Answering Questions in Management and Research Using Large-Scale Manipulative Experiments Robert J. Cooper George A. Gale Leonard A. Brennan Abstract—An adequate information base regarding management practices for migratory landbirds and other nongame species does not yet exist. Land managers, therefore, must act with inadequate knowledge of the resources they are charged with managing. We believe that part of the solution to this problem rests with the land managers themselves, in that they are in a position to gain new knowledge about this resource by combining research or monitoring with the management activities they currently use or anticipate using. By collaborating with researchers, they can modify some of these manipulations to take the form of well-designed, large-scale experiments. Ideally, such experiments should include features of sound experimental design, such as replication, randomization, and controls. Where such features are compromised, we offer some suggestions on how to modify designs appropriately. They also should include estimation of demographic parameters such as productivity and survival, rather than just assessment of presence/ absence. We present four examples from our own work with silviculture in two forest types, prescribed fire, and insect pest management. In each case, a long-term, large-scale, manipulative experiment was developed and funded through collaborative efforts among researchers, managers, and multiple partners. Benefits to managers include timely information directly pertinent to their lands. Benefits to researchers include increased funding opportunities for basic as well as applied research, and the knowledge that their research results are being used. Both groups benefit in that they are able to achieve more together than either could alone. Land managers (e.g., wildlife managers, foresters, range managers) typically carry out a variety of management practices to promote the resources they oversee. When faced with questions regarding management of their resource, they often resort to what is to them common sense, based on a career-long experience with the resource in question. When faced with questions for which they do not readily have answers, they often rely upon literature and/or expert In: Bonney, Rick; Pashley, David N.; Cooper, Robert J.; Niles, Larry, eds. 2000. Strategies for bird conservation: The Partners in Flight planning process; Proceedings of the 3rd Partners in Flight Workshop; 1995 October 1-5; Cape May, NJ. Proceedings RMRS-P-16. Ogden, UT: U.S. Department of Agriculture, Forest Service, Rocky Mountain Research Station. Robert J. Cooper, Daniel B. Warnell School of Forest Resources, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602 U.S.A. George A. Gale, Department of Biology, University of Memphis, Memphis, TN 38152 U.S.A. Current address: King Mongkut’s University of Technology, Thonburi School of Bioresources & Technology, Division of Natural Resources Management, Bangmod, Thungkru, Bangkok 10140 Thailand. Leonard A. Brennan, Tall Timbers Research Station, Route 1, Box 678, Tallahassee, FL 32312 U.S.A. 220 opinion. A current problem is that an adequate information base does not yet exist regarding management practices for migratory landbirds and many other nongame species. We often simply do not know the effect of a particular management practice on a particular bird species. Some of the most basic information needed by land managers to effectively manage migratory landbirds involves the effects of typical management practices on migrants. Despite recent advances in the knowledge of management of long-distance migrants, most reviews on the subject have been necessarily general (Finch and Stangel 1993b; Martin and Finch 1995). One of the most common complaints we hear from managers regarding managing their lands for migratory birds is the lack of such information specific to their needs. We maintain that the power to change this situation rests partially with land managers themselves. Often, important information can be obtained by monitoring, which can be done directly by the land manager if adequate resources and personnel exist, or by collaborating with researchers or volunteers. For example, monitoring conducted in places where different management practices have been applied in the past provides information on effects of those practices. If practices such as timber harvesting, prescribed burning, or pesticide applications are about to be performed, then monitoring efforts aimed at assessing the effects of those practices can constitute manipulative experiments. Attention to attributes of experimental design such as replication, randomization, and controls will help such assessments be more meaningful, and allow extrapolation of results to other areas. Input by experienced researchers, presumably knowledgeable in experimental design, is therefore desirable. By communicating and collaborating with researchers, land managers can make their areas more attractive as research sites. From the researcher’s viewpoint, input from managers is just as desirable to ensure that manipulations are done properly and that they reflect realistic management practices. Also, both groups often will be needed to combine scarce resources required to perform the project properly. The purpose of this paper is to provide suggestions on how such large-scale manipulative experiments can be designed and developed, with some examples of practical application, which come from our own unpublished work, in progress. The lack of sound, large-scale manipulative experiments in the literature at the time of the Cape May workshop reflected the past failure of researchers and land managers to collaborate. We note with optimism that many such experiments are now under way, however, this fact does not diminish USDA Forest Service Proceedings RMRS-P-16. 