Biological Conservation 89 (1999) 121±129 In¯uence of prerelease experience on reintroduced black-footed ferrets (Mustela nigripes) Dean E. Biggins a,*, Astrid Vargas b, Jerry L. Godbey a, Stanley H. Anderson c a U.S. Geological Survey, Midcontinent Ecological Science Center, 4512 McMurry Avenue, Fort Collins, Colorado 80525, USA U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, National Black-footed Ferret Conservation Center, 410 Grand Avenue, Suite 315, Laramie, Wyoming 82070, USA c U.S. Geological Survey, Wyoming Cooperative Fish and Wildlife Research Unit, University of Wyoming, Laramie, Wyoming 82071, USA b Received 24 June 1998; received in revised form 24 November 1998; accepted 4 December 1998 Abstract Captive breeding is increasingly being used to create supplies of endangered animals for release into natural habitats, but rearing strategies vary and debates arise over which methods are most ecient. We assessed postrelease behaviors and survival of three groups of black-footed ferrets, each with dierent prerelease experience. Eighteen ferret kits 60 days of age were moved with their dams from cages to 80-m2 outdoor pens with prairie dog burrows. These animals were compared to animals reared in standard cages (n 72), some of which were given experience killing prairie dogs (n 32). Ferrets were released onto white-tailed prairie dog (Cynomys leucurus) colonies in Wyoming, USA, in fall, 1992. Radio-tagged cage-reared ferrets made longer nightly moves and dispersed further from release sites than their pen-reared counterparts. The band return rate was 4-fold higher for pen-reared animals than for cage-reared animals during surveys conducted about 1 month after release. We recommend routine use of quasinatural outdoor pens for prerelease conditioning of black-footed ferrets. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd. Keywords: Behavior; Black-footed ferret; Mustela nigripes; Reintroduction; Survival 1. Introduction Populations of black-footed ferrets declined throughout this century to near extinction by the late 1970's (Biggins and Schroeder, 1988). A small population was discovered near Meeteetse, Wyoming, USA in 1981 (Fig. 1), but that population was decimated by canine distemper and plague (Yersinia pestis) in 1985 (Forrest et al., 1988). The last animals were removed by 1987 to establish a captive population (Miller et al., 1996), with the ultimate goal of reintroduction. Captive-raised animals of many species have had poor success after reintroduction into their natural habitat (see reviews in Wemmer and Derrickson, 1987; Grith et al., 1989; Beck et al., 1994; Wilson and Stanley-Price, 1994). To maximize survival, reintroduction candidates must be able to procure food and shelter, develop antipredator skills, interact properly with conspeci®cs, and orient (navigate, migrate, and/or disperse) in a structurally complex environment (Kleiman, 1989; Box, 1991). Prerelease ``training'' given to several mammal species * Corresponding author. Tel.: +1-970-226-9467; fax: +1-970-2269230; e-mail: dean-biggins@usgs.gov. 0006-3207/99/$ - see front matter Published by Elsevier Science Ltd. PII: S0006-3207(98)00158-X has altered behaviors in ways assumed to be bene®cial to survival (Kleiman et al., 1986; Stanley-Price, 1989; Phillips, 1990; Soderquist and Serena, 1994). Golden lion tamarins pre-conditioned in a naturalistic wooded area had higher survivorship after release in the Brazilian forests (Beck, 1991). Masked bobwhites (Colinus virginianus ridgwayi) cross-fostered to wild neutered Texas bobwhites (C. v. virginianus) and birds trained for physical ®tness and anti-predatory behaviors survived better than untrained birds (Ellis et al., 1978). Laboratory experiments have detailed additional eects of environmental enrichment. Behaviors of domestic gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus), for example, were altered toward the wild phenotype after one generation of rearing in tunnels (Clark and Galef, 1980). Compared to rats (Rattus sp.) raised in simple cages, rats raised in more complex environments were found to have more synaptic connections and heavier brains (Rosenzweig, 1979), and were better at negotiating mazes (Greenough and Juraska, 1979) and escaping predator models (Renner, 1988). Behavioral dierences between breeds of dogs (Canis familiaris) were no longer present when the animals were reared in a quasi-natural setting (Scott and Fuller, 1965). 122 D.E. Biggins et al. / Biological Conservation 89 (1999) 121±129 To gain knowledge and experience in preparation for black-footed ferret releases, studies were ®rst conducted on the closely related Siberian polecat (Mustela eversmanii) to investigate the development of food searching and predator avoidance behaviors (Miller et al., 1990a, b). When introduced to a novel arena, polecats familiar with underground burrow systems spent less time aboveground than did their cage-reared counterparts. Studies of captive polecats were followed by trial releases of neutered polecats reared in cages and pens similar to those described below, and of neutered wild-caught polecats translocated from China (Biggins et al., 1990, 1991). Behaviors and survival diered among some groups. Predation from coyotes (Canis latrans), badgers (Taxidea taxus), and avian predators caused rapid loss of both captive-reared and wild-caught polecats. After several years of successful captive breeding, 49 ferrets were released in Shirley Basin, Wyoming (Fig. 1), in the fall of 1991. No comparative evaluations of rearing or release techniques, however, were made using those animals. All ferrets were raised in cages, and were released with postrelease support in the form of cages that held animals during an adjustment period of about 10 days; after release, a ferret could return to its cage for food and refuge. In 1992, our objective was to evaluate the eect of three rearing strategies on behavior and survival of released black-footed ferrets. Some ferrets were raised in standard cages with either of two levels of experience killing prey; other ferrets were reared in outdoor pens. In the fall, 1992, these ferrets were released in Shirley Basin (Fig. 1) and samples of animals from each experimental group were monitored via radiotelemetry. 