0149-0400/95 $10.00 + .03 Copyright © 1995 Taylor & Francis Backcountry Llama Packing: Visitor Perceptions of Acceptability and Conflict DALE J. BLAHNA Department of Forest Resources Utah State University Logan, Utah, USA KARI S. SMITH Department of Recreation Education Ricks College. Rexburg, Idaho, USA JANET A. ANDERSON Department of Forest Resources Utah State University Logan, Utah, USA Abstract There is little research on visitors’ reactions to encountering nontraditional backcountry recreational activities. This paper concerns potential conflicts between llama packers and traditional backcountry visitors. Both on-site and mail questionnaires were completed by 337 visitors at trailheads in the Bechler Meadow region of Yellowstone National Park and the Jedediah Smith Wilderness on the Targhee National Forest. Respondents were asked about past encounters with llamas, perceptions of conflicts and problems resulting from llama use, and attitudes toward five dimensions of social acceptability of llamas: social conflict, safety, physical impacts, managerial equity, and philosophical “appropriateness.” About 30% of the respondents at both areas encountered llamas during the trip, and half had encountered llamas during backcountry trips during the past 5 years. Conflicts and problems related to llama use were low in both study areas, though horseback riders were more likely to have concerns than hikers. Llama management shouldfocus on informational and educational approaches rather than simple reliance on designating areas specifically for horse or llama use. Acceptability judgments about nontraditional activities must include estimates of safety and philosophical appropriateness as well as social, environmental, and managerial conditions. Keywords recreation conflict, asymmetrical conflict, social acceptability, llama packing, nontraditional activities, wilderness recreation Received 1 December 1994; accepted 9 June 1995. Research was supported by a grant from the Aldo Leopold Wilderness Research Institute and the Utah Agricultural Experiment Station. (Approved as AES paper no. 4715.) The authors would like to thank Mark Brunson and Rick Krannich for their advice and cooperation. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 5th International Symposium on Society and Natural Resources, Fort Collins, CO, June 7-10, 1994. Address correspondence to Dale J. Blahna, Department of Forest Resources, Utah State University, Logan, UT 84322-52 15, USA. 185 186 D. J. Blahna et al. There are many studies on recreational carrying capacity and use restrictions in backcountry areas. Most of the literature focuses on limiting use due to increasing numbers of existing user groups, rather than the acceptability of new or nontraditional user groups (White & Schreyer, I981). Although issues related to physical and social impacts of new activities may be similar to recognized carrying capacity problems, there are also unique concerns involved with the introduction of new activities. Issues such as the philosophical appropriateness of the new activity and potential for new forms of conflict with traditional user groups must be addressed. These issues are especially relevant for restricted-use public lands such as designated wilderness areas, wilderness study areas, wild and scenic river corridors, and national or state parks. Public land managers have been confronted with many nontraditional user groups in recent years (Ewert & Schreyer, 1990). These have included jet boaters on the Snake River (Cole, 1989; Stuebner, 1993). helicopter skiers in national forests, rock climbers and hang gliders in national parks (MacCracken & O’Laughlin, 1992; White & Schreyer, 1981), and mountain bikers practically everywhere (Baker, 1990; Jacoby, 1990; Martino, 1992; Watson, Williams, & Daigle, 1991). Although the total number of people involved in these activities is relatively low and use is localized, the consequences of ignoring the new activity groups may be quite severe. For example, conflicts between jet boaters and rafters in Hell’s Canyon of the Snake River have involved name calling, threats, boat ramming, and even gun shots (Cole, 1989). Unfortunately, there is little research on the social acceptability of nontraditional recreational activities in the backcountry, and there are virtually no guidelines for managing these activities (White & Schreyer, 1981). The activities are typically ignored until problems emerge, at which time the new activity is restricted or prohibited in an effort to reduce impacts or conflicts (Baker, 1990; Kelly, 1989; Kulla, 1991). Unfortunately, rather than reducing conflict, this approach can easily exacerbate it. One form of nontraditional recreation that is increasing in popularity is backcountry llama packing. There are few statistics on the use of llamas as recreational packstock, but an International Llama Association (ILA) study found that, during the 1980s there was a fivefold increase in the number of llamas in North America (from 5,000 to 25,000) and in ILA membership (from 282 to 1,469 members) (International Llama Association, 1992). A 1991 survey of ILA llama owners revealed that about 17% use their llamas as pack animals. In 1992, 24% of the hikers and 34% of the packstock users in the John Muir Wilderness, and 21% of the hikers and 29% of the packstock users in the Sequoia-Kings Canyon Wilderness, encountered llamas during their visits (Watson, Niccolucci, & Williams, 1993). In the present study, visitors’ reactions to llama encounters are examined in two western backcountry areas: the Bechler Meadow region of Yellowstone National Park and the Jedediah Smith Wilderness on the Targhee National Forest, just south of Yellowstone. This paper focuses on visitors’ perceptions of the acceptability of backcountry llama packing and the potential for social conflict between llama packers and traditional backcountry visitors. There are three specific objectives: (a) document visitors’ actual experiences and contacts with llama packing, (b) investigate visitors’ perceptions of social acceptability and conflict related to llama packing in the study areas, and (c) identify the dimensions of acceptability that may influence perceptions of conflict. Management implications for designing backcountry llama packing policies and for dealing with nontraditional wilderness recreation activities in general will be discussed. Backcountry Llama Packing 187 Social Conflict in Recreation Most research on outdoor recreation conflict focuses on measuring the potential for interpersonal conflict between participants in different activity groups. The theoretical approach for most of these studies is derived from Jacob and Schreyer’s (1980) hypothesis that recreational conflict occurs when the behavior of one group interferes with the goal attainment of another group. Himes (1980) compares this perspective to relative deprivation theory, and agrees that goal deprivation and resulting frustration and discontent form the “prime cause” of overt conflict (p. 43). However, most recreation research does not investigate overt conflict; rather, it focuses on preexisting evaluations by groups, each of the other, based on past contacts and future expectations (Jacob & Schreycr, 1980). There are a wide variety of social, psychological, and institutional conditions that determine the “structural preconditions” of overt conflict (Himes, 1980, p. 39). For example, based on past research, Jacob and Schreyer ( 1980) identified four factors that influence recreationists’ preexisting attitudes toward different groups: (a) differences in activity