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 70, 301–309. (doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330700305) Dean, M. C., Leakey, M. G., Reid, D. J., Schrenk, F., Schwartz, G. T., Stringer, C. & Walker, A. 2001 Growth processes in teeth distinguish modern humans from Homo erectus and earlier hominins. Nature 414, 628–631. (doi:10.1038/414628a) Demisch, A. & Wartmann, P. 1956 Calcification of the mandibular third molar and its relation to skeletal and chronological age in children. Child Dev. 27, 459–473. DenBesten, P. K., Machule, D., Gallagher, R., Grayson, W. M., Mathews, C. & Filvaroff, E. 2001 The effect of TGF-b2 on dentin apposition and hardness in transgenic mice. Adv. Dent. Res. 15, 39–41. Dirks, W. 1998 Histological reconstruction of dental development and age at death in a juvenile gibbon (Hylobates lar). J. Hum. Evol. 35, 411–425. (doi:10.1006/ jhev.1997.0185) Proc. R. Soc. B (2006) Dirks, W. 2003 Effect of diet on dental development in four species of catarrhine primates. Am. J. Primatol. 61, 29–40. (doi:10.1002/ajp.10106) Dirks, W. & Bowman, J.E. In press. Life history and dental development in four species of catarrhine primates. J. Hum. Evol. Feasby, W. H. 1981 A radiographic study of dental eruption. Am. J. Orthod. 80, 554–560. (doi:10.1016/00029416(81)90250-5) FitzGerald, C. M. 1998 Do enamel microstructures have regular time dependencies? Conclusions from the literature and a large scale study. J. Hum. Evol. 35, 371–386. (doi:10.1006/jhev.1998.0232) Furseth Klinge, R., Dean, M. C., Gunnaes, A. & Leakey, M. G. 2005 Microscopic structure and mineral distribution in tooth and periodontal tissues in a robust australopithecine fossil hominid from Koobi Fora, Kenya. In Current Trends in Dental Morphology Research, 13th Int. Symp. on Dental Morphology (ed. E. Ża˛dzińska), pp. 233–242. Łódż, Poland: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego. Gaunt, W. A. 1967 Quantitative aspects of developing tooth germs. J. Dent. Res. 46, 851–857. Gleiser, I. & Hunt, E. E. 1955 The permanent mandibular molar: its calcification, eruption and decay. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 13, 253–283. (doi:10.1002/ajpa. 1330130206) Godfrey, L. R., Samonds, K. E., Jungers, W. L. & Sutherland, M. R. 2001 Teeth, brains and primate life histories. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 114, 192–214. (doi:10.1002/10968644(200103)114:3!192::AID-AJPA1020O3.0.CO;2-Q) Guatelli-Steinberg, D., Reid, D. J. & Bishop, T. A. 2005 Anterior tooth growth periods in Neandertals were comparable to those in modern humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 14 197–14 202. (doi:10.1073/pnas. 0503108102) Gurven, M. & Walker, R. 2006 Energetic demand of multiple dependents and the evolution of slow human growth. Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 835–841. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3380) Harrison, T. 1987 The phylogenetic relationships of the early catarrhine primates: a review of the current evidence. J. Hum. Evol. 16, 41–80. (doi:10.1016/0047-2484(87) 90060-1) Harvey, P. H. & Clutton-Brock, T. H. 1985 Life history variation in primates. Evolution 39, 559–581. (doi:10. 2307/2408653) Harvey, P. H. & Nee, S. 1991 How to live like a mammal. Nature 350, 23–24. (doi:10.1038/350023a0) Harvey, P. H. & Zammuto, R. M. 1985 Patterns of mortality and age at first reproduction in natural populations of mammals. Nature 315, 319–320. (doi:10.1038/315319a0) Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., Blurton Jones, N. G., Charnov, E. L. & Alvarez, H. 1998 Grandmothering, menopause and the evolution of human life histories. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 1336–1339. (doi:10.1073/pnas.95.3.1336) Hill, R. S. & Walsh, A. 2005 Molecular insights into human brain evolution. Nature 437, 64–67. (doi:10.1038/ nature04103) Hill, K., Boesch, C., Goodall, J., Pusey, A., Williams, J. & Wrangham, R. 2001 Mortality rates among wild chimpanzees. J. Hum. Evol. 40, 437–450. (doi:10.1006/jhev. 2001.0469) Inoue, Y. & Suzuki, Y. 1992 Study on eruption of maxillary permanent incisors using panoramic tomography. Pediatr. Dent. J. 2, 83–92. Irving, J. T. 1963 The sudanophil material at sites of calcification. Arch. Oral Biol. 8, 583–590. Jones, S. J. & Boyde, A. 1984 Ultrastructure of dentine and dentinogenesis. In Dentine and dentinogenesis, vol. 2 (ed. A. Linde), pp. 81–134. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Review Keller, L. & Genoud, M. 1997 Extraordinary lifespans in ants: a test of evolutionary theories of ageing. Nature 387, 958–960. (doi:10.1038/40130) Kelley, J. 1997 Paleobiological and phylogenetic significance of life history in Miocene hominoids. In Function, phylogeny, and fossils: Miocene hominoid evolution and adaptations (ed. D. R. Begun, C. V. Ward & M. D. Rose), pp. 173–208. New York, NY: Plenum Press. Kelley, J. 2002 Life history evolution in Miocene and extant apes. In Human evolution through developmental change (ed. N. Minugh-Purvis & K. J. McNamara), pp. 223–248. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. Kelley, J. 2004 Life history and cognitive evolution in the apes. In The evolution of thought: evolutionary origins of great ape intelligence (ed. A. E. Russon & D. R. Begun), pp. 280–297. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Kelley, J. & Smith, T. M. 2003 Age at first molar emergence in early Miocene Afropithecus turkanensis and life-history evolution in the Hominoidea. J. Hum. Evol. 44, 307–329. (doi:10.1016/S0047-2484(03)00005-8) Kelley, J., Dean, M. C. & Reid, D. J. 2001 Molar growth in the late Miocene hominoid, Dryopithecus laietanus. In Dental morphology 2001 (ed. A. Brook), pp. 123–134. Sheffield, UK: Sheffield Academic Press. Kozlowski, J. & Weiner, J. 1997 Interspecific allometries are by-products of body size optimization. Am. Nat. 149, 352–380. (doi:10.1086/285994) Lacruz, R. S., Ramirez Rozzi, F. & Bromage, T. G. In press. Variation in enamel development of South African fossil hominids. J. Hum. Evol. Lesot, H., Lisi, S., Peterkova, R., Peterka, M., Mitola, V. & Ruch, J. 2001 Epigenetic signals during odontoblast differentiation. Adv. Dent. Res. 15, 8–13. Linde, A. 1984 Noncollagenous proteins and proteoglycans in dentinogenesis. In Dentine and dentinogenesis, vol. 2 (ed. A. Linde), pp. 55–92. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Linde, A. 1985 Dynamic aspects of dentinogenesis. In The chemistry and biology of mineralised tissues (ed. W. T. Butler), pp. 344–355. Birmingham, AL: Ebsco Media Inc. Lutz, R. A. & Rhoads, D. C. 1977 Anaerobiosis and a theory of growth line formation. Science 198, 1222–1227. McKinney, M. L. 2002 Brain evolution by stretching the global mitotic clock of development. In Human evolution through developmental change (ed. N. Minugh-Purvis & K. J. McNamara), pp. 173–188. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press. Mellars, P. 2006 A new radiocarbon revolution and the dispersal of modern humans in Eurasia. Nature 439, 931–935. (doi:10.1038/nature04521) Miani, A. & Miani, C. 1971 Circadian advancement rhythm of the calcification front in dog dentin. Minerva Stomatol. 20, 169–178. Moore, R. Y. 1999 A clock for the ages. Science 284, 2102–2103. (doi:10.1126/science.284.5423.2102) Nargolwalla, M. C., Begun, D. R., Reid, D. J. & Kordos, L. 2005 Dental development in Anapithecus hernyaki. J. Hum. Evol. 49, 99–121. (doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.03. 008) O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K. & Blurton Jones, N. G. 1999 Grandmothering and the evolution of Homo erectus. J. Hum. Evol. 36, 461–485. (doi:10.1006/jhev.1998.0285) Ohtsuka, M. & Shinoda, H. 1995 Ontogeny of circadian dentinogenesis in the rat incisor. Arch. Oral Biol. 40, 481–485. (doi:10.1016/0003-9969(95)00002-7) Ohtsuka-Isoya, M., Hayashi, H. & Shinoda, H. 2001 Effect of suprachiasmatic nucleus lesion on circadian dentin increment in rats. Am. J. Physiol. Reg. Comp. Physiol. 