WORKSHOP 2 Friday 25 March, Wellcome Trust Reading Faces: Physiognomy and Facial Typing ABSTRACTS LARA PERRY 19thC cartes de visites and the question of photographic likeness The carte de visite - small, multiple portrait photographs - soared to popularity amongst all classes of Britons in the early 1860s. This phenomenon of mass culture is of interest to its historians typically in its social dimension, but not in terms of its visual practice, since carte de visite are unprepossessing as objects: they are characteristically small, flimsy, repetitive and predictable. Even contemporary commentators tended to treat the iconic or indexical likeness produced in the carte de visite as inadequate at best, a judgment which was affirmed by variety of sharp practices and deceptions circulated around and through carte photography during the period. If the carte de visite did not achieve its likeness at least primarily through its convincing reproduction of the sitter's appearance, what were the mechanisms of likeness that did function in the carte? Two notable aspects of photographic culture in the 1860s - the performative/theatrical mode that is chiefly associated with 'art' photography, and the repetitive nature of carte photography, are here presented as alternative modes of likeness that functioned in the context of 1860s photographic portraiture, as documented in two carte albums held by the National Portrait Gallery. TIM VALENTINE (Goldsmiths, University of London) Face–space: A psychological perspective of similarity and difference between faces. Face processing poses a considerable challenge. To recognise a face we need to be able to distinguish it from many similar faces, seen under widely variable viewing conditions (e.g. changes in lighting, expression, viewpoint and age). To do this, we make some strong assumptions about faces. These assumptions can be revealed by facial illusions that will be demonstrated. Not only do we need to distinguish between different faces, but similarities help us to make judgements about people. For example, we can judge age, ethnicity, health and emotional state. The face-space framework, which helps us to think about the similarity structure of faces and its mental representation, will be introduced. Psychological insights into the perception of caricature, attractiveness and ethnicity will be discussed. VICKI BRUCE (Newcastle University) Resemblance and Identity My own interest in face recognition was stimulated in the mid 1970s by two seemingly contradictory observations. First, that memory for faces in laboratory conditions was very good – much better than for other apparently equally complex and meaningful patterns; second, that memory for faces by witnesses to crimes was very poor, and a large number of mistakes were made that led to the wrongful conviction of innocent people falsely identified. Much of my work in the 35 years since then has been aimed at understanding this paradox and also at exploring ways to improve how images of faces are used to identify criminals. In this talk I will describe how research into the identification of people in line-ups and the use of CCTV images shows how easily confused people are by a resemblance between face images, and how such resemblance can lead to a false assumption of identity. In contrast, one reason why it is difficult to get witnesses to ‘recall’ recognisable faces using composite systems (e.g. E-fit) is that these systems seem, at best, able to yield images of general ‘type’ resemblance, whereas what is needed for an investigation is an image that can be identified as someone in particular. However, a sense of resemblance can be very useful too. I will describe how some of the work on the new composite system ‘Evo-Fit’ has tried to build upon participant-witnesses’ feelings of resemblance to generate composite images more likely to be identified by those familiar with them. PETER HAMILTON (Maison des Sciences de l'Homme, Paris) "Policing the Face: Photography, Portraiture and Social Order" The invention of photography in the 1830s promised to unite art and science and create both novel and more accurate methods of depiction and documentation. Despite the difficulties of early processes for photographers and sitters alike, it was photographic portraiture's rapid rise to popularity that encouraged its diffusion to other spheres. The portrait photograph was rapidly adopted by the new sciences and technologies to provide empirical evidence for theories of evolution, phrenology, racial types, insanity and criminality. A system of scrutiny or 'surveillance' of the face - and to a lesser extent the body - emerged in the latter part of the nineteenth century. It has had a lasting and profound influence on society, culture and the arts ever since. This paper will discuss the multiple influences on photography during its emergent period, and trace some of the key implications in the development of photographic and organisational techniques for social surveillance through facial recognition. A key feature of the photographic surveillance process has been the creation of organisations and bureaucratic processes that "rationalise" the production and use of such images for purposes of social control. In contrast to the "official" institutional uses of portraiture as an (imperfect) device of social control, the "artistic" uses of photography as a method of depicting beauty, character, personality, social identity, etc, have proved to be unwilling bedfellows in the processes of surveillance. The paradoxes to which this leads will also be touched upon to conclude the paper. SIBYLLE ERLE (Bishop Grosseteste University College, Lincoln) “Read the text, look at the image, develop your ‘Physiognomical Discernment’: Problems of Representation in Johann Caspar Lavater’s Essays on Physiognomy” Johann Caspar Lavater’s physiognomy was first published in German (Physiognomische Fragmente) in the 1770s and then quickly abridged, revised, extended and translated into Dutch, French and English (Essays on Physiognomy). The physiognomy gained, in the first instance, in portraits. Lavater was an avid collector and included more and more engravings; and while introducing his new - scientific – approach to physiognomy, he not only commented on long-established face-reading traditions but also experimented with both methods and machines of likeness-taking and even devised a teaching programme. That the extra material made teaching ‘Physiognomical Discernment’ virtually impossible, didn’t concern him, because he believed that if readers looked at a representation of a true, that is, accurate likeness, they would feel exactly what he had felt when he examined the plate. Readers, according to Lavater, would be subjected to the same bodily experience and, therefore, agree with his physiognomical judgement. What complicated the development of “Physiognomical Discernment” as well as the so-called moments of recognition is that Lavater continually complained about the engravers and artists working for him: he had to