style, (b) resource specificity and significance of particular resources or areas, (c) different modes or ways of experiencing the environment, and (d) lifestyle tolerance. Research has verified that these factors help predict one group’s predisposition toward other activity groups, but they are less effective at predicting actual goal interference (Watson et al., 1993; Watson, Niccolucci, & Williams, 1994). This apparent contradiction follows from the conflict theory literature, which indicates that in addition to interpersonal attitudes, there are many other structural preconditions that influence conflict (cf. Himes, 1980) and that do not fit well into Jacob and Schreyer’s (1980) framework. Some examples include normative differences of participants (Lee, 1972; Ruddell & Gramann, 1994). direct competition for resources (Owens, 1985), institutional failures or inequities (Himes, 1980; Owens, 1985), and the effects of nonactivity-related factors that make up the conflict “catalyzing situation” (Himes, 1980, p. 45; Ruddell & Gramann, 1994; Watson & Niccolucci, 1994). In this study, we will start with standard questions of conflict based on the Jacob and Schreyer goal interference model. This will allow comparisons of llama packing with past studies of intergroup perceptions of conflict and allow for testing specific hypotheses. Recognizing the need to expand the study of conflict beyond intergroup characteristics and perceptions, we also implemented two multi-item measures of problems and social acceptability of using llamas in the backcountry. These measures are designed to investigate visitor attitudes toward other structural preconditions related to potential conflict involving llama packing. These acceptability measures are exploratory, so there are no acceptability-related hypotheses, but we hope the results help advance the study of recreation conflict and provide information on the social acceptability of nontraditional recreation activities in general. Conflict Measures and Hypotheses Most studies of wilderness conflict employ questions intended to tap affective responses of one group to another-.such as the extent to which they enjoy or dislike encounters or find contacts to be desirable or undesirabie. More recent studies have employed a more direct measure of goal interference: questions ask visitors the extent to which they felt encounters interfered with their enjoyment (Ivy, Stewart, & Lue, 1992; Watson et al., 188 D. J. Blahna et al. 1993; Watson et al., 1994). For this study, we used both an enjoy/dislike measure, to retain comparability with past studies, and a measure of perceived interference. A specific form of recreational conflict that is especially relevant to nontraditional activities is asymmetrical conflict. One of the most consistent findings in the recreation conflict literature is the tendency for asymmetrical antipathy between different activity groups (Adelman, Heberlein, & Bonnicksen, 1982, p. 42; Watson et al., 1991; Watson & Niccolucci, 1994). When two activity groups conflict, there is a tendency for one group to be more sensitive to the conflict than the other group. Trout fishermen on the AuSable River in Michigan disliked contacts with canoeists (Driver & Bassett, 1975), paddling canoeists were more sensitive to motor canoeists in the Boundary Waters Canoe Area (Adelman et al., 1982; Lucas, 1964; Stankey, 1973), cross-country skiers were more sensitive to snowmobilers in Minnesota (Knopp & Tyger, 1973), and hikers were more sensitive to mountain bikers in the Rattlesnake National Recreation Area (Watson et al., 1991). Studies of backcountry conflict have consistently documented asymmetrical antipathy between hikers and horseback riders as well (Stankey, 1973; Stankey, 1980; Watson et al., 1993; Watson et al., 1994). The presence of packstock is considered a major source of conflict by many backcountry hikers, but the role of llamas in the so-called “hiker/ packstock debate” is unclear. If packstock per se is the key precondition, as past studies suggest, hikers should be more sensitive than horseback riders to contacts with llama packers. And since llama packers also hike while in the backcountry, they should have greater antipathy toward horseback riders than vice versa. However, the limited data available do not support this simple relationship for llamas. Reports from managers and professional llama packers indicate that horseback riders are more concerned than hikers about encounters with llamas, which may result from the tendency of some horses to shy or bolt when meeting llamas (McClaran & Cole, 1993). Survey data from the John Muir and Sequoia-Kings Canyon Wilderness studies also question the simple hiker/packstock explanation for llamas. In both studies, results indicated that the classic asymmetrical antipathy exists between hikers and horseback riders, and between hikers and people using horses and mules for packstock (Watson et al., 1993), but the hiker/llama conflict results were mixed. Hikers in the John Muir were more likely to say they disliked meeting horses (36%) than llamas (17%), but there was no difference in responses from Sequoia-Kings Canyon visitors (44% for both). Stock users, on the other hand, were less averse to llamas than hikers in either area (9% in the John Muir and 27% in Sequoia-Kings Canyon). Thus, the potential for hiker/llama conflict is high in one area, while relatively low in the other-and it is higher than horseback/llama conflict in both areas. Unfortunately, the sample size was too small for the authors to analyze the conflict ratings of llama packers to get a complete picture of conflict symmetry. These results indicate that the hiker/packstock asymmetrical antipathy is more complex than simple explanations based upon travel mode and the presence of packstock. If it were that simple, then controlling the conflict would simply require a zoning approach, with packstock restricted to certain areas just as managers separate snowmobilers and cross-country skiers or motorbikers and hikers. If packstock is not the primary source of conflict, different and probably more complex management approaches will be needed. The California study results also indicate the potential for site-specific differences in conflict perceptions. The null hypothesis to test for asymmetrical conflict in the present study would be to expect no differences in perception of conflict by visitors with different travel modes Backcountry Llama Packing 189 (H10). Three alternative hypotheses exist, however. A hypothesis based on the hiker versus packstock notion would suggest that hikers are more likely than horseback riders to evaluate llama encounters negatively (Hl1 ,). The potential problems between horses and llamas would suggest an asymmetrical form of conflict where horseback riders would evaluate contacts more negatively than hikers (H12). And finally, if asymmetrical conflict is driven primarily by reaction against a new activity or user group, then one would expect that both hikers and horseback riders would rate llama contacts more negatively than llama packers would rate contacts with hikers or horseback riders (Hl3). A second set of hypotheses relates to the conflict perceptions of llama packers. Here the null hypothesis would be that there is no difference in llama packers’ perceptions of conflict with the other two user groups (H20). Asymmetrical conflict would predict that llama packers view hikers as less invasive than horseback riders (H2 1), and not significantly different from contacts with other llama packers (H22). . Social Acceptability Measures Unlike recreation