Preliminary results. Please do not quote A comparative study of public values in China, Denmark, Taiwan and the United States. Lotte Bøgh Andersen, Aarhus University, lotte@ps.au.dk, Torben Beck Jørgensen (corresponding author), University of Copenhagen, tbj@ifs.ku.dk Karsten Vrangbæk, Danish Institute of Governmental Research, kvr@akf.dk, Yahong Zhang, Rutgers, the State University of New Jersey yahongzh@andromeda.rutgers.edu Paper prepared for The Public Value Consortium Biennial Workshop in Chicago 2012 Introduction 1 A basic but rarely addressed question in public values research is whether and how public sector values vary from one country to another. We do know that public values vary according to task, sector and administrative level within one country (Vrangbæk, 2009) but comparative studies of public values are rare and limited to countries that are quite alike, cf. Van der Waal, Pekur and Vrangbæk (2008) and Palidauskaite (2010). This paper seeks to fill this gap in the literature as it compares public values in four countries – China, Taiwan, USA and Denmark – which differ widely on a number of potentially important dimensions such as size, age, constitution and culture. Our basic interest is to investigate how public values depend on political system and national culture. One would clearly expect to find value differences between democracies and dictatorships and between Western and Eastern cultures or between European continental and anglosaxon cultures. In this respect the selected countries present us with large differences. The biggest constitutional difference is between China which is a single-party state governed by the Communist Party of China in contrast to the other three countries which are constitutional democracies. As to cultural variation, several studies point to large differences (Hofstede; Schwartz, World Value Survey). The possible interplay between political system and culture is also of interest as national cultures may fit more or less well with specific political systems. On the other hand, one may also argue that is important to look for a set of universal public values because the core task of public administration – serving a master in ruling a country – simply is the same everywhere, regardless of how the political master is chosen and regardless of culture (references). Following this line of thought it is important looking for both similarities and differences and explaining identified patterns. Empirical studies of values are difficult because a value is such an airy and intangible phenomenon. When it comes to comparative studies of public values in countries with different administrative traditions and different political history the problems are immense as misunderstadings are beyond absolute control. ”Oxygen” is the same in Denmark and China – an ”agency” or ”directorate” is probably not, and certainly the understanding of political loyalty may depend heavily on both the past and actual circumstances – in contrast to oxygen. This is precisely why comparative studies are difficult - and why they are highly important (Beck Jørgensen, 2010). 2 Comparing public values in countries all over the world is costly if qualitative methods are used. For this first preliminary study we have chosen surveys. Our study builds on survey data on public values collected from students enrolled in a Master of Public Administration or Governance program in the four countries. These students represent a convenient and informative group of respondents for this type of comparative study. They have working experience from public administration in their home countries and they have completed some degree of higher education, which makes it likely that they are aware of abstract notions of public values and public service motivation (Svara, 2010). Furthermore, it is convenient to gather survey data from them as a part of their education and this is likely to have a positive impact on response rates. The weakness is a potential selection bias, e.g. being more committed to public sector values than other public sector employees. The questionnaire used in this study has been applied in a number of earlier investigations, reported in papers on Danish public values (also compared to Dutch and Estonian values), on testing the dimensions of public values, and on empirical and conceptual links between public values and public service motivation (references). Information on the data set is enclosed. In the next section we will develop hypotheses addressing the possible links between constitution and culture on the one hand and public values at the other hand. We consider constitution and culture as independent variables and public values as the dependent variable but we will discuss whether this is always the case. The following sections present our data and methods, the data analysis and discussion, and finally our conclusion. How public values may be framed by contextual factors – development of hypotheses We define public values as the ideals, coined as principles, to be followed in the public sector when producing a service and regulating citizens, business firms etc. (Beck Jørgensen, 2003). The public values literature insists that the desirable, not only the desired, is important and public values thus provide direction to public employees rather than drive action (Andersen et al. 2012). Public values are often considered important, because they are expected to a) form our perceptions of reality; b) give identity to individuals as well as organizations; c) guide behavior; d) give meaning to public service; and e) be necessary for the maintenance of communities and societies (cf. Kluckhohn, 1962; Lawton and Rose, 1994; Maguire, 1998; Verdikommisjonen, 1999). According to Bozeman (2007: 13) public values specify 3 • ‘the rights, benefits and prerogatives to which citizens should (and should not) be entitled; • the obligations of citizens to society, the state and one another; and • the principles on which governments and policies should be based.’ The latter – systemic or institutional – element in the definition is important, as it indicates that values can be considered as the basic building blocks of the public sector (Beck Jørgensen 1999: 581). We delimit ourselves to the systemic/institutional understanding of public values. We further narrow it down by excluding the policy-part of Bozeman’s definition because a broader understanding of public values (as the dependent variable) may overlap with political culture and possibly also with national culture, one of our independent variables. Also, and not least important, our questionnaire is designed to capture public values in the narrow sense. Universal governmental regime values? One may argue that there is one and only one contextual factor influencing public values and that is ownership. Public organizations are owned and governed by politicians and the main role of all public administration is to be loyal towards the legitimate masters with no regard to whether the master is elected in open competition between several parties, selected by a single party, designated by tradition or has inherited a kingdom by birth. Thus, the public apparatus has one core obligation, i.e. serving the master. If this argument is valid we should be able to identify a universal set of public values. Or to put it differently: Inherently governmental regime values can be identified (references). Suggestions could be political loyalty, adaptability and rule abidance as these values may facilitate obedient service. For a number of reasons, we do not choose identifying inherently governmental values as our point of departure. First, in general the degree of political authority (and economic authority) may vary along one or several dimensions. Following that argument, absolute political authority is a special case, and often no clear boundary exists between public and private organizations. There are degrees of publicness and some organizations are thus more public than others (Bozeman, 1987). Second, even if organizations are fully financed by public money and fully subject to political control, publicness may vary (Antonsen & Beck Jørgensen, 1997). Third, how to serve a political master efficiently and obediently may vary because of contextual factors. We will therefore proceed to discuss the contextual factors already mentioned: political system and culture. 4 Political system Political systems can be categorized in numerous ways. One may differentiate between unitary and federal states, decentralized and centralized states, corporatist regimes versus parliamentary regimes etc. For our test we have chosen the strongest contrast between political systems, i.e. democracy versus dictatorship, in this paper operationalized as multi-party systems versus single-party systems. In single-party systems, a single political party forms the government and no other parties or only approved minor parties are permitted to run candidates for election, or laws or practices prevent the opposition from legally getting power. In contrast, in multi-party states at least two independent parties are, both formally and in practice, allowed to run candidates for election and ultimately for office. The question is then which public values we theoretically associate with a single-party respective multi-party regime. A single-party regime calls for a monolithic top-down world with a strong emphasis on hierarchy, loyalty towards masters, flair for interpreting politics, and vertical climbing within the system. Thus, our first hypothesis includes public values stressing the vertical nature of a system (“verticality”). Next, we expect that values related to task and relations between administration and the public play a less important role. Consequently we expect values such as legality and professionalism to be less important. Finally, a single-party regime calls for limited public competition over power and a restrained public discourse. Therefore we do not expect transparency and listening to the public opinion to be important public values. Referring to our questionnaire (see appendix A) we formulate the following two hypotheses: H1: A country with a single-party system attach more importance to ability to interpret the political climate and signals, good relations with higher authorities, recognition from management, a high salary and career opportunities compared to master students from multi-party countries. H2: A country with multiple parties attach more importance to judicial values/due process, independent professional standards, public insight and transparency and consideration of the public opinion compared to master students from a single-party system. 