2000 the pertinence of this paper. It is targeted equally toward managers, researchers, and those interested in monitoring, because without the cooperation and collaboration of all three groups, making the large advances in the knowledge needed to effectively manage Neotropical migratory birds will be difficult. Experimental Design _____________ If the effect of a particular management practice on migratory landbirds is to be examined, then the investigators should follow rules of good experimental design in their investigation. By experimental design we mean “the logical structure of an experiment” (Fisher 1971). The reader is also directed to Hurlbert (1984), Johnson (this proceedings), and Nichols (this proceedings). In this paper we will largely use the terminology of Hurlbert (1984), who differentiated between a manipulative experiment, in which experimental units (i.e., those units to which treatments are applied) are somehow manipulated by the application, and a mensurative experiment, in which nothing is manipulated, but plots may be established and measured with the objective of comparing experimental units over space or time. We assume in the following that some management practice (e.g., timber harvest, prescribed burning, pesticide application, grazing) is to be (in the case of the manipulative experiment) or has been (for mensurative experiments) applied over a large area (e.g., a management compartment). For example, in the case of a management compartment of several hundred hectares, usually the whole compartment is scheduled to receive a particular treatment at a particular time; nothing is random about the selection of the site or the treatment allocated to it. However, monitoring that site still can be meaningful if another control site is monitored, and if a before-and-after, control and impact (BACI) design is used (Stewart-Oaten and others 1986). Alternatively, and this is the approach we have adopted, if plots can be established within a management compartment or other area, and if the land manager is willing to let various treatments be randomly assigned to the plots, then the study takes on the characteristics of a welldesigned manipulative experiment. Below, we briefly discuss some considerations involving the design of large-scale manipulative experiments. Objective This may seem obvious, but more than one major investigation has failed because the objectives of the study were not clearly defined. Objectives will dictate the treatments to be applied, the timing of the treatments, what should be measured, and how long the study should last. What should be measured? Point counts are desirable in that they cover a maximum amount of area with a minimum amount of effort, so that larger numbers of replicates are possible. Also, standardized methodologies have been developed (Ralph and others 1995b; Hamel and others 1996). However, point counts also provide minimal information, and are affected by a number of extrinsic factors. Methodologies should be determined by objectives more than by logistical or financial constraints. We believe that to truly understand USDA Forest Service Proceedings RMRS-P-16. 2000 the effects of alternative management practices, demographic data (productivity, survival) are required. Much of this section of the Cape May Proceedings is devoted to this proposition. Controls By control we mean a treatment that serves as a meaningful comparison to an actual management practice, such that the effects of the practice relative to not performing the action can be assessed. Often, but not always, developing a control involves taking measurements in an area where no management action is taken (see example 2 for a control involving a manipulation). Simply monitoring birds on a single study area before and after a management practice is applied does not provide useful information, because other factors that might influence the birds in question also might have changed over that time. For this reason we believe that, of the features of sound experimental design identified by Hurlbert (1984), controls are the most important. Randomization Ideally, treatments (alternative management practices, controls) should be allocated randomly to experimental units. If a particular management compartment, for example, is due to be thinned, but no other compartment is due, then one might divide