AN ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION OF Lorina S. Evans for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Human Development and Family Studies on July 10, 1996. Title: Amount of Care Given by Daughters and Perceived Relationship Quality with Care-Receiving Mothers Abstract approved: Redacted for Privacy Alexi J. Walker The relationship between mothers and daughters has been shown to be generally positive and strong throughout the life cycle. The relationship may become strained, however, as mothers become dependent due to deteriorating health or chronic health conditions. It has long been recognized in both the gerontological and intergenerational literatures that caring for a dependent elderly parent can be burdensome and stressful for some caregiving adult daughters. Using the elaborated wear-and-tear hypothesis, this study examined longitudinally the impact of the amount of care given on 140 caregiving daughters' perception of the quality of their relationship with their elderly dependent mothers. It was hypothesized that increases in the amount of care given by caregiving daughters over time would lead to a corresponding decrease in relationship quality. It was also hypothesized that relationship quality and amount of care given would be stable over time. Results indicated that providing more care does decrease relationship quality between caregiving daughters and care-receiving mothers over time although it does not do so initially. Further, the results of study showed that relationship quality and amount of care given, on average, are stable over time. Amount of Care Given by Daughters and Perceived Relationship Quality with Care-Receiving Mothers By Lorina S. Evans A DISSERTATION Submitted to Oregon State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Completed July 10, 1996 Commencement June 1997 Doctor of Philosophy dissertation of Lorina S. Evans presented on July 10, 1996 APPROVED: Redacted for Privacy Major Profes or, representing Human Development and Family Studies Redacted for Privacy Head of Department o Human Development and Family Sciences Redacted for Privacy Dean of Gr uate School I understand that my thesis will become part of the permanent collection of Oregon State University libraries. My signature below authorizes release of my thesis to any reader upon request. Redacted for Privacy Lorina S. Evans, Author ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to express my sincere appreciation to my major advisor, Dr. Alexis J. Walker, for her valuable time, advice, and guidance in this project. I also wish to thank my other committee members, Dr. Alan Acock, Dr. Mina Carson, Dr. Brooke Collison, Dr. Patricia Moran, and Dr. Anisa Zvonkovic for their encouragement and their support in bringing this project to completion. I am also indebted to both Dr. Alan Acock and Dr. Fuzhong Li for their assistance and guidance in the editorial process of the statistical analyses section. Additionally, I would like to give a special thank you to my friend Shari Sakashita, for her support over a difficult journey, encouraging me to pursue my dreams, and to hang on when things were tough. Lastly, I wish to express my gratitude to my parents and friends for their patience and support along this journey. This research was supported by grant R01-AG-06766 from the National Institute on Aging. TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter Chapter One: Introduction Page 1 Purpose of the Study 4 Definition of Terms 6 Chapter Two: Literature Review 7 Wear-and-Tear Hypothesis 7 Research on the Wear-and-Tear Hypothesis 8 Overview of Elderly Caregiving 11 Outcomes for Caregiving Daughters 13 Intergenerational Relationships 14 Elderly Caregiving and Mother-Daughter Relationships 16 Impact of Amount of Care Given and Relationship Quality 17 Covariates 18 Modeling of Change in Relationship Quality 20 Control Variables 20 Hypotheses 23 TABLE OF CONTENTS (Continued) Chapter Page Chapter Three: Method 24 Sample 24 Attrition 28 Procedures 30 Measures 31 Care given Perceived relationship quality Control variables 31 32 33 Data Analysis 34 Missing Data 36 Chapter Four: Results 38 Preliminary Data Analyses 38 Structural Equation Model 40 Measurement Model 41 Structural Model 45 Measurement Invariance 48 Loadings on the latent variables Path coefficients Effects of the exogenous variables 49 51 53 TABLE OF CONTENTS (Continued) Chapter Page Chapter Five: Discussion and Conclusion Primary Findings Stability of the endogenous latent variables Relationship between amount of care given and relationship quality Relationship between covariates and endogenous latent variables 55 56 56 58 60 Limitations and Strengths 62 Limitations Strengths 62 63 Implications 64 Directions for Future Research 64 Bibliography 66 Appendices 80 Appendix A: Caregiving Tasks and Activities Appendix B: Intimacy Scale Appendix C: Attachment Scale 81 84 85 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1 Conceptual Model 2 Structural and Measurement Model 22 3 Measurement Model of Latent Constructs 42 5 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1 Selected Sociodemographic Characteristics of Caregiving Daughters 26 2 Comparisons of Daughters in the Sample with Those Lost to Attrition 29 3 Correlations Among Observed Indicators and Exogenous Variables 39 4 Loadings of Measurement Model 44 5a Goodness-of-Fit Indices of Structural Models 46 5b Model Comparisons 47 6 Loadings for Structural Model 4 50 7 Parameter Estimates of the Hypothesized Model (Beta) 52 8 Parameter Estimates of the Exogenous Variables to Latent Variables 54 AMOUNT OF CARE GIVEN BY DAUGHTERS AND PERCEIVED RELATIONSHIP QUALITY WITH CARE-RECEIVING MOTHERS Chapter One Introduction Most elderly persons continue to be in good health despite advancing age. Some become dependent due to deteriorating health or chronic conditions, however. Kunkel and Applebaum (1992) projected that by the year 2020, 4.9 million elderly persons aged 65 and over will experience some type of chronic problems. When chronic conditions limit daily activities, the need for assistance is usually provided by a family member. Caregiving to impaired elderly persons usually begins when they are unable to perform such tasks as housekeeping, shopping, or meal preparation without some assistance (Brody, 1985; Brody & Schoonover, 1986; Cicirelli, 1981; Lang & Brody, 1983; Stephens & Christianson, 1986; Stone, Cafferata, & Sang!, 1987; Walker, Pratt, & Eddy, 1995). Spouses usually provide care to their dependent partner. In the absence of a spouse, adult children, particularly adult daughters, assume the responsibility of providing care needed by their aging parent(s) (Brody, 1986; Cicirelli, 1983; Mancini & Blieszner, 1989; Shanas, 1979; Stoller, 1983). 2 Providing care to an impaired parent has been shown to be costly, particularly for adult daughters. Caregivers often give up their personal and social activities (Abel, 1989) and are confined to their homes (Robinson & Thurnher, 1979). Additionally, caregivers report experiencing emotional costs such as frustration, resentment, and feeling overwhelmed (e.g., Archbold, 1983; Cicirelli, 1983; Robinson & Thurnher, 1979). Increasingly, it appears that the amount of care given is a critical factor in caregiving costs. That is, those who give more care, have more costs (Brody, 1985; Walker, Acock, Bowman, & Li, 1996). Indeed, the amount of care given is far more important in predicting the outcomes of caregiving than is caregiving duration. Despite the myth that aging parents are socially isolated and abandoned by their adult children, researchers have shown that both continue to interact and depend on each other throughout life (Brody, Johnsen, Fulcomer, & Lang, 1984; Cicirelli, 1983; Rossi & Rossi, 1990; Shanas, 1979). On average, adult children have frequent contact with their parents, live within a 30-mile radius of them, and provide emotional and instrumental assistance as they grow older (Shanas, 1979). As aging parents receive emotional support and assistance from their adult children in tasks they no longer are able to perform independently, aging parents may continue to provide monetary support as well as love and affection. Mother-daughter relationships in middle and later life have been shown to be strong and enduring ties, characterized by affection and by the exchange of aid (Bretherton, Biringen, & Ridgeway, 1991; Boyd, 1989; 3 Bromberg, 1982-1983; Cicirelli, 1983, 1991; Himes, 1994; Lawton, Silverstein, & Bengtson, 1994; Mancini & Blieszner, 1989; Rossi & Rossi, 1990; Stueve & O'Donnell, 1984). Relationships between mothers and daughters in middle and later life have also been shown to be positive and rewarding to both parties (Baruch & Barnett, 1983; Barnett, Baruch, Kibria, & Pleck, 1991). These relationships may be strained however, by experiences such as daughter's marital dissolution or even by the experience of caregiving (Johnson & Catalano, 1983; Umberson 1989a, 1989b, 1992). Caregiving has produced both positive and negative consequences for mothers and daughters. Some researchers have found that adult daughters generally have a good relationship with their parent(s) despite the strain and burden that caregiving often brings (e.g., Abel, 1986; Miller, 1989; Walker, Shin, & Bird, 1990). Other researchers report opposing results. Scharlach (1987), for example, found that adult daughters reported a deterioration in their relationship with their parent(s) as the need for care increased. Overall, little research has examined the potential detrimental effect on caregiving of elderly mother-adult daughter relationships. Furthermore, only a handful of longitudinal studies have examined the impact of caregiving (e.g., Johnson & Catalano, 1983; Townsend, Noelker, Deimling, & Bass, 1989). Most studies have been cross-sectional in nature and have looked at adult children caring for elderly parents with Alzhiemer's Disease or other types of dementia (e.g., Pratt, Schmall, Wright, & Cleland, 1985; Pruchno & Resch, 1989; Reed, Stone, & Nee le, 1990). Research to understand the impact of caregiving on 4 relationships between caregivers and care receivers is needed, particularly longitudinal research. Purpose of the Study The purpose of the study was to examine the impact of the amount of care given on daughters' perception of their relationship with their carereceiving mothers. The study investigated longitudinally the effect of the amount of care given over time on the perceived quality of the relationship between caregiving adult daughters and their mothers, thus filling in a gap in the existing literature (See Figure 1). The study builds on findings that the amount of care given and not duration is the important factor in determining caregiving outcomes. 5 Figure 1. Conceptual Model 6 Definition of Terms Caregivers. Caregivers refers to individuals, such as spouses and adult children, who assist dependent elderly individuals with activities of daily living (ADL) (Stone et al., 1987). Care recipients. Care recipients refers to elderly individuals who need assistance with activities of daily living (ADL) (Stone et al., 1987). 7 Chapter Two Literature Review Wear-and-Tear Hypothesis The wear-and-tear hypothesis, as applied to caregiving, proposes that as duration of caregiving increases, caregivers will experience increasing strain and burden (e.g., Townsend, Noelker, Deimling, & Bass, 1989). The wear-and-tear hypothesis is derived from social exchange theory. Social exchange theory has emerged as a major theoretical framework by which to explain social interactional processes in dyadic and small-group relationships (McDonald, 1981). The basic premise of social exchange theory is that individuals involved in social interaction attempt to maximize rewards and minimize costs to obtain the most profitable outcome (Thibaut & Kelley, 1959). That is, individuals will seek what is most beneficial to them at the lowest cost. Costs are defined as both unpleasant experiences and rewards foregone. Rewards are defined as experiences that lead to some type of gratification such as relationship satisfaction (Thibaut & Kelley, 1959). Relationship changes in later life as a consequence of care given by adult children to their aging parents can be explained by social exchange theory. Thibaut and Kelley (1959) posit that in order for a relationship to be satisfying, both members of a dyad must find interaction more rewarding than costly. When 8 one or both individuals is unable to reciprocate, an imbalance may be created that is costly to individuals in the relationship. Dowd (1975) applied social exchange theory to aging suggesting that as individuals age, their resources are diminished, thus creating an imbalance in the exchange of aid. In other words, as aging parents become more dependent on their adult children, their level of independence diminishes. This decline may result in their feeling helpless and useless. For caregiving adult children, the increasing amount of care given coupled with declines in aid received may lead to burden, strain, and relationship deterioration (Gerstel & Gallagher, 