5 Culture The nature of a political system is far from the only potential reason why certain public values would be prioritized. Most importantly, countries can be argued to be dominated by a national culture. The main argument is that a political system is embedded in a national culture that influences which public values are the important ones. A common sense understanding would lead us to think that West (USA, Denmark) contrasts East (China, Taiwan) but as will be shown a more nuanced and theoretical grounded point of departure than a common sense East West comparison is available. Several theories locate high numbers of countries on cultural maps. Hofstede and associates have developed cultural dimensions such as individualism-collectivism, masculinity-feminism, high-low power distance, and high-low uncertainty avoidance (Hofstede, 2001; Hofstede & Bond 1984). United States and Denmark have individualistic cultures, and China and Taiwan have collectivistic cultures. Here we find an East-West divide. On the other hand, China and United States have masculine cultures, while Denmark have a more feminine culture, and Taiwan is in-between. On the uncertainty avoidance dimension, there is very limited difference between the four countries. The World Values Survey locates countries on two dimensions: traditional values vs. secular/rational values and survival values vs. self-expression values. Both East and West have high scores on secular/rational values while there is a clear difference between East and West on the survival/self-expression dimension. The different scores on this dimension may however be related to wealth. Schwartz (1992, 1994, 1999) has developed a multidimensional measure of cultural values. He argues that countries can be placed according to the average scores of their citizens on seven dimensions. The seven values are: Embeddedness/conservatism, hierarchy, mastery, affective autonomy, egalitarianism, harmony and intellectual autonomy. Based on an analysis of 74 countries Schwartz creates a map with relatively distinct groups of countries. Our four case countries are placed into three different groups based on relative emphasis on the seven value dimensions. China and Taiwan belongs to a “Confucian” cultural group with a strong emphasis on hierarchy. USA belongs to a group of “English speaking” countries emphasizing “mastery”. Denmark is part of the group of European countries with two core values of “egalitarianism” and “intellectual autonomy”. 6 The “Confucian”/hierarchical group (China/Taiwan) has the following characteristics: A hierarchical, differential allocation of fixed roles and of resources is the legitimate, desirable way to regulate interdependencies. People are socialized to comply with the obligations and rules and sanctioned if they do not. The cultural emphasis is on the legitimacy of an unequal distribution of power, roles and resources (social power, authority, humility, wealth). USA on the other hand belongs to a group of “English speaking” countries where the dominant value is “mastery”. Mastery is characterized by: Groups and individuals should master, control, and change the social and natural environment through assertive action in order to further personal or group interests. The cultural emphasis is on getting ahead through active self-assertion (ambition, success, daring, competence). Denmark is located in the group of Western European countries with a strong emphasis on “egalitarianism” and “intellectual autonomy”. The cultural orientation towards egalitarianism is characterized by: Individuals are portrayed as moral equals, who share basic interests and who are socialized to transcend selfish interests, cooperate voluntarily with others, and show concern for everyone's welfare (equality, social justice, freedom, responsibility, honesty). Intellectual autonomy is characterized by: The person is an autonomous, bounded entity and finds meaning in his / her own uniqueness, seeking to express own internal attributes (preferences, traits, feelings) and is encouraged to do so. Intellectual Autonomy has a cultural emphasis on the desirability of individuals independently pursuing their own ideas and intellectual directions (curiosity, broadmindedness, creativity). Schwartz’ cultural theory is chosen as our point of departure for three reasons. First, our four countries are located in three categories and this distribution constitutes an interesting contrast to the distribution on political system; second, it is a well argued theory, which has been confirmed through empirical investigations; and, third, it is easier to relate our public values in the questionnaire to the Schwartz theory. Referring to our questionnaire (see appendix A) we formulate the following three hypotheses: 7 H3: A country belonging to the confucianist/hierarchical group attach more importance to political loyalty, loyalty to rules, good relations with higher authorities, recognition from management and adaptability compared to master students from other countries. H4: A country belonging to the mastery group attach more importance to innovation, the ability to innovate, business-like operations, willingness to take risks, learning and development on the job compared to master students from other countries. H5: A country belonging to the egalitarianism/intellectual autonomy group attach more importance to judicial values/due process, independent professional standards, equal treatment, ethical awareness and personal integrity compared to master students from other countries. Note that H1 and H5 to some extent unfortunately are overlapping because of the vertical nature both of the Chinese political system and the Confucianist culture. Research design and methods The hypotheses are investigated using surveys of Public Administration students in China, Taiwan, USA and Denmark, and this section explains why we compare masters of public administration students and why we have chosen the four investigated countries. It then describes the empirical data and our operationalizations. Finally, we discuss and explain our choice of methods to analyze the data. Analyzing Masters of Public Administration students We investigate master students for the following four reasons which will be discussed further below: Master of Public Administration students in the four countries are relatively comparable even in these very different contexts, they have practical experience in the public sector, they are aware of public values and public service motivation, and it is practically possible to gather survey data from them as a part of their education. We argue that Master of Public Administration students in the four countries are relatively comparable even in these very different contexts. The investigated educational programs differ 8 slightly in their relative weight of the policy analysis and public administration/management approaches to public service education, but Soto, Opheim and Tajalli (1999) find limited differences between these types of educational programs, indicating that this is inconsequential. Still, the comparison between the questionnaire answers in different countries should be done carefully because of the known difference in cross-cultural response behavior. "Response set" means the general tendencies to systematically agree more (or less) with all questions, and it is known to vary between countries (e.g. Hofstede and Bond, 1984). Many of the master students have practical experience in the public sector, and their values thus reflect the different values in the four countries’ public sectors. Svara (2010) found that Public Administration students are oriented toward contributing to society. He based his investigation on a survey of Masters of Public Administration students at five universities, and more than 80 percent agreed that meaningful public service and opportunities to help others are important them (ibid.). Svara (2010: 361) further argues that generational change in government employment presents numerical, attitudinal, and organizational challenges, and more knowledge about future public administrators around the world might help us handle these challenges. Finally, it is convenient to gather survey data from master students as a part of their education. As discussed below, the response rate was high, and we were able to get data from a relative high number of respondents. The weakness is a