conflict, there has been little research specifically on the dimensions of social acceptability. Social acceptability is a management-oriented concept that is gaining prominence as a result of federal agencies’ emerging emphasis on ecosystem management (Brunson, 1993; Stankey & Clark, 1991). Although a primary goal of ecosystem management is to increase ecological considerations in resource decision-making, it is also intended to better incorporate social and political considerations in decision-making. Research is needed to define more specifically the dimensions of social acceptability for different issues, and to link the concept to existing social science knowledge and precepts and to different geographic settings (Brunson, 1993; Stankey & Clark, 1991). Two areas of recreation management that provide insight into some of the dimensions of acceptabilthat are important for wilderness packstock are the Limits of Acceptable Change (LAC) and the National Park Service’s policies regarding the management of nontraditional activities. The LAC process was designed specifically to incorporate social and physical carrying capacity issues into wilderness planning (Stankey et al., 1985). The LAC process incorporates three general dimensions of acceptability: physical conditions, social conditions, and management practices. These three dimensions are becoming the basis for most planning and management considerations in wilderness areas (e.g., Hammitt & Cole, 1987; Hendee, Stankey, & Lucas, 1990) and natural resource-based recreation areas in general (e.g., Hof et al., 1994; Manning, 1986). The LAC dimensions are also relevant for acceptability decisions regarding nontraditional backcountry activities. For llama packing, acceptability judgments may be influenced by visitors’ or managers’ perceptions of physical impacts of llamas, such as trampling or transmitting diseases to wildlife (McClaran & Cole, 1993). And the extent to which crowding is exacerbated by the presence of llama packers would also influence acceptability judgments. In the Park Service policies, “nontraditional activities,” are defined as activities that are not necessarily dependent upon park resources and do not constitute “traditional or customary uses” of the parks (U.S. Department of Interior, 1978, p. III-7). Nontraditional activities may be permitted if they do not: (1) interfere with normal park usage, (2) constitute a consumptive form of use, (3) have an undesirable impact on park resources, (4) compromise the historic or natural scene, or (5) present a danger to public welfare and safety of both participants and other visitors. 190 D. J. Blahna et al. The Park Service criteria correspond with the LAC dimensions in terms of social (number 1 above) and physical (numbers 2 and 3) conditions, but there are two new acceptability dimensions introduced. Safety, for example, may be relevant in two ways: the safety of the participants in the new activity (e.g., risk recreation activities like hang gliding), and the safety of other visitor groups. Because llamas are easy to handle, safety is more of an issue for other visitors, namely horseback riders, inasmuch as some horses may bolt when they see llamas (McClaran & Cole, 1993). Another acceptability dimension suggested by the Park Service policy is the potential for compromising the natural or historic scene. This is not just a visual aesthetic judgment, which is an element of the physical condition, because it also implies a philosophical judgment. Virtually any human use could be interpreted as compromising a natural scene in the backcountry. and any technology-related uses (e.g., fiberglass canoes, backpacking tents, or modem fishing reels) could be interpreted as compromising the historic character of a scene. Thus, some judgment of philosophical appropriateness is needed. In responding to a particular activity or user group, this philosophical question is particularly relevant. Ethics scholars speak of two different types of information that people use to make evaluative judgments: empirical claims and moral claims (VanDeVeer & Pierce, 1994, p. 2). There is little research regarding the influence of moral judgments in the recreation literature. Because judgments of safety, physical impacts, social conflict, and the like are empirically based judgments, compromising the character of the backcountry was treated as a philosophical or moral claim for this study; that is, does it seem right or appropriate to use llamas in the backcountry? Two sets of questions were used to evaluate visitors’ perceptions of the acceptability of llama packing. First, a set of 18 problem statements were used to tap the social and physical impact dimensions of llama packing and to compare these perceptions with impacts of other packstock and other backcountry activities, Second, a 15-item question was developed to tap five specific social-acceptability dimensions (a) appropriateness, (b) social conflict, (c) managerial equity, (d) physical impacts, and (e) safety. Because this was an exploratory set of questions, no acceptability hypotheses were tested. In general, there are four potential outcomes of the role of llamas in the hiker/ packstock debate. Llamas may be generally found to be unacceptable and lead to social conflict for both hikers and horseback riders. This would indicate that a combination of conflict-related factors influences visitors’ perceptions, and might call into question the philosophical appropriateness of llamas in the backcountry. At the other extreme, if llamas are considered acceptable by both horseback riders and hikers, it is likely that none of the traditional conflict factors are a problem, and visitors have little concern for the appropriateness of llamas in the backcountry. If just hikers have negative reactions to llamas, then the traditional hiker/packstock conflict exists and zoning would be a logical management approach. And finally, if just horseback riders view llamas negatively, safety and resource competition may be the sources of conflict. In this case, a more complex management combination of zoning and education will be needed to minimize conflict. The literature discussed above also indicates that there may be site-specific differences in perceptions of both conflict and acceptability. Research Methods This study was conducted with visitors at three trailheads in two backcountry areas within 1 hour’s drive of each other: the Bechler Meadow region of Yellowstone National Park and the Jedediah Smith Wilderness on the Targhee National Forest. Sample subjects were Backcountry Llama Packing 191 asked to complete a short trailhead survey and to provide their name and address in order to receive a longer mail survey to be completed and returned by mail. To provide results comparable with past research, the survey’s focus on llama packing was not discussed with respondents at the trailhead, and only a few of the questions toward the end of the survey focused specifically on llamas. To investigate the generalizability of the results, two areas that attract very different types of visitors were selected for the study. Study Sites and Contact Survey The Jedediah Smith Wilderness comprises 123,451 acres administrated by the Targhee National Forest. It is located directly west of Teton National Park and south of Yellowstone, on the western slopes of the Teton Mountains. Trailheads lead to popular high alpine lake basins, often over fairly steep and rugged terrain. The two trailheads selected for the study were known to have horse, hiker, and llama use. In a Forest Service survey of wilderness managers, the