Archives of Sexual Behavior, Vol. 20, No. 5, 1991 Homosexual Mounting in Free-Ranging Female Hanuman Langurs (Presbytis Entellus) Arun Srivastava, Ph.D., 1 Carola Borries, Ph.D., 2 and Volker Sommer, Ph.D. 2,3 Female-female mounting was studied for 3233 hr in a bisexual one-male troop of free-ranging Hanuman langurs in northwestern India over a period of 6 years. The population breeds throughout the year and female-female mounts (n = 524) occurred during all months. All 15 adult females mounted and were mounted, although only 20% of the individuals exhibited more than half of all mounter activity and 33% more than half of all mountee activity. Various similarities with heterosexual mounting suggest that sexual arousal is a stimulus of the activity. Cycling, pregnant, and lactating females acted as mounters as well as mountees. However, ovulating individuals were greatly overrepresented concerning their likelihood of mounting and being mounted. Females of any given rank in the dominance hierarchy showed mounter as well as mountee activity, although mounters were higher ranking than mountees in 84% of cases. Young mounters were overrepresented and middle- to old-aged mounters underrepresented, whereas no such differences existed for mountees. Mounting relations among four young adult paternal half-sisters who occupied top positions in the dominance hierarchy were significantly overrepresented. Although the ultimate function of homosexual female mounting is not fully understood, it is suspected to be connected with intrasexual competition: If "pseudocopulations" induce mountees to reduce the number of solicitations addressed to the male, the probability of insemination decreases and mounters thus reduce the number of future competitors. KEY WORDS: Presbytis entellus; mounting; homosexuality; dominance rank; reproductive states. 1Department of Zoology, University of Jodhpur, Rajasthan, India. 2Institut f/ir Anthropologie, Universit~it G6ttingen, B/irgerstrasse 50, D-3400 G6ttingen, Germany. 3To whom correspondence should be addressed. 487 0004-0002/91/1000-0487506.50/0© 1991Plenum Publishing Corporation 488 Srivastava, Borries and Sommer INTRODUCTION Mounting behavior among females has been described for a large number of vertebrate animals (reviews in Young, 1941; Beach, 1968; Goy and Goldfoot, 1975; Denniston, 1980). It has been recognized that such behavior is generally neither associated with structural abnormalities or endocrinopathies nor represents "sex reversals," but "is instead to be considered a normal element in the behavioral repertoire" (Beach, 1968, p. 88). Unisexual mounts are of particular interest concerning the broader question of how nonreproductive patterns of sexual behavior (e.g., heterosexual activities during infertile periods such as pregnancy or lactation as well as homosexual activities) might have evolved. Hormones and social factors are important in triggering the behavior at the proximate level. In several species of nonhuman primates, female mounting occurs mainly during the ovulatory period of the cycle, when females are sexually receptive (Michael et al., 1974; Akers and Conaway, 1979). Age, sex, and dominance rank are likewise relevant to partner selection (e.g., Chevalier-Skolnikoff, 1976; Fedigan and Gouzoules, 1978). However, the behavior shows considerable interspecies variability concerning its frequency, form, and correlation with certain hormonal and social conditions. Therefore, the ultimate causes remain largely speculative. Functional explanations interpret female-female mounting as (i) a substitute for heterosexual mounting (Talmage-Riggs and Anschel, 1973; Wolfe, 1984), (ii) training for adult heterosexual roles (Akers and Conaway, 1979), (iii) an expression of dominance (e.g., Kempf, 1917; Zuckerman, 1932), (iv) a display of masculine behavior by receptive females which should stimulate male sexual activity (Ford and Beach, 1952; Parker and Pearson, 1976), or (v) an indication of Close affectional ties (Chevalier-Skolnikoff, 1976; Akers and Conaway, 1979). Most data on female-female mounting have been collected on domestic or laboratory animals and few quantitative studies exist for animals under seminatural conditions confined to large enclosures (Hanby and Brown, 1974; Gouzoules and Goy, 1983). Since there is general agreement that captivity might change the frequency and expression of sexual behavior (Beach, 1968; Chevalier-Skolnikoff, 1976; Akers and Conaway, 1979; Gouzoules and Goy, 1983) it seems necessary to investigate wild populations. For Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus), an Asian colobine monkey inhabiting wide parts of the Indian subcontinent, female-female mounting has been reported from several field sites (see Discussion). These observations are largely anecdotal and do not provide quantitative analyses of the possible influences of reproductive status, dominance rank, age, and Homosexual Mounting in Female Langurs 489 kinship. We examined these variables on the basis of data collected during a 6-year study of a free-ranging troop of Hanuman langurs in northwestern India and discuss them in the light of the potential adaptive significance of female-female mounting. MATERIAL AND METHODS Study Site In the vicinity of the city of Jodhpur (240 m above sea level) in Rajasthan, India, lives an isolated population of 1200-1300 Hanuman langurs which has been studied by various Indian and German researchers since 1967. The reproductive units are one-male-multi-female troops (harems). Females remain lifelong in their natal troop. Maturing males join bands of extratroop males, which besiege harems from time to time and try to oust the resident male. The climate is dry with maximum temperatures of up to 50 ° C, in May/June and minimum temperatures around 0 ° C in December/January; 90% of the average 360 mm of precipitation is received during the monsoon in July-September (for details, see Mohnot, 1974; Winkler et al., 1984; Sommer and Rajpurohit, 1989). Study Troop Data presented here concern a one-male troop living about 6 km west of Jodhpur city which is termed Kailana-I (KI; other publications sometimes use