2. Methods 2.1. Captive-breeding and rearing Black-footed ferrets used in these experimental releases were young-of-the-year raised at the Sybille Wildlife Research facility operated by the Wyoming Game and Fish Department, the Henry Doorly Zoo at Omaha, the Louisville Zoo and the National Zoo's Conservation and Research Center (Table 1). Captive-breeding and rearing procedures have been standardized (Williams et al., 1991), but modi®cations were added for animals that were to be involved in reintroductions. These animals were divided into the following 3 treatment groups (Table 1). (1) ``Standard operating procedures'' (SOP) ferret kits were reared in cages with a surface area of 1.5 m2 supported on 1 m legs (Vargas, 1994; Vargas and Anderson, 1996). The SOP ferrets initially were given live hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) three times weekly starting at age 8 weeks and were fed prairie dog meat about 3 times weekly from 60 days of age, until their release. The SOP group came from all 4 rearing facilities. Four SOP ferrets were hand-raised due to death of their mother, and one was cross-fostered to a Siberian polecat. Table 1 Group sizes and rearing facilities for young black-footed ferrets released during 1992 in Shirley Basin, Wyoming Facility of origin Sybille Omaha << ,, << ,, Louisville << ,, National Total << ,, SOPa (Radio-tagged) 12 (1) 5 (3) PDb (Radio-tagged) 18 14 (11) (6) 32 (17) PENc (Radio-tagged) 14 4 (7) (1) 18 (8) TOTAL (Radio-tagged) 66 (26) Rearing: a Fig. 1. Historic range of the black-footed ferret, location of the ®nal known population (Meeteetse, Wyoming, USA) extirpated in 1987 to protect the remnant ferrets from a disease outbreak and to initiate captive-breeding, and location of the ®rst reintroduction site (Shirley Basin, Wyoming). (Range map was adapted from Miller et al., 1996). 4 5 5 (2) (1) 10 (3) 7 (5) 12 (8) 2 0 2 (0) 40 (12) 90 (37) SOP=ferrets raised in standard indoor cages, and having no experience with live prairie dogs. b PD=ferrets raised cages and given experience with predation on prairie dogs. c PEN=ferrets born in cages, allowed access to outdoor pens with prairie dog burrows when kits were about 60 days old, and fed live prairie dogs. D.E. Biggins et al. / Biological Conservation 89 (1999) 121±129 (2) The second treatment group (PD) was raised at Sybille in SOP-type cages, fed live hamsters twice weekly starting at age 8 weeks, and exposed to live white-tailed prairie dogs (Cynomys leucurus) in weekly trials at 13± 16 weeks. A prairie dog was given to a family group of ferrets, consisting of kits and their mother, in each trial. Individual experience varied from witnessing kills and interacting with the freshly-killed prairie dog carcass to performing several kills (Vargas, 1994). (3) The third group (PEN) was raised in outdoor pens at the Sybille facility. Pens were about 80 m2 in surface area, with soil to a depth of 2.4 m. The enclosures were constructed of heavy-gauge wire mesh (``cyclone'' fencing) on 4 sides and the bottom, with roo®ng of solid materials to shed precipitation. White-tailed prairie dogs had been introduced previously and had established burrow systems. Pregnant female ferrets whelped in cages (1.1 m2) within these pens, and were allowed access to the pens when kits were about 2 months old. Prairie dogs were added to the pens as needed. At least one kit from each outdoor litter was individually exposed to a live prairie dog at 16.5 weeks of age as part of a study evaluating eciency of killing (Vargas, 1994). 2.2. Marking and radio-collaring Animals produced at Sybille were anaesthetized, given a thorough physical examination, vaccinated, marked with transponder implants (Fagerstone and Johns, 1987) and numerical tattoos in the ear, and radio-tagged (Fagerstone et al., 1985) several days before they were shipped to Shirley Basin. Ferrets from other facilities were handled and radio-tagged after their arrival at the Shirley Basin ®eld site. Thirty-seven ferrets were out®tted (Table 1) with collars consisting of a 6-gram transmitter package mounted with Te¯on heat-shrink tubing to a 1 cm wide wool neckband. The Te¯on coating resists mud buildup and the wool is degradable, with stretch and wear allowing the collar to be shed by the animal in several days to several weeks. 