Jedediah Smith Wilderness was named as being one of 14 wildernesses that received more than 50 llama packing groups between 1985 and 1990 (McClaran & Cole, 1993). Although it is in close proximity to two heavily visited National Parks, the Jedediah Smith Wilderness is relatively lightly used. It is less well known than the National Parks, access is more difficult, and more visitors are local residents. The third trailhead, Bechler Ranger Station, sits in the remote southwest comer of Yellowstone National Park. The Bechler trails cover fairly easy and moderate terrain through large meadows and up canyons with popular waterfall and geothermal feature destinations. This trailhead also gets a mix of horse and hiker use, and most of the commercial llama packers who use the Jedediah Smith Wilderness also have permits for the Bechler trailhead. Brief trailhead contact surveys were conducted during randomly selected days between July 30 and September 27, 1993. Due to the deep snowpack from the winter of 1992-93, much of the backcountry was inaccessible until late summer. The Bechler region was closed to all stock use until August due to wet trails. In order to maximize the number of contacts during this shortened visitor season, interviews were conducted every Saturday and Sunday in September. Afternoon and evening hours produced the most contacts. Every visitor group during sampling periods was approached at the end of its trip by an interviewer who asked them to fill out a one-page questionnaire. All members of each party were asked to respond. The contact survey solicited basic trip information (size of group, length of stay, and mode of travel) and general comments regarding trip satisfaction. At the end of the survey, visitors were asked to give their names and addresses if they were willing to complete a longer questionnaire that would be mailed to them. Trailhead contacts resulted in a final sample size of 454 backcountry visitors: 233 (51.3%) from the Bechler trailhead in Yellowstone National Park, and 221 (48.7%) from the Jedediah Smith trailheads. Mail Survey and Study Measures An 11-page mail survey was sent to all trailhead contacts. A three-wave mailing design was used; first a cover letter and mail survey were sent within 3 weeks after the trailhead contacts were made, and a reminder postcard and then a replacement survey were sent at three week intervals to nonrespondents. Experience with llama packing was measured three ways: (1) visitors’ current mode of travel, (2) number of backcountry encounters with llamas in the past, and (3) personal experience llama packing in the last 5 years. Packstock, hiker, and llama conflict measures 192 D. J. Blahna et al. and encounters with llamas on the current trip were both measured with the same question, which read: “Please evaluate your encounters with other user groups during this trip into the backcountry.” The response format was: “enjoyed meeting them,” “did not mind meeting them,” “ disliked meeting them,” or “did not meet any.” The goal-interference question asked respondents if meeting the different user groups interfered “a little,” “somewhat,” or “a lot” with their trip enjoyment. As discussed above, acceptability was measured two ways. First visitors responded to 18 problem statements on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “no problem” to “big problem.” There were two items dealing with social impacts of llamas (“meeting llamas on the trail” and “too many llamas on the trails”) and two items dealing with physical impacts of llamas (“trails impacted by llamas” and “llama manure on trails or in campsites”). There were also four items related to horse impacts that were comparable to the four llama impact items. Other resource management problems included crowding, hiker impacts, litter, cattle grazing, and aircraft noise. The data were treated as interval level data, and multivariate ANOVA analyses were run while controlling for travel mode and trailhead. The dimensions of acceptability were investigated with 15 individual statements designed to tap the five dimensions of llama acceptability discussed above: social conflict, physical impacts, managerial equity, philosophical appropriateness, and safety. Eight of the items were directly related to llamas (e.g., “llamas cause little impact to vegetation”), two were related specifically to horses and mules (e.g., “campers don’t mind camping in sites previously occupied by horses or mules”), and five items dealt with both llamas and other packstock (e.g., “use regulations should be the same for llama.: as they are for horses and mules”). Respondents ranked the items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree, and the results were treated as interval level data. Since these questions were designed to be more long-term and philosophical than the trip-specific conflict and problem measures, the independent variables for these analyses differed slightly from those used above. In addition to controlling for the study areas, the number of past llama encounters (coded “none,” “1 to 5,” and “over 5”), and travel mode preference (coded “horseback, " “hike,” and “both equally”) were used in bivariate analyses. Results Survey Response and Sample Characteristics A total of 337 useable surveys was returned for a 74% response rate. There were 182 responses from the Bechler subsample (78% response) and 155 (70%) from Jedediah Smith. Bechler respondents were more likely to be urban, have significantly higher levels of education and income, and to be hikers rather than horseback riders (Table 1). Jedediah Smith respondents, on the other hand, were more likely to be local residents, horseback riders, and on shorter trips than Bechler respondents. Bechler visitors also encountered significantly more hiking groups (mean = 4.9) than did Jedediah Smith visitors (mean = 1.9, t-value = 6.6, signif. < .00l) but about the same number of groups on horseback (2.8 to 2.1, respectively; t- value = 1.45, signif. = .52). Thus, the subsamples reflect two very different but typical western backcountry areas: the Jedediah Smith is used primarily by local, rural residents traveling with packstock, and Bechler is a more heavily used, destination recreation backcountry experience. Backcountry Llama Packing 193 Table 1 Visitor characteristics by study area Mode of travel Horseback riding Hiking Hiking with packstock In what type of community do you now live? Rural/small town (pop. l,000-5,000) Small city (5,000-50,000) Medium/large city (50,000-250,000 and larger) Location of residence: Within 2 hours Other MT, WY, ID Other U.S. Annual household income: <$20,000 20,000-49,999 50,000-74,999 75,000 and over Highest education level: High school or less Some college/vocational College graduate/graduate work Total sample Bechler Jed. Smith 113 (33.5) 209 (62.0) 15 (4.5) 30 (16.5) 149 (81.9) 3 (1.6) 83 (53.5) 60 (38.7) 12 (7.7) 146 (43.8) 51 (28.4) 95 (61.7) 107 (32.0) 69 (38.3) 38 (24.7) 81 (24.3) 60 (33.3) 21 (13.6) 208 (61.9) 31 (9.2) 97 (28.9) 86 (47.3) 23 (12.6) 73 (40.1) 122 (79.2) 8 (5.2) 24 (15.6) 61 (19.6) 119 (38.3) 60 (19.3) 71 (22.8) 32 60 27 50 39 (11.6) 95 (28.4) 201 (60.0) (18.9) (35.5) (16.0) (29.6) 29 (20.4) 59 (41.5) 33 (23.2) 21 (14.8) 13 (7.2) 52 (28.9) 115 (63.9) 26 (16.8) 43 (27.7) 86 (55.5) Chi-sq. (prob.) 66.5 (<.001) 45.6 (<.001) 36.2 (<.001) 10.5 (.03) 7.8 (.05) Experience and Encounters with Llamas Only 9 (2.7%) respondents were actually traveling with llamas: 7 (4.5%) of the Jedediah Smith visitors and 2 (1.1%) of the Bechler visitors. Likewise, more Jedediah Smith visitors ( n = 11, 8.3%) than Bechler visitors (n = 4,4.2%) had been on llama trips in the last 5 years. Nearly one third (29.5%) of the visitors encountered llamas on their trip, however, including 28.5% of the Bechler subsample and 31.7% of the Jedediah Smith 2 subsample (X = 2.34, signif. = .50). These results are similar to the California studies that used the same question format (Watson et al, 1993). For past encounters with llamas, 47.2% of the respondents said they had encountered llamas in the backcountry in the last 5 years, including 45.3% of the Bechler visitors and 2 50.3% of the Jedediah Smith visitors (X = .65, signif. = .42). About one third of the respondents had met one to three groups (29. 