1993; Miller, McFall, & Montgomery, 1991; Montgomery, Gonyea, & Hooyman, 1985; Stoller, 1983; Young & Kahana, 1985). Research on the Wear-and-Tear Hypothesis According to the wear-and-tear hypothesis, caregivers experience increasing burden and stress as duration of caregiving increases. Research on caregiving generally has focused on negative outcomes for caregivers (Archbold, 1983; Baruch & Speid, 1989; Cicirelli, 1983; Montgomery & Kamo, 1989). Mathews (1988), Stull, Bowman, and Smerglia (1994), and Zarit, Reeves, and Bach-Peterson (1989) suggested, however, that the majority of caregivers report low levels of strain, depression, and burden, thus questioning the validity of the wear-and-tear hypothesis. Stephens and Zarit (1989) proposed an adaptation 9 hypothesis in which caregivers adjust to the caregiving situation over time despite stressors (see Pear lin, Mullen, Semple, & Skaff, 1990). In other words, stress should level off as caregivers adapt and adjust to meeting the elderly person's needs. Only a few studies have looked at the wear-and-tear hypothesis longitudinally. Johnson and Catalano (1983) followed 115 elderly individuals recently discharged from the hospital and their caregivers over an 8-month period. They found that on average, caregivers reported experiencing stress and burden as the amount of care given increased. They also found that the caregiver-care recipient relationship deteriorated due to the increased care provided. Townsend, Noelker, Deimling, and Bass (1989) examined longitudinally the impact of caregiving to impaired elderly parents on adult children's mental health. They found substantial individual variability in adult children's level of stress and depression. For example, 36% (n = 40) of the adult child caregivers reported increased stress while 52% n = 57) reported decreased stress over time. Approximately 12% (n = 13) reported no change in their stress levels and 8% (n = 9) of the caregivers reported no change in depression. Depression decreased for 58% (n = 65) of the caregivers but increased for 34% (n = 38) The overall changes were small, however. More importantly, they . found that duration of caregiving did not influence adult children's mental health. Longitudinal studies of caregiving for persons with Alzheimer's Disease or dementia have produced mixed results (Haley & Pardo, 1989; Schulz & Williamson, 1991; Zarit, Todd, & Zarit, 1986). Zarit et al. (1986) followed 10 caregivers of family members with dementia for a 2-year period. They found that women (wives) initially reported being more burdened than men (husbands) at Time 1. At Time 2, women were able to adapt and adjust to the caregiving situation despite the progression of dementia. Schulz and Williamson (1991) also found considerable variability in depression scores for men and women giving care to persons with Alzheimer's Disease over a 2-year period. For example, only 27.3% (n = 6) of the men reported being depressed over the 2-year period. Approximately 47% (n = 24) of the women (both wives and daughters) reported higher levels of depression over time, thus, supporting the wear-and-tear hypothesis. Walker, Acock, Bowman, and Li (1996) extended the wear-and-tear hypothesis by explaining individual variability in the adaptation to caregiving over time. The researchers examined the impact of change on the amount of care given by 130 White caregiving daughters over a 4-year period. They found that on average, adult daughters gave increasing amounts of care over the 4-year period, and they reported high caregiving satisfaction. They also found that, as the amount of care increased over time, caregiving satisfaction decreased even though daughters' caregiving satisfaction scores were high. Additionally, they found that duration of caregiving (wear-and-tear hypothesis) was unrelated to change in daughters' caregiving satisfaction. The small decrease in caregiving satisfaction over the 4-year period was related to the increasing amount of care given, rather than duration or time. It appears then, that the wear-and-tear hypothesis must be modified to include change in the amount of care. The 11 increased amount of care that caregivers provide over time appears to be related to caregiving stress and burden. Therefore, researchers need to focus more on the amount of care given than on duration of caregiving to explain caregiving outcomes. Overview of Elderly Careaivinq Persons aged 65 and over comprise approximately 13% of the population (Harris, 1990). Increased life expectancy, the advancement of life-saving medical technology, low fertility rates, and low mortality rates have contributed to the increased need for elderly caregiving (Kunkel & Applebaum, 1992; Mancini & Blieszner, 1989; Treas, 1977). Although most elderly persons continue to be in good health and are selfsufficient, nearly 10% of those aged 65 and older, and one quarter of those over age 84 become dependent due to deteriorating health or chronic conditions such as arthritis, loss of hearing, and vision impairment (Nimes, 1994). Under such conditions, family members provide assistance to the elderly (Archbold, 1983; Brody, 1986; Cicirelli, 1983; Nimes, 1994; Lee, 1987; Mancini & Blieszner, 1989; Stoller, 1983). Shanas (1979) noted that spouses usually provide care for the dependent elderly who are married. In the absence of a spouse, however, adult children typically provide care to their aging parent(s). Increased longevity 12 means that such care is provided for longer periods of time (Brody, 1985; Shanas, 1979; Stoller, 1983). Stone, Cafferata, and Sang! (1987) provided a national profile of caregivers and care recipients using the 1982 National Long-Term Care Survey (also known as the channeling demonstration) and the matched Informal Caregivers Survey. These surveys were the first databases where national estimates of family members, friends, and other unpaid caregivers to the uninstitutionalized elderly were provided. Stone et al. (1987) found that the majority (72%) of caregivers are women, with adult daughters comprising 29% of those caring for an impaired aging person. Wives comprised 23% of this population. The average age for caregiving wives was 69 years with 48% between the ages 65 to 74 years. The average age for caregiving adult daughters was 52 years with 63% between the ages 45 to 64 years. Women caregivers provide assistance in personal care, medical care, and other in-home care (e.g., meal preparation, shopping, transportation). Stone et al. (1987) found that 60% of the care recipients are women and 51% are married. The mean age for care recipients was 78 years with 78% aged 65 and above. Approximately 40% lived with a spouse, 36% lived with a spouse and/or children, and 11% lived alone. Approximately 23% of the care recipients reported being in good health despite increasing chronic conditions. Elderly care recipients reported needing help performing activities of daily living (ADL), such as bathing, dressing, toileting, getting in and out of bed or chairs, and eating, and instrumental activities of daily living (IADL), such as shopping, cleaning house, 13 and laundry. Stone et al.'s (1987) study substantiated earlier findings that women are actively involved as caregivers and as care receivers (Archbold, 1983; Brody, 1985; Cicirelli, 1981; Lang & Brody, 1983; Stoller, 1983). Outcomes for Caregiving Daughters The impact of elderly caregiving on adult daughters continues to be a major focus in gerontological research (Abel, 1986; Aronson, 1992; Brody, Litvin, Albert, & Hoffman, 1994; Cicirelli, 1981; Dwyer & Coward, 1991; Dwyer & Seccombe, 1991; Nimes, 1994; Horowitz, 1985; Mancini & Blieszner, 1989; Stoller, 1983; Walker et al., 1992). With caregiving to their aging parent(s), adult daughters report being distressed and burdened (Barusch & Spaid, 1989; Gerstel & Gallagher, 1993; Montgomery et al., 1985; Montgomery & Kamo, 1989; Young & Kahana, 1986), depressed (Archbold, 1983; Pruchno & Resch, 1989), frustrated (Circirelli, 1983; Motenko, 1989) and having a restricted outlook for the future (Rakowski & Clark, 1985). Not all outcomes are negative, however. Abel (1986) suggested that the mother-daughter relationship can be strengthened as a consequence of caregiving. Archbold (1983) found that caregivers reported being satisfied when they managed rather than gave care to their dependent elderly parent(s). She also found that caregivers learned much about themselves and about aging in the caregiving process, and reported that their relationship with their dependent elderly parent(s) improved as a result of 14 caregiving. Walker, Martin, and Jones (1992) found that daughters who have a good relationship with their dependent elderly mothers reported less caregiving costs (i.e., insufficient time, frustration, anxiety). Intergenerational Relationships Most intergenerational relationships in later life are continuous in that these relationships are shaped by past intimate experiences, histories, patterns, and behaviors. Research has indicated that family members live in close proximity to each other, maintain contact, share important emotional ties, and exchange aid (Cicirelli, 1983; DeWitt, blister, & Burch, 1988; Frankel & DeWitt, 1988; Lee, 1988; Lee & Ellithorpe, 1982; Mancini & Blieszner, 1989; Stoller, 1985; Walker & Pratt, 1991; Walker, Pratt, & Oppy, 1992). For example, parents and adult children provide all types of services (e.g., babysitting, major help with housework, financial assistance) to each other throughout life (Hogan, Eggebeen, & Clogg, 1993; Wellman & Wortley, 1990). Barnett, Baruch, Kibria, and Pleck (1991) studied the effect of relationship quality on mental health (i.e., subjective well-being and psychological distress) in 403 adult daughters. They found that adult daughters generally reported having positive relationships with their aging parent(s). They also found that relationship quality was positively related to daughters' well-being. In other words, daughters who reported having 15 a good relationship with their aging parent(s) had higher psychological wellbeing. In addition to examining intergenerational solidarity, such as type and frequency of interactions and activities, and affectional sentiments (see Harootyan & Bengtson, 1994; Bengtson & Roberts, 1991 for a review), researchers have focused their attention on the effect of early family relationships in later life (Rossi & Rossi, 1990; Whitbeck, Simons, and Conger, 1991). For example, Rossi and Rossi (1990) examined parent-child relationships across the life course. They found that parents who were affectionate, accessible, and had a close relationship with their children in adolescence were more intimate with their adult children later in the life course. They also found that early family cohesiveness influenced intimacy and closeness between adult children and their parents in later life. Whitbeck, Simons, and Conger (1991) also examined the effects of early parent-child relationships on current relationships. They found that adult children's perceptions of early relationships with their elderly parents did impact their relationships with them in later life. Adult children who perceived their parents as rejecting and hostile and as harsh disciplinarians early in life reported having poorer relationships with them currently. They also found depression to be negatively related to current relationships. Their study substantiates Rossi and Rossi's (1990) findings that parents who had poor relationships with their children earlier in life were more likely to have poor relationships with them in later life. 16 Elderly Caregiving and Mother-Daughter Relationships The relationships between elderly parents and adult daughters in later life have been studied in both the gerontological and intergenerational literatures (Abel, 1989; Barer & Johnson, 1990; Brody & Schoonover, 1986; Cicirelli, 1983; Nimes, 1994; Lang & Brody, 1981; Sheehan & Nuttall, 1988; Walker & Allen, 1991). Research has shown that on average, adult daughters live near their elderly parents, have frequent contact with them, and generally view their relationships as positive (Abel, 1986; Bengtson, Cutler, Mangen, & Marshall, 1985; Lawton, Silverstein, & Bengtson, 1994; Mancini & Blieszner, 1989; Silverstein, Lawton, & Bengtson, 1994). Baruch and Barnett (1983) found that adult daughters perceived their relationship with their mothers as rewarding and positive. Walker, Shin, and Bird (1990) studied perceived relationship changes as a consequence of caregiving in 133 elderly mother-adult daughter pairs. The researchers found that 50.4% of daughters reported a positive impact of caregiving on their relationship, but 44.2% reported no change. Additionally, daughters who reported a positive impact or no change in their relationship with their mothers were satisfied with caregiving. On the negative side, Boyd (1987) and Walker and Allen (1991) found that cargiving adult daughters who were in conflict with their elderly mothers reported their relationship as unrewarding. Additionally, caregiving daughters experienced greater strain or costs (e.g., time