B19). The habitat is open shrub dominated by Prosopis juliflora, Acacia senegal, and Euphorbia caducifolia. Water is available throughout the year from artificial ponds. The langurs are not shy, due to provisioning by local people for religious reasons, which accounts for one-third of the total feeding time. Apart from feral dogs, natural predators are absent. From 1977 through 1988, the troop was subject to a long-term study. All members have been known individually since January 1977. Data Sampling Between October 1981 and September 1987, data on female-female mounting interactions totaling 3233 hr of ad-lib, sampling were collected during three distinct blocks of observation, each covered by one of the authors (Table I; for details, see Sommer, 1985; Borries, 1989; Srivastava, 1989). The numbers of adult females were 11, 13, and 12, respectively, but 490 Srivastava, Borries and Sommer Table I. Observation Schedule and Composition of the Study Troop Troop compositiona Ad lib. Observer Sommer Borries Srivastava E Adult Juvenile-young adult Infant Period sampling (hr) M F M F M F Total Oct. 81-Dec. 82 Jan. 85-Oct. 85 Oct. 86-Sep. 87 942.1 1018.5 1272.0 1 1 1 11 13 12 2 2 1 5 1 2 1 1 3 3 20 17 22 3232.6 a Censuses refer to 1 January of 1982, 1985, and 1987. Subsequently, 6 births occurred in 1982, 5 in 1985, and 7 in 1987. The number of adult females remained constant within a given block. The number of adult and immature males was higher during periods of resident male changes (Oct.-Nov. 81, June 82, Sep.-Dec. 82, see Sommer, 1988). remained constant within a given block. If not stated otherwise, all results refer to mounting interactions between postmenarchal females only and have been corrected for differences in observation time related to the number of females, their reproductive states, dominance ranks, and age-classes. For each category of analysis (e.g., age class, reproductive state, dominance rank) the number of female days were computed (expected value) and compared with the number of episodes during which female mounters or mountees belonged to certain categories of age, reproductive state, or dominance (observed value). The goodness-of-fit-test (Chi-square, twotailed, Siegel, 1985, p. 101 ft.) was applied, to reveal significant differences between observed and expected scores. Reproductive Parameters Jodhpur langurs have no breeding season, although there is a birth peak during March and a minimum in November (Sommer and Rajpurohit, 1989). The only nonbehavioral sign of the females' cycles are menstruations, which are mostly visible. Physical indications of estrus (such as genital swellings) are absent. However, more than 90% of all sexual solicitations addressed to males occur around the time of presumed ovulation. Copulations are almost invariably initiated by females (not males) through head shaking, presentation of the hindquarters, and lowering of the tail. On the basis of menstruations and solicitations/copulations, the duration of the follicular stage (menstruation to first copulation) and luteal stage (first copulation to menstruation) could be assessed with fair accuracy, as Homosexual Mounting in Female Langurs 491 revealed by comparison with hormonal studies (review in Sommer, 1985). To calculate expected vs. observed scores for different stages of the menstrual cycle, the duration of menstruation has been subtracted from the follicular stage and that of the ovulation phase from the luteal phase, resuiting in the following reproductive parameters: cycle length 24.1 _+ 3.6 days (n = 161); menstruation 1.9 _+ 1.0 days (n = 212); follicular stage 8.9 _+ 3.5 days (n = 80); ovulatory phase 4.0 - 2.3 days (n = 239); luteal stage 14.9 -2_4.2 days (n = 69). Gestation lengths average 200.3 +- 3.4 days (n = 31). On the basis of postconception estrous behavior, pregnancies were divided into four periods (cf. Sommer, 1987): Days 1-52 (no estrous signs); Days 53-88 (extended estrous behavior with frequent solicitations and copulations); Days 89-136 (less frequent solicitations, no copulations); Days 137-term (no estrous signs). The lactational amenorrhea is considered to be over with the renewed onset of a regular menstrual cycle. Females reach menarche at 2.4 _+0.3 years of age (n = 10; for details, see Sommer et al., in press). Dominance Hierarchy The direction of dyadic displacement activities (n = 2940 episodes) were used to generate a dominance hierarchy that produced the fewest reversals. As a rule of thumb, females gain top positions in the hierarchy as soon as they start reproducing and descend to bottom ranks as they grow older (for details, see Borries et al., 1991). Age Classes Age classes include the following categories: young (menarche-9 years); middle-aged (10-19 years); old (20+ years) (Borries et al., 1991). RESULTS During the study, 524 episodes of mounting between adult females were observed (207 in 1981/1982, 178 in 1985, 139 in 1986/1987), in which mounter as well as mountee could be identified (Table II). Variables such as dominance rank, reproductive status, age, etc. of the partners could be determined for varying proportions of the total sample. 492 Srivastuva, Borries and Sommer © ~ ,= I r~ < O ("4 OOt"4 r'--~ I TM [ ¢~ I - . ~ Homosexual Mounting in Female Langurs 493 ~.= ~ I I ~ .~ ~= ~..u~ ~o0 ,-, ~ ~ ~,~ "u II .,..c " ~ •-~ ~ . ~ .