1%), 47 (14.1%) had met 4 to 10 groups, and 14 (4.2%) had met more than 10 groups with llamas. In general, the results indicate that while very few visitors had llama packing experience, about one third encountered llamas on this trip, and nearly half encountered llamas during the last 5 years. D. J. Blahna et al. 194 Conflict Perceptions The results of the intergroup-conflict questions are shown in Table 2. In general, there were more negative replies given in response to the goal-interference question than to the more affective enjoyment/dislike question. As expected, the classic pattern of asymmetrical conflict emerged in the responses of hikers and horseback riders. Hikers rated encounters with llamas more negatively than encounters with other hikers, but not as negatively as their encounters with horses. Horseback riders, on the other hand, rated llama encounters more negatively than encounters with either hikers or horseback riders, but the differences were not statistically significant. These results are strong enough, however, to reject the first null hypothesis and indicate support for H13 (i.e., both hikers and horseback riders rated encounters with llamas more negatively than encounters with their own activity groups). This indicates asymmetrical conflict is not driven simply by the presence of packstock. There is also an indication that there may be support for H1 2 (horseback riders would rare llamas more negatively than hikers), but the differences are not statistically significant. Based on the small sample size of llama packers, no firm conclusions can be drawn about the second set of hypotheses; the null hypothesis cannot be rejected (i.e., llama packers do not evaluate contacts with any groups more negatively than encounters with other llama groups). Table 2 Perceptions of conflict by travel mode (for respondents who encountered the type of visitors listed) Travel mode Total. Hiker Horseback rider Llama 2 packing x Sig. a Hikers/backpackers Horseback riders Llamas % Disliked encounters with 9 (3.3) 4 (2.3) 5 (5.6) ( n = 271) n = 172 n = 90 37 (14.2) 36 (23.5) l(l.0) n = 100 n = 153 ( n = 261) 22 (22.7) 9 (19.6) 13 (28.3) n =46 n =46 ( n = 95) 0 (0.0) n=9 0 (0.0) n= 8 0 (0.0) n =3 2.4 .667 78.6 <.001 7.5 .ll .29 .865 43.5 <.00l .74 .692 b Hikers/backpackers Horseback riders Llamas a 6 1 (22.3) ( n= 274) 93 (27.6) ( n= 266) 39 (11.6) ( n = 100) % Interfered with 20 (22.0) 39 (22.8) ( n= 171) ( n = 91) 11 (11.0) 78 (5 1.3) (n = 100) ( n = 152) 20 (42.6) 17 (36.2) ( n = 47) ( n = 47) 1 (14.3) ( n = 7) 2 (25.0) (n = 8) 1 (25.0) ( n =4) For each type of user group they encountered, respondents were asked if they “enjoyed meeting, ” “did not mind meeting,” or “disliked meeting” that type of group. b Respondents we re asked if meeting each type of group interfered with their enjoyment, and if so, if it interfered “a little,” “somewhat,” or “a lot.” Backcountry Llama Packing 195 Acceptability: Perceptions of Backcountry Problems All 18 items were rated as relatively small problems (means range from 1.17 to 2.36) in both study areas (Table 3). Consistent with past research, the top eight problems were related to horse impacts, human impacts, and crowding. The llama impact items were four of the five lowest rated problems on the list, and there were no differences in the ranking of llama-related problems in the two study areas. Although this is certainly not surprising, inasmuch as llama use is relatively low, it does indicate that visitors feel that llama impacts are not at an unacceptable level. It is also interesting to note that the horse-related impact items were considered significantly more problematic at Bechler, and humancaused problems were rated higher by Jedediah Smith visitors. Although these results indicate that visitors feel llama impacts are not currently problematic, they say little about the potential for future problems if llama numbers increase. To address these broader issues, Tables 4 and 5 present the mean problem ratings for horse and llama impacts, respectively, while controlling for the effects of respondents’ mode of travel (hiking or horseback), the study trailhead, and encounters with horses or llamas.’ Results in Table-4 illustrate that, for horse-related impacts, the classic pattern of asymmetrical conflict exists in both study areas. Horse encounters and travel mode proTable 3 Perceptions of backcountry problems by study area Trailhead Problem a Horse manure Horse/mule trail impacts Too many people at certain locations Litter Meeting horses on trail Too many horses on trail Human vegetation damage Too many large groups Cattle grazing damage Sheep grazing damage Too many hikers on trail Firewood Human waste Meeting llamas Too many llamas on trail Aircraft Llama trail impacts Llama manure a N Grand mean Bechler N= 182 Jed Smith T b value Signif. N= 155 337 332 335 2.01 1.94 I .73 2.36 2.21 1.77 1.61 1.61 1.65 5.52 4.10 .97 ** ** 335 337 337 332 335 328 332 336 325 329 332 335 333 330 328 1.65 1.64 1.61 1.52 1.48 1.42 1.44 1.44 1.41 1.39 1.32 1.28 I .25 1.19 1.17 1.51 1.84 1.81 1.53 1.51 1.19 1.08 1.49 1.43 1.29 1.24 1.26 1.28 1.19 1.15 1.82 1.41 1.37 1.50 1.44 1.76 1.87 1.38 1.38 1.51 1.41 1.31 1.20 1.19 1.18 -3.13 3.78 3.77 .24 .69 -4.94 -6.40 1.14 .46 -2.27 -1.78 -.52 .97 -.07 -.38 ** ** ** ** ** * Perceptions of problems measured on 5-point scale where 1 = “no problem at all” to 5 = “big problem.” b Significance levels = * < .05. ** < .0l. 196 D. J. Blahna et al. vide most of the significant main effects for each impact measure. Hikers rated all four impact measures, especially the physical impact items, as significantly larger problems than horseback riders did. For the social impact items, there is also a significant interaction between encounters and travel mode; in both study areas, hikers who encountered horseback riders were more likely to rate the social impacts of horses higher than horseback riders rated them. Although this result seems intuitive, it indicates that hikers’ responses to physical impacts of horses are the result of the combined effect of seeing horses and of recognizing the physical impacts from horses, whether or not horses were present at the time. For problems related to llamas, however, a different conflict dynamic exists. The problem ratings for llama impacts are lower than for horse impacts, and the physical impacts were rated lower than social impacts by horseback riders, and about the same (Bechler) or lower (Jed Smith) by hikers (Table 5). Encounters with llamas was by far the most significant factor variable, and there were no significant differences by travel mode, except in an interactive relationship with encounters for the physical impact items. (Horseback riders were more sensitive to llama impacts in the Jedediah Smith Wilderness than at Bechler.) Furthermore, physical impacts were only noted by hikers who actually saw llama groups, which indicates they may be responding to the mere presence