put up with lack of precision and much incompetence. The two main, competing English translations - on which this paper will focus – were published from 1789 onwards and are referred to as the Holcroft translation (named after Thomas Holcroft) and the Hunter translation (named after Henry Hunter). According to Lavater’s friend, the Swiss-born painter Henry Fuseli, the Hunter translation was the best ‘Lavater’ to date: the plates were excellent, because they had been done with the ‘most discriminating exactness and attention’. When taking a closer look at Fuseli’s contributions to the Hunter translation, however, it emerges that he not only saw to correcting the mistakes of the French edition but also made some small adjustment which enhanced as well as undermined Lavater’s physiognomical readings. NATASHA RUIZ-GóMEZ (University of Essex) Individual and Multiple: Rodin and Physiognomy This paper proposes a new reading of the sculpture of Auguste Rodin (18401917) based on an examination of his ambivalent relationship to the theory of physiognomy and its project of reading individuals’ physical characteristics for morality and intellect. Rodin used physiognomy and the related study of regional type to motivate sculptures—from intimate studies of hands to major commissions, such as the Burghers of Calais (1895) and the Monument to Balzac (1898)—but also subverted both discourses by forcing multivalent readings of the human form. Rodin’s preparations for both the Burghers of Calais and the Monument to Balzac involved intensive research on physiognomy and regional type—he studied natives of Calais and of Touraine, where Honoré de Balzac (1799– 1850) was born—but the completed works resist physiognomic legibility. The visage of the Monument to Balzac is grotesquely distorted, its body enshrouded in a long cloak. The figure serves as a monument to genius rather than as a testament to the validity of the well-known theories that informed its genesis. In the final version of the Burghers of Calais, Rodin used the same head for three of the figures, simultaneously exploiting and flouting physiognomy’s popular authority—the innovation that Leo Steinberg calls ‘multiplication’ announces Rodin’s modernity as it undermines the physiognomic project. Considering the works in the framework of physiognomy and regional type reveals that the two discourses were factors that prompted Rodin’s experiments with replication. In other words, the challenge that Rodin perceived in the pseudo-scientific discourse of physiognomy helped push him to become the ‘sculptor of modern life’. KAREN INGHAM (Swansea Metropolitan University) A Type of Uncertainty: an interdisciplinary response to the UCL Francis Galton Archive The Victorian polymath Francis Galton is a controversial figure yet the importance of his legacy to 21st century medical and surveillance technologies cannot be over stated, from finger prints to retinal identity scans, reproductive technologies, bioinformatics and genetics. But Galton is also of interest to those in the arts and humanities, not least because of the uneasy beauty of his photographic composite typologies. For the past year I have been in discussion with Galton curator Natasha McEnroe and have had access to the Galton archive. With the intent of creating an interdisciplinary artwork for the 2011 Galton centenary, I have focused on two aspects of his prodigious research: the composite typologies and his iris studies as manifested in the glass eye collection framed in moulded metal. In response to the typologies I am developing a short film and a series of large-scale photographic ‘thought portraits’, with the working title ‘Variance’, a term derived from Galton’s pioneering work on statistics. The portraits, which I refer to as oxymoronic typologies, incorporate confocal and scanning electron microscopy images of brain activity. These were made during my Science and Art Research Fellowship with Cardiff Neuroscience Research Group, and are characteristic of a new generation of interpretively complex brain imaging ‘snapshots’. Photographic portraits of friends and family are layered with these purportedly ‘living thought’ images creating a visual effect similar to Galton’s composites. The histories, philosophies and narratives of biomedical imaging in relation to identity are brought into question, challenging traditional notions of portraiture by referencing those aspects of the self that reside below the surface. ‘Variance’ plays with these questions of identity and is situated in the gaps between knowledge, where speculation and interpretation contest certainty. The glass eye collection is reminiscent of Isaac Oliver’s 1600 portrait of Elizabeth the 1st with her rainbow cloak of many eyes. The cloak is a metaphor for surveillance and I am designing a contemporary cloak incorporating Galton’s glass eyes; a piece of wearable technology that acts as a surveying device. I will talk about these artworks and show examples of previous Galton informed practice during the workshop. RESEARCH QUESTIONS LARA PERRY What are the different ways that we understand the sitter's role/agency in the production of a photographic portrait? What are the historical specificities of concepts of 'likeness' in nineteenth century cartes de visites? What is the role of second-order information (i.e. that which is external to the image itself) in the generation of a likeness? VICKI BRUCE Why are we so good at identifying familiar faces and so poor at unfamiliar ones? How can we make better use of images of faces from crime scenes (via cameras or via the witness recall of composites)? TIM VALENTINE How do we bind unfamiliar faces to specific events? How do we know where and when we last saw somebody before? What skills underlie exceptional face recognition ability? PETER HAMILTON How do we develop a history of photography that looks at social processes of image production, rather than the history of pictorial aesthetics? Why are there such profound links between science and photography in the nineteenth century? What are the social factors that lead to the rise of surveillance through facial recognition? What have been the social and cultural consequences of using photographic techniques to aid social control? SIBYLLE ERLE The events of our lives will shape our personalities, which will be expressed through our bodies - although our bodies may restrict, to some extent, how we respond to these events. Our bodies are our modes of being in the world, of interacting with it, and as such, will help to shape this interaction. Lavater was one of the last to take the soul seriously. His physiognomy was propelled by the search for divine likeness, which he believed to be represented in the human face. Lavater’s problem was that he couldn’t find the perfect portrait. What do Lavater’s struggles tell us about body-soul relationships in the late 18th century? Lavater prefers analyzing physiognomical portraits to examining faces. What does this preference tell us about the use and understanding of the body?