280, R1364–R1370. Proc. R. Soc. B (2006) M. C. Dean 2807 Okada, M. 1943 Hard tissues of animal body: highly interesting details of Nippon studies in periodic patterns of hard tissues are described. Shanghai evening post special edition, health, recreation and medical progress, pp. 15–31. Promislow, D. E. L. & Harvey, P. H. 1990 Living fast and dying young: a comparative analysis of life-history variation among mammals. J. Zool. Soc. Lond. 220, 417–437. Promislow, D. E. L. & Harvey, P. H. 1991 Mortailty rates and the evolution of mammal life histories. Acta Oecol. 12, 119–137. Rakic, P. 1995 A small step for the cell, a giant leap for mankind: a hypothesis of neocortical expansion during evolution. Trends Neurosci. 18, 383–388. (doi:10.1016/ 0166-2236(95)93934-P) Ramirez Rozzi, F. V. & Bermudez de Castro, J. M. 2004 Surprisingly rapid growth in Neanderthals. Nature 428, 936–939. (doi:10.1038/nature02428) Reid, D. J. & Dean, M. C. 2005 Variation in modern human enamel formation times. J. Hum. Evol. 50, 329–346. (doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.09.003) Risinger, R. K. & Proffitt, W. R. 1996 Contiuous overnight observation of human premolar eruption. Arch. Oral Biol. 41, 779–789. (doi:10.1016/S0003-9969(96)00062-3) Rosser, H., Boyde, A. & Stewart, A. D. G. 1967 Preliminary observations of the calcium concentration in developing enamel assessed by scanning electron-probe X-ray emission microanalysis. Arch. Oral Biol. 12, 431–440. (doi:10. 1016/0003-9969(67)90018-0) Schour, I. & Poncher, H. G. 1937 Rate of apposition of enamel and dentine, measured by the effect of acute fluorosis. Am. J. Dis. Child. 54, 757–776. Schultz, A. H. 1940 Growth and development of the chimpanzee. Contrib. Embryol. Carnegie Inst. 28, 1–63. Schultz, A. H. 1969 The life of primates. New York, NY: Universe Books. Schumaker, D. B. & El Hadary, M. S. 1960 Roentgenographic study of eruption. J. Am. Dent. Assoc. 61, 535–541. Schwartz, G. T., Samonds, K. E., Godfrey, L. R., Jungers, W. L. & Simons, E. L. 2002 Dental microstructure and life history in subfossil Malagasy lemurs. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 6124–6129. (doi:10.1073/pnas.092685099) Scrutton, C. T. 1964 Periodicity in Devonian coral growth. Palaeontology 7, 552–558. Shellis, R. P. 1984 Variations in growth of the enamel crown in human teeth and a possible relationship between growth and enamel structure. Arch. Oral Biol. 29, 697–705. (doi:10.1016/0003-9969(84)90175-4) Shinoda, H. 1984 Faithful records of biological rhythms in dental hard tissues. Chem. Today 162, 34–40. Sloan, A. J., Matthews, J. B. & Smith, A. J. 2000 Transforming growth factor-b isoform expression in mature human molar teeth. Histochem. J. 31, 247–252. (doi:10.1023/A:1004007202404) Smith, B. H. 1986 Dental development in Australopithecus and early Homo. Nature 323, 327–330. (doi:10.1038/ 323327a0) Smith, B. H. 1989 Dental development as a measure of life history in primates. Evolution 43, 683–688. (doi:10.2307/ 2409073) Smith, B. H. 1991 Dental development and the evolution of life history in Hominidae. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 86, 157–174. (doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330860206) Smith, B. H. 1992 Life history and the evolution of human maturation. Evol. Anthropol. 1, 134–142. (doi:10.1002/ evan.1360010406) Smith, T. M. 2006 Experimental determination of the periodicity of incremental features in enamel. J. Anat. 208, 99–113. (doi:10.1111/j.1469-7580.2006.00499.x) 2808 M. C. Dean Review Smith, B. H. & Tompkins, R. L. 1995 Towards a life history of the Hominidae. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 24, 257–279. (doi:10.1146/annurev.an.24.100195.001353) Smith, C. E., McKee, M. D. & Nanci, A. 1987 Cyclic induction and rapid movement of sequential waves of new smooth-ended ameloblast modulation bands in rat incisors as visualized by polychrome fluorescent labelling and GBHA-staining of maturing enamel. Adv. Dent. Res. 1, 162–175. Smith, B. H., Crummett, T. L. & Brandt, K. L. 1994 Ages of eruption of primate teeth: a compendium for aging individuals and comparing life histories. Yearb. Phys. Anthropol. 37, 177–231. (doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330370608) Smith, T. M., Reid, D. J. & Sirianni, J. E. 2006 The accuracy of histological assessments of dental development and age at death. J. Anat. 208, 125–138. (doi:10.1111/j.14697580.2006.00500.x) Stack, M. V. 1967 Vertical growth rates of deciduous teeth. J. Dent. Res. 46, 879–882. Stearns, S. 1992 The evolution of life histories. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Stringer, C. B., Dean, M. C. & Martin, R. D. 1990 A comparative study of cranial and dental development within a recent British sample and among Neandertals. In Primate life history and evolution: monographs in primatology, vol. 14 (ed. C. Jean De Rousseau), pp. 115–152. New York, NY: Wiley-Liss. Takeshima, T., Suda, N., Ishikawa, M., Takagi, Y. & Ohyama, K. 2004 Relationship between formation and eruption of permanent mandibular buccal dentition and vertical height of the mandibular body: longitudinal study of Japanese boys from 4 to 9 years of age. World J. Orthod. 5, 332–339. Thesleff, I., Keranen, S. & Jernvall, J. 2001 Enamel knots as signalling centers linking tooth morphogenesis and odontoblast differentiation. Adv. Dent. Res. 15, 14–18. Thomas, H. F. 1995 Root formation. Int. J. Dev. Biol. 39, 231–237. Tompkins, R. L. 1996 Relative dental development of Upper Pleistocene hominids compared to human population Proc. R. Soc. B (2006) variability. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 99, 103–118. (doi:10. 1002/(SICI)1096-8644(199601)99:1!103::AID-AJPA6O 3.0.CO;2-1) Trinkaus, E. & Tompkins, R. L. 1990 The Neandertal life cycle: the possibility, probability and perceptibility of contrasts with recent humans. In Primate life history and evolution: monographs in primatology, vol. 14 (ed. C. Jean De Rousseau), pp. 153–180. New York, NY: Wiley-Liss. Tziafas, D., Smith, A. J. & Lesot, H. 2000 Designing new treatment strategies in vital pulp therapy. J. Dent. 28, 77–92. (doi:10.1016/S0300-5712(99)00047-0) Veis, A., Sharkey, M. & Dickenson, I. 1977 Non-collagenous proteins of bone and dentine extracellular matrix and their role in organised mineral deposition. In Calcium binding proteins and calcium function (ed. R. H. Wasserman, R. A. Corradino & E. Carafoli), pp. 409–418. New York, NY: Elsevier. Walker, A. 1997 Proconsul: function and phylogeny. In Function, phylogeny and fossils: Miocene hominoid evolution and adaptation (ed. D. R. Begun, C. V. Ward & M. D. Rose), pp. 209–224. New York, NY: Plenum Press. Walker, A. & Shipman, P. 2005 The ape in the tree: an intellectual and natural history of Proconsul. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. Watts, E. S. 1990 Evolutionary trends in primate growth and development.In Primate life history and evolution: monographs in primatology, vol. 14 (ed. C. Jean De Rousseau), pp. 89–104. New York, NY: Wiley-Liss. Wells, J. W. 1963 Coral growth and geochronometry. Nature 197, 948–950. Wich, S. A., Utami-Atmoko, S. S., Mitra Setia, T., Rijksen, H. D., Schurmann, C., van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. & van Schaik, C. P. 2004 Life history of wild Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii ). J. Hum. Evol. 47, 385–398. (doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.08.006) Wolpoff, M. H. 1979 The Krapina dental remains. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 50, 67–114. (doi:10.1002/ajpa. 1330500110) Wuthier, R. E. 1984 Lipids in dentinogenesis. In Dentine and dentinogenesis, vol. 2 (ed. A. Linde), pp. 93–106. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.