of llamas as much as to the actual signs of llama impacts. In general, horseback riders were less likely to consider the physical effects of llamas a problem than the social impacts, even if they encountered llama groups in the backcountry. And while hikers were more likely to rate the physical llama impacts higher if they encountered llamas, they considered these impacts less problematic than the physical or social impacts of horses. There was also an indication of a geographical effect in the responses to the llama problem questions (Table 5). For the social impact items, there was a significant interaction between llama encounters and the study area. Both hikers and horseback riders in the Jedediah Smith subsample rated “meeting llamas on the trail” and “trails impacted by llamas” more of a problem than the Bechler subsample. This is not simply a response to crowding because use levels and group encounters are actually higher in the Bechler region than in the Jedediah Smith Wilderness (data not shown), and there were no differences in llama encounters at the two sites (discussed above). Another possible explanation is a structural-effects hypothesis (cf. Bultena & Field, 1980). The social setting of the Jedediah Smith Wilderness has always supported horseback riding, and the norms of the riders seem to be reflected in the responses of hikers using the area, at least compared to the responses of hikers in Bechler. This interpretation of the results is also supported by the results of the horse impact questions (Table 4), where hikers from Jedediah Smith rated the effect of horses lower than the Bechler subsample for all four items, although the differences were only marginally significant (.06) for two items. Dimensions of Social Acceptability The results reported above indicate that llama packing is not viewed as a problem at current use levels, but there is potential for social conflict, especially between horseback riders and llama packers. These results, however, tell us little about the specific dimensions of acceptability and conflict, which can have important management implications. Responses to the questions relating to different dimensions of acceptability indicate there was little support for statements suggesting llamas are inappropriate in the backcountry (items H and M), and the support that did exist was primarily from horseback riders (Table 6). Visitors in all analysis subgroups were most likely to support statements Table 4 ANOVA results testing for significant effects on perceptions of horse-related problems by travel mode, study area, and horse encounters Problems a Meeting horses on trail Travel mode b ANOVA results Jed Smith study area Bechler study area c c Horse encounters No Yes Horse encounters No Yes c 1.25 (40) 1 .00 (5) 2.30 ( 106) 1 .00 (25) e 1.00 (I I) I .OO (6) 2.04 (49) 1.07 (76) Too many horses on trail Hiker Horseback rider 1.18 (40) 1 .00 (5) 2.21 (106) 1.24 (25) e 1.00 (I 1) 1 .OO (6) I .94 (49) 1.04 (76) Trails impacted by horses Hiker Horseback rider 1.78 (40) 1 .00 (5) Horse manure on trail or in campsites Hiker Horseback rider 1.98 (40) 1 .00 (5) Hiker Horseback rider a d F = 36.7 <.00l Travel mode <.001 Encounters <.001I Travel & encounters = .004 F = 27.7, .00l Mode <.00l Encounters <.001 Study area = .06 Travel & encounters = .02 e 2.41 (49)’ 1.04 (76) F = 35.6, .00l Mode <.001 Encounters <.001 d 2.45 (49)’ 1.05 (76) F = 50.7, <.001 Mode <.001 Encounters <.00 1 Study area = .06 d 2.68 (106) 1.12 (25) e 2.00(11) 1.17 (6) d 2.85 (106) 1.12 (25) e 1.82 (ll) 1 .00 (6) Problems measured on 5-point scale where 1 = “no problem at all” and 5 = “big problem." F-value given for entire subtable; the only other statistics listed are probabilities for statistically significant main effects and interactions. c Numbers in ( ) are cell n sizes. d Indicates cell means between 1.50 and 1.99. e Indicates cell means 2.00 and over. b Table 5 ANOVA results testing for significant effects on perceptions of problems with llamas by travel mode, study area, and llama encounters Problems” Meeting llamas on trail Travel mode Hiker Horseback rider b ANOVA results Jed Smith study area Bechler study area C c Llama encounters Yes No Llama encounters Yes No d e 2.20 ( 10) e 2.27 (33) 1.04 (110) 1.13 (16) 1.75 (36) d 1.77 (13) Too many llamas on trail Hiker Horseback rider 1.08 (107) 1.13 (16) d 1.61 (36) d 1.85 (13) 1.02 (47) 1.02 (48) 2.00 (10) e 2.07 (30) Trails impacted by llamas Hiker Horseback rider I.01 (107) 1.13 (16) I .78 (36) 1.08 (13) d 1.11 (47) 1 .00 (48) 1.60 (l0) d 1.60 (30) Llama manure on trail or in campsites Hiker Horseback rider a 1.02 (107) 1.13 (16) 1.61 (36) 1.08 (13) 1.06 (47) 1.13 (48) d 1.11 (47) 1.00 (48) F = 31.6, <.001 Encounters <.001 Encounters and study area = .05 e d d 1.80 (10) 1.47 (30) F = 18.7, <.00l Encounters = .00l F = 15.7, <.001 Encounters <.001 Encounters, mode, and study area = .02 F = 12.6, <.00l Encounters <.001 Encounters and mode = .02 Problems measured on 5-point scale where 1 = “no problem at all” and 5 = “big problem.” F-value given for entire subtable; the only other statistics listed are probabilities for statistically significant main effects and interactions. c Numbers in ( ) are cell n sizes. d lndicates cell means between 1.50 and 1.99. e Indicates cell means 2.00 and over. b Table 6 a Measures of acceptability dimensions by trailhead. encounters with llamas, and travel mode b Llama Encounters Travel Mode Preference (TMP) Trailhead Item A) Use regulations same for llamas. and horses B) Limits for llamas same as for horses C) Safety problems when llamas meet horses D) Llamas cause little impact E) Llamas should be led off trail when meeting horses F) Llama packers are experienced G) Meeting llamas makes trip more interesting H) Seeing llamas seems out-of-place I) Hikers don’t mind camping in sites used by llamas I) Llamas threat intro. disease K) Llamas threat introduction of exotic plants L) Llama safety problems worse for mules than horses M) Horses more appropriate than llamas in backcountry N) Hikers don’t mind camping in horse/mule sites 0) Llamas. may escape and compete with wildlife a b Grand N Mean Acceptability Dimension Bcchler Jed Smith None N = 1 8 2 N= 155 l-5 Over 5 Horse N= 178 N =98 Hike Both Contrast N =61 N =28 N =37 N = 5 6 for TMP 272 3.89 Managerial equity 3.87 3.91 3.96 4.06 3.41** 4.1 I 3.55 3.96 264 3.78 Managerial equity 3.83 3.73 3.80 3.83 3.60 4.58 3.00 3.91** ** 195 3.40 Safety 3.21 3.53 3.19 3.48 3.62 3.69 2.64 3.28* ** 164 3.38 230 3.36 Physical Impact Safety 3.34 3.19 3.42 3.50 3.41 3.07 3.47 3.55 3.22 3.62** 3.17 4.12 3.67 3.13 3.38 3.54** ** 199 3.29 Conflict 291 3.29 Appropriateness 3.32 3.33 3.26 3.24 3.33 3.41 3.32 3.44 3.17 2.79** 2.88 2.80 3.52 3.49 3.68* 3.31 * * 304 2.84 197 2.71 Appropriateness Conflict 2.86 2.49 2.81 2.95 2.90 2.66 2.64 2.79 3.00 2.70 3.41 2.41 2.65 3.27 2.55% 3.29* * * 174 2.71 180 2.69 Physical Impact Physical Impact 2.82 2.58 2.61 2.79 2.77 2.61 2.57 2.85 2.77 2.64 2.80 3.31 2.46 2.33 2.32 2.55* * 132 2.69 Safety 2.67 2.71 2.76 2.67 2.59 2.94 2.44 2.54 260 2.44 Appropriateness 2.14 2.75** 2.38 2.38 2.64 3.63 1.71 2.52** ** 259 2.24 Conflict 1.96 2.57** 2.33 2.30 1.91 3.19 1.89 2.66** ** 212 2.12 2.10 2.15 2.43 1.78 1.95** 2.00 2.16 2.08 Physical Impact Scale = 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree); Statistical probability: * <.05, ** <.l0 Horseback rider/hiker contrast only (T-test) 200 D. J. Blahna et al. suggesting that management regulations should be the same for llamas as for horses in the backcountry (items A and B). and horseback riders were especially likely to