= ~ ~ " ~ ~__~ ~ Srivastava, Borries and S u m m e r 494 Table III. Age-Sex Class Distribution of Nonrcproductive Mounting in the O n e Male Study Troop" Mounter Mountee Episodes Deviation from expectation (for A F only ) (%) AF AF JF JM AM 286 48 13 2 + 33.1 -3.3 -8.4 -4.1 JF AF JF JM AM 24 +++ ++ -9.0 (p < 0.01) JM AF JF JM AM 14 ++ +++ + -8.2 (p < 0.01) AM JF JM + + Infant either sex + Infant either sex (p < 0.001) (ns) (p < 0.01) (p < 0.05) a A F = adult female, JF = juvenile female, JM = juvenile male, A M = adult male. Observed versus expected frequencies could be calculated for interactions involving adult females only, since they were subjects to focal animal sampling. F o r interactions b e t w e e n o t h e r age-sex classes, the following qualitative categories were applied: - = not observed; + = rare, + + = moderate, + + + = frequent. Data for 1981-1982 and 1986-1987 only. Age--Sex Class Distribution of Nonreproductive Mounting Nonreproductive mounting (i.e., mounts involving partners other than sexually mature male mounters with adult female mountees) did not occur only among adult females but involved all possible combinations between different age--sex-classes. However, adult females clearly preferred each other whereas all other categories involving adult females were significantly underrepresented, except for the combination adult female-juvenile female (Table III). The youngest female mounted by an adult female was an infant of 7.6 months, the youngest male 7.9 months; the youngest individuals, who mounted adult females (or at least tried to do so) were a female of 3.1 months and a male of 5.1 months. If not indicated otherwise, all results refer to adult female-adult female mounting only. Homosexual Mounting in Female Langurs 495 Fig. 1. Female-female mounts closely resembled male-female copulations. Female 4 (right) solicited Female 3 through head shaking, whereupon Female 3 clings jusl below the knees of Female 4. The pelvic thrusting of Female 3 is accompanied by grunt-and-grimacing (22 Apr. 82). Qualitative Features and Contexts Female-female mountings were brief interactions, lasting about 5 sec on average with a maximum of about 10 sec. In the majority of the cases, mounters apply a double foot-clasp on the mountee's lower legs (Fig. 1). Less frequently, they may stand on the ground. Unlike mounting positions described for Macaca mulatta (Akers and Conaway, 1979) or Macaca arctoides (Chevalier-Skolnikoff, 1976), the mounter does not rub her genitals on the rump of the mountee but thrusts against her buttocks. The clitoris of the mounter is thus not directly stimulated, although that of the mountee might be. However, indirect mounter 496 Srivastava, Borries and Sommer Table IV. Behaviors Connected with Female-Female Mounting a Behavior Mountee solicits adult male or copulates Mountee solicits female mounter Pelvic thrusts of mounter Preceding mounting During Succeeding mounting mounting Proportion b (%) 19 11 13.4 28 38 29.5 39.3c 29 10 18.8 4.9 88 Mounter grooms mountee Mountee grooms mounter 13 1 Embracing, pulling of facial fur Mounter mounts quadrupedicallyd Jumping display of mounter Jumping display of mountee Mountee resists (slaps mounter) Teeth grinding of mounter 14 Mounter displaces mountee Mountee displaces mounter Mountee displaces 3rd individual 3rd individual displaces mountee 3rd individual displaces mounter "~ J "~ 42.9 23.7 J 7.6 2.7 I 1 1 0.9 I,. 2 J 1 1 1.3 1.3 0.9 4 0 1 4 1 3 0 3.1 0.0 0.4 1.8 0.9 } 1 2.2 6.3 a224 episodes, data for 1981-1982 only; in 66 cases, either mounter or mountee was a nonadult individual, usually a perimenarchal female. t'Totai > 100% since a given episode could be accompanied by several variables. c During 1986-1987, pelvic thrusts occurred in 33.6% out of 128 closely monitored episodes. aStanding on mountee's back. stimulation might be achieved due to mechanical pressure on body portions surrounding the clitoral region. There were no visible indications of orgasm for either mounter or mountee, but these are likewise absent in mountees participating in heterosexual copulations. Manual or oral stimulation of the genitalia were not observed. Moreover, a relationship existed between the mounting syndrome and various agonistic behavioral patterns (Table IV). In the context of mounting, mounters sometimes exhibited head bobbing (a mild threat gesture), grunt-and-grimacing (indicating excitement), teeth grinding (indicating tension or threat), or displaced mountees. Both mounter and mountee sometimes participated in jumping displays or embraced each other (a common gesture of reassurance). Behaviors accompanying female-female mounting were in some respect strikingly similar to the sequence of male-female copulations (Table IV). Although the majority of interactions seemed to be initiated by the mounter, in 19 cases (8.5%) out of 224 episodes, female mounters responded to solicitations which were directed towards a male. In 28 cases Homosexual Mounting in Female Langurs % 20 o AD. F E M A L E - AO. FEMALE • ALL N O N R E P R O D U C T I V E 497 MOUNTS MOUNTS [n= 128 ( n= 266 ] } - p, / 10 - "o__o ' l 6 ~ I 7 I 8 ,', I I i I I I I I I I DAy9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 TIME Fig. 2. Diurnal distribution of female-female mounting. Data for 1986-1987 only, when observation hours were equally distributed over the day. Broken line indicates expectation. (12.5%) the typical heterosexual solicitation was directly displayed towards the subsequent female mounter. In 38 cases (16.7%) unsuccessful solicitations occurred after the mount. Pelvic thrusting occurred in more than one-third of all cases. The mean number of thrusts was 6.0 (n = 41 episodes, range 2-11 thrusts). As during heterosexual contacts of estrous females with males, mountings were frequently accompanied by allogrooming. Following copulations, females often groom the male, whereas males rarely groom females. However, such a clear-cut role of "mountee = active groomer" vs. "mounter = passive groomee" could not be found in femalefemale interactions. The reation was rather reversed, since out of 53 grooming interactions in the context of mounting, 79.2% were performed by the mounter. Heterosexual copulations were subject to sexual harassment by nonmating individuals, especially adult females, in more than 80% of all cases (Sommer, 1989a). In few instances, unisexual female "couples" were likewise harassed. Diurnal and Annual Distribution On average, each female participated in a mounting episode every 59.6 hr, i.e., once every 5th day. The diurnal activity had a morning peak, a depression around noon, and another less pronounced peak in the late afternoon, )~z(12) = 46.60, p < 0.001 (Fig. 2). Thus it resembles the activity pattern observed for monitoring, feeding, and locomotion tWinkler, 1988; Srivastava, 1989). It is higher during the cool hours of the day and lower during the hot hours. Mounts occurred during all months, although not equally