potential selection bias as our respondents have actively selected to pursue public management training and careers and/or receive support from their workplace. This can imply that they are more committed to public sector values than other public sector employees. Furthermore it may mean that they are more focused on management values than professional or user values. However, since we sample from similar respondent groups in all four countries we do not expect this to affect the results. Comparing mainland China, Denmark, United States, and Taiwan The biggest difference in political system is between Mainland China which is a single-party state (where only the Communist Party and a limited number of approved minor parties under “the 9 United Front” is permitted to run candidates for election) and the other three countries which are multi-party systems. In terms of culture, China and Taiwan belong to the “Confucian”group, USA belongs to the “mastery” group, and Denmark is part of the group of European countries with two core values of “egalitarianism” and “intellectual autonomy”. Differences in culture and political systems are not, of course, the only differences between the countries, but we argue that they capture the most relevant distinctions. Clearly, one could also look to size and age. Two countries are small (Denmark and Taiwan) and two countries are big (China and the US) and China as a nation is the far oldest of the four countries. Table 1 sums up the differences between China, Taiwan, the Unites States and Denmark in culture, number of inhabitants and political systems. Table 1: Investigated countries Country Inhabitants China Taiwan United States Denmark Political system Cultural group Number of respondents 1,341m Single-party state Confucian/hierarchical 307 23m Multi-party state Confucian/hierarchical 223 310m Multi-party state Mastery 265 6m Multi-party state Egalitarianism/intellectual autonomy 455 Data and operationalizations In Denmark, the questionnaires were sent out by post as part of their education. A stamped return envelope was enclosed. In the other three countries, data was collected as part of lectures held in Public Administration. The response rate in Denmark was 62, while almost all present students in the other three countries completed the questionnaires. The number of respondents in each of the 10 countries can be seen in Table 1 above. The translations were done by native speaking researchers and language specialists (the English version of the questionnaire is attached). In order to measure how much importance the students attached to different values, the respondents were asked to evaluate the importance of firstly general organizational values in pursuing the daily tasks of their organization, secondly values related to the desirability of certain staff competences and thirdly values about the dominant motivational factors for the staff. Examples are political loyalty and high productivity (general values), ethical awareness and adaptability (values related to staff competences), and good relations to users and staying within the budget (values related to staff motives). The general values are linked to performance of daily organizational tasks in the text and the respondents are thus encouraged to consider the desirability of abstract values in light of specific working tasks. The options for answering were for general values and staff competences: Should be fundamental: very important (5); should normally be important (4); should occasionally be important (3); shouldn’t be prominent (2); shouldn’t play any role (1) and don’t know (coded as missing). The options for answering for the questions about motives were: extremely important (5), very important (4), fairly important (3), less important (2), not important (1) and don’t know (coded as missing). The answering options for staff motivation are thus more neutral in tone, while the questions on general values and staff competencies more clearly aims to elicit respondents’ perspectives on desirability. Note that all questions address what should be, not what is. Our data thus reflect the desirability of certain normative universes, not the actual presence of certain public values. Methods of analysis Analytically, our strategy is to give an overview of what the respondents actually answered and to test whether their answers differed between the countries. The mean score on a given value in each of the countries gives a parsimonious summary of the students’ answers, but the comparison involves four challenges. The first challenge is that differences between countries may be due to differences in language rather than political system or culture. Although the translation has been validated with native speaking researchers, culture and language are so closely intertwined that all interpretations between countries need to be done very carefully. Still, we can analyze what answers the respondents gave and then discuss whether should be seen as an actual difference in their 11 values. This is also the main reason for primarily presenting descriptive analyses (graphs showing the mean scores for each of the four countries) and only secondary using more advanced statistics. The second challenge is, however, that value importance is measured on ordinal scales (see the options for answering above), and this suggests that advanced statistics are necessary as a supplement to the descriptive graphs. The available measure of central tendency for ordinal variables (the median) does not capture nuances in the distribution of answers, which is the reason for using mean scores in the Graphs. But this makes it necessary to supplement with ordinal regressions (shown in the appendix) to make sure that our results are robust when we use a proper ordinal scale technique. When we compare one country to the other three countries, this country is reference category, and the country coefficients express estimated difference from the students from this country. If we compare groups of countries (e.g. the two countries belonging to the Confucianist/hierarchical group with the two non-Confucianist countries) we construct a dummy variable for whether a given student comes from the group in question (here: a Confucianist country). If there is only one country in a group (e.g. United States in the mastery group) we use the country dummy variable instead of coding countries together. The ordinal regressions also handle the third challenge, namely that the gender composition and average age differ between the countries. The ordinal regressions control for these variables to make sure that differences, if any, are not due to these personal characteristics. We use a logit transformation in all models, and all models are acceptable in term of significant tests of model fitting information (-2 log Likelihood). The fourth challenge relates to the fact that we measured 16 general values, 10 values linked to staff competences and 11 values related to staff motives. Comparisons of all these values between four countries without adjustment for the number of comparisons performed would mean that we could get significant differences by chance, only because of the big number of comparisons. We have therefore theoretically selected a more limited number of comparisons to present in the paper and we also performed post hoc tests to adjust the p-values of the comparisons to the fact that we perform many comparisons. We consistently use Scheffe post hoc tests. 12 Results The presentation of the results follows the hypotheses starting with the expectations concerning single-party versus multi-party systems followed by the hypotheses concerning culture. Political system matters According to hypothesis 1, the expectation is that master students from a country with a singleparty system attach more importance to the ability to interpret the political climate and signals, good relations with higher authorities, recognition from management, high salary and career opportunities compared to master students from multi-party countries. Given that China is the only investigated single-party system, we test whether master students from China attach more importance to the mentioned values compared to master students from Denmark, United States and Taiwan. Concerning the value of being able to interpret political climate and signals, the variation between the four countries is as expected according to Figure 1. The students from China attach higher importance to this value than students from the other three countries. In a post hoc test (Scheffe, including all items related to staff competences), these differences are all highly significant (p<0.001). This is also the case if the differences are analyzed using ordinal regression (see Table A1 in the appendix). Figure 1: Comparison of importance of being able to interpret political climate and signals Importance 5 Ability to interpret the political climate and signals 5=Very important 4 4= Should normally be important 3 3= Should occasionally be impo. 2 2= Shouldn’t be important 1 1= Shouldn’t play any role 0 Denmark China US Taiwan Note: Comparison of average importance reported by the participating Master of Public Administration students from the four countries. The averages are calculated from the following classification: Very important (5); should normally be important (4); should occasionally be important (3); shouldn’t be important (2); shouldn’t play any role (1). 