the compartment designated for treatment in half, thinning a randomly selected portion of the compartment, but leaving the other half as a control. Such a procedure is not always possible, because sometimes a particular treatment must be applied to a particular area. However, if randomization is not used, then the areas studied are not representative of the population from which they were selected, and meaningful extrapolation of results to other areas is not valid. Also, an effort should be made to intersperse treatments, to avoid an unlucky draw in which all replicates of a particular treatment are lumped together in space (Hurlbert 1984). Replication If treatments are applied to one experimental unit each, then any differences found may result not from the treatment, but could be attributable to preexisting differences on those sites. In other words, the effect of treatment is confounded with location, and inferences applied to the specific site where the study is being performed are limited. Because of expense and other considerations, only one set of treatments may be possible at a particular site (i.e., no replication at all). Is a study that compares a number of treatments with no replication worthwhile? While certainly not as desirable as a replicated study, we believe the answer is yes. First, an unreplicated study still can provide the land manager with some insight concerning the potential effects of management practices on birds. Results of unreplicated studies can be thought of as working hypotheses, which the manager can support or refute by further investigations. This approach can be expanded into the area of adaptive resource management (Conroy, this proceedings). Second, if the management practice in question is of interest to one 221 manager, it probably is of interest to others, and hopefully the study can be, or already is, being replicated elsewhere (perhaps also using a design that is similarly compromised). Power Power is defined as the probability of rejecting a false null hypothesis, therefore, having high power in an experiment is desirable. Unfortunately, the only way to increase the power of an experiment while minimizing Type I error is to increase the number of replicates. Because of the relationship between power and sample size, the power of most large-scale manipulative experiments is low. There are several ways to partially overcome the low power inherent in studies with a small number of replicates. First, rather than investigate many different levels of a particular treatment (e.g., silvicultural options), one can reduce the number of treatment levels, and for the same effort, increase the number of replicates per level. Second, standardization of methods among studies with similar objectives can increase power by combining studies from different locations in one analysis (meta-analysis, see Fernandez-Duque and Valeggia 1994). See Cohen (1977) for a more complete treatment of the subject of power. Manipulation Sometimes, research aimed at understanding the effects of alternative management practices on birds has involved taking measurements in areas where alternative management practices were applied in the past. This assumes that the different areas were similar to begin with, which probably is not the case. It also ignores the important consideration of randomization. However, short-term effects (≤3 yrs) of alternative management practices are often not the same as long-term effects (say, ≥10 yrs); understanding long-term effects requires a long period of time that we often do not have. Therefore, while mensurative experiments often are not as rigorous or insightful as manipulative experiments, they are sometimes necessary. Furthermore, short-term effects can be misleading. For example, site fidelity can cause migratory birds to occupy breeding territories in habitats severely altered by timber cuts or other management activities that occurred during the previous winter, even though the habitats have become suboptimal. Assessment of both short- and long-term effects are important; therefore, we conclude that mensurative experiments can be valuable. They are not, however, a substitute for good longterm research. Pretreatment Data Are pretreatment data necessary in manipulative experiments such as these? Strictly speaking, a treatment effect can be detected on most parameters without pretreatment data if adequate controls are used (e.g., Cooper and others 1990). Pretreatment data are desirable for several reasons, however. First, we have found that a season or two is often required for a project to operate smoothly. Often, we are unsure which species we will monitor for demographic work 222 until we have gained some experience in the study area. Second, estimation of survival necessitates a large number of banded birds, and obtaining adequate sample sizes can take several years. Third, because of low power or no replication, detecting treatment effects is often difficult. An alternative to data analysis involves a largely graphical approach, in which means and standard errors are plotted for each treatment over time (Hurlbert 1984). Pretreatment data are necessary in this type of analysis. Also, a BACI analysis can be employed with this design (Stewart-Oaten and others 1986). Realism Researchers tend to want to conduct experiments in an ordered, efficient, elegant fashion. Managers, on the other hand, generally are not as concerned with methodology as they are with results (i.e., which management option produces the desired results). For example, in a study of the effect of different logging practices on avian productivity (see below), it was suggested that different treatments should consist of removing the same total volume of timber on each plot, but the size and number of individual cuts should differ among plots. While this is attractive from a research standpoint, it is not the way that timber is marked and cut in an actual harvest operation on these national wildlife refuges. The objective of harvesting was to promote wildlife habitat. Different prescriptions result in different percentages of standing timber removed. Therefore, the individual trees that were selected for harvest were selected and marked by the refuge silviculturalist using prescriptions typically employed on the refuge. The other approach was simply not realistic, and inferences made from that study design would have had limited application elsewhere. Plot Size The size of the study plots will vary with the study and its objectives. We have adopted BBIRD (Breeding Bird Inventory and Research Database) protocols, which call for plots to be at least 20 ha to allow enough area to conduct point counts and to find a sufficient number of nests to make valid inferences (Martin and others 1997). Collaboration ___________________ Research such as that described above requires collaboration and cooperation between researchers and managers. Researchers who attempt large-scale manipulative experiments without close collaboration with the managers on whose land they are working risk conducting a presumably well-designed experiment that has limited application elsewhere; they also risk a certain amount of resentment. Most managers also are willing to share resources such as housing and vehicles if available. On the other hand, most managers are not researchers; they risk conducting a poorly designed experiment if they do not consult with researchers wellversed in experimental design. Large-scale manipulative experiments with adequate replication are expensive, and may be beyond the means of most USDA Forest Service Proceedings RMRS-P-16. 2000 managers. Even on well-funded projects, replication may be minimal. In such cases, collaboration with other land managers and researchers at other locations is helpful. By bringing other resources to bear, a well-designed study can be replicated at another location. If done carefully so that the design and methodology are identical, then different locations can serve as blocks in a randomized design. Even if the different studies are similar only in the questions they pose, they sometimes can be combined in a meta-analysis, which again serves to increase the power of the overall analysis. Examples 1. Alternative Silvicultural Practices in Bottomland Hardwood Forests—This study was initiated in 1993 by the University of Memphis collaborating with the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service (USFWS), and was continued in 1997 in a similar collaborative arrangement between the USFWS and the University of Georgia. The study area is the White River National Wildlife Refuge (NWR), one of many federal refuges along the lower Mississippi Alluvial Valley (MAV) originally established for management of migratory waterfowl. Federal refuges also provide some of the largest tracts of unfragmented bottomland hardwood forest remaining in the MAV. Most of the forest on refuges is managed to promote within-stand diversity and the production of hard mast; timber harvest alternatives designed to promote oak regeneration are selective cuts (thinnings) or small (<2 ha) patch cuts. In 1993, six 50-ha plots were established in the same management compartment (which was due to be harvested in 1995), so that all plots were initially somewhat similar. Three treatment options (thinnings, patch cuts, no cuts) were randomly allocated to two plots each. After two seasons of pre-treatment data collection in 1994-1995, treatments were applied in the late summer-fall of 1995 under the supervision of refuge silviculturalists. Post-treatment data collection occurred in 1996-2000. Measurements taken include point counts, survival, productivity, habitat characteristics, and foraging behavior. Methods generally follow BBIRD protocols. Since its inception, the project has been funded by a partnership between the University of Memphis (and later the University of Georgia), the USFWS, the U.S. Forest Service (USFS), the Biological Resources Division of the U.S. Geological Survey, and the Arkansas Game and Fish Commission and the Arkansas Chapter of The Nature Conservancy. The refuge staff was present and contributed to every aspect of planning the project, including personnel, equipment, and most importantly housing, which served to defray costs and logistical problems. Despite the favorable aspects of the design of this experiment, there are still only two replicates of each treatment, providing low power. Therefore, we are collaborating with researchers and managers at Tensas River NWR (D. J. Twedt, unpublished data), who are performing a very similar experiment, also with two replicates of each treatment. For some questions, then, this collaboration will double our number of replicates. USDA Forest Service Proceedings RMRS-P-16. 