2.3. Release Ferrets were delivered to the 20,596 ha prairie dog complex in Shirley Basin (Fig. 1) between 9 September and 12 October at 16.5±18 weeks, held in release cages for 10 days after their arrival, and released in the ®rst 2 h after sunset. Supplemental food (prairie dog meat) was provided in the release cages as long as evidence of ferret use existed (ferret sightings, scat, disappearing food) and for several days thereafter. Two types of release cages, an aboveground version and a surface/belowground combination, were used. Aboveground cages were 0:9 1:2 m in ¯oor area, were on 0.9 m legs, and had a plastic tunnel extending to 123 ground level for release of ferrets. The second version had a similarly-sized cage of wire mesh at ground level, connected to a belowground nest box that was protected by a sheet metal outer box to deter badgers. Thirty-six animals were released from belowground units, including all 8 radio-tagged PEN animals. All non-Sybille animals (n 24) were released from aboveground cages. Radio-tagged ferrets were released at a cluster of 3 locations centered about 5 km northeast of the 4 locations where non-instrumented ferrets were released. Con¯icting operational objectives prevented the preferred experimental design of intermixing radio-tagged and non-tagged ferrets (for details, see Miller et al., 1996; Biggins et al., 1997). Most release locations within the clusters were separated by about 2 km. Estimated densities of prairie dogs were 7.3 haÿ1 on the ``good'' habitat (that portion of the colony with density of active burrows 40 haÿ1) where the radio-tagged ferrets were released, but only 31% of that colony was rated as ``good'' (Hnilicka and Luce, 1993). The average prairie dog density on the 3 colonies where non-radioed ferrets were released (weighted by the number of ferrets released on each) was 10.2 haÿ1, and 77% of the area was rated as ``good''. 2.4. Radio-tracking Radio-tracking, conducted 3 October±26 November, mostly involved triangulation from ®xed stations, producing positional ®xes by intersecting bearings from 2 stations. The stations were camp trailers ®tted with 11element dual-beam Yagi antennas, situated on hilltops. Accuracy of each station was tested by taking a sample of at least 30 paired bearings, one bearing with the telemetry system and another with a transit, to a beacon transmitter moved in an arc around the station. Dierences between actual and radio bearings were plotted against azimuths in an attempt to identify patterns of error. If bias patterns were obvious, correction formulae were developed to adjust subsequent bearings. A second test sample was used to evaluate the eect of bias correction on the standard deviation of dierences between true and telemetric bearings. If variation was reduced, the bias correction algorithm was added to the computer program used to process bearings. The standard deviation of dierences between pairs of bias-adjusted telemetric and transit bearings was used to produce a con®dence interval to predict the distribution of future bearings. Accuracy of each ®x was indicated by area of the error quadrangle, formed by intersecting 2 stationspeci®c con®dence interval arcs, and lengths of its diagonals (White and Garrot, 1990). Multiple beacons (2±4) were used to reference the stations at least once every 8 h. For stations with identi®able bias patterns, bearings were adjusted for bias 124 D.E. Biggins et al. / Biological Conservation 89 (1999) 121±129 before referencing calculations were made. A laptop computer with TRITEL (a BASIC program by DEB) was used in one of the tracking stations to perform referencing calculations and to process bearings as they were taken. The program assisted technicians in selecting appropriate tracking stations by presenting estimates of accuracy and a simple map of each ®x. Monitoring was conducted in ®xed stations from sunset to sunrise. Mobile stations, trucks with collapsible or permanently erect null-peak yagi antennas, were used to track rapidly-moving or distant animals. Hand-held equipment often was used to ®nd burrows occupied by ferrets, shed collars, and predator-killed or injured ferrets. A ®xed-wing aircraft was used on 2 occasions to locate dispersing animals. Extremes of dispersal (the straight-line distance from release site to most distant ®x during the ®rst 3 days postrelease) and movements (the greatest cumulative movement over any 3 day period) were estimated for each animal. Long cumulative moves and dispersals had mostly negative implications (wasted energy, increased risk of predation, failure to explore a novel area methodically). Although extreme behaviors may be rare, we assumed they would disproportionately in¯uence retention of ferrets on good quality habitat and long-term survival. We agree, therefore, with Gaines and Denny (1993) that ``...many problems of biological interest concern the extremes...'' but we chose to use conventional statistics of central tendency and variation to assess these measures. To ®nd the longest cumulative movement in a 3 day period, we summed distances between consecutive ®xes for 72 h, examining all possible start times. Moves were calculated without regard to inter®x time interval (x 19 min). An animal was assumed to remain at a point until it was found at a new point. We did not assess movements for animals monitored <3 days. The primary objective was to compare groups of animals with dierent prerelease experience. The SOP group of radio-tagged ferrets contained 5 hand-fed or cross-fostered individuals, and represented 3 rearing facilities. We emphasized the comparison between PEN and PD groups because of greater uniformity of treatment within these groups, and because both groups were from Sybille (Table 1). Preliminary comparisons between sexes and types of release cages were done to evaluate the potential for confounding the interpretation