distributed, Z2 (11) = 78.56, p < 0.001 (Fig. 3). Seasonal fluctua- Srivastava, Borries and S o m m e r 498 % 20 10 ~ _ J FM A M J J A S 0 N D Fig. 3. A n n u a l distribution of female-female mounting, n = 497 episodes recorded b e t w e e n 1981-1987. Broken line indicates expectation. tion did not correlate with the overall reproductive activity (e.g., conceptions, births). Mounting and Reproductive Status Cycling, pregnant, and lactating individuals all acted as mounter and mountee, although several deviations from the given expectations were significant (Fig. 4). During menstruation, follicular, and luteal periods, participation was significantly underrepresented for mounters as well as mountees. Around ovulation, mounter activity was 1.6 times and mountee activity even 4.7 times higher than expected. Mounters had additional significant peaks during the main phase of postconception estrus (Days 53-88 of pregnancy) and during lactation (Days 1-50, Days 151-200). Mountees had significant minima during the last anestrous phase of pregnancy (Days 137-term) as well as during late lactation (Days 151-200). Cases in which mounter as well as mountee were anestrous accounted for only 14.5% of all episodes. Mounting and Dominance Mounting behavior clearly paralleled the female displacement hierarchy. Mounters were higher ranking than mountees in 84.2% of all cases (N = 436), a significant difference found during each period of the study (80.8% out of 130 cases during 1981/1982; 91.6% out of 178 cases during 1985; 77.3% out of 128 cases during 1986/1987). Homosexual Mounting in Female Langurs 499 ICycling I 4s2 MENSTRUATING I FOLLICULAR 34 ] 11114 [ 231 OVULATORY [ '°'11:~, f ~ .,k ,,k "lc'~/a I 4c'41 LUTEAL Pregnant ANESTROUS DAy 26 I 6 8 6 I--, ' --" , :Ft'I~: E S TR O U S / C O P U L A T I N G I 517 I DAY I~3 - - Sip I 1271 ESTROUS/SOLICITiNG DAY 1t9 - - 1~6 IANESTROUS IDAY 137 - - TERM I ,r I ~ A I 155 I "~'1231 69 I 43 ]61211 ,.~.~ ', ! i JLactating ll*x~ oA..--5o i ]5.o,;;, r J DAY°" 5, -- 15o I~, ] "'[~:r 20° J .,is:l __J m MOUNTER DAYS ~] MOUNTEE EPISODES - ! EXPECTATION i I ii ,.. I I 2 I M tttt~ I 6 [Mounts/Day I ,//--7 I 22 10 3 X 10 - 2 Fig. 4. Reproductive status of female mounters and mountees. "Days" refer to the total number of observation days of females in a given reproductive status, "episodes" indicates the total number of mounting events. Significance values for observed versus expected: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Females of any rank position did act as mounter as well as mountee. Mounters were significantly overrepresented in the high-ranking class and significantly underrepresented in the middle- and low-ranking classes (Fig. 5). For mountees, a similar trend held true. When the actual rank difference between mounter and mountee was analyzed, dyads with mounters ranking 1-3 positions higher in the hierarchy turned out to be significantly overrepresented, whereas most dyads with mounters ranking 3 or more positions lower were significantly underrepresented (Fig. 6). 500 Srivastava, Borries and Sommer DEVIATION FROM EXPECTATION % • MOUNTER [] MOUNTEE *'k'k ÷40 *20 -20 -k~Ik -40 RANK H I GH EPISODES 305,/187 MIDDLE 78 / 126 LOW 53 / 123 Fig. 5. Female-female mounting: Distribution over rank classes. High-ranking females (first third of displacement hierarchy; depending on the total number of females, usually the first 4 positions); middle-ranking (second third); low-ranking (last third, usually the last 4 positions). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.0t, ***p < 0.001. Mounting and Age Concerning age classes, there was no difference between the observed and expected distribution for mountees, whereas young mounters were significantly overrepresented and middle-aged as well as old ones significantly underrepresented (Fig. 7). These results closely resemble correlations between mounting and rank (see Fig. 5). This is not surprising, since there is a significant negative correlation between age and dominance rank in free-ranging female Hanuman langurs (Bottles et al., 1991). The relative age difference of the partners was known for 97 dyads. Out of these, 66.0% represented the combination "younger mounter/older mountee" with a mean of 5.7 episodes per dyad (n = 367); 34.0% combined "older mounter/younger mountee" with a mean of 2.8 episodes per dyad (n = 92). Interindividual Variability In the course of the study (1981-1987), all adult females acted as m o u n t e r as well as m o u n t e e (see Table II; the number of individuals Homosexual Mounting in Female Langurs EPISODES 501 dr~rtk *,A~r ] EXPECTED ] OBSERVED 84 '°1 78 i i~i !ililili~,!i~!~ii!! 72 30-- 24-- 12-- iiiii! iii!, i!~ii!ili!!iil~i!i!i!ii!!!i~iiii '~|2 "11 410 ~'9 ~11 * 7 46 t S ~4 • 3 ¢'2 ÷1 MOUNTER HIGHER RANKING ilill! ,,;',i!iiii ~iii!!ii!l -1 - 2 - 3 - 4 - 5 - 6 - 7 - I - 9 MOUNTER LOWER RANKING -10 - I ! -12 RANK DIFFERENCE Fig. 6. Observed vs. expected rank differences between female mounters and mountees. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. acting as mounter/mountee was 10/11 for 11 females during 1981/1982, 10/12 for 13 females during 1985, and 8/12 for 12 females during 1986/1987). However, there was considerable interindividual variability: 20.0% of all mountees accounted for more than half (50.8%) of all episodes (n = 524) and 33.3% of all mountees accounted for 54.8%. Individual ratios of mounter:mountee activity varied from 12.0:1 (Female 3.2) to 1:22.0 (Female 4). Out of 194 possible dyads" between individuals (i.e., matrix cells in Table II), 39.1% were zero dyads. Of the remaining nonzero dyads, 31.4% were reciprocal, i.e., both individuals involved acted sometimes as mounter and sometimes as mountee. Of all dyads, 8.1% accounted for more than half (50.4%) of the total events. Moreover the majority (59.7%) of all mount-giving activity was restricted to 3 mountees only and the majority (60.5%) of all mount-receiving activity to 2 mounters only. Mounting relations were extraordinarily well developed between the four paternal half-sisters, who occupied the top positions in the domi- 502 Srivastava, Borries and Summer DEVIATION