How Citizens Respond to Combat Casualties

advertisement
Public Opinion Quarterly, Vol. 76, No. 4, Winter 2012, pp. 761–770
How Citizens Respond to Combat Casualties
The Differential Impact of Local Casualties
on Support for the War in Afghanistan
douglas l. kriner*
francis x. shen
International relations scholarship, from Kant’s (1983 [1795]) theory of perpetual peace to contemporary analyses of the importance of ballot box constraints in understanding state conflict behavior (see Russett 1990; Bueno de
Mesquita and Lalman 1992; Reiter and Stam 1998; Bueno de Mesquita et al.
2003), has long emphasized the political importance of domestic public opinion. Most analyses of wartime opinion formation have conceptualized the process by which citizens decide whether or not to support a war as a cost-benefit
Douglas L. Kriner is an Associate Professor of Political Science at Boston University, Boston,
MA, USA. Francis X. Shen is an Associate Professor of Law at the University of Minnesota,
Minneapolis, MN, USA. *Address correspondence to Douglas Kriner, Boston University,
Department of Political Science, 232 Bay State Rd., Boston, MA, USA; e-mail: dkriner@bu.edu.
doi:10.1093/poq/nfs048Advance Access publication 19 October 2012
© The Author 2012. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the American Association for Public Opinion Research.
All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com
Downloaded from http://poq.oxfordjournals.org/ at Boston University Libraries on August 22, 2013
Abstract Scholars have long analyzed the influence of combat casualties on public support for war. However, the mechanisms through
which casualties—particularly local casualties—affect wartime opinion formation have received much less attention. We employ a novel
survey experiment to test three mechanisms that might explain previously observed cleavages in war support between residents of high- and
low-casualty communities. We find that subjects who read a news story
concerning a casualty from their home state were significantly more
likely to oppose the war in Afghanistan than were subjects who read an
identically worded news story in which the fallen soldier was not identified as being from the respondent’s home state. Moreover, this difference emerged regardless of whether the story followed the coverage
patterns and emphasis typical of national or local media reporting. We
conclude that the local connections triggered by learning of a homestate casualty, not the emotionally charged nature of local media reporting, is most responsible for generating opinion cleavages observed in
previous research.
762
Kriner and Shen
Three Potential Mechanisms
Most prior analyses of the political effects of local casualties emphasize the
greater exposure to the human costs of war that residents of high-casualty communities receive versus their peers in low-casualty communities. We term this
the differential exposure mechanism. Given the absence of mass mobilization
Downloaded from http://poq.oxfordjournals.org/ at Boston University Libraries on August 22, 2013
calculation (see Page, Shapiro, and Dempsey 1987; Gartner and Segura 1998;
Gelpi, Feaver, and Reifler 2009). And, in the low-mobilization wars waged
since World War II, casualties have been the primary way in which most
Americans see a conflict’s costs (Aldrich et al. 2006; Gartner 2008). Across
a range of military actions, from Vietnam and Korea, to more minor missions
in Panama and the Persian Gulf, to recent conflicts in Afghanistan and Iraq,
scholars have shown that public support declines as casualties accumulate
(e.g., Mueller 1973; Larson 1996; Mueller 1994; Eichenberg, Stoll, and Lebo
2006; Jacobson 2010).
However, recent scholarship also finds that casualties are far from uniformly
distributed across the country. This unequal exposure to casualties, in turn,
has produced significant cleavages in policy opinions and political behaviors
between Americans who live in communities that have suffered high versus
low numbers of casualties. For example, in both the Vietnam and Iraq wars,
residents of high-casualty communities were more likely to believe each war
was a mistake and to favor an early withdrawal than demographically identical residents of communities that had experienced substantially lower casualty
rates (Gartner, Segura, and Wilkening 1997; Hayes and Myers 2009; Kriner
and Shen 2010; Althaus, Bramlett, and Gimpel 2011). Moreover, these cleavages reappeared at the ballot box; state and county casualties strongly influenced both House and Senate races during the Vietnam era (Gartner, Segura,
and Barratt 2004; Kriner and Shen 2010), as well as the 2004 presidential
election (Karol and Miguel 2007) and House and Senate races during the 2006
midterms (Kriner and Shen 2007; Grose and Oppenheimer 2007; Gartner and
Segura 2008).