13 The next value mentioned in hypothesis 1 concerns good relations with higher authorities, and the students have been asked whether this is an important motive for core employees. Figure 2 shows that although the average importance attached to this value is highest for China, the difference to US and Taiwan is not very substantial. Still, according to the logistic regression in table A2 Chinese students attach significantly higher importance to good relations with higher authorities as a motive among core employees compared to students from all the other countries. Post hoc tests do, however, indicate that only the difference from Danish and Taiwanese students is robust. Figure 2: Comparison of good relations with higher authorities as a motive 5= Extremely important 4= Very important 3= Fairly impo. 2= Less important 1= not important Note: See the note to Figure 1. Recognition from management is also expected to be seen as a more important motive for core employees for the Chinese students, and Figure 3 shows that this is actually the case. Post hoc tests also show that Chinese students on average find this value significantly more important than students from the three other countries, and the ordinal regression (table A3) confirms this. 14 Figure 3: Comparison of importance of recognition from management as a motive 5=Extremely important 4= Very important 3= fairly important 2= Less important 1= Not important Note: See the note to Figure 1. The next value expected to be more important for Chinese students is high salary as a motive for core employees. Again, our expectations are met in Figure 4, and the logistic regression (table A4) also shows that the differences are statistically significant controlled for age and gender. Still, the post hoc test shows that the difference between China and Taiwan is not robust when we control for the many comparisons performed. Figure 4: Comparison of importance of high salary as a motive 5=Extremely important 4= Very important 3= fairly important 2= Less important 1= Not important Note: See the note to Figure 1. Finally, career opportunities are expected to be seen as more important in China compared to the other three countries. Figure 5 shows that Chinese students (as expected) attach more importance to career opportunities as a staff motive than the other students (right hand, yellow bars), and this difference is also statistically significant for all the countries (both in post hoc tests and in Table A6). However, the students from the four countries do not differ in their importance attached to 15 whether the organization should ensure career opportunities (left hand, blue bars). The ordinal regression (Table A5) indicates that the difference between Chinese and Danish students is statistically significant after control for age and gender, but the substantial difference is inconsequential. Figure 5: Comparison of importance of career opportunities as general value and motive 5=Very important/extremely important (motive) 4= Should normally be important/very important (motive) 3= Should occasionally be impo./fairly importan (motive) 2= Shouldn’t be important/less important (motive 1= Shouldn’t play any role/not important Note: See the note to Figure 1. In sum, hypothesis 1 on the relation between a single party system and public values reflecting verticality is confirmed. Hypothesis 2 expects that a country with a multi-party system attach more importance to values related to task and relations between system and environment: judicial values/due process, independent professional standards, public insight and transparency and consideration of the public opinion compared to master students from a single-party system. In this study, the implication is that master students from Denmark, United States and Taiwan are expected to attach more importance to the above mentioned values compared to master students from China. Concerning the first value, judicial values/due process, our expectations are definitely not met. On the contrary, Figure 6 shows that Chinese students attach substantially more importance this value than the other students, and both logistic regression (Table A7) and post hoc tests confirm this. On the other hand, it is worth mentioning that all countries attach very high importance to judicial values. 16 Figure 6: Comparison of judicial values/due process as general value 5 Judicial values/due process Importance 4 3 2 1 0 Denmark China US Taiwan Concerning the importance of independent professional standards, our results are not in line with the expectations either. Figure 7 shows that Danish students attach more importance to this value than all other master students, but although Chinese students attach slightly less importance to this value than US students and Taiwanese students, these differences are not statistically significant in either ordinal regression (Table A8) or post hoc tests. Figure 7: Comparison of independent professional standards as general value Independent professional standards 5 Importance 4 3 2 1 0 Denmark China US Taiwan Chinese master students were expected to attach less importance to public insight and transparency than master students from Denmark, United States and Taiwan. This expectation is not met. There is no difference between Denmark, China and Taiwan, while students from United States score 17 significantly higher than all the other students (see Figure 8 below, and Table A9 for the relevant ordinal regression). Figure 8: Comparison of public insight and transparency as general value Public insight and transparency 5 Importance 4 3 2 1 0 Denmark China US Taiwan The last part of hypothesis 2 was that master students from multi-party states were expected to think that consideration of the public opinion was more important than students from a single-party state (China). Again, this expectation is not met (see Figure 9). Similar to public insight and transparence, master students from United States attach higher importance to considerations of the public opinion than students from the other three countries, while Danish students attached significantly lower importance to this value than the other students (Table A10). Figure 9: Comparison of consideration of the public opinion as a general value Consider the public opinion 5 Importance 4 3 2 1 0 Denmark China US Taiwan 18 These results leave us with a highly interesting discussion. While hypothesis 1 concerning public values in single-party countries is confirmed, hypothesis 2 concerning public values in multi-party countries is not met. One may say that the hierarchical/vertical orientation in a single-party country (hypothesis 1) is not at the expense of other values we believed to be present in a democracy (hypothesis 2). Other factors may be present. Also note that all countries attach very high importance to all values in hypothesis 2 with one exception: considering the public opinion where Denmark has an interestingly low score. Possibly, this leaves us with three candidates to a set of universal public values. Culture matters Hypothesis 3 expected that a country belonging to the Confucianist group attach more importance to political loyalty, loyalty to rules, good relations with higher authorities, recognition from management and adaptability compared to master students from other countries. This implies that master students from China and Taiwan attach more importance to these values compared to master students from United States and Denmark. Figure 10 shows that China has the highest level of importance attached to this value followed by Taiwan and Denmark with approximately the same level and United States with the lowest level. This implies that although the students from Confucianist countries on average have higher scores (as also illustrated in the significant dummy variable in Table A11), it does not explain why Taiwan does not, as expected, differ from Denmark. 19 Figure 10: Comparison of political loyalty as a general value Political loyalty 5 Importance 4 3 2 1 0 Denmark China US Taiwan Loyalty to rules was also expected to be seen as more important in Confucianist countries, and it is correct that if we combine the students from China and Taiwan in a category in the ordinal regression, then they score significantly higher than the other students. But as Figure 11 also illustrates, the differences are inconsequential, and China is the only country which differs significantly from the other countries (all three of them) in the post hoc tests. Figure 11: Comparison of importance of being loyal to rules Loyalty to rules 5 Importance 4 3 2 1 0 Denmark China US Taiwan We already discussed good relations with higher authorities as a motive in relation to hypothesis 1 (which expected higher scores for students from single-party systems), and although the ordinal regression finds that students from Confucianist countries score higher on average, this is primarily 20 due to the high score in China. Students from United States and Taiwan do not differ (see also figure 2 above), and our interpretation is therefore that the high score in China has more to do with the political system than with the Confucianist culture. The conclusion is basically similar for recognition from management as a motive. The significant difference between Confucianist countries and other countries can be seen in Table A14, but Figure 3 above illustrates that the high score of Chinese students on this value may have more to do with political system than with culture (given that the difference between US and Taiwan is neither statistically nor substantially significant, while China and Taiwan differ significantly (both in post