regarding rearing groups. To avoid grossly unbalanced designs, subsets of the overall dataset were used in these initial comparisons. Comparisons of males and females were made within the pooled data for PD and SOP groups to avoid confounding by the PEN group, in which 7 of 8 radiotagged individuals were males. The 2 types of release cages were compared within the radio-tagged PD group (10 belowground, 7 aboveground) because all PEN animals were released with the belowground strategy and all radio-tagged SOP animals were released with the aboveground strategy. Ferrets who left the prairie dog colonies where they were released were classi®ed by departure direction, either northeasterly (310 ±360 and 0 ±90 ) or southwesterly (91 ±309 ). Habitat conditions were disparate in these 2 categories. Ferrets departing within the NE arc could encounter 175 ha of prairie dog colonies within 20 km, while ferrets departing in the SW arc had 16 752 ha of prairie dog colonies available after departing the colony of release (Hnilicka and Luce, 1993). Habitat quality of prairie dog colonies at these release sites (Hnilicka and Luce, 1993) was assessed using transects to estimate density of active burrows (Biggins et al., 1993). Preliminary examination of the behavioral datasets revealed heteroscedasticity and skewed distributions. Signi®cance of dierences between groups was assessed with Kruskal±Wallis and Mann±Whitney tests; values presented are the chi-square approximations (H) and associated probabilities (p). 2.5. Spotlighting and capture In the 2-week period 4±18 November, an eort was made to ®nd, capture and identify as many animals as possible. Spotlighting was conducted by a crew of 32 observers, who were coordinated to provide as complete as possible coverage on the prairie dog colonies where reintroduction occurred, and on selected colonies away from the reintroduction site. Trapping and handling (Thorne et al., 1985) was conducted by a few of the most experienced persons. Tattoo identi®cation was facilitated by shining a small ¯ashlight though the tattooed ear while the animal was in the trap. Ferrets were transferred to a cloth sack for transponder identi®cation with an electronic reader. Animals were released from the bag at the locations of capture. Data from spotlighting/recapture consisted of simple counts (within treatment groups) of animals captured, with statistical tests on representation of groups. If an animal was captured more than once during the sampling period, the last date of capture was used to calculate time spent free-ranging. A previous assessment (Biggins et al., 1998) incorporated these recapture data into a multi-site summary, but pooled the SOP and PD categories as variations of cage rearing. Band recovery rate is the probability of capture times the number of animals remaining after losses due to mortality and emigration. Because these components of recovery rate were not separately estimable in this study, we could not examine the relationship between mortality rate and rearing condition using non-telemetric data. Recovery rates, however, gave estimates of D.E. Biggins et al. / Biological Conservation 89 (1999) 121±129 125 minimum survival. For evaluating signi®cance of recapture data, the ``exact'' Chi-square procedure (Berry and Mielke, 1985) was used. Mean numbers of days that each group had been free-ranging when captured was compared with one-way analysis of variance. For evaluation of null hypotheses, we used 0:05. 3. Results 3.1. Radio-telemetry After bias correction, radio-tracking stations produced bearing readings varying 0.29 to 0.91 with 90% con®dence, assuming the best case scenario of nonmodulating signal, line-of-sight, and matched vertical polarity of transmitting and receiving antennas (test conditions). Reception range for ferret transmitters varied with elevation of the station and position of the animal; ranges to 10 km were common from 2 stations located about 500 m above the surrounding terrain, and 1 animal's signal was received from 27 km. Radiotracking of 37 tagged ferrets produced 1224 locational ®xes. Most data were obtained by triangulation, but 7 animals were found during aerial searches. Ferrets were telemetrically monitored an average of 6.3 days each. The mean length of the longest diagonal of estimated error quadrangles (Saltz and Alkon, 1985) for all ®xes was 316 m. Lengths of longest diagonals did not dier signi®cantly between rearing groups (H 1:777, d:f: 1, p 0:411), evidence that variation due to tracking error was distributed equally among groups. Eleven radio-tagged ferrets were known to have died during the monitoring period, including 7 killed by coyotes, 1 killed by a badger, 1 from a diurnal bird of prey, 1 hit by an automobile, and 1 fatally injured by an unknown assailant. The latter animal had head injuries and punctures into the thoracic and abdominal cavities, similar to injuries observed in cases of predation. Eects of type of release cage and sex were not signi®cant for dispersal (cage type, H 0:000, d:f: 1, p 0:000; sex, H 0:277, d:f: 1, p 0:599) or cumulative movements (cage type, H 0:193, d:f: 1, p 0:661; sex, H 1:636, d:f: 1, p 0:201). Thus, data were merged across levels of these categories for all further analyses. Although we did not use parametric statistics for tests regarding movements or dispersal, standard errors were larger for the SOP group than for other groups for those attributes (by factors of 43±175%). The relatively heterogeneous rearing