Although these political consequences are well established, existing scholarship tells us significantly less about the mechanisms through which local
casualties generate these political effects. How, precisely, do local casualties
affect citizens’ cost-benefit calculations when forming their wartime policy
preferences? Are residents of high-casualty communities simply more aware
of the human costs of war than residents of low-casualty communities? Are
local casualties covered differently in the media in ways that disproportionately sway public opinion? Or do local casualties cause citizens to update their
cost-benefit calculations differently than information about national casualties? The answers to these questions may compel us to fundamentally reexamine how citizens respond to war and its human costs.
How Citizens Respond to Combat Casualties
763
Downloaded from http://poq.oxfordjournals.org/ at Boston University Libraries on August 22, 2013
in America’s post–World War II conflicts and the widespread lack of information concerning casualty totals observed in the Iraq War (Berinsky 2007;
Myers and Hayes 2010), there are at least three reasons that residents of highcasualty communities may be more likely to hear about casualties than other
Americans. First, Gartner, Segura, and Wilkening (1997, p. 670) argue that
“interpersonal and community-based information networks” make local casualties “better known” to citizens. Second, residents of high-casualty communities may receive significantly more elite cues critical of the war than
residents of low-casualty communities (Kriner and Shen 2010). Third, media
scholars continue to find that a large percentage of Americans rely on local
news outlets for their information on international affairs (Gilliam and Iyengar
2000), and that local news outlets give significantly more attention to local
casualties than they do to casualties from other communities (Gartner 2004).
Alternatively, Americans may simply respond differently to casualties suffered in their communities than to casualties with whom they share no local
connection. We term this the differential processing mechanism, as it implies
that citizens may weight information about local casualties differently than
nonlocal casualties when forming their policy judgments and preferences. This
mechanism is suggested in Gartner, Segura, and Wilkening’s (1997, p. 670)
argument that local war deaths are not only “better known” but also more
“highly salient” in citizens’ decision calculus. In a similar vein, in an analysis of Gallup polling data from the Iraq War, Kriner and Shen (2010) found
that respondents from high-casualty states were significantly more likely to
report that they personally knew a soldier killed or wounded in Iraq than were
respondents from low-casualty communities. And those who perceived a personal connection were significantly more likely to oppose the war and support
withdrawal than were respondents who did not.
A third possibility, a hybrid of the differential exposure and differential
processing mechanisms, emphasizes the key role played by the local media.
Local media outlets do more than simply report on casualties in their coverage
areas; they provide a certain type of coverage that may be particularly suited
to triggering the personal, emotive connections that influence public opinion.
Even a cursory comparative review of casualty coverage in national and local
news outlets reveals that the two report on casualties in dramatically different
ways. With some exceptions, most coverage mentioning casualties in national
news outlets does so within the frame of a larger story concerning the battle
or incident in which the casualties occurred and what that incident reveals
about the overall strategy in the theater. Local media coverage of casualties, by
contrast, is often intensely personal. It routinely features interviews with surviving family members, friends, neighbors, high school teachers, and athletic
coaches. The details emphasized are not of the firefight and what the battle
tells us about the larger strategy; rather, the emphasis is on personal details
of the fallen soldier and the effects of his or her death on family, friends,
and neighbors left behind. In short, local media coverage appears designed to
764
Kriner and Shen
produce the emotional reaction and sense of connection with a fallen soldier
that might lead readers to weight local war deaths disproportionately in their
cost-benefit calculations.
As a result, residents of high-casualty communities may become more critical of a war than their peers—not because of a shared local connection with
fallen soldiers, but because they receive more emotionally charged media coverage that personalizes a war’s human costs. We label this the emotional news
coverage mechanism.
Traditional observational methods are ill suited to testing between these three
competing mechanisms. However, by manipulating the information respondents receive, a survey experiment allows us to assess the influence of both
whether or not a casualty is “local” and the nature of media coverage of that
casualty on respondents’ support for war.
In summer 2010, 849 subjects were recruited to take an online survey via
Mechanical Turk.1 Although the sample is not nationally representative, it
is considerably more diverse than are undergraduate samples routinely used
in many international relations studies of public opinion (e.g., Gaines et al.