hoc test and in Table A3). The last value expected to be more important for student from Confucianist countries compared to other countries is adaptability. Here the results show the opposite pattern (Table A15). Students from the United States attach highest importance to this value followed by Danish and Chinese students with approximately the same average importance, while Taiwanese students see this value as significantly less important compared to students from the other three countries. Figure 11: Comparison of importance of being adaptable Adaptability 5 Importance 4 3 2 1 0 Denmark China US Taiwan So far the conclusion is that culture does not matter, at least not in the expected way. Note that with regard to political loyalty and loyalty towards rules we see the highest scores from Chinese students, fitting well with the results from testing hypothesis 1. An alternative conclusion could thus 21 be, that culture matters, but that the confucianist culture has been diluted in the Taiwanese case, while it remains stronger in China. Hypothesis 4 expects that a country belonging to the mastery group attach more importance to innovation, the ability to innovate, business-like operations, willingness to take risks, learning and development on the job compared to master students from other countries. This implies that master students from United States attach more importance to these values compared to master students from Denmark, China and Taiwan. We have asked the students two questions about innovation: Both as a general value and about the importance of the ability to innovate. Concerning innovation as a general value, Figure 12 shows that students from United States and Denmark attach most importance to this values and that students from United States as expected attach significantly more importance to the value than students from China and Taiwan (both in post hoc tests and ordinal regression, see Table A16). There is no significant difference between Denmark and United States. For the ability to innovate, we find the exact same pattern (see Figure 12 and Table A17). This points to an East-West divide. Figure 12: Comparison of importance of innovation Innovation 5 Innovation as a general value Ability to innovate Importance 4 3 2 1 0 Denmark China US Taiwan Concerning business-like operations (Figure 13), Danish students score significantly higher than the other students followed by students from United States and then Taiwan and finally China. Our expectation is not met, given that US students score significantly lower than Danish students and do 22 not differ from Taiwanese students (although they score higher than Chinese students, Table A18). Given China’s solitary score, the explanation may differences in economic systems. Figure 13: Comparison of importance of business-like operations as a general value Business-like operations 5 Importance 4 3 2 1 0 Denmark China US Taiwan Concerning willingness to take risks (Figure 14), Chinese and Taiwanese students see this as more important than both US and Danish students. Our expectation is therefore not met here either (see also table A19). Figure 14: Comparison of importance of willingness to take risks as a staff competence Willingness to take risks 5 Importance 4 3 2 1 0 Denmark China US Taiwan 23 In contrast, our expectation is met for learning and development on the job where students from the United States scores significantly higher than students from all other countries (Figure 15 and table A20). Figure 15: Comparison of importance of learning and development on the job as a staff motive Learning and development on the job 5 Importance 4 3 2 1 0 Denmark China US Taiwan We didn’t find much support to hypothesis 4. Only one value, the value of learning and developing on the job, was considered more important in the US compared to the three non-mastery countries. On the other hand, it should be noted that we identified results that point to the importance of the Confucianist - non-Confucianist cultural split. Both China and Taiwan score lower than Denmark and the US on innovation and business-like operations and they similarly score higher than Denmark and the US on willingness to take risks. The low scores can be interpreted as in accordance Confucianism but the high score on willingness to take risks demands a special explanation which we don’t have. Hypothesis 5 expects that a country belonging to the egalitarianism/intellectual autonomy group attach more importance to judicial values/due process, independent professional standards, equal treatment, ethical awareness and personal integrity compared to master students from other countries. This implies that master students from Denmark are expected to attach more importance to these values compared to master students from China, Taiwan and United States. Concerning judicial values/due process, our expectations are not met, given that master students from China have the highest score followed by students from Denmark and United State and finally 24 Taiwan (see Figure 6 above). Both post hoc tests and the ordinal regression (see table A7) show that Danish students have lower scores than Chinese students and higher scores than Taiwan (there is no significant difference between Denmark and United States). For independent professional standards, our expectations are met. According to Figure 7 (above), Danish students have the highest score, and they differ significantly from students from the other three countries in post hoc test and ordinal regression (see also Table A8). As can be seen from Figure 16, students from the United States attach higher importance to equal treatment than the other countries, while there is no difference between students from Denmark, Taiwan and China (Table A22). Figure 16: Comparison of importance of equal treatment as a general value Equal treatment 5 Importance 4 3 2 1 0 Denmark China US Taiwan Taiwanese students do, as expected attach less importance to ethical awareness than Danish students, but this is not the case for students from United States and China (according to Figure 17, the ordinal regression (table A24) and post hoc tests). Denmark (as a country from the egalitarianism/intellectual autonomy group) can therefore not be said to be characterized by a higher level of importance of this value compared to other countries. 25 Figure 17: Comparison of importance of having ethical awareness as a staff motive Having ethical awareness 5 Importance 4 3 2 1 0 Denmark China US Taiwan The same conclusion can be drawn for personal integrity where the country mean scores can be seen in Figure 18. Taiwanese students score lower than Danish students, but students from the United States score significantly higher (according to ordinal regression, although the difference is substantially inconsequential and not significant in post hoc test). Figure 18: Comparison of importance of personal integrity as a staff competence Personal integrity 5 Importance 4 3 2 1 0 Denmark China US Taiwan In sum, hypothesis 5 can not be confirmed. We found the expected distribution only in the case of independent professional values. If we on the other hand take a look at tables 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5, Denmark is rather consistently the country that shows the lowest score on all public values related to the vertical nature of a single-party country. This fits nicely with the Schwartz location of Denmark among egalitarian countries just as well as the high score on professional standards fits well with Denmark’s location in the intellectual autonomy group. (Nice test – bad hypothesis!) 26 Discussion and conclusion Our first two hypotheses were derived from a theoretical expectation of differences in values between students of different political regimes. We expected students from the single-party Chinese system to differ systematically from students in the other three countries with multi-party systems. The first hypothesis concerned expectations about the importance of the values “ability to interpret the political climate and signals”, “good relations with higher authorities” and the motivational factors “recognition from management”, “high salary” and “career opportunities”. The analysis confirmed our expectations that Chinese students attach higher importance to these values and motivational factors above students from the other three countries. Our second hypothesis related to political regimes included a set of expectations concerning judicial values/due process, independent professional standards, public insight and transparency and consideration of the public opinion. Chinese students were expected to attach lower importance to these values than their counterparts in multiparty systems. However, these expectations were not met. Chinese students actually emphasized judicial values/due process higher than other students, and they do not differ significantly from the other three countries in regards to the other values in this hypothesis. What is our overall conclusion concerning political regime then? Firstly that it the regime type seems to have some importance, as a fair number of expectations are met. But secondly that it does not provide a perfect theory for understanding value differences. The theory may be wrong, or our translation into survey questions could be imprecise. Considering the high importance attached to judicial values/due process by Chinese students it seems fairly plausible that Chinese students perceive the question differently from the others. We speculate that they do not