FROM EXPECTATION l MO U N T E R [] M O U N T E E % , k ~,,llr ÷ 40 -- + 20 -- - 20 - - 4 0 - % AGE EPISODES YOUNG 350/152 MIDDLE 50 /116 OLD 94 / 196 Fig. 7. Female-female mounting: Distribution over age classes. Young (menarche-9 years), middle-aged (10-19 years), old (20+ years). ***p < 0.001. nance hierarchy for most of the time through at least 1987 (cf. Borries et al., 1991). This age cohort performed 54.7% of all their combined mount-giving activity among themselves and 72.0% of all mountreceiving, which is 10-13 times more than expected. Much of the pattern of rank differences between mounters and mountees (see Fig. 6) is due to the fact that Females 3.2 and 6.3 for most of the time maintained a rank difference of 3 positions and Females 4.4 and 6.3 a difference of 1-2 positions. The only other known kinship relations involved the four mothers of the half-sisters. However, m o t h e r - d a u g h t e r dyads were clearly underrepresented (n = 6 episodes). They account for only 1.3% of all activity performed by daughters and 2.8% of all maternal activity. DISCUSSION F e m a l e - f e m a l e mounting among J o d h p u r langurs in general matches the pattern reported for other study sites in India, Nepal, and Sri Lanka (Table V). There is a single study site (Junbesi, Nepal) for which the absence of female mounting is explicitly mentioned. Most of Homosexual Mounting in Female Langurs 503 these qualitative reports likewise emphasize the participation of adult females in nonreproductive mounting with other age-sex-classes, especially male and female immatures (cf. Table III). The extremely rare occurrence of adult female-adult male mounts seems to be a speciesspecific pattern. Male-male mounts, which are especially frequent in male bands, are the subject of a forthcoming paper (Sommer and Moore, in preparation). Hormonal Influences Among mammals, the degree of pre- and postnatal endocrine influence on female-female mounting varies considerably (reviews in Beach, 1968; Goy and Goldfoot, 1975). As a rule, mounting activity is n o t n e c e s sarily dependent on certain hormonal conditions; nevertheless, it is m o r e likely to coincide with certain reproductive states. This is true for langurs as well; mounter as well as mountee activity occurred during all stages of the menstrual cycle, pregnancy, and lactation, although significant peaks and minima could be recognized (cf. Fig. 4). In other mammals, mounting occurs frequently during estrus when females are sexually receptive to males. Nevertheless, whereas the mounter might be estrous o r anestrous, the individual most likely to be mounted is a second female in estrus (e.g., lions, domestic cats, dogs, cattle, Beach, 1968; rhesus macaques, Carpenter, 1942; p. 150; Akers and Conaway, 1979; Japanese macaques, Hanby and Brown, 1974; chimpanzees, Yerkes, 1939; gorillas, Harcourt, 1979; but see stumptail macaques, Chevalier-Skolnikoff, 1976). Langurs behave similarly since both mounter and mountee activity was significantly higher around the time of presumed ovulation (for hormonal studies, see Chowdhury and Chandra, 1984). Mounting did not cease during pregnancy. However, the peak in heterosexual postconception activity (Days 53--88) coincides with an increased activity of mounters but not mountees. This suggests that endocrine correlates of estrous activity during ovulation differ from those postconception. Reports about postconception mountee activities are lacking for other primates. In rhesus macaques, mounter activity ceases with the onset of pregnancy (Akers and Conaway, 1979), whereas in J a p a n e s e macaques, early p r e g n a n c y facilitated m o u n t e r activity (Gouzoules and Goy, 1983). During lactation, mounting activity showed an irregular distribution for Jodhpur langurs. Mounter activity was overrepresented during Months 504 Srivastava, Borries and Sommer E o 0 ~ "~ 0 ~E e~ Z 0 E ,'~ I N <, g- z z Homosexual Mounting in Female Langurs v ~ c: 0 ~5 o "6 Z t~ o Z Z 0 z < O z ~'~ ~ 2 5{}5 506 Srivastava, Borries and Sommer 1-2 and from Month 6 onwards, whereas mountee activity was underrepresented from Month 6 onwards. One would expect that mounting activity generally increases after infants develop motoric independence. However, locomotor abilities of infants are probably not crucial in this regard since langurs have a well-expressed system of infant transfer (review in Hrdy, 1976) which frees the mothers for foraging and social activities (Sommer, 1989b). In fact, almost all mounting occurred while troop members other than the mother took care of the babies (Sommer, 1985). For stumptail macaques, where mounting females were always lactating (Chevalier-Skolnikoff, 1976), caretaking behavior by nonmothers was likewise reported. In Japanese macaques, however, having a dependent infant clearly inhibited mounter activity (Gouzoules and Goy, 1983). Sexuality, Dominance, and Gender Role Wickler (1967) argued that in a heterosexual context, presentation of the hindquarters (a "signal" commonly preceding mounting) can be liberated from a strictly sexual motivation and employed to turn aggressive motivations of a dominant individual into sexual ones. Accordingly, for the part of the signal sender, he believed that social and sexual presentation have the same form but different motivations, whereas "the re.