constraints, burden) when their relationships with their mothers were poor (Scharlach, 1987; Sheehan & Nuttall, 17 1988). Very few researchers, however, have examined the impact of caregiving on relationship quality. Impact of Amount of Care Given and Relationship Quality In order to understand factors that contribute to relationship quality, researchers have measured this latent construct as intimacy (Walker & Thompson, 1983), attachment (Cicirelli, 1983; Thompson & Walker, 1984), and affection (Jarrett, 1985; Lawton, Silverstein, & Bengtson, 1994). Although adult children and in particular, adult daughters, do not have to have strong emotional ties to provide care to an elderly parent(s), some research has shown that caregiving brings the mother-daughter relationship closer (Abel, 1989; Baruch & Barnett, 1983; Cicirelli, 1983; Lang & Brody, 1983; Walker, Acock, Bowman, & Li, 1996). For example, Cicirelli (1983) examined 148 adult children's helping behavior to their elderly parents. The researcher found that adult children provided more care to their elderly parent when they felt a strong attachment. Lawton, Silverstein, and Bengtson (1994) also found that affectional ties bring the mother-daughter relationship closer. Other research has shown that relationship deterioration and strain are consequences of care given (Scharlach, 1987; Sheehan & Nuttall, 1988). Mancini and Blieszner (1989) and Walker, Pratt, Shin, and Jones (1990) noted that more research is needed on the impact of care given on the quality of mother-daughter relationships in later life. It is 18 noteworthy that there is little research on relationship quality as it applies to mother-daughter relationships in middle and later life. Covariates A number of variables may influence the amount of care given. It is important to control for these influences in any examination of the effect of the amount of care given on relationship quality. Age. Age is often used as a demographic variable in many gerontological studies because it is often assumed that individuals change in predictable patterns as they age (e.g., Kart, 1985; Markides & Cooper, 1989). Age is multidimensional, however, and encompasses biological, psychological, and chronological age. Most research on caregiving has shown that a person's age is related to care received (see Cicirelli, 1981; Hareven, 1995; Kinsella, 1995). As individuals live longer, they face an increased likelihood of becoming dependent and of requiring care over time. Age, however, has not been found to be associated with relationship quality (Walker & Allen, 1991). Health. The health of an elderly parent is associated with the amount of care given. Some researchers have found that elderly parents' deteriorating health is related to an increase in helping behaviors by caregiving daughters (Abel, 1986; Archbold, 1983; Lang & Brody, 1983; Markides & Cooper, 1989; 19 Walker et al., 1992). Walker, Shin, and Bird (1990), however, found no association between mother's health and intimacy. Coresidence. The majority of elderly individuals do not live alone. Of those aged 65 and older, 67% live with a family member, predominately a spouse. Approximately 13%, most of them widowed, live with other family members, such as an adult daughter (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1994). When adult children and their elderly parents live together, they so do for a variety of reasons, such as high housing costs, unemployment, and divorce (Aquilino, 1990, Aquilino & Supple, 1991; Ward & Spitze, 1992). Chappell (1991), Lang and Brody (1983), and Walker and Pratt (1991) found that coresidence with adult children is associated with higher rates of assistance from them. Regarding the association between coresidence and relationship quality, Aquilino and Supple (1991) found that coresidential parents generally reported having as positive a relationship with their adult children as noncoresidential parents. Past Relationship Quality. As stated previously, past relationship history influences current relationships. Research has shown that parents who had poor relationships earlier in life are likely to have poor relationships in the present (Rossi & Rossi, 1990; Whitbeck et al., 1991; Whitbeck et al., 1994). Whitbeck et al. (1991) found that adult children's past or current relationship with their parents was not related to giving assistance to their parents. 20 Modeling of Change in Relationship Quality Past research has suggested either stable levels of perceived relationship quality (Baruch & Barnett, 1983; Lang & Brody, 1983) or decreasing levels of perceived relationship quality (Johnson & Catalano, 1983; Scharlach, 1987; Sheehan & Nuttall, 1988) with caregiving. Walker et al. (1996) found that increased amount of care given is associated with negative caregiving outcomes; that is, the more care given, the poorer the outcomes. Structural equation modeling is used to examine the impact of the amount of care given on perceived relationship quality over a two-year period (see Figure 2). In accordance with the mothers' chronic rather than acute conditions, it was predicted that on average, amount of care given would remain stable over time. Perceived relationship quality too, would remain stable over time, on average. In accordance with the elaborated wear-and-tear hypothesis, it was predicted that caregiving daughters would report a deterioration in relationship quality when the amount of care given increases over time. Control Variables Age and deteriorating health are associated with the amount of care given by caregiving adult daughters (e.g., Lang & Brody, 1983); that is, older persons and persons in poorer health receive more care. In addition, Chappell (1989) and 21 Walker et al. (1996) found that caregivers give more care to care recipients when they coreside. Furthermore, age, health, and coresidence have been found to be correlated. Therefore, these variables were controlled in the model. Because Whitbeck et al. (1991, 1994) and others (e.g., Caspi & Elder, 1988; Rossi & Rossi, 1990) found an association between past and present relationship quality, it also was important to control for prior relationship quality in examining the impact of amount of care given on relationship quality. Relationship Quality (Wave 1) 12 Figure 3. Measurement Model of Latent Constructs 23 Hypotheses Drawing specifically on the elaborated wear-and-tear hypothesis and findings from the literature, the following hypotheses were derived: 1. On average, amount of care given will remain stable over time. 2. On average, relationship quality will remain stable over time. 3. The initial amount of care given in Wave 1 will be negatively related to the initial level of relationship quality in Wave 1; that is, respondents who initially give higher levels of care will have lower initial levels of relationship quality. 4. For any increase in amount of care over time, there will be a corresponding decrease in relationship quality. 5. Mothers' age will be positively associated with the initial amount of care given. 6. Daughters' perception of mothers' health will be negatively related to the initial amount of care given; that is, the better the perceived health, the less care given. 7. Daughters who reside with their mothers will give more care than daughters who live separately from their mothers. 8. Daughters' perceived past relationship quality with their mothers will be positively related to their perceptions of relationship quality at Wave 1. 24 CHAPTER THREE Method Sample The sample consists of 140 (136 White, 2 African American, 1 American Indian, 1 Other) caregiving daughters, who, with their mothers, participated in a two-year longitudinal study on mother-daughter relationships in middle and later life between 1986 and 1989 (see Walker et al., 1996). This sample was drawn from a larger sample of 173 elderly mother-caregiving adult daughter pairs from Western Oregon. Participants were recruited primarily through local newspaper articles describing a study of mother-daughter relationships wherein caregiving sometimes occurred. To participate in the study, the following criteria had to be met: (a) mothers were aged 65 and older and unmarried (i.e., widowed, divorced, or deserted); (b) mothers had no cognitive impairment as reported by daughters or by project interviewers; (c) daughters lived with or within 45 miles of their mothers; (d) dependent mothers needed assistance in one or more of the following areas: transportation, housekeeping, meal preparation, laundry, personal care, or financial aid; and (e) daughters reported providing more than half of the assistance needed by their dependent mothers. The mean age of caregiving daughters was 52.7 years (Mdn = 53.0 years, SD = 10.3) with ages ranging from 29 to 73 years at Wave 1. The majority (62.9%) were married (n = 88) although 21.4% (n = 30) were divorced. The 25 average length of marriage was 21.7 years (Mdn = 19.0 years, SD = 15.2). Approximately 24.3% completed high school while the majority (75.7%) had some college. The mean years of education was 14.1 (Med. = 14.0 years, SD = 2.2). (See Table 1). The median annual family income was $28,000 (M = $30,181, SD = $20,458) at Wave 1 and 59.3% (n = 83) reported being employed. Caregiving daughters reported giving care to their mothers for a median of 5 years (M = 7.3 years, SD = 7.1) with a range from 0 months to 51 years. The estimate of 0 months came from daughters who did not describe their activities as caregiving, even though they met the study criteria. The estimate of 51 years of caregiving came from a daughter who felt she had been taking care of her mother since her father had abandoned the family years earlier. Regarding living arrangements, 80% (n = 112) of the caregiving daughters did not live with their mothers while 20% (n = 28) did. Mothers' median age was 84 years (M = 81.5, SD = 8.8) and ranged from 64 years to 101 years. Daughters reported that their mothers had, on average, 4.1 (Mdn = 4.0, SD = 1.8) of 14 health problems common among the elderly. The four most common health problems experienced by their mothers, as reported by caregiving daughters, were arthritis (n = 94, 67.1%), loss of vision (n = 90, 64.3%), loss of hearing (n = 55.0%), and high blood pressure (n = 53, 37.9%). Despite the chronic health problems, daughters perceived their mothers to be in good health (M = 2.8, SD = .9). 26 Table 1: Selected Sociodemooraphic Characteristics of Caregiving Daughters Characteristic n % 18 13.0 23.7 34.5 23.7 5.0 Age 29-39 40-49 50-59 60-69 70-79 33 48 33 7 Marital Status Never married Married 52 88 37.1 8 5.7 24.3 32.9 25.7 11.4 62.9 Education < High school High school Some college 4 years college > 4 years college 34 46 36 16 Income < $10,000 $10,000-$19,999 $20,000-$29,999 $30,000-$39,999 $40,000 and above 15 32 36 24 29 11.0 23.5 26.5 17.7 21.3 57 83 40.7 59.3 Employment Status Not employed Employed 27 Table 1 (Continued) Characteristic n ok Coresidence Lived with mother Other 28 112 20.0 80.0 44 31.4 22.1 12.9 21.4 Years in marital status 0-10 11-20 21-30 31-40 > 41 31 18 30 17 12.1 Parental Status Mothers No children 12 91.4 8.6 106 75.7 127 Children Under 18 0 1 2 3 4 5 17 10 5 1 1 Note. Cases with missing data are excluded. 12.1 7.1 3.6 .7 .7 28 Attrition Of the original 173 mother-daughter pairs, 33 daughters were lost to the study over two years resulting in a total sample of 140. The 33 who dropped out of the study did so for the following reasons: (a) mother's death ( = 19, 57.6%), (b) lack of interest (n = 9, 27.3%), (c) mother's physical illness (n = 3, 6.1%), daughter's cognitive decline (n = 1, 3.0%) and daughter's physical illness (n = 1, 3.0%). To determine if differences existed between these women and those who remained in the sample over the two waves, I compared them on the following Wave 1 measures: marital status, education, income, employment status, number of children under age 18, and all the variables in the model. As shown in Table 2, the two groups did not differ significantly on marital status (married or not married), education, employment status (employed or not employed), number of children under age 18, mothers' age, coresidence, past relationship quality, or on the indicators that make up the relationship quality construct (i.e., closeness, good relationship quality, attachment, and intimacy). Daughters' perceptions of their mother's health, however, did differ between the two groups such that daughters who dropped out of the study reported their mothers being in poorer health, x2 (3, n = 173) = 17.97, p. = .001. The two groups also differed in the amount of care given as measured by their IADL and ADL scores. Daughters who were lost to the study gave more IADL and ADL care to their mothers than those who remained. Additionally, daughters who stayed in the study had more income than those who left the study (See Table 2). 