support these statements. Horseback riders were also more likely than hikers to agree with statements that llamas caused safety problems (items C and E) and statements implying that horses are more appropriate than llamas in the backcountry (items H and M), and to disagree with the statement that “meeting llamas makes the trip more interesting” (item G). Hikers and horseback riders tended to agree that llamas cause little physical impact (items D, K, and O), but social concerns (items F and I were more likely to be registered by horseback riders than by hikers. Horseback riders also predicted that hikers would be more likely to want to camp in sites used by horses rather than llamas, which was the opposite of hikers’ actual opinions (items I and N). There were few statistical differences in the responses to acceptability dimensions based on visitors’ past llama encounters or the study area (Table 6). In general, visitors with past llama encounters were more likely than those without past encounters to rate correctly the acceptability items that had a factual basis. For example, visitors with past llama contacts were less likely to agree that llamas could escape and compete with wildlife and more likely to agree that llamas should be led off the trail when they meet horses. Visitors with more than five past encounters with llama groups were also least likely to say that meeting llamas makes the trip more interesting. Apparently, with increasing encounters, the novelty of seeing llamas wears off. The only differences between the visitors to the two study areas were that Jedediah Smith visitors were more likely to agree that horses are more appropriate in the backcountry and that hikers don’t mind camping in sites used by horses or mules. This probably reflects the higher number of horseback riders in the Jedediah Smith Wilderness and the structural-effects results discussed above. Discussion and Implications The results indicate there is relatively low use of llamas in the Bechler or Jedediah Smith backcountry areas. About 3% of the sample subjects were packing with llamas, and fewer than 5% had gone llama packing in the last 5 years. Many more visitors saw llamas in the backcountry, however. Approximately 30% saw llamas sometime during their trip in the study areas, and about half the sample have seen backcountry visitors with llamas in the past. Although ratings were very low for all backcountry problem items, llama-related problems were especially low. Hikers who encountered horses and llamas felt that the physical and social impacts of horses are more problematic than llama impacts. Compared to hikers, horseback riders were more concerned with the social impacts of llamas and less concerned with the physical impacts. The social concerns appear to be the result of potential safety problems and, to a lesser extent, a question of the philosophical appropriateness of llamas in the backcountry. Hikers had neither of these concerns, and appear to consider llamas as acceptable in the backcountry as horses. Most of the variance in responses to llama problems was explained by encounters with llamas, but for horse-related problems, there was also a strong effect of travel mode, reflecting the classic hiker/horseback rider form of asymmetrical social conflict. Thus, although there is evidence for asymmetrical conflict with llama packing (hypothesis H13), it is a different form of conflict than between hikers and horseback riders. Hikers are less sensitive to encounters with llamas than with horses, and horseback riders are more sensitive to the presence of llamas than are hikers (hypothesis HI 2). These results are similar to those found in the John Muir and Sequoia-Kings Canyon Wildernesses (Watson Backcountry Llama Packing 201 et al. 1993), except that horseback riders seem to be more negative than hikers toward llama encounters, while in California, horseback riders were slightly less negative than hikers. For horseback riders, the basis of this asymmetry seems to be related to safety and philosophical considerations, but it may also result from backlash of horseback riders who are feeling increased competition for resources. This dimension of acceptability was not included in the survey. In general, the results indicate that social acceptability of new or nontraditional activities is not just the result of judgments related to social, environmental, and managerial conditions. Although these factors form the analytic basis for the LAC process and the literature on wilderness management, other factors such as safety and philosophical appropriateness were also important elements in visitors’ assessments of acceptability of llama packing. There has been little data and research on these factors, however, and measures of direct competition for resources also need to be incorporated in future research. The results also indicate that acceptability judgments regarding nontraditional recreational activities will vary among social groups and geographical settings. While social group differences have long been recognized in the recreation conflict and preference literature, there is little systematic research on site differences. Although the specific causal factors leading to differences between the Bechler and Jedediah Smith subsamples are unclear, the results are consistent with social structural effects being a critical influence. Horseback riders in the Bechler area (which is dominated by hikers) were more likely to respond like hikers, while hikers in the Jedediah Smith Wilderness (which is dominated by horseback riders) were more likely to respond to problems as horseback riders respond. Because this outcome was not expected, it needs to be verified with further research and analysis. Management Implications Managers cannot assume that a nontraditional activity is unacceptable and should be restricted simply due to conflict with traditional visitor groups. In the case of llama packing, using llamas was actually perceived by hikers to cause fewer problems than horseback riding, especially in the area dominated by hikers. To simply prohibit llama packing to control conflict based on either social or physical impact perceptions would be unjustified. Most of the potential for social conflict is between horseback riders and hikers, not between other traditional users and llama packers. If physical impacts are being used to justify restricting llama use, one could argue that llama use should be favored over horse use because the perceived impacts of horseback riding are greater than the perceived impacts of llamas. Due to the conflict dynamics described above, it would also be a mistake to zone the backcountry to restrict all packstock to certain areas. This would actually exacerbate the potential for conflict, as horseback riders and llama packers are forced into closer proximity. It may make more sense to zone one area for horse use and another for hiking and llama packing, With or without zoning, however, some combination of information and education is needed to help reduce the potential for conflict between llama and horse users, especially while llama use is still relatively localized. Horseback riders need to be informed if there is a potential for contacts with llamas, and llama packers need to be made aware of potential safety problems in encounters with horses. Llama packers should also be responsible for leading llamas off the trail and keeping them still when meeting horses on the trail. The infrastructure for an informational approach already exists at the Bechler 202 D. J. Blahna et al. Ranger Station, where rangers are required