conditions present in the SOP group may have contributed to its large variation, reemphasizing the preferred use of the PD group for comparing cage-reared ferrets with pen-experienced ferrets. Dispersing animals were found in a rectangular area of about 1200 km2 (Fig. 2). Known overall maximum Fig. 2. Release area and last known locations of radio-tagged blackfooted ferrets released at Shirley Basin, Wyoming in 1992. dispersal was >7 km for 19 animals. Eight animals, all without pen experience, dispersed >15 km, and 4 of those dispersed >20 km over the entire study period. Some ferrets began dispersing during the night of release (up to 10 km), and 6 had dispersed >10 km by the third night postrelease (up to 24.7 km). Maximum dispersal during the ®rst 3 days was greater for PD animals than for PEN animals (H 4:900, d:f: 1, p 0:027), but the dierence between dispersal of SOP and PEN animals was not signi®cant (H 2:149, d:f: 1, p 0:143) (Fig. 3). Dispersal distances were associated with direction of dispersal for ferrets departing their colonies of release (H 4:24, d:f: 1, p 0:040); the 10 animals in the NE group dispersed further than the 17 SW animals (Fig. 4). The NE group and the SW group, however, did not dier in maximum cumulative 3 day movements (H 0:06, d:f: 1, p 0:800). The longest cumulative movement in a 3 day period was 49.6 km, made by a radio-tagged male of the PD group who ®rst moved 10 km to the east, then reversed course, travelling during the next two nights to a point 26.5 km northwest of the release site. Another male of the PD group initially moved about 17 km west of the reintroduction site, and was found in pine forest in the Shirley Mountains. He was relocated back to the release site, and was lost to trackers when he had reached a point 23.5 km east of the release site. Six ferrets moved >30 km in 3 days, and 19 ferrets moved more than 7 km in a single night. Seventy-seven percent of the cage-reared (PD and SOP) animals moved >7 km within a 12 h period and 50% sustained that rate for 72 h (>21 km). PEN animals exhibited less extreme 3 day maximum cumulative moves (H 6:80, d:f: 1, p 0:009) than did animals of the 126 D.E. Biggins et al. / Biological Conservation 89 (1999) 121±129 Fig. 3. Maximum postrelease dispersal (distance separating release site and most distance telemetric ®x during the ®rst 3 days postrelease) of radio-tagged black-footed ferrets with 3 rearing histories (medians, ranges, 25th±75th percentiles). The SOP ferrets were raised in standard indoor cages, and had no experience with live prairie dogs. The PD ferrets were cage-reared, and had experience with predation on prairie dogs. The PEN ferrets were born in cages, were allowed access to outdoor pens with prairie dog burrows when kits were about 60 days old, and were fed live prairie dogs. The PEN and PD groups diered signi®cantly (H 4:900, d:f: 1, p 0:027), but the PEN and SOP groups did not (H 2:149, d:f: 1, p 0:143). Fig. 5. Maximum cumulative 72 h movement (over any 3 day period) made by radio-tagged black-footed ferrets with 3 rearing histories (medians, ranges, 25th±75th percentiles). The SOP ferrets were raised in standard indoor cages, and had no experience with live prairie dogs. The PD ferrets were cage-reared, and had experience with predation on prairie dogs. The PEN ferrets were born in cages, were allowed access to outdoor pens with prairie dog burrows when kits were about 60 days old, and were fed live prairie dogs. The PEN and PD groups diered signi®cantly (H 6:80, d:f: 1, p 0:009), but the PEN and SOP groups did not (H 0:893, d:f: 1, p 0:345). been confounded. Using a more conservative comparison of Sybille PD and PEN animals only, the return rate for PEN animals was higher than the return rate for PD animals (X2 8:32, d:f: 1, p 0:007). Nonradioed ferrets, released on better habitat, also can be treated as a separate subset of ferrets, and again the PEN animals had the highest return rate (X2 8:17, d:f: 2, p 0:015). Fig. 4. Maximum postrelease dispersal (distance between release site and most distant telemetric ®x) of radio-tagged ferrets moving (southwesterly) further into the prairie dog complex and (northeasterly) away from the prairie dog complex (medians, ranges, 25th± 25th percentiles). The eect of dispersal direction was signi®cant (H 4:24, d:f: 1, p 0:040). PD group, but cumulative movements of the PEN and SOP groups did not dier signi®cantly (H 0:893, d:f: 1, p 0:345) (Fig. 5). 3.2. Spotlighting and capture The November spotlighting/capture eort detected 14 animals that were released in 1992, 4 of which had been radio-tagged (all had lost their collars). Neither transponders nor ear tattoos were totally reliable for identi®cation of animals; ear tattoos were not legible for some animals, and transponder loss occurred in captivity and postrelease. At least one of the two identi®ers, however, was usable for every animal recaptured. Seven of the 14 animals captured were from the PEN group, the highest band return rate (39% of animals in that group) of the three groups (X2 9:85, d:f: 2, p 0:006). Mean elapsed times from release to recapture were similar (about 1 month) for the three groups (F 1:55; d:f: 2; 11; p 0:25). Because all PEN animals came from Sybille and SOP animals came from 4 facilities, eects of rearing and origin facility could have 4. Discussion Movements of males exceeded movements of females for free-ranging ferrets near Meeteetse, Wyoming (Biggins et al., 1985), contrasting with the similar