29 Table 2: Comparisons of Daughters in the Sample with Those Lost to Attrition Study Sample Variable Respondents lost due to attrition M or % SD or n M or % SD or n Daughters' age 52.68 11.17 51.30 10.26 .50 Mothers' age 81.48 8.79 79.69 1.55 .24 Education 14.11 2.28 13.42 2.22 .11 % Married 62.9 140 45.5 33 .37 Years married 21.74 15.22 22.24 14.40 .86 $30.18 $20.46 $22.26 $12.98 .01 33 .87 Income in thousands % Employed No. of Children < 18 59.3 140 63.6 2 .44 .92 .30 .64 .87 IADL help 2.17 .71 2.60 .78 .003 ADL help 1.40 .72 1.88 1.07 .02 Attachment 3.27 .70 3.25 .87 .91 Intimacy 4.04 .67 3.92 .83 .40 Daughters' perception of mothers' health 2.82 .88 2.09 1.01 Closeness 3.46 .60 3.42 .66 .78 Good relationship quality 3.49 .62 3.25 .80 .13 Past relationship quality 3.12 .76 3.18 .85 .71 .001 Note. n = 140 in the study sample; n = 33 respondents lost to attrition. 30 Procedures The data were drawn from a preexisting data set in which data were collected during face-to-face interviews with daughters conducted primarily in their homes by trained graduate research assistants and the principal investigator. Interviews lasted approximately 1.5 hours. Daughters received $20 in Wave 1 and $15 in Wave 2 to thank them for their participation. Daughters were asked a series of questions about background characteristics, caregiving tasks and activities, mother's health, coresidence, and relationship quality. Respondents also completed paper-and-pencil measures assessing perceived relationship quality. To help maintain interest in the study, daughters and mothers received birthday cards each year, and a general report about the study was distributed to respondents at the end of the second wave of data collection. At the end of each interview, the interviewers asked respondents if they would be willing to participate in the study in the following year. Before the next interview was scheduled, respondents were sent a postcard indicating that someone would call them to set up a time for the next interview. When possible, respondents had the same interviewer across waves. 31 Measures Two latent variables were of central interest in this study: amount of care given and perceived relationship quality. Four additional variables, coresidence, age, health, and past relationship quality were included as controls. Care given. Daughters were asked about the specific tasks with which they helped their mothers in seven IADL categories: (a) shopping/errands, (b) indoor maintenance, (c) financial tasks, (d) food preparation/clean up, (e) financial aid, (f) outdoor maintenance, and (g) bureaucratic mediation. In addition, they were asked about personal care (i.e., bathing, dressing) or ADLs. For each activity in every category, daughters were asked: "Does your mother do herself or do you help her to do it?" For each category, responses were coded: (1) no help at all. (2) help with half or less than half of the activities in the category, (3) help with more than half of the activities in the category, or (4) help with all of the activities in the category. Categories not applicable (e.g., outdoor maintenance for mothers who lived in retirement homes or apartments) were coded (5). A mean level of IADL care given was calculated by adding across the seven categories any score from 1 to 4, and dividing by the total number of categories in which scores from 1 to 4 were obtained. The level of ADL care given was the code given for that category (i.e., 1, 2, 3, or 4) (Appendix A). 32 Perceived relationship quality. Perceived relationship quality consisted of four measures: (a) daughter's intimacy score, (b) daughter's attachment score, (c) daughter's perception of overall relationship quality with her mother, and (d) daughter's perception of the closeness or distance of the present relationship with her mother. The intimacy scale (Walker & Thompson, 1983), reflecting generalized affection for the intergenerational partner, consisted of 17 items. Examples of the items are 'We want to spend time together," "She shows that she loves me," "Our lives are better because of each other," and "We love each other." Daughters rated each item from (1) not true to (5) always true. A mean score was computed for each participant with higher scores indicating greater intimacy. Cronbach's alphas for this measure have ranged from .91 to .97 across mother-daughter relationships (Walker & Thompson, 1983) (Appendix B). The attachment scale (Thompson & Walker, 1984) consisted of nine items that ranged from (1) not true to (5) always true. Examples of the items include: 'We are dependent on each other," 'We anticipate each other's moods," "Our best times are with each other," and "We are emotionally dependent on each other." A mean score was computed for each participant with higher scores indicating greater attachment. Cronbach's alphas have ranged from .86 to .91 across relationship reports (Thompson & Walker, 1984) (Appendix C). Daughters were asked the following question to describe their current relationship with their mother: "How would you describe your relationship with 33 fMother's name] at the present time?" Daughters responded from (1) poor to (4) excellent. In addition, daughters were asked the following question: "How (emotionally) distant or close would you say you are?" Responses ranged from (1) very distant to (4) very close. Control variables. Mothers' age was measured in years. Coresidence was measured by asking the daughter the following question: "What is your current living arrangement?" Coresidence was dichotomized into two categories (0) other (e.g., live alone, live with other relatives but not mother) or (1) live with mother. Daughters' perception of mothers' health was measured by the following question: "In general, how would you describe your mother's health right now?" (1) poor, (2) fair, (3) good, or (4) excellent. Daughter's perception of past relationship quality was measured by the following question: "How would you describe your relationship with your mother in the past?" (1) poor, (2) fair, (3) good, or (4) excellent. 34 Data Analysis Data on the 140 participants were obtained from the measures described above. The SAS statistical program was used for descriptive analyses. Descriptive statistics such as the mean, median, standard deviation, range, frequency, and percent were generated. Intercorrelations of independent and dependent variables were computed for caregiving daughters. The method used to analyze change in the amount of care given by daughters to care-receiving mothers and change in perceived relationship quality from Wave 1 to Wave 2 was the LISREL (Linear Structural Relations) computing program (Version 8.12a; J6reskog & Sorbom, 1993). All analyses were conducted using maximum likelihood estimation on covariance matrices, although the major emphasis was on parameter estimates for the completely standardized solution. Structural equation modeling (SEM) takes into account multiple indicators allowing for more valid measurements of latent constructs (i.e., amount of care given and relationship quality) (Bent ler, 1988). As Byrne (1994) and others (Bent ler, 1988; Bollen, 1989; JOreskog & SOrbom, 1993) noted, SEM allows for testing and modeling visually the adequacy of fit of a theoretical model to data by using a covariance matrix (see Bent ler, 1988; Joreskog & SOrbom, 1993 for further explanations). SEM consists of two conceptually distinct submodels: a measurement model that deals with the relationship between observed measures and their underlying latent constructs, and the structural model that 35 specifies the relationship between latent and observed variables that are not indicators of the latent construct (see Bent ler, 1988; Hoyle, 1995 for a review). The model, which included both the measurement and structural models, is presented in Figure 2. There were four exogenous variables (mothers' age, daughters' perception of mothers' health, coresidence, and past relationship quality) and four endogenous variables (amount of care given and relationship quality measured at Waves 1 and 2). In structural equation terminology, the four exogenous variables are called Ksi's, (4)'s and the four endogenous variables are called Eta's, (Ti)'s. The structural parameters from the exogenous variables to the endogenous variables are called the Gamma's, (y)'s. The structural parameters from one endogenous construct to the next are called Beta's, (3)'s. The Zeta (0 coefficients are the unexplained variance for each endogenous latent variable. The measurement model includes paths from the latent constructs to the measurement indicators. These are called Lamda's, (A)'s. The Epsilon's, (E)'s represent the error (residuals) for the indicators of the latent endogenous variables. There are several advantages to using structural equation modeling in the present study. First, SEM allows for the joint specification and estimation of the hypothesized measurement and structural models to account for the observed data (Long, 1983). Second, SEM takes into account measurement errors and uses multiple indicators to improve the construct validity of measurements (Bent ler, 1980; Hayuduk, 1987). Third, SEM allows for "restricted" models that 36 include systematic constraints on relationships among theoretical constructs and between observed variables and theoretical constructs. A key implication of such restrictions is that models can be tested that include only relationships that are hypothesized a priori. Fourth, SEM provides a systematic basis for evaluating the "fit" of the hypothesized model to the data. Thus, SEM allows for theory development and construct validation (see Anderson & Gerbing, 1988 for further explanation). Missing data Missing data are often a major problem in longitudinal research. Standard analysis of missing data such as pairwise deletion, listwise deletion, mean substitution, and imputation assume that data are missing at random (Little & Rubin, 1987). Due to sample bias, inefficient estimates, and discarding of potentially useful data, these analyses often produce biased results (Muthen, Kaplan, & Hollis, 1987). Structural equation modeling was applied to test whether missing data were problematic. Of the 140 sample daughters, 7 have missing data on at least one of the variables in the model. The Expectation Maximization (EM) Algorithm was used to account for missing data patterns (Dempster, Laird, & Rubin, 1977; Little & Rubin, 1987). EM Algorithm uses multiple-regression-based imputation to predict the missing variable using all other relevant variables. EM Algorithm assigns the 37 missing variables an imputed value that includes the error component (see Dempster et al., 1977; Little & Rubin, 1987 for further explanation). 38 Chapter Four Results This study examined the effect of the amount of care given on the quality of mother-daughter relationships in middle and later life. Using SEM methodology, a model describing mother-daughter relationships was empirically tested. This chapter is organized into three sections. First, univariate descriptive statistics as well as correlations among observed variables are presented. Second, results of the structural equation modeling analyses are presented. Finally, the results are discussed in relation to the hypotheses presented in Chapter 2. Preliminary Data Analyses Means, standard deviations, and correlations for all observed variables are presented in Table 3. An inspection of Table 3 shows that the two observed measurement indicators, IADL and ADL, for the latent construct of amount of care given, were highly correlated at Wave 1 (r = .67, p < .001) and Wave 2 (r = .74, p. < .001). The correlations of the four observed measurement indicators, closeness, good relationship quality, attachment, and intimacy, for the latent construct of relationship quality, were all positively related to each other. Table 3: Correlations Among Observed Indicators and Exogenous Variables Indicators V1 1. IADL1 2. ADL1 3. Close1 4. Good1 1.00 .67 .02 -.07 .16 .00 .77 .60 .00 -.12 .04 -.04 .22 -.27 5. Attachment) 6. Intimacy1 7. IADL2 8. ADL2 9. Close2 10. Good2 11. Attachment2 12. Intimacy2 13. MAGE 14. Health 15. Coresidence 16. Past Rel.Q1ty. MEANS SD V2 1.00 -.05 -.09 .12 .04 .53 .51 -.07 -.14 -.02 -.10 .22 V3 V5 V6 V7 V8 V9 V10 1.00 .65 .59 .68 1.00 .46 .68 V13 V14 V15 V16 .31 -.11 .43 1.00 .23 .08 .15 1.00 .00 .09 1.00 -.06 1.00 3.06 .72 4.01 .69 81.48 8.79 2.82 .88 .20 .40 3.12 .76 V11 V12 1.00 .76 -.06 -.08 -.04 1.00 -.01 .00 1.00 .61 .45 .47 .06 -.02 .54 .39 .48 .45 -.03 -.02 .31 -.30 .24 .05 .02 -.21 .28 2.17 1.40 .72 3.46 .60 .71 V4 1.00 .44 .64 -.12 -.09 .56 .45 .47 .61 .04 .07 -.12 .39 3.49 .62 1.00 .58 .14 .09 .54 .28 .75 .56 -.05 -.15 -.04 .36 3.27 .70 1.00 .00 .01 .64 .52 .54 .74 .04 -.05 -.15 .36 4.04 .67 1.00 .74 .00 -.10 -.05 -.12 .27 -.29 .15 .02 1.00 .00 -.10 -.10 -.14 .22 -.22 .13 -.01 2.53 1.68 .81 .91 -.10 -.02 -.20 .37 3.44 .64 .01 .03 -.17 .38 3.45 .72 Note. IADL1, (ADL2 - Instrumental Activities of Daily Living in waves 1 & 2; ADL1, ADL2 - Activities of Daily Living in Waves 1 & 2; Closel, Close2 Closeness in Waves 1 & 2; Good1, Good2 - Good relationship quality in Waves 1 & 2; MAGE - Mothers' age; HEALTH - Daughters' perception of mothers' health. 