2007; Gartner 2008; Nyhan and Reifler 2010). Moreover, recent research by
Berinsky, Huber, and Lenz (2012) demonstrates that replicating experiments
on samples recruited in this way yields very similar results to previously published studies with nationally representative samples.2 Thus, while the nature
of our sample provides some barriers to generalizability, we believe that the
observed results are reflective of how a large segment of the American public
would respond to the experimental stimuli. Summary statistics for the sample’s demographics are presented in the online appendix.
The survey began by asking subjects several basic demographic questions,
including their gender, age, highest level of educational attainment, and the
state in which they currently reside. Respondents then received a number of
1. Between August 15 and October 4, 2010, 864 respondents were recruited to take an online
survey via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk service. Four subjects who began the survey dropped out
before answering the first question (gender), and another eleven subjects were thanked for their
willingness to participate but excused from the survey because they did not reside in one of the fifty
U.S. states. All 849 of the remaining subjects answered the war-support question and subsequent
demographics; the only exception is that four subjects refused to answer the question on income.
2. On the use of Mechanical Turk samples, see also Kittur, Chi, and Suh (2008); Paolacci,
Chandler, and Ipeirotis (2010); Mason and Suri (2010); Ross et al. (2010); and Buhrmester,
Kwang, and Gosling (2011). An additional concern regards the quality of the answers given by
an online convenience sample. As a measure of quality control, we concluded the survey with an
attention filter validated by previous research (Oppenheimer, Meyvis, and Davidenk 2009). More
than 80 percent of subjects answered the attention filter correctly. Results are virtually identical if
those who did not answer the attention filter correctly are excluded from the analysis.
Downloaded from http://poq.oxfordjournals.org/ at Boston University Libraries on August 22, 2013
Experimental Design
How Citizens Respond to Combat Casualties
765
Table 1. Experimental Design
Home state
Non-home state
National story
Local story
Treatment 1
Treatment 2
Treatment 3
Treatment 4
unrelated questions before being asked to read a short news story about the war
in Afghanistan, which discussed the death of a hypothetical soldier, “Corporal
Jeremy Smith.” Respondents were randomly assigned to one of four versions
of the news story, which varied along two dimensions: whether the news story
followed the form of “national” media coverage of war casualties or of “local”
news coverage of casualties, and whether the fallen soldier was identified as
hailing from the subject’s self-reported home state. The resulting 2x2 matrix
of treatments is summarized in table 1.
Treatments 1 and 2 both presented subjects with a news story typical of
national media coverage of war casualties. This “national” news story led
with the information that Corporal Smith was killed in a firefight with Taliban
forces outside Kandahar. It then proceeded to describe the specifics of the
attack and to connect this battle with the military’s larger strategy of trying to
stabilize Kandahar province in southern Afghanistan. In treatment 1, Corporal
Smith was identified as hailing from the subject’s home state. In treatment 2,
no home state was reported for Corporal Smith.
Treatments 3 and 4 both presented subjects with a news story typical of local
media coverage of war deaths. This “local” news story also led with the specific
details of Corporal Smith’s death in Afghanistan. However, the remainder of
the article focused on those Corporal Smith left behind, and the article reported
reflections from family and friends. In treatment 3, Corporal Smith was identified as hailing from the subject’s home state. In treatment 4, Corporal Smith
was identified as hailing from a different state, assigned at random. The full text
of the local and national story variants are presented in the online appendix.
After reading the story, all subjects were asked the same question (on a fivepoint Likert scale): “Do you support or oppose the U.S. war in Afghanistan?”
For the initial analysis, we collapse “strongly oppose” and “oppose” answers
to generate a percentage opposing the war in each of the four treatments.
Comparing levels of war support across the four treatment groups allows us
to assess the three mechanisms posited previously. If the differential exposure
mechanism alone is driving previously observed cleavages in war opinions
between high- and low-casualty residents, then we would not expect to see
any differences in responses across the four treatments. In our experiment, all
Downloaded from http://poq.oxfordjournals.org/ at Boston University Libraries on August 22, 2013
Note.—The two rows indicate whether or not the story identified the fallen soldier as hailing
from the respondent’s home state. The two columns denote the type of coverage in the treatment
story—whether the experimental treatment presented information in the format characteristic of
national media outlet reporting or local news reporting of Afghanistan casualties.