~ponse of the partner has remained sexually unaltered" (Wickler, 1967; p. 108). However, concerning same-sex mounting, Wickler used the term "homosexual" only if an individual clearly prefers same-sex partners in a purely sexual context (Wickler, 1969, p. 58). Moreover, same-sex mounts are generally interpreted as "social dominance gestures" (Wickler, 1967, p. 108). At the same time, mount-giving behavior is linked with a male gender and mountreceiving with a female gender: "In the social context, male sexual actions imply dominance and female sexual actions submissiveness--both sexes are capable of exhibiting both types of interaction with an animal of either sex" (Wickler, 1967, p. 109). Thus, the mountee is assumed to play the "feminine" role in a male-male mounting whereas the mounter in a female-female mounting plays the "masculine" role. This concept of "inversion," which originally referred to human homosexuality (e.g., Freud, 1905/1961), postulates a sex-reversal during all mounts that are not malefemale. However, at least for Hanuman langurs it seems difficult to establish such a dichotomy: Female mounters can hardly be labeled as "androgenized" or "masculinized" individuals because the same hormonal conditions around midcycle stimulate to "play the female role" in a heterosexual Homosexual Mounting in Female Langurs 507 mounting as well as to "play the male role" in a female-female mounting (see Fig. 4). Presbytis entellus clearly belongs to those species where the pattern "dominant mounter-subordinate mountee" is followed rather rigidly (Figs. 5, 6). But even then, the equation of dominance and mounter role can nevertheless be criticized, because Wickler assumed that same-sex mounts almost always demonstrate dominance. However, cause and effect can hardly be distinguished, i.e., whether mounting actually influences the hierarchy or simply reflects its existence. Moreover, female-female mounts include behavioral patterns which--in a heterosexual context--would be viewed as clear indicators of sexual interest: solicitation, pelvic thrusting, grooming, touching of the genital regions (see Table IV) might be expressions of true "homosexual" motivations (as already stated by Weber and Vogel, 1970, Vogel, 1976, p. 132). Hence, it seems virtually impossible to separate "sexual mounting" from "dominance mounting" as tried by Ripley (1965, p. 213). Furthermore, it seems questionable to almost equate the mounter position with dominance and that of the mountee with submissiveness, notwithstanding the fact that in many species female mounters are usually dominant individuals (nonprimates, e.g., white leghorn hens, Guhl, 1948; mountain sheep, Geist, 1968; primates, e.g., Papio cynocephalus, Anthoney, 1968; Saimiri sciureus, T a l m a g e - R i g g s and Anschel, 1973, Macaca arctoides, Chevalier-Skolnikoff, 1976; Macaca mulatta, A k e r s and Conaway, 1979; cf. review in Wickler, 1967, p. 109). However, this pattern does not always hold even within the same species: for example, males of Macaca mulatta do tolerate mounting by females although males clearly dominate females (Michael et al., 1974). In other species (e.g., Miopithecus talapoin, Wolfheim and Rowell, 1972; Macaca fuscata, Gouzoules and Goy, 1983) there is no relation at all between the dominance hierarchy and same-sex mounts. Sexual arousal and dominance are obviously not mutually exclusive in langur females, since female-female mounting is related to both dominance and sexuality. The Question of Function Ford and Beach (1952) proposed that the temporary female display of "masculine behavior," such as mounting, functions to prompt sexually sluggish males into action. Beach (1976) considered female-male mounting one form of "proceptivity" that should arouse males specifically at the time 508 Srivastava, Borries and S o m m e r females are fertile. Parker and Pearson (1976) assumed that, by mimicking a potential rival through display of male copulatory patterns, a female mounter could attract a male's attention, resulting in copulation. In captive rhesus monkeys, if males failed to ejaculate despite repeated female sexual solicitations, females were more likely to mount the males who could be aroused by such mountings (Michael et al., 1974). Although female-male mountings are very rare in langurs (cf. Tables III, V), female-female mounts might theoretically stimulate a male as well, by either "arousing" him or making him "jealous." In fact, harem residents do often not respond to female solicitations (cf. Mohnot, 1974, p. 165; Vogel, 1976, p. 119; Hrdy, 1977, p. 141; Sommer, 1985, p. 68). The hypothesis draws some support because in 8.5% of all cases, unsuccessful solicitations of males preceded a mounting episode between females and in another 4.9% solicitations of males followed female-female mounts (of. Table IV). Since males sometimes inspect the genital region of presenting females, it is likely that males try to assess the reproductive condition of females. If female-female mounting would in fact stimulate males, one must not necessarily assume that mounters act cooperatively (by increasing the chances to copulate for the mountee) if a mounter is herself motivated to copulate. However, several indications suggest that langur female mounting did not evolve to stimulate males. First, female mounting often occurred when males were definitely out of sight. Second, mounters were potentially fertile in only 14.3% of all episodes and mountees in 40.7%, whereas the majority of mounting took place during infertile stages (see Fig. 4). Similarly, Gouzoules and Goy (1983) rejected the hypothesis for Japanese macaques, since a significantly higher proportion of female-male and female-female mounts occurred after the females had conceived. Nevertheless, we believe that the functional explanations of Beach (1976) and Parker and Pearson (1976) have to be rejected for a different reason. In langur one-male troops, copulations during infertile periods might be a strategy of females to deplete sperm otherwise available to competing fertile females (Sommer, 1989a, Sommer and Rajpurohit, 1989; cf. also Small, 1988): (i) 81% of all copulations were harassed. Higher ranking females harassed copulations of lower rankings twice as often as vice versa, copulations of top-ranking females were only rarely disturbed and 75% of all copulations of pregnant females were likewise harassed. (Female-female mounts, on the other hand, were rarely harassed; see Table V.) Sperm competition is assumed to be more intense in larger harems. In fact, interbirth intervals in larger harems were significantly longer than those in smaller harems, although this difference cannot be ascribed to food availability (Srivastava, 1989, p. 219). (iii) Females copulated significantly more fre- Homosexual Mounting in Female Langurs 509 quently during conception estrus. During days of conception, the average number of females copulating at the same day was significantly smaller than