to meet with all overnight visitor groups to give an interpretive talk about recreational impacts (both physical and social) and safety issues before they begin a backcountry trip. In fact, this program may help explain why visitors at Bechler rate llama acceptability more positively than Jedediah Smith-visitors. Recreation researchers and policy makers need to be more concerned with special problems and information needs confronting managers when dealing with new or nontraditional activity groups. Traditional conceptions of acceptability and social conflict do not provide all of the analysis framework or management tools needed to address issues related to nontraditional uses-and such uses are likely to increase in the future. Notes 1. Cell-by-cell means are displayed because there is a high amount of interaction among the control variables. Cell n sizes were too small for a separate category for “llama packers” as a travel mode. References Adelman, B. J. E., Heberlein, T. A., & Bonnicksen, T. M. ( 1982). Social psychological explanations for the persistence of a conflict between paddling canoeists and motorcraft users in the Boundary Walers Canoe Area. Leisure Sciences, 5, 45-61. Baker, N. (1990). Mountain bike management: A tale of three cities. Western Wildlands, 16 (3), 36-39. Brunson, M. W. (1993). “Socially acceptable” forestry: What does it imply for ecosystem management? Western Journal of Applied Forestry, 8 (4), 116-119. Bultena, G. L., & Field, D. R. (1980). Structural effects in National Parkgoing. Leisure Sciences, 3, 22 l-240. Cole, M. L. (1989). Troubled waters: User conflicts on the Snake River of Hells Canyon. Western Wildlands, 15(3), 8-13. Driver, B. L., & Bassett, J. (1975). Defining conflicts among river users: A case study of Michigan’s AuSable River. Naturalist, 26 (1). 19-23. Ewert, A., & Schreyer. R. (1990. March). Risk recreation: Social trends and implications for the 1990s. In O’Leary. J. T., Fesenmaier, D. R., Brown, T., Stynes, D., & Driver, B. (eds.) Proceedings of the National Outdoor Recreation Trends Symposium III (pp. 480-489). University Place Executive Conference Center, Indianapolis, IN. Hammitt, W. E.. & Cole, D. N. (1987). Wildland recreation: Ecology and management. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Hendee, J. C., Stankey, G. H.. & Lucas, R. C. (1990). Wilderness management (2nd ed.). Golden, CO: North American Press. Himes, J. S. (1980). Conflict and conflict management. Athens: University of Georgia Press. Hof, M., Hammett, J., Rees, M., Belnap, J., Poe, N., Lime, D., & Manning, R. (1994). Getting a handle on visitor carrying capacity: A pilot project at Arches National Park. Park Science, 14 (l), 11-13. International Llama Association. (1992). Questionnaire summary: Use of llamas on public lands. Unpublished manuscript. Ivy, M. I., Stewart, W. P., & Lue, C. (1992). Exploring the role of tolerance in recreational conflict. Journal of Leisure Research, 24, 348-360. Jacob, G. R., & Schreyer, R. (1980). Conflict in outdoor recreation: A theoretical perspective. Journal of Leisure Research, 12, 368-380. Jacoby, J. (1990). Mountain bikes: A new dilemma for wildland recreation managers? Western Wildlands, 16(1), 25-28. Backcountty Llama Packing 203 Kelly, C. (1989). Evolution of an issue: From California fad to nationwide dilemma-tracing the trail access problem. Bicycling (May), 104-I 10. Knopp. T., & Tyger, J. (1973). A study of conflict in recreational land use: Snowmobiling vs. ski-touring. Journal of Leisure Research, 5, 6-17. Kulla, A. (1991). A new perspectives approach in national forest recreation and its application to mountain bike management. Unpublished paper prepared for the Utah State University Outdoor Recreation Shortcourse, Department of Forest Resources, Logan, UT. Lee, R. G. (1972). The social definition of outdoor recreation places. In R. W. Burch, N. H. Cheek, & L. Taylor (Eds.), Social behavior, natural resources, and the environment (pp. 68-84). New York: Harper & Row. Lucas, R. C. (1964). Wilderness perception and use: The example of the Boundary Waters Canoe Area. Natural Resources Journal, 3, 394-411. McClaran, M. P., & Cole, D. N. (1993). Packstock in wilderness: Use, impacts, monitoring, and management. General Technical Report INT-301, USDA Forest Service, Intermountain Research Station, Ogden, UT. MacCracken, J. G., & O’Laughlin, J. (1992). A National Park in Idaho? Proposals and Possibilities. Report NO. 7, Idaho Forest. Wildlife and Range Policy Analysis Group. University of Idaho, Moscow. Manning, R. E. (1986). Studies in outdoor recreation: A review and synthesis of the social science literature in outdoor recreation. Corvallis, OR: Oregon State University Press. Martino. S. ( 1992). Mountain bikes make their mark. Milwaukee Journal (Waukesha ed.). July 27, pp. W1, W4. Owens, P. L. (1985). Conflict as a social interaction process in environment and behavior research: The example of leisure and recreation research. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 5, 243-259. Ruddell, E. J., & Gramann. J. H. (1994). Goal orientation, norms, and noise-induced conflict among recreation area users. Journal of Leisure Research, 26, 93-104. Schreyer, R. (1990). Conflict in outdoor recreation. In Vining, J. (Ed.), Social science and natural resource recreation management (Chapter 2). Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Stankey, G. H. (1973). Visitor perception of wilderness recreation currying capacity. Research Paper INT 142, USDA Forest Service, Intermountain Forest Experiment Station, Ogden, UT. Stankey, G. H. (1980). A comparison of carrying capacity perceptions among visitors to two wildernesses. Research Paper INT 242, USDA Forest Service, Intermountain Forest Experiment Station. Ogden, UT. Stankey, G. H., & Clark, R. N. (1991). Social aspects of new perspectives in forestry: A problem analysis. Pinchot Institute for Conservation Monograph Series. Milford, PA: Grey Towers Press. Stankey, G. H., Cole, D. N., Lucas, R. C., Peterson, M. E., & Frisell, S. S. (1985). The Limits of Acceptable Change (LAC) system for wilderness planning. General Technical Report INT 176, USDA Forest Service, Intermountain Forest and Range Experiment Station, Ogden, UT. Stuebner, S. (1993). A timely solution for the Hells Canyon river war. High Country News 25(21), 6. U.S. Department of Interior. (1978). Management policies. USDI, National Park Service, Washington, DC. VanDeVeer, D., & Pierce, C. (1994). The environmental ethics and policy book: Philosophy, ecology, and economics. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Watson, A. E., & Niccolucci, M. J. (1994, February). Conflicting goals of wilderness management: Natural conditions vs. natural experiences. Paper presented at the 2nd Symposium on Social Aspects and Recreation Research, San Diego, CA. Watson, A. E., & Niccolucci, M. J., & Williams, D. R. (1993). Hikers and recreational packstock users: Predicting and managing recreation conflicts in three wildernesses. Research Paper INT-468, USDA Forest Service, Intermountain Research Station, Ogden, UT. 204 D. J. Blahna et al. Watson, A. E., Niccolucci, M. J.. & Williams, D. R. (1994). The nature of conflict between hikers and recreational stock users in the John Muir Wilderness. Journal of Leisure Research, 26, 372-385. Watson, A. E., Williams, D. R., & Daigle, J. J. (1991). Sources of conflict between hikers and mountain bike riders in the Rattlesnake NRA. Journal of Park and Recreation Administration, 9(3), 59-71. White, R. G., & Schreyer, R. (I 98 1). Nontraditional uses of the National Parks. Leisure Sciences, 4, 325-341.