2000 2. Alternative Gypsy Moth Management Practices in the Middle Appalachians—This study was initiated in 1994 by the University of Memphis collaborating with West Virginia University (WVU) and the USFS, and was continued in 1997 with collaboration between the University of Georgia, WVU, and the USFS. The study areas are located in the George Washington National Forest in Virginia and the Monongahela National Forest in West Virginia. The gypsy moth (Lymantria dispar), accidentally introduced from Eurasia in 1869, has become the most important insect pest of eastern deciduous forests, annually defoliating millions of hectares. Pesticides are also applied to hundreds of thousands of hectares annually. Although the pesticide most commonly used is the bacterium Bacillus thuringiensis (Bt), which affects larval insects only and has no direct effects on vertebrates, it still may affect birds indirectly by eliminating their most important prey, caterpillars. The gypsy moth is expected to eventually invade most of the lower 48 states, and is expected to be most devastating in the southern states, whose forests have a high oak component (oaks are preferred hosts). Eighteen 200-ha plots were established in 1994, nine in each forest. Within each plot, a 30-ha core plot was established where most of the bird and insect work is being performed. Three treatment options (Bt, no-action [defoliation], and controls) were randomly allocated to the plots to provide six replicates of each treatment option. Unlike many studies, controls are not untreated treatments. Rather, we expect the untreated areas to be defoliated, so they will not serve as controls as defined above. Therefore, six plots will be sprayed with Gypchek, the commercial formulation of the nucleopolyhedrosis virus that affects only the gypsy moth (it is not in widespread production and is available only for experimental purposes). This will assure that the only difference between defoliated sites and controls will be defoliation (neither will have Bt), and that the only difference between Bt-treated sites and controls will be Bt (neither will have defoliation). Pre-treatment data collection occurred in 1995-1996, and pesticide treatments were applied in1997 and 1998. With post-treatment monitoring, the project is scheduled to be a 10-year study. Measurements taken include point counts, territory mapping, survival, productivity, habitat, diet and foraging behavior, and arthropod sampling. Methods again generally follow BBIRD protocols. This project was funded by a competitively awarded grant to a team headquartered at WVU. The team consists of entomologists, a toxicologist, avian ecologists, and a herpetologist. Plots were located through collaboration with USFS personnel. Spraying was done as part of the gypsy moth eradication programs of the respective national forests, and involved state and federal biologists. Spray volumes and rates reflected normal applications. 3. Alternative Prescribed Burning Practices in Longleaf Pine Forests—In the southeastern coastal plain, applications of prescribed fire for management of northern bobwhite (Colinus virginianus) quail in longleaf pine (Pinus palustris) forests have been conducted during February and March for more than 70 years (Stoddard 1931). The genesis for this idea came from the understanding that lightningseason ignitions were a natural process of coastal plain 223 ecosystems. However, elements of controversy (fire burns nests), as well as convenience (February and March are pleasant times to burn, whereas June and July can be miserable), resulted in development of a paradigm of dormant season applications of prescribed fire for many years. Over time, enlightened managers realized that if prescribed fire was to be used to mimic an ecosystem process, ignitions should be generated during the peak of lightning season (April-August) rather than dormant season (late winter). Although winter months are not outside the temporal range of natural fire occurrence in this region, research and anecdotal observation indicate that applications of lightning season fire are essential for keystone plants like wiregrass (Aristida stricta) to flower and set viable seed. The existing literature on comparison of dormant and lightning season fire applications is limited, controversial, and conflicting (Robbins and Myers 1992). Therefore, an experiment was developed to examine how dormant and lightning season applications of prescribed fire influence the abundance and distribution of vertebrates in longleaf pine forests. In one