consider individual citizen rights and rule based administration, but rather refer to general anti-corruption and system preservation values. Judicial values might for example refer more to “what you as a citizen or civil servant should do for the system” than what the system should deliver in terms of upholding and supporting the rights of the individual. Chinese students do not differ significantly from their counterparts in multiparty systems in regards to independent professional standards, public insight and transparency and consideration of the public opinion. This could indicate that even in multiparty systems public administrators must consider external interests, although not necessarily 27 act according to them. Finding a way of coping with this is probably part of the professional standards emphasized in all four countries. It thus points toward our null hypothesis of a common set of value orientations among civil servants throughout the globe, given similarities in their core job functions. The results from our analysis of hypotheses derived from culture theory were also quite mixed. The first group of expectations concerned political loyalty, loyalty to rules, good relations with higher authorities, recognition from management and adaptability. We argued that Confucianist countries (Taiwan and China) were likely to attach higher importance to these values than their counterparts in Denmark and the United states. The overall picture is that these expectations were confirmed for China, but not for Taiwan. This could lead to several different conclusions. Firstly we can speculate that culture matters, but that the impact depends on the political regime, as partly confirmed in our first two hypotheses. Confucianist values may have better conditions for thriving in some settings (China) than in others (Taiwan). A second possible interpretation is that Taiwan has moved away from a classical confucianist culture and towards a more hybrid cultural form due to stronger interaction with western countries than in China. Schwarz indicates this in his overview studies of cultural characteristics across countries, where China and Taiwan belong to the same family, but with China as a cultural “outlier” and Taiwan closer to western countries (Schwarz and Bardi 2001). The relatively lower importance placed on adaptability by “confucianist” students seems to falsify our initial hypothesis, which was based on the premise that a high degree of willingness to accept hierarchical decisions and accommodate system needs was an inherent part of this culture. Yet, our formulation of the question is ambiguous and it is possible that the high score given by other students reflects an entirely different understanding of adaptability, which is more in line with innovation and entrepreneurship than adapting to system needs. Hypothesis four stated that master students from a country belonging to the mastery group attach more importance to innovation, the ability to innovate, business-like operations, willingness to take risks, learning and development on the job. These expectations were not met, except for learning and development on the job. Both Danish and US students place high importance on innovation. It is possible that they attach slightly different meanings to this concept. Referring to hypothesis five it could be that Danish students link innovation to the personal integrity and development, while US students perhaps are more clearly linking innovation to “mastery” concepts. But we cannot confirm 28 these speculations. That Danish students attach high importance to “business like” operation may reflect the general public administration rhetoric in the country over the past two decades, whereas US students either take this for granted, or have grown weary of this after the early introduction of “entrepreneurship” ideas in the 1980s. The final hypothesis derived from culture theory was that master students from a country belonging to the egalitarianism/intellectual autonomy group would attach more importance to judicial values/due process, independent professional standards, equal treatment, ethical awareness and personal integrity. These expectations were not confirmed, except for the high importance attached to independent professional standards by Danish students. This result can be seen in light of the consistently low scores on all public values related to the vertical nature of a single-party country in table 1,2,3,4 and 5. The emerging picture when looking across these results fits nicely with the Schwartz location of Denmark among egalitarian countries just as well as the high score on professional standards fits well with Denmark’s location in the intellectual autonomy group. The overall conclusion from our deductively based analysis must be that we find some support for regime based hypothesis and some for culture based hypothesis. Furthermore, we speculate that there might be interaction between the two types of explanations. In particular we suggested that regimes may create more or less benign environments for cultural factors to have an impact. A more inductive approach might reveal other patterns or possible interpretations, but we do not have space to move into this in greater detail here, except for a brief consideration of our initial discussion about the possibility of finding values that are shared across the public administrators in all four countries. One way to operationalize this is to look for values that receive an average score of importance above four in all countries. The following values meet this criterion: judicial values/due process; independent professional standards; public insight and transparency; loyalty towards rules; adaptability; learning and development on the job; equal treatment; ethical awareness and personal integrity. Interestingly, these values are also found in Codes of Good Government in 14 countries around the world (Beck Jørgensen & Sørensen (forthcoming). Quite a few of these values are related to a Weberian understanding of bureaucracy, and taken together they point to an understanding of the public machinery as self-propelled and self-disciplined, not to say selfreferential. This indicates that future research needs to look at similarities as well as differences, 29 even when very different countries are compared. 30 References Andersen, L.B., Beck Jørgensen, T., Kjeldsen, A.M., Pedersen, L.H., Vrangbæk, K. (in press): "Public Service Motivation and Public Values: Conceptual and Empirical Relationships". Accepted to be published in American Review of Public Administration January 9th. 2012. Antonsen, M. & Beck Jørgensen, T. (1997): The ’publicness’ of public organizations. Public Administration, vol. 75, no. 2, 337-358 Beck Jørgensen, T. 1999. The public sector in an in-between time: searching for new public values. Public Administration, 77(3), 565–84. Beck Jørgensen, T. (2003): Værdier og den offentlige sector. I: Beck Jørgensen, T. (ed): På sporet af en offentlig identitet. Århus: Aarhus Universitetsforlag, 11-38. Beck Jørgensen, T. (2010). ”Danish PA and PA in Danish”, In G. Bouckaert & W. van de Donk: The Europaen Group of Public Administration (1975-2010). Perspectives for the Future. Bruxelles: Bruylant, 223-227. Beck Jørgensen, T., & Sørensen, D.-L. (forthcoming). Codes of good governance: National or global public values? Accepted for Public Integrity. Bozeman, B. (1987): All Organizations are Public. Bridging Public and Private Organizational Theories. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Bozeman, B. 2007. Public Value and Public Interest: Counterbalancing Economic Individualism. Washington DC: Georgetown University Press. Hofstede, G. & M. H. Bond (1984) “Hofstede's Culture Dimensions. An Independent Validation Using Rokeach's Value Survey” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 15 (4): 417-433. Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s Consequences. Comparing Values, Behaviors, Institutions and Organizations across Nations. Second Edition. Thousand Oaks, London & New Delhi: Sage Publications. Kluckhohn, C. (1962). Values and value-orientations in the theory of action: An exploration in definition and classification. In T. Parsons & E. A. Shils (Eds.), Toward a general theory of action (pp. 388-433). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Lawton, A., & Rose, A. G. (1994). Organisation and management in the public sector. London: Pitman. Liu, Ying & Aaron Cohen (2010) Values, commitment, and OCB among Chinese employees International Journal of Intercultural Relations 34: 493–506. 31 Maguire, M. (1998). Ethics in the public service: Current issues and practice. In A. Hondeghem (Ed.), Ethics and accountability in a context of governance and new public management. EGPA Yearbook (pp. 23-34). Amsterdam: IOS Press. Meyer, P. J., & Allen, J. N. (1984). Testing the side bet theory of organizational commitment: Some methodological considerations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 372–378. Mintzberg, H. (1983). Structure in Fives. Designing Effective Organizations. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall. Palidauskaite, J. 2010. “Value profile of civil servants in new European democracies through the lens of embedded ethics”. Paper presented at the European Group for Public Administrationconference, Toulouse. Soto, W. D.; C. Opheim, H.T. Tajalli (1999) Apples and Oranges? Comparing the Attitudes of Public Policy Versus Public Administration Students The American Review of Public Administration 29 (1): 77-91. Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theory and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 1–65). New York: Academic Press. Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the content and structure of values? Journal of Social Issues, 50, 19-45. Schwartz, S. H. (1999). Cultural value differences: Some implications for work. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 48, 23-47. Schwartz, S. H., & A, Bardi, A. (1997). Influences of adaptation to communist rule on value priorities in Eastern Europe. Political Psychology, 18, 385-410. Schwartz, S. H. & A. Bardi, A. (2001). Value hierarchies across cultures: Taking a similarities perspective. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 268-290. Svara, J.H. (2010). The next generation challenge: Finding and incorporating the local (and other) government managers of the future. Journal of Public Affairs Education, 16(3), 361-377. Van der Waal, Z., Pekur, A. and Vrangbæk, K. 2008. Public sector value congruence among old and new EU member-states? Empirical evidence from the Netherlands, Denmark and Estonia. Public Integrity, 10(4), 317–33. Verdikommisjonen [The National Value Commission]. (1999). Verdier – fellesskap og mangfold. Verdikommisjonens midtveisrapport [Values – community and diversity. The national value commission mid-term report]. Verdikommisjonens styringsgruppe, Oslo. 32 Vrangbæk, K. (2009). Public sector values in Denmark: A survey analysis. International Journal of Public Administration, 32(6), 508-535. 33 Appendix A: Questionnaire concerning values, motives and competences in the public sector 1. How important do you think these values should be in your organization? Please mark one option in each row Should Fundamenta Should occasionl, very normally be nally be important important important Shouldn’t be important Shouldn’t play any role Don’t know A. Political loyalty B. Judicial values/Due process C. Independent professional standards (freedom to use professional discretion in a qualified manner) D. Balance societal interests (e.g., considering special interests without letting them dominate) E. Satisfying immediate needs of users F. High productivity G. Businesslike operations H. Accountability to society in general I. Public insight and transparency (citizens and others can always ask questions and look us over the shoulder) J. Listen to the public opinion (ensure alignment between public opinion and our activity) K. Equal treatment (avoid discrimination based on gender, ethnicity and religion in encounters with users or recruitment) L. Continuity (look at the long term and maintain traditions) M. Innovation N. Ensure good career opportunities for employees O. Strengthen user democracy (e.g., help users gain influence using existing channels or creating new ones) P. Networking (moving beyond sector limits and traditional jurisdictions) Q. Other values, please specify: 34 2. In your opinion, how should core employees in a public organization rate the importance of the following competences? Please mark one option in each row Should be fundamental; very important Should Should normally be occasionally important be important Shouldn’ Shouldn’ t be t play Don’t important any role know A. Ability to interpret the political climate and signals B. Ethical awareness C. Willingness to take risks D. Loyalty to rules E. Adaptability F. Personal integrity (e.g., honesty and credibility) G. Professional drive H. Economic awareness J. Strong interpersonal skills I. Ability to innovate K. Other competences, please specify: 3. In your opinion, how should core employees in a public organization rate the importance of the following motives? Please mark one option in each row Should be extremely important Should be Should be very fairly important important Should be Shouldn’t less be important important Don’t know A. Professional commitment B. Commitment to the organizational task/mission C. Good social work environment D. A high salary E. Career opportunities F. Staying within budget G. Good relations with higher authorities H. Learning and development on the job I. Recognition from management J. Good relations to users K. Recognition from peers L. Other types of motives, please specify: 35 Appendix B Table A1: Ordinal regression of ability to interpret the political climate and signals (variable 2a) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 2a = 1] .257 .359 .513 1 .474 [variable 2a = 2] 1.309 .336 15.211 1 .000 [variable 2a = 3] 2.949 .328 80.594 1 .000 [variable 2a = 4] 4.804 .347 191.553 1 .000 Age -.014 .009 2.253 1 .133 .011 .112 .010 1 .920 Taiwan=1 -1.708 .184 86.426 1 .000 US=1 -1.468 .175 70.362 1 .000 Female =1 Denmark=1 -2.773 .303 83.716 1 .000 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.21. Reference category is China and the country coefficients express estimated difference from China. Table A2: Ordinal regression of importance of good relations with higher authorities as a motive (variable 3G) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 3G = 1] -2.687 .411 42.827 1 .000 [variable 3G = 2] -.371 .317 1.365 1 .243 [variable 3G = 3] 1.662 .313 28.201 1 .000 [variable 3G = 4] 3.898 .333 137.155 1 .000 Age -.004 .009 .185 1 .667 Female =1 .429 .112 14.660 1 .000 Taiwan=1 -.747 .176 18.030 1 .000 US=1 -.451 .166 7.376 1 .007 Denmark=1 -2.648 .304 76.060 1 .000 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.27. Reference category is China and the country coefficients express estimated difference from China. Table A3: Ordinal regression of importance of recognition from management as a motive (var.3i) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 3i = 1] -5.512 .514 114.796 1 .000 [variable 3i = 2] -3.577 .351 104.044 1 .000 [variable 3i = 3] -1.361 .319 18.169 1 .000 [variable 3i = 4] .534 .317 2.830 1 .093 Age .010 .009 1.175 1 .278 Female =1 .392 .111 12.559 1 .000 Taiwan=1 -.632 .174 13.111 1 .000 US=1 -.981 .166 34.855 1 .000 Denmark=1 -.698 .291 5.746 1 .017 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.057. Reference category is China and the country coefficients express estimated difference from China. 36 Table A4: Ordinal regression of importance of high salary as a motive (variable 3d) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 3d = 1] -5.561 .393 200.565 1 .000 [variable 3d = 2] -2.853 .331 74.244 1 .000 [variable 3d = 3] -.759 .322 5.562 1 .018 [variable 3d = 4] 1.588 .324 23.967 1 .000 Age .016 .009 2.965 1 .085 Female =1 .060 .111 .289 1 .591 Taiwan=1 -.412 .176 5.475 1 .019 -1.791 .174 106.022 1 .000 US=1 Denmark=1 -2.380 .301 62.668 1 .000 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.314. Reference category is China and the country coefficients express estimated difference from China. Table A5: Ordinal regression of importance of the organization ensuring career opportunities (variable 1n) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 1n = 1] -5.563 .496 125.922 1 .000 [variable 1n = 2] -3.815 .358 113.272 1 .000 [variable 1n = 3] -1.651 .325 25.745 1 .000 [variable 1n = 4] .653 .322 4.116 1 .042 -.019 .009 3.961 1 .047 Female =1 .398 .113 12.491 1 .000 Taiwan=1 -.211 .175 1.457 1 .227 .177 .167 1.125 1 .289 Age US=1 Denmark=1 -.626 .296 4.481 1 .034 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.021. Reference category is China and the country coefficients express estimated difference from China. Table A6: Ordinal regression of importance of career opportunities as a motive (variable 3e) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 3d = 1] -6.918 .552 157.065 1 .000 [variable 3d = 2] -4.235 .345 150.510 1 .000 [variable 3d = 3] -1.692 .322 27.531 1 .000 [variable 3d = 4] .336 .317 1.120 1 .290 Age .001 .009 .012 1 .912 Female =1 .256 .111 5.272 1 .022 Taiwan=1 -.733 .175 17.602 1 .000 US=1 -.942 .166 32.109 1 .000 Denmark=1 -2.331 .298 61.100 1 .000 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.223. Reference category is China and the country coefficients express estimated difference from China. 37 Table A7: Ordinal regression of importance of judicial values/due process as general value (variable 1b) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 1b = 1] -6.185 .538 132.374 1 .000 [variable 1b = 2] -5.076 .422 144.913 1 .000 [variable 1b = 3] -3.471 .366 90.062 1 .000 [variable 1b = 4] -1.221 .350 12.190 1 .000 Age .004 .010 .154 1 .694 Female =1 .016 .123 .016 1 .898 Taiwan=1 -1.841 .201 83.734 1 .000 US=1 -1.064 .195 29.792 1 .000 Denmark=1 -.731 .323 5.110 1 .024 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.094. Reference category is China and the country coefficients express estimated difference from China. Table A8: Ordinal regression of importance of independent professional standards (variable 1c) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 3d = 1] -6.942 1.050 43.715 1 .000 [variable 3d = 2] -4.363 .424 105.927 1 .000 [variable 3d = 3] -1.922 .332 33.452 1 .000 [variable 3d = 4] .375 .325 1.334 1 .248 -.010 .009 1.114 1 .291 Female =1 .154 .116 1.766 1 .184 Taiwan=1 .198 .176 1.261 1 .262 US=1 .024 .168 .020 1 .888 Age Denmark=1 .945 .300 9.923 1 .002 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.096. Reference category is China and the country coefficients express estimated difference from China. Table A9: Ordinal regression of importance of public insight and transparency (variable 1i) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 1i = 1] -7.170 1.050 46.591 1 .000 [variable 1i = 2] -3.852 .375 105.595 1 .000 [variable 1i = 3] -1.759 .330 28.434 1 .000 [variable 1i = 4] .190 .325 .342 1 .559 -.006 .009 .451 1 .502 Female =1 .215 .112 3.684 1 .055 Taiwan=1 -.206 .173 1.420 1 .233 .651 .170 14.606 1 .000 Age US=1 Denmark=1 -.338 .298 1.289 1 .256 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.034. Reference category is China and the country coefficients express estimated difference from China. 