766
Kriner and Shen
Results
Table 2 presents the results. Each cell displays the percentage of subjects who
said they opposed the war in Afghanistan. The high levels of war opposition in
each of the four cells echo results from contemporary national surveys showing a majority of Americans opposed to the war. Nevertheless, we do see significantly different levels of opposition across the treatment conditions. Only
53 percent of subjects who read the baseline national news story in which no
home state was given for the fallen soldier opposed the war. However, among
subjects who read an identical story with one exception—Corporal Smith was
identified as hailing from the respondent’s home state—that figure increased
dramatically to 62 percent. The near 10-percent increase in war opposition
is particularly striking because the difference between the two stories was so
minimal. The home-state version of the “national” news story did not provide
any additional detail about Corporal Smith, his family, or the broader effects
of his death on others. Rather, the only difference is that it identified Corporal
Smith as being from the subject’s home state. This local connection between
the subject and the casualty mentioned in the story, alone, significantly
increased the probability of that subject opposing the war in Afghanistan. This
result is evidence for the differential processing mechanism.
The “local” news treatments also yielded somewhat surprising results.
Subjects in treatment 4 read a newspaper article patterned after typical local
Downloaded from http://poq.oxfordjournals.org/ at Boston University Libraries on August 22, 2013
subjects received the same basic information concerning an American soldier
killed in Afghanistan.
By contrast, if home-state casualties resonate more with Americans than
those from beyond the state lines, as posited by the differential processing
mechanism, we would expect to see the most opposition to the war in the
national and local home-state treatments (1 and 3). If the emotional and personal nature of local media coverage is more important than whether the casualty is actually from a respondent’s home state—as posited by the emotional
news coverage mechanism—we would expect to see the most war opposition
in the two local news treatments (3 and 4), regardless of whether or not the
casualty is identified as from the respondent’s state of residence. Finally, if
both of these latter mechanisms matter, then we should see the highest level of
war opposition in the local home-state story treatment (3) and the lowest level
of war opposition in the national non-home-state treatment (2).
Two additional features of the experiment merit notice. First, to mitigate
external validity concerns, our experimental stimuli involve an ongoing war,
instead of a hypothetical military venture. Second, the use of Afghanistan substantially reduces the probability that any of our treatments will have a significant effect on public opinion, as attitudes toward the war should have calcified
over the preceding nine years.
How Citizens Respond to Combat Casualties
767
Table 2. Influence of Home State Connection vs. Local Media
Coverage on Opposition to the War in Afghanistan
Home state
Non-home state
National story
Local story
62%
53%
62%
50%
news coverage of Afghanistan war casualties—coverage that emphasized the
toll the soldier’s death had exacted on family, friends, and the greater community. However, this story identified Corporal Smith as hailing from a different
state than the subject. In this treatment, 50 percent of respondents answered
that they opposed the war in Afghanistan. Interestingly, this percentage is even
lower than (though not statistically different from) the percentage of respondents opposing the Afghan War in the national non-home-state story treatment.
In sharp contrast to expectations derived from the emotional news coverage
mechanism, the humanizing details and emotional tone of the local news story
did not heighten opposition to the conflict.
When the soldier in the local news story is identified as hailing from the
subject’s home state, the percentage of respondents opposing the war soars
to 62 percent. Yet, this percentage is exactly the same as that observed in the
home-state national story treatment. Surprisingly, the additional personal
details and interviews with family members and friends had no additional
impact on increasing war opposition beyond that generated by identifying the
soldier as from a respondent’s home state. Thus, strongly consistent with the
differential processing mechanism, the local connection triggered by learning
about a soldier from one’s home state—not the nature of the coverage of that
casualty—appears to have the greatest effect on Americans’ evaluations of the
war in Afghanistan.
As a robustness check, we also estimated an ordered logit model that used
the dependent variable’s full five-point scale and controlled for respondents’
demographic characteristics. These results, discussed in the online appendix,
are virtually identical to those presented in table 2. Reading a story about
a casualty from one’s home state significantly increased the probability of
that subject opposing the war in Afghanistan, regardless of whether the mock
news article reflected local or national casualty coverage. By contrast, the
nature of the coverage—whether it mimicked “national” or “local” coverage norms—had no impact on war support. The local ties evoked by reading
about a fallen soldier from the subject’s home state were the only experimentally manipulated factor that significantly affected a subject’s probability of
opposing the war.