40 The correlations ranged from .44 to .61 at Wave 1 and .46 to .76 at Wave 2, respectively. All correlations were statistically significant. Table 3 also provides the correlations between exogenous variables (mothers' age, daughters' perceptions of mothers' health, coresidence, and past relationship quality). The correlations ranged from -.06 to .23. Results indicated that mother's age and daughter's perception of mother's health were positively associated with each other (r = .23, p < .01). Mother's age was not associated with past relationship quality or coresidence. Finally, coresidence was not associated with past relationship quality or daughter's perception of mother's health. Structural Equation Model A two-step process was used in analyzing the structural model (See Figure 2). Step 1 involved assessing the adequacy of the measurement model and step 2 verified the structural model. The two-step process has the advantage of minimizing the interpretational confounding of both the structural and measurement model covariances (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988). 41 Measurement Model A preliminary confirmatory factor analysis was conducted to test the measurement indicators of the four latent endogenous constructsamount of care given, measured at Waves 1 and 2, and relationship quality, measured at Waves 1 and 2. A structural equation modeling program, LISREL8.12a (Joreskog & Sombom,1993), was employed using the maximum likelihood (ML) estimation method. The ML estimation assumes that the data being analyzed have multivariate normal distribution. When the data are not normally distributed, the weighted least squares (WLS) estimation method is usually recommended. Although the WLS estimation method provides unbiased estimates (e.g., chisquare, standard errors), it requires a very large sample (see Bollen, 1989; Joreskog & Sombom,1993). The sample size used in this study precludes the use of WLS. Two observed measurement indicators (IADL and ADL) were loaded on the latent construct, amount of care given. Four observed measurement indicators (closeness, good relationship quality, intimacy, and attachment) were loaded on the latent construct, relationship quality. Because the same items were used at both waves (i.e., closeness, Wave 1 and closeness, Wave 2), corresponding error terms were allowed to be correlated. These correlations are not shown in Figure 2 for simplicity. Finally, the four latent variables were allowed to be correlated in confirmatory factor analyses (see Figure 3). ci Mothers' Age E2 IADL ADL W1 y11 kl Mothers' Health IADL W2 W1 X2 C3 1331 Amount of Care Coresidence Amount of Care (Wave 2) (Wave 1) E3 rI3 71 [343 c4 Past Relation. Quality y 24 Relationship Quality (Wave 2) Relationship Quality (Wave 1) r14 112 19 x3 W1 63 'Attach I nt. W1 W1 64 e6 rial ood I 17 IW2 Frri W2 I 69 (G W2 I 610 W2 I 61 I Figure 2. Structural and Measurement Models Note. Correlated error terms among observed measurement indicators are not shown in order to simplify the presentation. 1 el2 43 The initial analysis of the measurement model yielded a statistically significant chi-square, x2 (42, N = 140) = 96.26, p < .001. Contrary to the conventional chi-square test, a nonsignificant chi-square indicates a good fit of the model to the data. The chi-square statistic, however, has been criticized for its practical use in model evaluation due to its dependency on large sample sizes and its sensitivity to multivariate normality violation (Bent ler & Bonett, 1980). Other measures of fit indices such as the Non-Normed Fit Index (NNFI) = .92, the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) = .95 and the Incremental Fit Index (IFI) = .95, indicated an adequate fit of the model to the data. While this model does not fit the data perfectly, the fit indices were all above the recommended .90 minimum criterion (JOreskog & Sorbom, 1993). The root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) is another goodness-of-fit index that corrects for degrees of freedom. The RMSEA for the measurement model is .10. Although the RMSEA exceeds the recommended level of .08 (Browne & Cudeck, 1993), inspection of the loadings for the measurement model indicated that all loadings were statistically significant and strong, ranging from .63 to .94 (completely standardized estimates) (See Table 4). The results indicated that all observed variables were good indicators of their underlying constructs. 44 Table 4: Loadings of Measurement Model Parameter estimates Unstandardized estimates Completely standardized estimates t-ratio IADL, Wave 1<== Amount of care given, Wave 1 .56 .79 6.37 ADL, Wave 1<== Amount of care given, Wave 1 .62 .86 6.02 Closeness, Wave 1<== Relationship quality, Wave 1 .38 .63 7.78 Good relationship quality, Wave 1 <== Relationship quality, Wave 1 .47 .77 10.01 Attachment, Wave 1<== Relationship quality, Wave 1 .48 .67 8.75 Intimacy, Wave 1<== Relationship Quality, Wave 1 .56 .78 11.48 IADL, Wave 2<== Amout of care given, Wave 2 .64 .94 6.52 ADL, Wave 2<== Amout of care given, Wave 2 .89 .75 7.10 Closeness, Wave 2<== Relationship quality, Wave 2 .48 .73 10.09 Good relationship quality, Wave 2 <== Relationship quality, Wave 2 .52 .73 9.58 Attachment, Wave 2<== Relationship quality, Wave 2 .57 .79 11.06 Intimacy Wave 2,<== Relationship quality, Wave 2 .65 .93 14.03 Note. IADL Instrumental Activities of Daily Living; ADL - Activities of Daily Living. t-ratios above 3.21 are significant at p < .001 45 Structural Model Table 5a shows the goodness of fit indices of the structural models. The initial analysis of the structural model, Model 1a structural model where corresponding error terms were allowed to covary because the same items were used in both waves (i.e., IADL, Wave 1 and IADL, Wave 2), yielded a statistically significant chi-square, x2 (88, N = 140) = 146.80, 2 < .001; NNFI = .93; CFI = .95; IFI = .95; RMSEA = .07. Upon inspection of the residuals and the LISREL modification indices (Joreskog & SOrbom, 1993), however, the hypothesized model was respecified to include correlations between additional errors as specified below. Model 2 allowed closeness and good relationship quality errors to covary within each wave because the two items contain similar wordings. The resulting model yielded the following fit: x2 (86, N = 140) = 122.18, 2 < .01; NNFI = .96; CFI = .97; IFI = .97; RMSEA = .06. The final model, Model 3, allowed attachment and intimacy error terms to covary within each wave at both time periods due to the conceptual similarity between the two scales. Model 3 resulted in the following fit: x2 (84, N = 140) = 114.11, 2 < .05; NNFI = .96; CFI = .97; IFI = .98; RMSEA = .05 (See Table 5a). With these respecifications, the structural models (Models 1 and 2) were improved, showing a better fit to the data. As shown in Table 5b, Model 2 was significantly better than Model 1 and Model 3 was significantly better than models 1 and 2. Table 5a: Goodness-of-Fit Indices of Structural Models Model Equality Constraints df NNFI /2 CFI IFI RMSEA Model 1 None 88 146.80 .00 .93 .95 .95 .07 Model 2 Closet error term correlated to Good1, Close2 error term correlated to Good2 86 122.18 .01 .96 .97 .97 .06 Model 3 Model 2 plus correlated attachment) and intimacy) error terms, attachment2 error term correlated intimacy2 84 114.11 .02 .96 .97 .98 .05 Model 4 Model 3 with equality constraints 88 118.76 .02 .96 .97 .97 .05 Note. NNFI Non-Normed Fit Index; CFI Comparative Fit Index; IFI Incremental Fit Index; RMSEA Root Mean Square of Approximation. Close 1, 2 -Closeness in Waves 1 & 2; Good 1, 2 Good relationship quality in Waves 1 & 2; attach - Attachment in Waves 1 & 2; int. Intimacy in Waves 1 & 2. 47 Table 5b: Model Comparisons Zdiff Model df if Model 3 - Model 1 32.69*** 2 Model 2 24.62*** 2 Model 3 - Model 2 8.07* 2 Model 4 - Model 3 4.65 4 *p<.05 Model 1 < .001 48 A fourth model, Model 4, was constructed to test invariance of the measurement indicators of the latent endogenous variables across time as detailed below. Measurement Invariance A concern that relates to longitudinal studies is whether constructs measured across waves are equivalent (Byrne, Shavelson, & Muthen, 1989; Pentz & Chou, 1994). Testing for measurement invariance addresses whether a particular measurement instrument operates equivalently across different populations (e.g., gender, age, race) and/or on the same population longitudinally. It concerns primarily the invariance of regression intercepts, factor loadings (regression slopes), and error/uniqueness variances. Joreskog (1971) developed a procedure that enabled researchers to test for invariance simultaneously across groups and waves. Conventionally, tests of invariance begin with a baseline model. This model represents the most parsimonious as well as substantively meaningful model specification (Byrne, 1994). In the present study, Model 3 was chosen for this purpose as the most parsimonious and meaningful model. In testing for invariance, each measurement indicator for the latent construct was tested for equivalence across waves. A fourth model (Model 4) tested the loadings of the observed indicators on the endogenous latent variables. Equality constraints were imposed on loadings in Model 4 so that each 49 observed indicator at Wave 1 was equal to the corresponding indicator at Wave 2. For example, the loadings of IADL at Wave 1 and IADL at Wave 2 were forced to be equal. Notice that loadings to IADL and intimacy were set to one for scaling purposes in Waves 1 and 2. As shown in Table 5a, although the overall chi-square for Model 4 was significant x2(88, N = 140) = 118.76, p < .05, practical fit indices suggested an adequate fit to the data; NNFI = .96; CFI = .97; IFI = .97; RMSEA = .05. Comparing Model 4 to Model 3, the chi-square difference test, x2d,ff = 4.65, (2 = n.s.), was not statistically significant, indicating that constraints imposed on Model 4 were reasonable (See Table 5b). On the basis of this result, it was concluded that the indicators measuring the latent variables were invariant over time. Parameter estimates from Model 4 are used to present the results below. Loadings on the latent variables. Table 6 shows the loadings of the indicators on the four latent variables of Model 4. Inspection of the loadings indicated that all loadings were statistically significant (2 < .001) and substantial, ranging from .62 to .91 (completely standardized estimates). The completely standardized loadings for IADL and ADL were .87 and .80 in Wave 1 and .91 and .79 in Wave 2. The completely standardized loadings for indicators of the latent construct, relationship quality, ranged from .62 to .82 in Wave 1 and .73 to .91 in Wave 2. The most prominent 50 Table 6: Loadings for Structural Model 4 Parameter estimates Unstandardized estimates Completely standardized estimates t-ratio IADL, Wave 1<== Amount of care given, Wave 1 1.00® .87 ADL, Wave 1<== Amount of care given, Wave 1 .99 .80 8.92 Closeness, Wave 1<== Relationship quality, Wave 1 .72 .62 11.25 Good relationship quality, Wave 1 <== Relationship quality, Wave 1 .84 .73 12.82 Attachment, Wave 1<== Relationship quality, Wave 1 .84 .64 12.04 Intimacy, Wave 1<== Relationship Quality, Wave 1 1.00ED .82 IADL, Wave 2<== Amout of care given, Wave 2 1.00® .91 ADL, Wave 2<== Amout of care given, Wave 2 .99 .79 8.92 Closeness, Wave 2<== Relationship quality, Wave 2 .72 .75 11.25 Good relationship quality, Wave 2 <== Relationship quality, Wave 2 .84 .73 12.82 Attachment, Wave 2<== Relationship quality, Wave 2 .84 .74 12.04 1.00® .91 Intimacy, Wave 2,<== Relationship quality, Wave 2 Note. IADL - Instrumental Activities of Daily Living; ADL - Activities of Daily Living. t-ratios above 3.21 are significant at p. < .001 ED Parameters set to 1.0 51 indicator for measuring relationship quality was intimacy (.82 Wave 1 and .91 at Wave 2) followed by good relationship (.73 Wave 1, .73 Wave 2), attachment (.64, Wave 1, .74, Wave 2), and closeness (.62 Wave 1, .75 Wave 2). Path coefficients. The maximum-likelihood estimates and associated t-values for the structural parameter from Model 4 are shown in Table 7. Examination of the hypothesized completely standardized paths in the model indicated that hypothesis 1 was supported, showing that the latent construct, amount of care given, is stable over time, (completely standardized estimate, = .81, t = 11.48, < .001). Results also showed that perceived relationship quality was stable over time (completely standardized estimate, 0 = .93,1 = 13.45, 2 < .001), thus supporting hypothesis 2. The next two hypotheses, H3 and H4, dealt with the relationship between amount of care given and relationship quality at each wave. H3 was not supported (completely standardized estimate, = -.01, t = -.08, p = n.s.) in that amount of care given was not associated with relationship quality at Wave 1. H4, however, was supported. As predicted, as the amount of care given increased, perceived relationship quality decreased (completely standardized estimate, 13 = .16, = -2.67, < .01). 