38 Table A10: Ordinal regression of importance of listening to the public opinion (variable 1j) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 1j = 1] -4.326 .355 148.185 1 .000 [variable 1i = 2] -3.072 .332 85.861 1 .000 [variable 1j = 3] -1.277 .320 15.961 1 .000 [variable 1j = 4] .740 .317 5.433 1 .020 -.007 .009 .638 1 .424 Female =1 .081 .110 .538 1 .463 Taiwan=1 .028 .171 .026 1 .872 US=1 .498 .164 9.242 1 .002 Age Denmark=1 -1.848 .295 39.226 1 .000 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.204. Ref. category is China. Country coefficients express estimated difference from China. Table A11: Ordinal regression of importance of political loyalty (variable 1a) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 1a = 1] -2.998 .161 346.367 1 .000 [variable 1a = 2] -1.849 .133 194.194 1 .000 [variable 1a = 3] -1.014 .122 69.468 1 .000 [variable 1a = 4] .481 .120 16.164 1 .000 Age -.052 .005 125.509 1 .000 Female =1 -.159 .110 2.105 1 .147 Confucianism =1 2.427 .152 255.399 1 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.223. Non- Confucianist culture is reference (students from Denmark and US) Table A12: Ordinal regression of importance of loyalty to rules (variable 2d) Estimate Std. Error .000 Wald df Sig. [variable 2d = 1] -6.878 1.006 46.791 1 .000 [variable 2d = 2] -3.680 .230 256.419 1 .000 [variable 2d = 3] -1.587 .132 145.107 1 .000 [variable 2d = 4] .764 .123 38.812 1 .000 Age .001 .005 .019 1 .890 Female =1 .133 .112 1.414 1 .234 Confucianism =1 .290 .142 4.208 1 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.008 Non- Confucianist culture is reference (students from Denmark and US) Table A13: Ordinal regression of importance of good relations to higher authorities as a motive (variable 3g) Estimate Std. Error Wald df .040 Sig. [variable 3g = 1] -3.406 .295 133.358 1 .000 [variable 3g = 2] -1.112 .135 68.133 1 .000 [variable 3g = 3] .827 .123 45.489 1 .000 [variable 3g = 4] 2.955 .150 386.032 1 .000 Age .051 .005 112.591 1 .000 Female =1 .386 .111 12.133 1 .000 Confucianism =1 .434 .140 9.579 1 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.208 Non- Confucianist culture is reference (students from Denmark and US) .002 39 Table A14: Ordinal regression of recognition from management as a motive (variable 3i) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 3i = 1] -4.841 .420 132.727 1 .000 [variable 3i = 2] -2.906 .186 242.846 1 .000 [variable 3i = 3] -.692 .120 33.345 1 .000 [variable 3i = 4] 1.187 .124 92.419 1 .000 Age .001 .005 .085 1 .771 Female =1 .358 .110 10.616 1 .001 Confucianism =1 .672 .139 23.217 1 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.045 Non- Confucianist culture is reference (students from Denmark and US) .000 Table A15: Ordinal regression of importance of being adaptable (variable 2e) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 2e = 2] -4.352 .281 239.693 1 .000 [variable 2e = 3] -2.211 .145 231.576 1 .000 [variable 2e = 4] .322 .122 6.942 1 .008 Age .009 .005 3.610 1 .057 Female =1 .137 .114 1.455 1 .228 Confucianism =1 -.595 .145 16.824 1 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.019 Non-Confucianist culture is reference (students from Denmark and US) .000 Table A16: Ordinal regression of innovation as a general value (variable 1m) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 1m = 1] -6.118 .666 84.474 1 .000 [variable 1m = 2] -4.307 .406 112.387 1 .000 [variable 1m = 3] -1.314 .336 15.257 1 .000 [variable 1m = 4] .746 .334 4.989 1 .026 Age .008 .009 .755 1 .385 Female =1 .234 .112 4.353 1 .037 -1.025 .180 32.308 1 .000 .571 .306 3.478 1 .062 Taiwan=1 Denmark=1 China=1 -1.139 .169 45.467 1 .000 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.127. Reference category is US and the country coefficients express estimated difference from US students. 40 Table A17: Ordinal regression of ability to innovate (variable 2i) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 2i = 1] -6.239 .778 64.236 1 .000 [variable 2i = 2] -3.863 .391 97.449 1 .000 [variable 2i = 3] -1.103 .332 11.055 1 .001 [variable 2i = 4] .858 .331 6.724 1 .010 Age .009 .009 .932 1 .334 Female =1 .268 .111 5.850 1 .016 Taiwan=1 -.631 .178 12.560 1 .000 .454 .303 2.254 1 .133 Denmark=1 China=1 -.732 .165 19.732 1 .000 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.058. Reference category is US and the country coefficients express estimated difference from US students. Table A18: Ordinal regression of businesslike operations as a general value Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 1g = 1] -3.583 .372 92.801 1 .000 [variable 1g = 2] -1.748 .331 27.844 1 .000 [variable 1g = 3] .154 .324 .227 1 .634 [variable 1g = 4] 2.155 .331 42.373 1 .000 Age .015 .009 2.554 1 .110 Female =1 .286 .110 6.765 1 .009 Taiwan=1 -.214 .174 1.512 1 .219 Denmark=1 1.065 .299 12.665 1 .000 China=1 -1.512 .169 80.437 1 .000 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.187. Reference category is US and the country coefficients express estimated difference from US students. Table A19: Ordinal regression of being willing to take risks (variable 2c) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 2c = 1] -3.225 .369 76.560 1 .000 [variable 2c = 2] -2.189 .339 41.589 1 .000 [variable 2c = 3] .168 .325 .267 1 .606 [variable 2c = 4] 2.121 .333 40.628 1 .000 Age .007 .009 .562 1 .453 Female =1 .085 .111 .584 1 .445 Taiwan=1 1.456 .180 65.082 1 .000 Denmark=1 -.228 .299 .581 1 .446 China=1 1.601 .170 88.831 1 .000 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.207. Reference category is US and the country coefficients express estimated difference from US students. 41 Table A20: Ordinal regression of learning and development on the job as a motive Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 3h = 1] -7.213 .784 84.558 1 .000 [variable 3h = 2] -5.122 .422 147.369 1 .000 [variable 3h = 3] -2.563 .348 54.391 1 .000 [variable 3h = 4] -.163 .339 .231 1 .631 Age -.004 .010 .198 1 .656 .404 .114 12.570 1 .000 Taiwan=1 -1.104 .184 36.014 1 .000 Denmark=1 -1.397 .314 19.848 1 .000 Female =1 China=1 -.482 .170 7.996 1 .005 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.081. Reference category is US and the country coefficients express estimated difference from US students. Table A21: Ordinal regression of satisfying immediate needs of users as a general value (variable 1e) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 2e = 1] -5.121 .498 105.647 1 .000 [variable 2e = 2] -3.124 .355 77.593 1 .000 [variable 2e = 3] -.962 .330 8.497 1 .004 [variable 2e = 4] 1.203 .331 13.217 1 .000 Age -.012 .009 1.699 1 .192 Female =1 .412 .111 13.663 1 .000 Taiwan=1 .777 .178 19.026 1 .000 -.537 .302 3.165 1 .075 Denmark=1 China=1 .920 .167 30.363 1 .000 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.052. Reference category is US and the country coefficients express estimated difference from US students. Table A22: Ordinal regression of equal treatment (variable 1k) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 1k = 1] -5.191 .395 172.785 1 .000 [variable 1k = 2] -3.344 .192 303.211 1 .000 [variable 1k = 3] -1.905 .140 184.103 1 .000 [variable 1k = 4] -.075 .124 .368 1 .544 Age -.021 .010 4.223 1 .040 Female =1 .333 .118 7.942 1 .005 Taiwan=1 .043 .353 .015 1 .902 1.959 .362 29.314 1 .000 US=1 China=1 .011 .321 .001 1 .972 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.131. Reference category is Denmark and the country coefficients express estimated difference from Danish students. 42 Table A23: Ordinal regression of user democracy as a general value (variable 1o) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 1o = 1] -4.165 .279 222.824 1 .000 [variable 1o = 2] -2.792 .170 270.916 1 .000 [variable 1o = 3] -.673 .121 30.764 1 .000 [variable 1o = 4] 1.269 .126 101.227 1 .000 Age -.011 .009 1.546 1 .214 Female =1 .367 .111 10.932 1 .001 Taiwan=1 .402 .320 1.583 1 .208 US=1 .554 .303 3.349 1 .067 China=1 .179 .292 .377 1 .539 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.017. Reference category is Denmark and the country coefficients express estimated difference from Danish students. Table A24: Ordinal regression of having ethical awareness (variable 2b) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 2b = 1] -5.733 .588 94.915 1 .000 [variable 2b = 2] -4.257 .303 197.563 1 .000 [variable 2b = 3] -2.585 .174 220.906 1 .000 [variable 2b = 4] -.260 .132 3.901 1 .048 Age -.005 .011 .232 1 .630 Female =1 .566 .131 18.691 1 .000 Taiwan=1 -.840 .377 4.974 1 .026 US=1 1.229 .376 10.664 1 .001 China=1 1.006 .357 7.958 1 .005 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.147. Reference category is Denmark and the country coefficients express estimated difference from Danish students. Table A25: Ordinal regression of having personal integrity (variable 2f) Estimate Std. Error Wald df Sig. [variable 2f = 2] -6.104 .521 137.272 1 .000 [variable 2f = 3] -3.273 .194 285.065 1 .000 [variable 2f = 4] -1.227 .151 66.025 1 .000 -.001 .012 .004 1 .948 .320 .140 5.222 1 .022 -1.666 .404 16.994 1 .000 .826 .417 3.925 1 .048 Age Female =1 Taiwan=1 US=1 China=1 -.423 .374 1.278 1 .258 Note: Logit link function. Nagelkerke=0.153. Reference category is Denmark and the country coefficients express estimated difference from Danish students. 43