Downloaded from http://poq.oxfordjournals.org/ at Boston University Libraries on August 22, 2013
Note.—The differences between the home-state and non-home-state treatments in both
the national story and local story categories are statistically significant, p < .05, one-tailed test.
Neither of the differences across rows is statistically significant.
768
Kriner and Shen
Conclusion
Supplementary Data
Supplementary data are freely available online at http://poq.oxfordjournals.org/.
References
Aldrich, John, Christopher Gelpi, Peter Feaver, Jason Reifler, and Kristin Sharp. 2006. “Foreign
Policy and the Electoral Connection.” Annual Review of Political Science 9:477–502.
Althaus, Scott, Brittany Bramlet, and James Gimpel. 2011. “When War Hits Home: The
Geography of Military Losses and Support for War in Time and Space.” Journal of Conflict
Resolution 56(3):382–412.
Berinsky, Adam. 2007. “Assuming the Costs of War: Events, Elites, and American Public Support
for Military Conflict.” Journal of Politics 69:975–97.
Berinsky, Adam, Greg Huber, and Gabriel Lenz. 2012. “Evaluating Online Labor Markets for
Experimental Research: Amazon.com’s Mechanical Turk.” Political Analysis 20(3):351–368.
Bueno de Mesquita, Bruce, and David Lalman. 1992. War and Reason: Domestic and
International Imperatives. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Bueno de Mesquita, Bruce, James Morrow, Randolph Siverson, and Alastair Smith. 2003. The
Logic of Political Survival. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Buhrmester, Michael, Tracy Kwang, and Samuel D. Gosling. 2011. “Amazon’s Mechanical
Turk: A New Source of Inexpensive, Yet High-Quality Data?” Perspectives on Psychological
Science 6:3–5.
Eichenberg, Richard, Richard Stoll, and Matthew Lebo. 2006. “War President: The Approval
Ratings of George W. Bush.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 50(6):783–808.
Gaines, Brian, James Kuklinski, Paul Quirk, Buddy Peyton, and Jay Verkuilen. 2007. “Same
Facts, Different Interpretations: Partisan Motivation and Opinion on Iraq.” Journal of Politics
69:957–74.
Downloaded from http://poq.oxfordjournals.org/ at Boston University Libraries on August 22, 2013
A significant lacuna in the wartime opinion literature involves the mechanisms
by which casualties—particularly local casualties—influence support for war.
Employing a novel survey experiment, we show that an important component
within the proverbial “black box” is the local connection that citizens feel when
“one of their own” falls on a foreign battlefield. Holding all other details about
the death constant, reading about a fallen soldier from one’s own state makes
one significantly more likely to oppose the war effort. Although this local connection has been alluded to in previous scholarship, it has not been isolated
from other plausible mechanisms. Our survey experiment demonstrates that
the local connection to a casualty, not the emotional tenor characteristic of
local media coverage, has the greatest influence on opinion formation.
Additional experimental and observational work on causal mechanisms is
needed to further our understanding of the nuanced ways in which a soldier’s
death on a distant battlefield is communicated to, and understood by, U.S. citizens. How these mechanisms operate, in turn, significantly influences the
extent to which battlefield deaths translate into political pressure brought to
bear on policymakers in Washington.
How Citizens Respond to Combat Casualties
769
Downloaded from http://poq.oxfordjournals.org/ at Boston University Libraries on August 22, 2013
Gartner, Scott. 2004. “Making the International Local: The Terrorist Attack on the USS Cole,
Local Casualties, and Media Coverage.” Political Communication 21:139–59.
———. 2008. “The Multiple Effects of Casualties on Popular Support for War: An Experimental
Approach.” American Political Science Review 102:95–106.
Gartner, Scott, and Gary Segura. 1998. “War, Casualties, and Public Opinion.” Journal of Conflict
Resolution 42:278–320.
———. 2008. “All Politics Are Still Local: The Iraq War and the 2006 Midterm Elections.” PS:
Political Science and Politics 41:95–100.
Gartner, Scott, Gary Segura, and Bethany Barratt. 2004. “War Casualties, Policy Positions, and
the Fate of Legislators.” Political Research Quarterly 53:467–77.