at given nonconception days (Sommer et aL, in press). Therefore, female-female mounting could be related to femalefemale competition in order to minimize the probability that a competitor conceives. Weber and Vogel (1970) ascribed a "drive-reducing function" to langur male-male mounting. Similarly, pseudocopulations among females-which often include pelvic thrusting (see Tables IV, V)--could provide a surrogate sexual satisfaction to the mountee especially if a male ignores heterosexual solicitations (Sommer, 1985, 1988). An estrous female might consequently reduce her frequency of solicitation. With that, the probability of insemination (and corresponding sperm depletion of the male) decreases. Mounters thus reduce the number of future resource competitors (and increase their own chances to conceive). The likelihood of being rejected is low for mounters (see Table IV) probably because the mountee is usually lower ranking and estrous (see Figs. 4, 6). Hrdy (1977) reported that estrous females sometimes tried to avoid mounting by other females by placing her rump closely to the resident male while soliciting him. However, during the present study, many solicitations were addressed to females, which is somewhat contradictory to the proposed hypothesis. On the other hand, estrous females gave the impression of being under a considerable "hormonal pressure" prompting them to accept mounters of any sex. Resource competition is reduced if the competitor is closely related. In fact, mounters obviously discriminated according to the degree of relatedness (r). Mother-daughter pairs rarely mounted; with both future grandchildren and half-siblings, r is on average 0.25. Half-sisters (different mothers, same fathers) mounted each other significantly more than expected; with a half-sister's offspring, r is on average 0.125 (see Table II). The hypothesis calls for another prediction: Females, who are seldom mounted should produce more offspring than females who are often mounted. In fact, during a 12-year period, high-ranking females of the study troop produced significantly more offspring than low-ranking females (Borries et al., 1991). However, it would be premature to draw a causal conclusion to female-female mounting, since the likelihood of conception might well be connected with other rank-related factors such as food resources, physical condition, or frequency of sexual harassment. Moreover, it remains unclear why mountees so rarely resist. Further clarification of the proximate and ultimate causes underlying female-female mounting in langurs requires an interspecific comparison Srivastava, Borries and Sommer 510 that relates the degree of same-sex m o u n t i n g to different social and mating structures. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS T h e fieldwork was sponsored by grants from M a n - a n d - t h e - B i o s p h e r e P r o g r a m m e , D e p a r t m e n t o f Environment, G o v e r n m e n t of India, New Delhi (A.S.), the I n d o - G e r m a n Cultural Exchange P r o g r a m m e o f the G e r m a n A c a d e m i c E x c h a n g e Service ( D A A D ) , Bonn and the Ministry o f Education, Culture, and Social Welfare, G o v e r n m e n t of India, New Delhi (C.B., V.S.). S u b s e q u e n t f i n a n c i a l s u p p o r t was g r a n t e d by D A A D (A.S.), Graf~Sg Niedersachsen, the Richard-Winter-Foundation (C.B.), t h e G e r m a n R e s e a r c h Council (V.S.), a n d the A c a d e m y o f Sciences, University o f G6ttingen. W e are gratefully indebted to S. M. M o h n o t , J o d h p u r , and Christian Vogel, G 6 t t i n g e n , for supervision. F o r critical c o m m e n t s on an earlier draft, we t h a n k S u z a n n e Chevalier-Skolnikoff, Jutta K~ister, Jim M o o r e , A n d r e a s Paul, E c k a r t Voland, Paul Winkler, and 3 a n o n y m o u s j o u r n a l referees. REFERENCES Akers, J. S. and Conaway, C. H. (1979). Female homosexual behavior in Macaca mulatta. Arch. Sex. Behav. 8: 63-80. Anthoney, T. (1968). The ontogeny of greeting, grooming, and sexual motor patterns in captive baboons (superspecies Papio cynocephalus). Behavior 31: 399-412. Beach, F. A. (1968). Factors involved in the control of mounting behavior by female mammals. In Diamond, M. (ed.), Perspectives in Reproduction and Sexual Behavior, Indiana University Press, Bloomington, pp. 83-131. Beach, F. A. (1976). Sexual attractivity, proceptivity, and receptivity in female mammals. Horm. Behav. 7: 105-138. Bishop, N. (1975). Social behavior of langur monkeys (Presbytis entellus) in a high altitude environment. Ph.D. thesis, University of California, Berkeley. Boggess, J. E. (1976). Social behavior of the Himalayan langur (Presbytis entellus) in eastern Nepal. Ph.D. thesis, University of California, Berkeley. Borries, C. (1989). Konkurrenz unter freilebenden Langurenweibchen (Presbytis entellus). Ph.D. thesis, University of G6ttingen, Germany. Borries, C., Sommer, V., and Srivastava, A. (1991). Dominance, age, and reproductive success in free-ranging female Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). Int. J. Primatol. 12: 231-258. Carpenter, C. R. (1942). Sexual behavior of the free-ranging rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). II. Periodicity of estrus, homosexual, autoerotic, and non-conformist behavior. J. Com. Psychol. 33: 143-162. Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. (1976). Homosexual behavior in a laboratory group of stumptail monkeys (Macaca arctoides): Forms, context, and possible social functions. Arch. Sex. Behav. 