movements and dispersal of released males and females in this study. Perhaps individuals of both sexes were overwhelmed by release into a strange area, and other sources of variation masked intersexual dierences (Biggins et al., 1998). Over time, patterns of movement unique to each sex would be expected to develop; the duration of this study was too short to gather such information. The longest total movement in one night by a radiotagged native black-footed ferret at Meeteetse, Wyoming slightly exceeded 7 km (Biggins and Fagerstone, 1984). By comparison, 19 released ferrets moved (cumulatively) >7 km in one night and 8 ferrets dispersed >15 km, perhaps re¯ecting release of naive animals into a novel environment. We may have underestimated dispersal for some animals, because dispersing animals sometimes were lost to radiotrackers, and some probably continued to disperse after losing their collars. The geographical distribution of ferrets captured in November is consistent with habitat selection by ferrets. Habitat quality in the immediate vicinity of the release site may have aected the likelihood that an animal would remain there. During the recapture period, 1 of D.E. Biggins et al. / Biological Conservation 89 (1999) 121±129 the 14 ferrets captured was found on the colony where radio-tagged ferrets were released. The individual found on that presumed poorer habitat was a radio-tagged ferret that had been recaptured and translocated back to its initial release site shortly before the recapture eort began; it had initially dispersed about 17 km to the west and, without our intervention, probably would not have been present at the reintroduction site during the survey. The other 3 (formerly) radio-tagged individuals located in the November survey had moved to colonies with higher estimates of burrow density and prairie dog density. Nevertheless, ®nding fewer ferrets on the telemetry area could also be explained by the poorer conditions for spotlighting there; access roads were fewer and of poorer quality, and topographic relief was more pronounced, circumstances expected to decrease detectability of ferrets (Marinari, 1992). Our data suggesting in¯uence of density of active prairie dog burrows and prairie dogs should be regarded as tentative. Recapture data must be interpreted cautiously because surveys were conducted only in the core portion of the reintroduction area. Interpretation of our band recovery rate was complicated by interanimal variation in times of release and recapture. Concern about the eect of time is reduced, however, by having releases of all groups distributed over much of the release period, and by the similarity between groups in mean times that recaptured animals had been free-ranging. The radio-tagged subsample helped explain the 4-fold dierence in recovery rates of PEN and PD animals. PEN ferrets, by making shorter aboveground movements and dispersals, may decrease their probability of encountering aboveground predators (Miller et al., 1992). Dispersing ferrets frequently found themselves away from prairie dog colonies, where food and cover were scarce, and the ferrets expended energy traveling; thus, dispersing ferrets may suer higher mortality from predation and starvation. What factor(s) in the outdoor pen environment could lead to improved adaptation to the wild? There are many possibilities (Biggins et al., 1998), but few experiments have been conducted to isolate their separate in¯uences on ferrets. Ferrets raised outdoors were subjected to oscillations in climatic conditions, and were pre-exposed to soil microorganisms and parasites such as ¯eas and ticks. Perhaps PEN animals were more cautious because of less frequent stimulation by humans or were imprinted on burrows as refugia from having been raised in them. Pen-raised black-footed ferrets may possess improved motor coordination or may be in better physical condition from having more exercise during their rearing. If they were in better physical condition, they should have been capable of longer moves than cage-reared ferrets, making their signi®cantly shorter moves more striking. 127 Ferrets gained hunting experience in pens, and during pre-release trials, cage-raised black-footed ferrets exposed to live prairie dogs (PD group) were signi®cant more pro®cient at killing this prey than SOP animals exposed to live hamsters only (Vargas, 1994). These superior predatory abilities, however, did not translate into a post-release survival advantage for the PD group. Similarly, cage-raised golden lion tamarins trained in food processing and locomotory skills did not dier signi®cantly from untrained counterparts (Beck et al., 1988). 5. Recommendations As many ferret kits as possible should be exposed to quasi-natural environments (in outdoor pens) before release. Monetary costs of this strategy should be evaluated relative to the number of surviving, free-ranging ferrets in short and long time scales. Nevertheless, monetary outlays and counts of surviving ferrets are proximate costs and bene®ts. Ultimate costs of not maximizing survivorship could include irretrievable loss of genetic diversity due to slower expansion of the ferret population, and erosion of survivorship skills due to increasing the generational time spent in captivity by a large proportion of the ferret population. Finally, it seems advisable to avoid releasing ferrets in areas of low prairie dog density, at the edges of colonies, or on small colonies at the edges of larger aggregations