study, four pairs of 12 ha plots were each established in Apalachicola National Forest in Florida, and the Sandhills region of North Carolina. In each pair, one of the two fire application options was randomly assigned. The plots are imbedded in much larger management units that average 50-100 ha, which is the level of management at which prescribed fire is applied. Here, controls were not used because the “no treatment” option is not a viable management alternative. Therefore, pre- and post-treatment assessments are a central part of the experimental design (Engstrom and others 1996). In this study, bird abundance was estimated by spot mapping. Other work done on these plots and others in ancillary studies include vegetation sampling, arthropod abundance, and quail radiotelemetry. The project is funded through a partnership between the Department of Defense, the North Carolina Wildlife Resources Division, National Fish and Wildlife Foundation, four private foundations, several key donors from the Red Hills Shooting Plantations, the USFS, USFWS, Tall Timbers Research Station, and Mississippi State University (Brennan and others 1996). 4. Longleaf Pine Restoration/Midstory Removal Experiment—Longleaf pine forest once covered nearly 90 million acres of the southeastern coastal plain (Simberloff 1993). Today, less than 1 million ha remain. Much of the remnant longleaf stands are degraded from lack of frequent fire, which causes hardwoods to encroach and invade the midstory of longleaf stands. In many cases, encroachment of hardwood midstory causes keystone species such as redcockaded woodpecker (Picoides borealis), gopher tortoise (Gopherus polyphemus), and bobwhite quail to leave or undergo local extinction. Personnel from Eglin Air Force Base, The Nature Conservancy, the University of Florida, and Tall Timbers Research Station have been collaborating on a comprehensive, largescale hardwood midstory removal experiment in longleaf pine stands at Eglin. The primary objective of the study is to understand how three methods of hardwood removal (fire, chainsaw, and herbicide) affect soil properties, vegetation, 224 arthropods, and birds. Each treatment area is 100 ha. There are 6 replicates per treatment option (18 total), in addition to 3 untreated stands. Thus, overall, treatments were applied to 1800 ha under various local contracts by the Jackson Guard Natural Resources Division of Eglin. The project features point counts, vegetation and arthropod sampling, and measurements of avian foraging behavior. Discussion _____________________ The benefits of large-scale manipulative experiments involving management questions are clear: Rigorous experimental design, realism, a good blend of management and research, and immediate application of results. Another benefit is that more resources are brought to bear to obtain funding. How is funding obtained? This sort of research is expensive, and is only likely to be funded extramurally through competitive grants. The need for partnerships is critical, both to make the research stronger and to increase the funding base. Also, with more partners the funding base is more secure. Even if a project is well funded but from a single source, it is in jeopardy of ending if that single source is suddenly gone. Researchers who have spent their careers conducting basic research might question the wisdom of becoming involved with management questions. We have found that several benefits result from conducting this sort of applied research. First, it is generally easier to obtain funding to do applied research, especially when agencies involved with management are providing some of the funding. Second, basic questions involving the ecology of migratory birds can always be addressed within the framework of the applied study. For example, Bt application can be viewed as an experimental manipulation of prey abundance, providing the basis for interesting questions involving bird/insect interactions and habitat selection theory, among others (e.g., Rodenhouse and Holmes 1992). Third, researchers in conservation biology often wonder if their research is making a difference. We have found that the results of this research are of great interest to managers, and that results are often applied before they find their way into print. Further, if conducted properly, the results of these studies can be applied not only on the study site but elsewhere with similar habitat types. Finally, collaboration among researchers and managers is the essence of Partners in Flight, an ad hoc group whose members, while diverse in interests and backgrounds, all work toward the single goal of conserving populations of Neotropical migratory birds. Acknowledgments ______________ We wish to acknowledge all of our collaborators and contributors, too numerous to mention individually, on these projects. We also acknowledge the contributions made by the many technicians, interns, and graduate students on these projects. Without all of you our work would be far less meaningful, and this publication would not have been possible. USDA Forest Service Proceedings RMRS-P-16. 2000