Gartner, Scott, Gary M. Segura, and Michael Wilkening. 1997. “All Politics Are Local: Local
Losses and Individual Attitudes toward the Vietnam War.” Journal of Conflict Resolution
41:669–94.
Gelpi, Christopher, Peter D. Feaver, and Jason Reifler. 2009. Paying the Human Costs of War:
American Public Opinion and Casualties in Military Conflicts. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Gilliam, Franklin, and Shanto Iyengar. 2000. “Prime Suspects: The Influence of Local Television
News on the Viewing Public.” American Journal of Political Science 44:560–73.
Grose, Christian, and Bruce Oppenheimer. 2007. “The Iraq War, Partisanship, and Candidate
Attributes: Variation in Partisan Swing in the 2006 Election.” Legislative Studies Quarterly
32:531–57.
Hayes, Andrew, and Teresa Myers. 2009. “Testing the ‘Proximate Casualties Hypothesis’: Local
Troop Loss, Attention to News, and Support for Military Intervention.” Mass Communication
and Society 12:379–402.
Jacobson, Gary. 2010. “A Tale of Two Wars: Public Opinion on the U.S. Military Interventions in
Afghanistan and Iraq.” Presidential Studies Quarterly 40:585–610.
Kant, Immanuel. 1983 [1795]. Perpetual Peace, and Other Essays on Politics, History, and
Morals, translated by Ted Humphrey. Indianapolis: Hackett Publishing.
Karol, David, and Edward Miguel. 2007. “The Electoral Cost of War: Iraq Casualties and the
2004 U.S. Presidential Election.” Journal of Politics 69:633–48.
Kittur, Aniket, Ed H. Chi, and Bongwon Suh. 2008. “Crowdsourcing User Studies with Mechanical
Turk.” Proceedings of CHI 2008, 453–456.
Kriner, Douglas, and Francis Shen. 2007. “Iraq Casualties and the 2006 Senate Elections.”
Legislative Studies Quarterly 32:4.
———. 2010. The Casualty Gap: The Causes and Consequences of American Wartime
Inequalities. New York: Oxford University Press.
Larson, Eric. 1996. Casualties and Consensus: The Historical Role of Casualties in Domestic
Support for U.S. Military Operations. Santa Monica, CA: RAND.
Mason, Winter, and Siddharth Suri. 2010. “Conducting Behavioral Research on Amazon’s
Mechanical Turk.” Behavior Research Methods 5:411–19.
Mueller, John. 1973. War, Presidents, and Public Opinion. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
———. 1994. Policy and Opinion in the Gulf War. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Myers, Teresa, and Andrew Hayes. 2010. “Reframing the Casualties Hypothesis: (Mis)perceptions of Troop Loss and Public Opinion About War.” International Journal of Public Opinion
Research 22:256–75.
Nyhan, Brendan, and Jason Reifler. 2010. “When Corrections Fail: The Persistence of Political
Misperceptions.” Political Behavior 32:303–30.
Oppenheimer, Daniel M., Tom Meyvis, and Nicolas Davidenk. 2009. “Instructional Manipulation
Checks: Detecting Satisficing to Increase Statistical Power.” Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology 45:867–72.
Page, Benjamin, Robert Shapiro, and Glenn Dempsey. 1987. “What Moves Public Opinion?”
American Political Science Review 81:23–44.
770
Kriner and Shen
Paolacci, Gabriele, Jesse Chandler, and Panagiotis Ipeirotis. 2010. “Running Experiments on
Amazon Mechanical Turk.” Judgment and Decision Making 5:411–19.
Reiter, Dan, and Alan Stam. 1998. “Democracy, War Initiation, and Victory.” American Political
Science Review 92:377–89.
Ross, Joel, Andrew Zaldivar, Lilly Irani, Bill Tomlinson, and M. Six Silberman. 2010. “Who Are
the Crowdworkers? Shifting Demographics in Mechanical Turk.” Proceedings of CHI 2010:
Imagine All the People, 2863–2872.
Russett, Bruce. 1990. Controlling the Sword: The Democratic Governance of National Security.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Downloaded from http://poq.oxfordjournals.org/ at Boston University Libraries on August 22, 2013
Download