of colonies, the cautionary note on reliability of habitat data notwithstanding. Acknowledgements This project involved a great deal of cooperation from diverse groups, and multiple pages would be needed to identify all who contributed. Many individuals of the Wyoming Game and Fish Department contributed to the success of the captive breeding program that led to these reintroductions, and for managing the complex logistics of the reintroductions themselves. Zoos that have volunteered to participate in the captive breeding program are to be commended; these presently include Omaha, Louisville, the National Zoo's Conservation and Research Center, Colorado Springs, Phoenix, and Toronto. We greatly appreciate the assistance of the spotlight-capture crew who came from several states and agencies including the Wyoming Game and Fish Department, the Park Service and Forest Service (South Dakota), the Bureau of Land Management (Wyoming, Colorado and Montana), the Fish and Wildlife Service (Wyoming, Colorado, Montana, and South Dakota), the University of Wyoming and Yale University. We are indebted to the landowners of Shirley Basin who 128 D.E. Biggins et al. / Biological Conservation 89 (1999) 121±129 allowed us access to their ranches. Sincere thanks are due to the Legacy Program of the Department of Defense, Wildlife Preservation Trust International, Chevron USA, and the National Fish and Wildlife Foundation for their contributions that augmented funding from the US Fish and Wildlife Service. We appreciate the critical reviews of John Oldemeyer, Richard Reading, and Roger Powell. References Beck, B.B., 1991. Managing zoo environments for reintroduction. In: American Association of Zoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedings. September 15±19, San Diego, California, pp. 436±440. Beck, B., Castro, I., Kleiman, D., Dietz, J., Rettberg-Beck, B., 1988. Preparing captive-born primates for reintroduction. Int. J. Primatology 8, 426. Beck, B.B., Rappaport, L.G., Stanley Price, M.R., Wilson, A.C., 1994. Reintroduction of captive-born animals. In: Olney, P., Mace, G., Feistner, A. (Eds.), Creative Conservation: Interactive Management of Wild and Captive Animals. Chapman and Hall, London, pp. 265±286. Berry, K.J., Mielke Jr, P.W., 1985. Subroutines for computing exact Chi-square and Fisher's exact probability tests. Ed. and Psych. Meas. 45, 153±159. Biggins, D.E., Fagerstone, K.A., 1984. Movements and activity of black-footed ferrets (Mustela nigripes). American Society of Mammalogists, 64th Annual Meeting, Arcata, California, Abstract. Biggins, D.E., Godbey, J.L., Hanebury, L.R., Luce, B., Marinari, P.E., Matchett, M.R., Vargas, A., 1998. The eect of rearing methods on survival of reintroduced black-footed ferrets. J. Wildl. Manage. 62, 643±653. Biggins, D.E., Hanebury, L.R., Miller, B.J., 1991. Trial release of Siberian polecats (Mustela eversmanni)Ðprogress report, 1 May 1991. Unpublished, Midcontinent Ecol. Sci. Center, Fort Collins, Colorado. Biggins, D.E., Hanebury, L.R., Miller, B.J., Powell, R.A., 1990. Trial release of Siberian polecats (Mustela eversmanni)Ðprogress report, 28 February 1990. Unpublished, Midcontinent Ecol. Sci. Center, Fort Collins, Colorado. Biggins, D.E., Miller, B.J., Hanebury, L.R., Oakleaf, R., Farmer, A.H., Crete, R., Dood, A., 1993. A technique for evaluating blackfooted ferret habitat. In: Oldemeyer, J.L., Biggins, D.E., Miller, B.J. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Symposium on the Management of Prairie Dog Complexes for Reintroduction of the Black-footed Ferret. U.S. Fish and Wildl. Ser. Biol. Rep. 93-1, Washington, DC, pp. 73±88. Biggins, D.E., Miller, B.J., Clark, T.W., Reading, R.P., 1997. Management of an endangered species: the black-footed ferret. In: Mee, G.K., Carroll, C.R. (Eds.), Principles of Conservation Biology, 2nd ed. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, Massachusetts, pp. 420±426. Biggins, D.E., Schroeder, M.H., 1988. Historical and present status of the black-footed ferret. In: Uresk, D.W., Schenbeck, G.L., Cefkin, R. (Eds.), Eighth Great Plains Wildlife Damage Control Workshop Proceedings. USDA For. Ser. Gen. Tech. Rep. RM-154. Fort Collins, Colorado, pp. 93±97. Biggins, D.E., Schroeder, M.H., Forrest, S.C., Richardson, L., 1985. Movements and habitat relationships of radio-tagged black-footed ferrets. In: Anderson, S.H., Inkley, D.B. (Eds.), Black-footed Ferret Workshop Proceedings. Wyoming Game and Fish Dep, Cheyenne, Wyoming, pp. 11.1±11.17. Box, H.O., 1991. Training for life after release: simian primates as examples. In: Gipps, J.H.W. (Ed.), Beyond Captive Breeding: Reintroducing Endangered Mammals to the Wild. Clarendon Press, Oxford, U.K, pp. 111±124. Clark, M.M., Galef, B.G., 1980. Eect of rearing environment on adrenal weights, sexual development, and behavior of gerbils: an examination of Richter's domestication hypothesis. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 94, 857±863. Ellis, D.H., Dobrott, S.J., Goodwin Jr, J.G., 1978. Reintroduction techniques for masked bob-whites. In: Temple, S.A. (Ed.), Endangered Birds. U. Wisconsin Press, Madison, pp. 345±354. Fagerstone, K.A., Biggins, D.E., Campbell III, T.M., 1985. Marking and radio-tagging of black-footed ferrets (Mustela nigripes). In: Anderson, S.H., Inkley, D.B. (Eds.), Black-footed Ferret Workshop Proceedings. Wyoming Game and Fish Dep, Cheyenne, pp. 10.1± 11.10. Fagerstone, K.A., Johns, B.E., 1987. Transponders as permanent identi®cation markers for domestic ferrets, black-footed ferrets, and other wildlife. J. Wildl. Manage. 