5: 511-527. Homosexual Mounting in Female Langurs 511 Chowdhury, S. R., and Chandra, H. (1984). Induction of menstrual cyclicity and circulating levels of plasma progesterone and estradiol in the iangurs (Presbytis entellus). In Roonwal, M. L., Mohnot, S. M., and Rathore, N.S. (eds.), Current Primate Researches, University of Jodhpur, India, pp. 437-440. Denniston, R. M. (1980). Ambisexuality in animals. In Marmor, J. (ed.), Homosexual Behaviour- A Modern Reappraisal, Basic Books, New York, pp. 25-40. Fedigan, L. M., and Gouzoules, H. (1978). The consort relationship in a troop of Japanese monkeys. In Chivers, D. J., and Herbert, J. (eds.), Recent Advances in Primatology, VoL 1: Behaviour, Academic Press, London, pp. 493-495. Ford, C. S., and Beach, F. A. (1952). Patterns of Sexual Behavior, Eyre & Spottiswoode, London. Freud, S. (1961). Drei Abhandlungen zur Sexualtheorie, Fischer, Frankfurt am Main. (Originally published 1905). Geist, V. (1968). On the interrelation of external appearance, social behavior and social structure of mountain sheep. Z. TierpsychoL 25" 199-215. Gouzoules, H., and Goy, R. W. (1983). Physiological and social influences on mounting behavior of troop-living female monkeys (Macaca fuscata). Am. J. PrimatoL 5: 39-49. Goy, R., and Goldfoot, D. (1975). Neuroendocrinology: Animal models and problems of human sexuality. Arch. Sex. Behav. 4: 405-420. Guhl, A. M. (1948). Unisexual mating in a flock of white leghorn hens. Transact. Kansas Acad. Sci. 51: 107-111. Hanby, J. P., and Brown, C. E. (1974). The development of sociosexual behaviors in Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata. Behavior 49: 152-196. Harcourt, A. H. (1979). Social relationships among adult female mountain gorillas. Anita. Behav. 27: 251-264. Hrdy, S. B. (1976). Care and exploitation of nonhuman primate infants by conspecifics other than the mother. In Rosenblatt, J. S., Hinde, R. A., Shaw, E. A., and Beer, C. (eds.), Advances in the Study ofBehavios, VoL 6, Academic Press, New York, pp. 101-158. Hrdy, S. B. (1977). The Langurs of Abu, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. Jay, P. (1963). The social behavior of the langur monkey, Ph.D. thesis, University of Chicago, Chicago. Kempf, E. J. (1917). The social and sexual behavior of infrahuman primates with some comparable facts in human behavior. PsychoanaL Rev. 4: 127-154. McKenna, J. J. (1975). An analysis of the social roles and behavior of seventeen captive Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). Ph.D. Thesis, University of Oregon, Eugene. Michael, R. P., Wilson, M. I., and Zumpe, D. (1974). The bisexual behavior of female rhesus monkeys. In Friedman, R. C., Richart, R. M., and Vande Wiele, R. L. (eds.), Sex Differences in Behavior, Wiley, New York, pp. 399-412. Mohnot, S. M. (1974). Ecology and behavior of the common Indian langur, Presbytis entellus Dufresne. Ph.D. thesis, University of Jodhpur, India. Parker, G. A., and Pearson, R. G. (1976). A possible origin and adaptive significance of the mounting behavior shown by some female mammals in oestrus. Z Natur. Hist. 10: 241-245. Ripley, S. (1965). The ecology and social behavior of the Ceylon gray langur, Presbytis entellus thersites. Ph.D. Thesis, University of California, Berkeley. Siegel, S. (1985). Nichtparametrische statistische Methoden, 2nd ed., Fachbuchhandlung fiir Psychologie, Eschborn. Small, M. F. (1988). Female primate sexual behavior and conceptionRAre there really sperm to spare? CULT.Anthropol. 29: 81-100. Sommer, V. (1985). Weibliche und miinnliche Reproduktionsstrategien der Hanuman Languren (Presbytis entellus) von Jodhpur, Rajasthan/Indien. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Grttingen, Germany. Sommer, V. (1987). Infanticide among free-ranging langurs (Presbytis entellus) at Jodhpur (Rajasthan/India): Recent observations and a reconsideration of hypotheses. Primates 28: 163-197. Sommer, V. (1988). Female-female mounting in langurs (Presbytis entelhts), hTt. Z PrimatoL 8: 478. 512 Srivastava, Borries and Sommer Sommer, V. (1989a). Sexual harassment in langur monkeys (Presbytis entellus): Competition for nurture, eggs, and sperm? Ethology 80: 205-217. Sommer, V. (1989b). Infant mistreatment in langur monkeys-Sociobiology tackled from the wrong end? In Rasa, A. E., Vogel, C., and Voland, E. (eds.), The Sociobiology of Sexual and Reproductive Strategies, Chapman and Hall, London, pp. 110-127. Sommer, V., and Rajpurohit, L. S. (1989). Male reproductive success in harem troops of Hanuman langurs (Prbsbytis entellus). Int. Z PrimatoL 10: 293-317. Sommer, V., Srivastava, A., and Borries, C. (in press). Cycles, sexuality, and conception in free-ranging langurs (Presbytis entellus). Am. J. Primatol. Sommer, V., and Moore, J. (in preparation). Male-male mounting in free-ranging langurs (Presbytis entellus). Srivastava, A. (1989). Feeding ecology and behavior of Hanuman langur (Presbytis entellus). Ph.D. thesis, University of Jodhpur, India. Talmage-Riggs, G., and Anschel, S. (1973). Homosexual behavior and dominance in a group of captive squirrel monkeys (Saimh~ sciureus). Folia PrimatoL 19: 61-72. Vogel, C. (1976). Okologie, Lebensweise und Sozialverhalten der grauen Languren in versehiedenen Biotopen Indiens. Fortsehr. Verhaltensforschung, Beihefte zur Z. Tierpsychol. 17, parey, Berlin. Weber, I. (1973). Tactile communication among free-ranging langurs. Am. Z Phys. Anthropol. 38: 481-486. Weber, I., and Vogel, C. (1970). Sozialverhalten in ein- und zweigeschlechtlichen Langurengruppen. Homo 21: 73-80. Wickler, W. (1967). Socio-sexual signals and their intra-specific imitation among primates. In Morris, D. (ed.), Primate Ethology, Aldine, Chicago, pp. 69-147. Wickler, W. (1969). Sind wir Siinder? Natulgesetze der Ehe, Droemer/Knaur, Miinchen. Winkler, P. (1988). Feeding behavior of a food-enhanced troop of Hanuman langur (Presbytis entellus) in Jodhpur, India. In Fa, J. E., and Southwick, C. H. (eds.), The Ecology and Behavior of Food-Enhanced Primate Groups, Alan R. Liss, New York, pp. 3-24. Winkler, P., Loch, H., and Vogel, C. (1984). Life history of Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus): Reproductive parameters, infant mortality, and troop development. Folia Primatol. 43: 1-23. Wolfe, L. (1984). Mounting patterns of female Japanese macaques. Am. J. Phys. AnthropoL 63: 235. Wolfheim, J. and Rowell, T. (1972). Communication among captive talapoin monkeys (Miopithecus talapoin). Folia Primatol. 1: 224-255. Yerkes, R. M. (1939). Social dominance and sexual status in the chimpanzee. Quart. Rev. Biol. 14: 115-136. Young, W. C. (1941). Observations and experiments on mating behavior in female mammals. Quart. Rev. Biol. 16: 135-156. Zuckerman, S. (1932). The Social Life of Monkeys and Apes, Kegan Paul, London.