52 Table 7: Parameter Estimates of the Hypothesized Model (Beta) Parameter estimates Amount of care given, Wave 1 ==> Amount of care given, Wave 2 Unstandardized estimates Completely standardized estimates t-ratio .99 .81 11.48 Amount of care given, Wave 1 ==>Relationship quality, Wave 1 -.01 -.01 -.08 Relationship quality, Wave 1 ==> Relationship quality, Wave 2 1.11 .93 13.45 Relationship quality, Wave 2 ==> Amount of care given, Wave 2 -.14 -.16 -2.67 Note. t-ratios above 2.58 and 3.21 are significant at p < .01 and p < .001 respectively. 53 Effects of the exogenous variables. The last four hypotheses dealt with the relationships between the exogenous variables (i.e., mothers' age daughters' perception of mothers' health, coresidence, and past relationship quality) and the latent endogenous variables, amount of care given and relationship quality at Wave 1. Table 8 presents the maximum-likelihood estimates (both unstandardized and completely standardized) and associated t-values. Results indicate that H5was supported. Mothers' age was positively associated with the initial amount of care given at Wave 1. Mothers who were older reported needing more care than their younger counterpart (completely standardized estimate, y = .35, t = 4.26, 2 < .001). H6 was also supported. Daughters who perceived their mothers to be in good health provided less care in the form of IADLs and ADLs (completely standardized estimate, y = -.42, t = -5.04, 2 < .001). Additionally, hypothesis H7 was also supported. Daughters who lived with their mothers provided more care than those who did not (completely standardized estimate, y =.30., t = 3.70, p < .001). Finally, support was also observed for H8, which showed that perceived past relationship quality is positively related to perceived current relationship quality (completely standardized estimate, y = .51, t = 6.10, 2 < .001). Taken together, results from these analyses offered empirical support for the hypothesized structural relationships specified in Model 4. 54 Table 8: Parameter Estimates of the Exogenous Variables to Latent Variables Parameter estimates Unstandardized estimates Completely standardized estimates t-ratio Mothers', age, Wave 1==> Amount of care given, Wave 1 .02 .35 4.26 Daughters' perception of mothers' health, Wave 1==> Amount of care given, Wave 1 -.29 -.42 -5.04 Coresidence, Wave 1==> Amount of care given, Wave 1 .44 .30 3.70 Past relationship quality, Wave 1==> Relationship quality, Wave 1 .34 .51 6.10 Note. t-ratios above 3.21 are significant at p < .001. 55 Chapter Five Discussion and Conclusion The purpose of this study was to examine longitudinally the effect of the amount of care given on daughters' perceptions of their relationship with their care-receiving mothers. Specifically, this study used data from 140 caregiving daughters to test the elaborated wear-and-tear hypothesis, which proposes that daughters' perception of relationship quality with their mothers will deteriorate as the amount of care given increases. Caregiving daughters in the present study reported helping their mothers with IADLs such as shopping and errands, indoor maintenance, and bureaucratic mediation. Nearly one in five gave help with ADLs such as bathing and dressing. The need for help with these activities is characteristic among the elderly population with similar needs (Stone et al., 1987). Additionally, the rate of care provided for ADLs is comparable to the rates of care for women over age 75 (Spitze & Logan, 1989). Caregiving daughters, on average, reported high attachment and intimacy scores and good and close relationships with their care-receiving mothers. 56 Primary Findings Stability of the endogenous latent variables. The first two hypotheses dealt with the stability of the two endogenous latent constructs, amount of care given and relationship quality. H1 and H2 predicted that amount of care given and relationship quality would remain stable over time. The results supported both hypotheses. The amount of care given by caregiving daughters to their physically dependent elderly mothers ramained stable from Wave 1 to Wave 2. This is not surprising given that these mothers had chronic rather than acute health conditions, such as arthritis and sensory impairments, which are common among elderly persons. Such conditions tend to change only gradually over time. Although the needs of care-receiving elderly mothers may gradually increase, the finding that amount of care given is stable over two years suggests that caregiving daughters have time to adapt and adjust to the stresses of giving care (Stephens & Zarit, 1989). That relationship quality is stable over time also is not surprising. Others have found that adult children who perceived their relationships with their parents as being warm, intimate, and close in early life, tend to report a feeling of intimacy and closeness in mid- and later life as well, 57 suggesting that such relationships are stable (Rossi & Rossi, 1990; Whitbeck et al., 1994). From a life span perspective, (Kahn & Antonucci, 1980), mother-daughter relationships in mid- and later life are a continuation of early relationships, and they reflect past intimate experiences and histories. Perhaps relationship stability is due also to the socialization process that encourages parents and children to keep connected to each other throughout the life course (Atkinson, 1989). For example, women, especially mothers and daughters, have been socialized to be active participants in maintaining kin relationships. Another reason may be due to the high attachment levels between mothers and daughters. Research has shown that mothers and daughters in mid- and later life show high levels of attachment by living in close proximity to each other, communicating with each other by telephone or letters, exchanging periodic visits, and sharing common experiences that lead to increased emotional intimacy (Cicirelli, 1983; Lawton et al., 1994; Umberson, 1992). Finally, most daughters (n = 127, 91%) were mothers. Hess and Waring (1978) argued that stability in relationship quality may be due to the common role experience daughters shared with their mothers by becoming parents themselves (Walker, Thompson, & Morgan, 1987). 58 Relationship between amount of care given and relationship quality. Hypotheses 3 and 4 dealt with the relationship between amount of care given and relationship quality at each wave. The literature has shown that high amounts of care given may result in detrimental effects for caregiving daughters, (i.e., Lang & Brody; 1983; Townsend et al., 1989). Daughters had been providing care to their dependent elderly mothers prior to the onset of the study. H3 predicted that care given in Wave 1 would be negatively related to perceived relationship quality in Wave 1. This hypothesis was not supported. No association between amount of care given at Wave 1 and relationship quality at Wave 1 was found. The amount of care provided by caregiving daughters was minimal in Wave 1. For example, approximately two-thirds of the caregiving daughters reported minimal or no care for the following IADLs in Wave 1: financial management, food preparation, outdoor maintenance, and financial support. The need for personal care was also reported to be minimal or nonexistent for the majority of daughters. Perhaps the type of care provided by daughters (i.e., shopping, indoor maintenance, bureaucratic mediation) is not particularly stressful. H4, however, was supported. H4 predicted that over time, amount of care given would be negatively related to relationship quality. Daughters who provided increased care in the form of IADLs and ADLs to 59 their dependent mothers from Wave 1 to Wave 2 showed a decline in relationship quality in Wave 2. In other words, relationship quality declined over time in response to an increased amount of care given. This finding is consistent with the elaborated wear-and-tear hypothesis in that providing more care erodes relationship quality over time even though it does not do so initially (Pear lin et al., 1990; Scharlach, 1987; Sheehan & Nuttall, 1988; Townsend et al., 1989; Walker et al., 1996). As mothers need more care, perhaps they are less able to contribute to the relationship, thus leading to declines in relationship quality (Dowd, 1975). It is important to note that although relationship quality decreased slightly, the means of the observed relationship quality indicators showed that caregiving daughters, on average, reported having good and close relationships with their care-receiving mothers, relationships that were characterized by attachment and intimacy. This is not surprising given other research indicating close relationships throughout adulthood between mothers and daughters (Baruch & Barnett, 1983). Furthermore, Abel (1990) found that the mother-daughter bond strengthened as a result of caregiving; that is, daughters reported feeling very attached and had strong feelings of affection for their dependent parent. The high relationship quality scores found in this study may be due also, in part, to the nature of the sample. As stated previously, respondents volunteered for this study, probably overrepresenting positive situations. Additionally, only a small percentage of mothers in this study 60 required assistance with personal care, which may be more stressful to relationships. Finally, that this study examined daughters whose mothers had acute or chronic health problems and not any form of dementia may account for the daughter's high perception of relationship quality. Studies have shown that caring for someone with Alzheimer's Disease or other forms of dementia is stressful and burdensome for the caregiver (Birkel, 1987; Schulz & Williamson, 1991; Zarit, Todd, & Zarit, 1986). Relationship between covariates and endogenous latent variables. Hypotheses H5 through H8 dealt with the relationships between the covariates and the endogenous latent variables. As predicted, mothers' age was positively associated with the initial amount of care given (H5). This substantiates earlier findings that increasing age is related to poorer health (Cicirelli, 1981; Hareven, 1995; Kinsella, 1995). The results also showed that daughters who perceived their mothers to be in better health provided less care in the form of IADLs and ADLs (H6), thus supporting previous findings that poorer health is associated with an increase in helping behaviors by caregiving daughters (Abel, 1986; Archbold, 1983; Lang & Brody, 1983; Markides & Cooper, 1989; Walker et al., 1992). 61 As stated previously, 20% of the caregiving daughters in this study lived with their dependent elderly mothers. As H7 predicted, the results showed that caregiving daughters who lived with their mothers provided more care than caregiving daughters who lived separately. A coresidential caregiving daughter may do all of her mother's cooking and laundry, for example, even if the mother is not dependent on that help, because the daughter does these activities in her homemaking role. Others have supported this finding as well (Chappell, 1991; Lang & Brody, 1983; Walker & Pratt, 1991; Walker et al., 1996). H8 predicted that daughters' perceived past relationship quality with their mothers would be positively related to their perceptions of current relationship quality. This hypothesis was supported. From a social learning perspective (see Patterson, 1982, 1986), Whitbeck et al., (1991) found that learned patterns of interactions from the early parent-child relationship did shape the current elderly parent-adult child relationship Rossi and Rossi (1990) also found that parents and children mutually reinforce each other throughout life in the way they communicate, negotiate and renegotiate rules and roles, and deal with family conflicts. The continuous reinforcement of interactional patterns between parents and children caries over to current relationship quality. 62 Limitations and Strengths Limitations. As with all research, there are limitations to the present study. First, results of this study cannot be generalized to the family caregiving population because it was selective, excluding caregiving spouses, sons, and care-receiving fathers. Respondents were recruited both from urban and rural areas, and having a wide range of caregiving daughters were included who were similar to representative samples of White daughters providing care to an impaired elderly parent(s). Second, this study focused on caregiving daughters whose mothers did not have cognitive deficiencies. As noted in the intergenerational and caregiving literatures, giving care to a person with some form of dementia may cause more wear-and-tear on the caregiver than the type of caregiving represented in this study (e.g., Blieszner & Shifflift, 1990). A third limitation is that 98% of the sample in this study is White so the results cannot be generalized to other ethnic/racial populations. A final limitation is that the geographical characteristics of the Pacific Northwest may be different from other locations. 