51, 294±297. Forrest, S.C., Biggins, D.E., Richardson, L., Clark, T.W., Campbell, T.M.III, Fagerstone, K.A., Thorne, E.T., 1988. Population attributes for the black-footed ferret (Mustela nigripes) at Meeteetse, Wyoming, 1981±1985. J. Mammal. 69, 261±273. Gaines, S.D., Denny, M.W., 1993. The largest, smallest, highest, lowest, longest, and shortest: extremes in ecology. Ecology 74, 1677± 1692. Greenough, W.T., Juraska, J.M., 1979. Experience induced changes in brain ®ne structure: Their behavioral implications. In: Hahn, M.E., Jensen, C., Dudek, B.C. (Eds.), Development and Evolution of Brain Size: Behavioral Implications. Academic Press, New York, pp. 263±294. Grith, B., Scott, J.M., Carpenter, J.W., Reed, C., 1989. Translocation as a species conservation tool: Status and strategy. Science 345, 447±480. Hnilicka, P., Luce, B., 1993. Habitat evaluation of Primary Management Zone 1 in the Shirley Basin/Medicine Bow black-footed ferret management area. In: Oakleaf, B., Luce, B., Thorne, E.T., Williams, B. (Eds.), Black-footed Ferret Reintroduction in Shirley Basin, WyomingÐ1992 Annual Completion Report. Unpublished, Wyoming Game and Fish Dept., Cheyenne, pp. 31±38. Kleiman, D.G., 1989. Reintroduction of captive mammals for conservation. Bioscience 39, 152±161. Kleiman, D.G., Beck, B.B., Dietz, J.M., Dietz, L.A., Ballou, J.D., Coimbra-Filho, A.F., 1986. Conservation program for the golden lion tamarin: Captive research and management, ecological studies, educational strategies, and reintroduction. In: Benirschke, K. (Ed.), Primates: The Road to Self-sustaining Populations. Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 959±979. Marinari, P.E., 1992. Detectability of black-footed ferrets (Mustela nigripes) using spotlighting. MS Thesis, U. Wyoming, Laramie. Miller, B., Biggins, D., Hanebury, L., Conway, C., Wemmer, C., 1992. Rehabilitation of a species: The black-footed ferret (Mustela nigripes). In: Ninth Annual Proceedings of the National Wildlife Rehabilitation Association. Chicago, Illinois, pp. 183±192. Miller, B., Biggins, D., Wemmer, C., Powell, R., Hanebury, L., Horn, D., Vargas, A., 1990a. Development of survival skills in captiveraised Siberian polecats (Mustela eversmanni): I. Locating prey. J. Ethol. 8, 89±94. Miller, B., Biggins, D., Wemmer, C., Powell, R., Calvo, L., Hanebury, L., Wharton, T., 1990b. Development of survival skills in captiveraised Siberian polecats (Mustela eversmanni): II. Predator avoidance. J. Ethol. 8, 95±104. Miller, B., Reading, R., Forrest, S., 1996. Prairie night. Smithsonian Inst. Press, Washington, D.C. Phillips, M.K., 1990. Measures of the value and success of a reintroduction project: Red wolf reintroduction project in Alligator River National Wildlife Refuge. Endangered Species Update 8, 24±26. D.E. Biggins et al. / Biological Conservation 89 (1999) 121±129 Renner, M.J., 1988. Learning during exploration: the role of behavioral topography during exploration in determining subsequent adaptive behavior. Int. J. of Comp. Psych. 2, 43±56. Rosenzweig, M.R., 1979. Responsiveness of brain size to individual experience: Behavioral and evolutionary implications. In: Hahn, M.E., Jensen, C., Dudek, B.C. (Eds.), Development and Evolution of Brain Size: Behavioral Implications. Academic Press, New York, pp. 263±294. Saltz, D., Alkon, P.U., 1985. A simple computer-aided method for estimating radio-location error. J. Wildl. Manage. 49, 664±668. Scott, J.P., Fuller, J.L., 1965. Genetics and Social Behavior of the Dog. U. Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois. Soderquist, T.R., Serena, M., 1994. An experimental reintroduction program for bush-tailed phascogales (Phascogale tapoatafa): the interface between captivity and the wild. In: Olney, P., Mace, G., Feistner, A. (Eds.), Creative Conservation: Interactive Management of Wild and Captive Animals. Chapman and Hall, London, pp. 431±438. Stanley-Price, M.R., 1989. Animal reintroductions: The Arabian oryx in Oman. Cambridge U. Press, Cambridge, U.K. Thorne, E.T., Schroeder, M.H., Forrest, S.C., Campbell III, T.M., Richardson, L., Biggins, D.E., Hanebury, L.R., Belitsky, D., Williams, E.S., 1985. Capture, immobilization and care of black- 129 footed ferrets for research. In: Anderson, S.H., Inkley, D.B. (Eds.), Black-footed Ferret Workshop Proceedings. Wyoming Game and Fish Dep, Cheyenne, pp. 9.1±9.8. Vargas, A., 1994. Ontogeny of the black-footed ferret (Mustela nigripes) and eects of captive-upbringing on predatory behavior and post-release survival. Ph.D. Diss., U. Wyoming, Laramie. Vargas, A., Anderson, S.H., 1996. Growth and development of captive-raised black-footed ferrets (Mustela nigripes). Am. Midl. Nat. 135, 43±52. Wemmer, C., Derrickson, S., 1987. Reintroduction: the zoologists dream. In: Annual Proceedings, American Association of Zoological Parks and Aquariums, Wheeling, W. Virginia, pp. 48±65. White, G.C., Garrot, R.A., 1990. Analysis of Wildlife Radio-tracking Data. Academic Press, San Diego, California. Williams, E.S., Thorne, E.T., Kwiatkowski, D.R., Anderson, S.L., Lutz, K., 1991. Reproductive biology and management of captive black-footed ferrets (Mustela nigripes). Zoo Biol. 10, 383± 398. Wilson, A.C., Stanley-Price, M.R., 1994. Reintroduction as a reason for captive breeding. In: Olney, P., Mace, G., Feistner, A. (Eds.), Creative Conservation: Interactive Management of Wild and Captive Animals. Chapman and Hall, London, pp. 243±264.