63 Strengths. A major strength of this study is that it investigated longitudinally the effect of the amount of care given on perceived relationship quality between mothers and daughters in middle and later life, thus filling a gap in both the gerontological and intergenerational literatures. Another strength of the study is that it used structural equation modeling methodology to compensate for previous methodological problems. A third strength was controlling for the care recipients' impairment; that is, this study only focused on care recipients who had acute or chronic health conditions. A fourth strength was that the research examined only the mother-daughter relationship, thus, controlling for the gender of the caregiver and the care-recipient as well as excluding daughters with married mothers as to reduce confounding effects in the data (Boyd, 1989; Horowitz, 1985; Stafford, 1988). Finally, this research addressed directly the problem of missing data that often plagues longitudinal studies by using EM Algorithm to account for missing data patterns (Dempster et al., 1977; Little & Rubin, 1987; Malone Beach & Zarit, 1991; Mancini & Blieszner, 1989; Young & Kahana, 1989). These strengths offset many weaknesses in the caregiving literature and help to minimize some of the study's limitations. 64 Implications Although the finding that an increase in the amount of care given over time leads to a decrease in relationship quality, caregiving daughters generally reported good and close relationships with their care-receiving mothers. Service providers should target those daughters who are giving increasing amounts of care by offering respite services that allow them to take a break from the demands of caregiving. Service providers should also make classes available in stress management. The finding that coresidential daughters provide more care is important in that such individuals may need to learn coping skills to reduce potential stress and burden as the amount of care provided increases. Additionally, service providers should target daughters who report having a poor relationship with their mothers. Interventions such as individual or family counseling may help them focus on positive relationship aspects rather than the negative aspects of the relationship. Directions for Future Research The findings reported here are based on the average caregiving daughter and does not address the issue of individual variability within and between caregivers. As Walker et al.(1996) and Townsend et al. 65 (1989) found, there is mounting evidence of variability among caregivers. Techniques such as latent growth cure methodology will help in assessing the variability among caregiving daughters in the caregiving literature. Further, more research is needed to examine the association between relationship quality and amount of care given. For example, what other factors contribute to decline in relationship quality? Do mothers and daughters argue in their perceptions of the amount of care given and relationship quality? If not, how do they differ? Finally, when the amount of care given remains stable for many years, does relationship quality also remain stable? These are just a few of the questions that should be addressed in the family caregiving literature. 66 Bibliography Abel, E. K. (1989). The ambiguities of social support: Adult daughters caring for frail elderly parents. Journal of Aging Studies, 3, 211-230. Abel, E. K. (1986). Adult daughters and care of the elderly. Feminist Studies, 12, 479-493. Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103.411 -423. Aquilino, W. S. (1990). The likelihood of parent-adult child coresidence: Effects of family structure and parental characteristics. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 405-419. Aquilino, W. S., & Supple, K. R. (1991). Parent-child relations and parent's satisfaction with living arrangements when adult children live at home. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 13-27. Archbold, P. G. (1983). Impact of parent-caring on women. Family Relations, 32, 39-45. Aronson, J. (1992). Women's sense of responsibility for the care of old people: But who else is going to do it? Gender and Society, 6, 8-29. Atkinson, M. P. (1989). Conceptualizations of the parent-child relationship: Attachment, crescive bonds, and identity salience. In J. A. Mancini (Ed.), Aging parents and adult children (pp. 81-98). Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath. Atkinson, J., & Huston, T. L. (1984). Sex role orientation and division of labor in early marriage. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 330345. 67 Barer, B. M., & Johnson, C. L. (1990). A critique of the caregiving literature. The Gerontologist, 30, 26-29. Barnett, R. C., Kibria, N., Baruch, G. K., & Pleck, J. H. (1991). Adult daughter-parent relationships and their associations with daughters' subjective well-being and psychological distress. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 29-42. Baruch, G., & Barnett, R. C. (1983). Adult daughters' relationships with their mothers. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 601-606. Barusch, A. S. (1988). Problems and coping strategies of elderly spouse caregivers. The Gerontologist, 28, 677-685. Barusch, A. S., & Spaid, W. M. (1989). Gender differences in caregiving: Why do wives report greater burden? The Gerontologist, 29, 667-676. Bengtson, V. L., Cutler, N. E., Mangen, D. J., & Marshall, V. W. (1985). Generations, cohorts, and relations between age groups. In R. H. Binstock & E. Shanas (Eds.), Handbook of aging and the social sciences (2nd ed., pp. 304338). New York, New York: Van Nostrand. Bengtson, V. L., & Roberts, R. L. (1991). Intergenerational solidarity in aging families: An example of formal theory construction. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 856-870. Bent ler, P. M. (1988). Causal modeling via structural equation systems. In J. R. Nesselroade & R. B. Cattell (Eds)., Handbook of multivariate experimental psychology (2nd ed., pp. 317-335). New York: Plenum. Bent ler, P. M. (1980). Multivariate analysis with latent variables: Causal modeling. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 419-456. Blieszner, R., & Mancini, J. (1987). Enduring ties: Older adults' parental role and responsibilities. Family Relations, 36, 176-180. 68 Blieszner, R., & Shifflift, P. A. (1990). The effects of Alzheimer's disease on close relationships between patients and caregivers. Family Relations, 39, 57-62. Bollen, K. E. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: John Wiley. Boyd, C. J. (1989). Mothers and daughters: A discussion of theory and research. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51, 291-301. Bretherton, I., Biringen, Z., & Ridgeway, D. (1991). The parental side of caregiving. In K. Pillemer & K. McCartney (Eds.), Parent-child relations in later life (pp. 1-24). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Brody, E. M. (1986). Filial care of the elderly and changing roles of women (and men). Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14, 175-200. Brody, E. M. (1985). Parent care as a normative family process. The Gerontologist, 25, 19-29. Brody, E. M., Johnsen, P. T., & Fulcomer, M. C. (1994). What should adult children do for elderly parents: Opinions and preferences of three generations of women. Journal of Gerontology, 39, 736-746. Brody, E. M., Litvin, S. J., Albert, S. M., & Hoffman, C. J. (1994). Marital status of daughters and patterns of parent care. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 49, S95-S103. Brody, E. M., & Schoonover, C. B. (1986). Patterns of parent care when adult daughters work and when they do not. The Gerontologist, 26, 372-381. Bromberg, E. M. (1982-1983). Mother-daughter relationships in later life: Negating the myth. Aging, 340, 15-20. 69 Browne, K. D., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136-162). Newbury Park, CA : Sage. Byrne, B. M. (1994). Structural equation modeling with EQS and EQSndows: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Byrne, B. M., Shavelson, R. J., & Muthen, B. (1989). Testing for invariance of factor covariance and mean structures: The issue of partial measurement invariance. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 456-466. Caspi, A., & Elder, G. H. (1988). Emergent family patterns: The intergenerational construction of problem behavior and relationships. In R. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Relationships within families. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Chappell, N. L. (1991). Living arrangements and sources of caregiving. The Gerontologist, 46, S1-S8. Cicirelli, V. G. (1991). Attachment theory in old age: Protection of the attached figure. In K. Pillemer & K. McCartney (Eds.), Parent-child relations in later life (pp. 25-41). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Cicirelli, V. G. (1983). Adult children's attachment and helping behavior to elderly parents: A path model. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 815-825. Cicirelli, V. G. (1981). Helping elderly parents: The role of adult children. Boston, MA: Auburn House Publishing Company. Dempster, A. P., Laird, N. M., & Rubin, D. B. (1977). Maximum likelihood estimation with incomplete data via the EM algorithm. Journal of Research and Statistical Society, B39, 1-38. 70 DeWitt, D. J., blister, A. V., & Burch, T. K. (1988). Physical distance and social contact between elders and their adult children. Research on Aging, 10, 56-80. Dowd, J. J. (1975). Aging as exchange: A preface to theory. Journal of Gerontology, 30, 584-594. Dwyer, J. W., & Coward, R. T. (1991). A multivariate comparison of the involvement of adult sons versus daughters in the care of impaired parents. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 46, S259-S269. Dwyer, J. W., & Seccombe, K. (1991). Elder care as family labor: The influence of gender and family position. Journal of Family Issues, 12, 229-247. Frankel, B. G., & DeWitt, D. J. (1988). Geographic distance and intergenerational contact: Methodological issues. Journal of Aging Studies, 34, 238-250. Gerstel, N., & Gallagher, S. K. (1993). Kinkeeping and distress: Gender, recipients of care, and work-family conflict. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 55 598-607. Haley, W. E., & Pardo, K. M. (1989). Relationship of severity of dementia to caregiving stressors. Psychology and Aging, 4, 389-392. Hareven, T. K. (1995). Historical perspectives on the family and aging. In R. Blieszner & V. H. Bedford (Eds.), Handbook of aging and the family (pp. 1331). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Harootyan, R. A., & Bengtson, V. L. (1994). Intergenerational linkages: The context of the study. In V. L. Bengtson & R. A. Harootyan (Eds.), Intergenerational linkages: Hidden connections in American society. (pp. 1-18). New York, NY: Springer Publishing Co. Harris, D. (1990). Sociology of aging. New York, NY: Harper & Row. 71 Hayuduk, L. A. (1987). Structural equation modeling with LISREL. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins. Hess, B., & Waring, J. M. (1978). Parent and child in later life: Rethinking the relationship. In R. M. Lerner & G. B. Spanier (Eds.), Child influences on marital and family interaction (pp. 241-273). New York: Academic. Himes, C. L. (1994). Parental caregiving by adult women: A demographic perspective. Research on Aging, 16, 191-211. Hogan, D. P., Eggebeen, D. J., & Clogg, C. C. (1993). The structure of intergenerational exchange in American families. American Journal of Sociology, 98 1428-1458. Horowitz, A. (1985). Sons and daughters of caregivers to older parents: Differences in role performance and consequences. The Gerontologist, 25, 612617. Hoyle, R. H. (1995). The structural equation modeling approach: Basic concepts and fundamental issues. In R. H. (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications (pp. 1-15). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Jarett, W. H. (1985). Caregiving within kinship systems: Is affection really necessary? The Gerontologist, 25, 5-10. Johnson, C. L., & Catalano, D. J. (1983). A longitudinal study of family supports to impaired elderly. The Gerontologist, 23, 612-618. Joreskog, K. G. (1971). Statistical analyses of sets of congeneric tests. Psychometrika, 36, 109-134. Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8: Structural equation modeling with the SIMPLIS command language. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International. 72 Inc. Kart, C. S. (1985). The realities of aging. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon, Kinsella, K. (1995). Aging and the family: Present and future demographic issues. In R. Blieszner & V. H. Bedford (Eds.), Handbook of aging and the family (pp. 32-56). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Kunkel, S. R., & Applebaum, R. A. (1992). Estimating the prevalence of long-term care for disability for an aging society. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 47, S253-S260. Lang, A. M., & Brody, E. M. (1983). Characteristics of middle-aged daughters and help to their elderly dependent mothers. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 193-202. Lawton, L., Silverstein, M., & Bengtson, V. L. (1994). Affection, social contact, and geographic distance between adult children and their parents. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56, 57-68. Lawton, L., Silverstein, M., & Bengtson, V., L. (1994). Solidarity between generations in families. In V. L. Bengtson & R. A. Harootyan (Eds.), Intergenerational linkages: Hidden connections in American society. (pp. 19-42). New York, NY: Springer Publishing Co. Lee, G. R. (1987). Aging and intergenerational relations. Journal of Family Issues. 8. 448-450. Lee, G. R., & Ellithorpe, E. (1982). Intergenerational exchange and subjective well-being among the elderly. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 44, 217-224. Levitt, M. J., Guacci, N., & Weber, R. A. (1992). Intergenerational support, relationship quality, and well-being. Journal of Family Issues, 13, 465-481. 73 Little, R. J. A., & Rubin, D. B. (1987). Statistical analysis with missing data. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Malone Beach, E. E., & Zarit, S. H. (1991). Current research issues in caregiving to the elderly. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 32, 103-114. Mancini, J. A., & Blieszner, R. (1989). Aging parents and adult children: Research themes in intergenerational relations. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51, 275-290. Markides, K. S., & Cooper, C. L. (1989). Aging, stress, social support, and health: An overview. In K. S. Markides & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Aging, stress, and health (pp.1-12). NY: John Wiley & Sons. Mathews, S. H. (1988). The burdens of parent care: A critical evaluation of recent findings. Journal of Aging Studies, 2, 157-165. McDonald, G. W. (1981). Structural exchange and marital interaction. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 43, 825-839. Miller, B. (1989). Adult children's perceptions of caregiver stress and satisfaction. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 8, 275-293. Miller, B., McFall, S., & Montgomery, A. (1991). The impact of elder health, caregiver involvement, and global stress on two dimensions of caregiver burden. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 46. S9-S19. Montgomery, R. J. V., Gonyea, J. G., & Hooyman, N. R. (1985). Caregiving and the experience of subjective and objective burden. Family. Relations, 34, 19-25. Montgomery, R. V. J., & Kamo, Y. (1989). Parent care by sons and daughters. In J. Mancini (Ed.), Aging parents and adult children (pp. 213-230). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. 74 Motenko, A. K. (1989). The frustrations, gratifications, and well-being of dementia caregivers. The Gerontologist, 29, 166-172. Muthen, B., Kaplan, D., & Hollis, M. (1987). Structural equation modeling for data that are not missing completely at random. Psychometrika, 51, 431-462. Nunnally, F. C. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co. Patterson, G. R. (1986). Performance models for antisocial boys. American Psychologist, 41, 432-444. Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive Family Process. Eugene, OR: Castillia. Pear lin, L. I., Mullan, J. T., Semple, S. J., & Skaff, M. M. (1990). Caregiving and the stress process: An overview of concepts and their measures. The Gerontologist, 30, 583-591. Pentz, M. A., & Chou, C. P. (1994). Measurement invariance in longitudinal clinical research assuming change from development and intervention. Special Section: Structural equation modeling in clinical research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 450-462. Pratt, C. C., Schmall, V., Wright, S., & Cleland, M. (1985). Burden and coping strategies of caregivers to Alzheimer's patients. Family Relations, 34, 2733. Pruchno, R. A., & Resch, N. L. (1989). Husbands and wives as caregivers: Antecedents of depression and burden. The Gerontologist, 29, 159165. Rakowski, W., & Clark, N. M. (1985). Future outlook, caregiving, and carereceiving in the family context. The Gerontologist, 25, 618-623. 75 Reed, B. R., Stone, A. A., & Neale, J. M. (1990). Effects of caring for a demented relative on elders' life events and appraisals. The Gerontologist, 30, 200-205. Roberts, R. L., Richards, L. N., & Bengtson, V. L. (1991). Intergenerational solidarity in families that bind. Marriage and Family Review, 13, 11-43. Robinson, B., & Thurnher, M. (1979). Taking care of aged parents: A family cycle transition. The Gerontologist, 19, 586-593. Rossi, A. S., & Rossi, P. H. (1990). Of human bonding: Parent-child relations across the life course. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Scharlach, A. E. (1994). Caregiving and employment: Competing or complementary roles? The Gerontologist, 34, 378-385. Scharlach, A. E. (1987). Role strain in mother-daughter relationships in later life. The Gerontologist, 27, 627-631. Schulz, R., & Williamson, G. M. (1991). A 2-year longitudinal study of depression among Alzheimer's caregivers. Psychology and Aging, 6, 569-578. Shanas, E. (1979). The family as a social support system in old age. The Gerontologist, 19, 169-174. Sheehan, N. W., & Nuttall, P. (1988). Conflict, emotion, and personal strain among family caregivers. Family Relations, 37, 305-309. Silverstein, M., Lawton, L., & Bengtson, V. L. (1994). Types of relations between parents and adult children. In V. L. Bengtson & R. A. Harootyan (Eds.), Intergenerational linkages: Hidden connections in American society. (pp. 43-76). New York, NY: Springer Publishing Co. 76 Spitze, G., & Logan, J. (1990). Sons, daughters, and intergenerational support. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 420-430. Stephens, S. A., & Christianson, J. B. (1986). Informal care of the elderly. Lexington, MA: D.C. Heath. Stephens, M. A., & Zarit, S. H. (1989). Symposium: Family caregiving to dependent older adults: Stress, appraisal, & coping. Psychology and Aging, 4, 387-388. Stoller, E. P. (1985). Exchange patterns in the informal support networks of the elderly: The impact of reciprocity on morale. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 47, 335-341. Stoller, E. P. (1983). Parental caregiving by adult children. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 851-858. Stoller, E. P., & Pugliesi, K. L. (1989). Other roles of caregivers: Competing responsibilities or supportive resources. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 44, S231-S238. Stone, R., Cafferata, G. L., & Sangl, J. (1987). Caregivers of the frail elderly: A national profile. The Gerontologist, 27, 616-626. Stueve, A., & O'Donnell, L. (1984). The daughter of aging parents. In G. B. Barach & J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds). Women in midlife. (pp. 203-225). New York: Plenum. Stull, D. E., Bowman, K., & Smerglia, V. (1994). Women in the middle: A myth in the making? Family Relations, 43, 319-324. Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The social psychology of groups. New York: John Wiley & Sons. 77 Thompson, L., & Walker, A. J. (1984). Mothers and daughters: Aid patterns and attachment. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 46, 313-322. Townsend, A., Noelker, L., Deimling, G., & Bass, D. (1989). Longitudinal impact of interhousehold caregiving on adult children's mental health. Psychology and Aging, 4, 393-401. Treas, J. (1977). Family support systems for the aged: Some social and demographic considerations. The Gerontologist, 17, 486-491. Umberson, D. (1992). Relationships between adult children and their parents: Psychological Consequences for both generations. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 54, 664-674. Umberson, D. (1989a). Parenting and well-being: The importance of context. Journal of Family Issues, 10, 427-439. Umberson, D. (1989b). Relationships with children: Explaining parents' psychological well-being. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51, 999-1012. U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1994). Current Population Reports, P251095. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Press. Walker, A. J., & Allen, K. A. (1991) Relationships between caregiving daughters and their elderly mothers. The Gerontologist, 31, 389-396. Walker, A. J., & Pratt, C. C. (1991). Daughters' help to mothers: Intergenerational aid versus caregiving. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 3-12. Walker, A. J., & Thompson, L. (1983). Intimacy and intergenerational aid and contact among mothers and daughters. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45. 841-849. 78 Walker, A. J., Acock, A. C., Bowman, S. R., & Li, F. (1996). Amount of care given and caregiving satisfaction: A latent growth curve analysis. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 51, P130-P142. Walker, A. J., Martin, S. K., & Jones, L. L. (1992). The benefits and costs of care receiving for daughters and mothers. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 47, S130-S139. Walker, A. J., Pratt, C. C., & Eddy, L. (1995) Informal care to aging family members: A critical review. Family Relations, 44, 402-411. Walker, A. J., Pratt, C. C., & Oppy, N. C. (1992). Perceived reciprocity in family caregiving. Family Relations, 41, 82-85. Walker, A. J., Shin, H. Y., & Bird, D. N. (1990). Perceptions of relationship change and caregiver satisfaction. Family Relations, 39, 147-152. Walker, A. J., Thompson, L., & Morgan, C. S. (1987). Two generations of mothers and daughters: Role position and interdependence. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 195-208. Walker, A. J., Pratt, C. C., Shin, H. Y., & Jones, L. L. (1990). Motives for parental caregiving and relationship quality. Family Relations, 39, 51-56. Ward, R. A., & Spitze, G. (1992). Consequences of parent-adult child coresidence. Journal of Family Issues, 13, 553-572. Wellman, B., & Wortley, S. (1990). Different strokes from different folks: Community ties and social support. American Journal of Sociology, 96, 558-588. Whitbeck, L. B., Hoyt, D. R., & Huck, S. M. (1994). Early family relationships, intergenerational solidarity, and support provided to parents by their adult children. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 49, S85-S94. 79 Whitbeck, L. B., Simons, R. L., & Conger, R. D. (1991). The effects of early family relationships on contemporary relationships and assistance patterns between adult children and their parents. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 46, S330-S337. Young, R. F., & Kahana, E. (1989). Specifying caregiver outcomes: Gender and relationship aspects of caregiving strain. The Gerontologist, 29, 660666. Zarit, S. H., Reeves, K. E., & Bach-Peterson, J. (1990). Relatives of the impaired elderly: Correlates of feelings of burden. The Gerontologist, 20, 649655. Zarit, S. H., Todd, P. A., & Zarit, J. M. (1986). Subjective burden of husbands and wives as caregivers: A longitudinal study. The Gerontologist, 20, 260-266. 80 Appendices 81 Appendix A Caregiving Tasks and Activities In the past 24 hours, which of the following did you do or arrange to have done for your mother? (We will also ask you about how much time or money was involved in each of these activities.) A. Shopping/Errands B. Indoor Maintenance Went grocery shopping Made the bed Ran errands (to bank, cleaners, gas station, etc.) Changed the bed Drove mother somewhere Bought household supplies Bought clothes Made an expensive purchase C. Financial Tasks Paid bills Wrote checks Budgeted, planned, or reviewed expenses D. Food Preparation/Clean-Up Made breakfast Made lunch Made dinner Did the dishes Straightened the house, put things away Cleaned the house (mopped, waxed, dusted, vacuumed) Took out the garbage Did the laundry Ironed clothes Decorated the house, arranged furnishings Did repairs around the house E. Outdoor Maintenance Gardened (pulled weeds, planted vegetables, cut shrubs) Repaired car or other vehicle Washed or waxed car or other vehicle 82 F. Personal Care G. Financial Contributions Fed your mother Paid for groceries Changed or dressed your mother Paid for utilities (gas, water, electricity) Bathed your mother Put your mother to bed Helped use toilet or bed pan Helped your mother exercise Paid for household supplies Paid for clothes Paid rent/mortgage H. Psychological Support Combed your mother's hair Listened to mother Helped with medication Discussed problems with mother Helped in and out of bed Gave understanding Helped to walk Gave advice Put on make-up Cheered up Helped with stairs Improved mother's morale Changed diapers Gave warmth Helped in and out of chairs Gave affection I. Leisure Activities Read to your mother Gave respect J. Bureaucratic Mediation Took mother to church Got information for mother Took mother to a meeting Made application for mother Took mother to visit friends Arranged for service for mother Took mother to restaurant Filled out forms Went for a walk with mother Talked with doctors for mother 83 I. Leisure Activities (continued) K. General Activities Played a table game (cards) Talked with mother on telephone Took to park or picnic Talked/chatted with mother in person Took shopping or browsing Took for a drive Adapted from Atkinson and Huston (1984). 84 Appendix B Intimacy Scale 1. We want to spend time together. 2. She shows that she loves me. 3. We're honest with each other. 4. We can accept each other's criticisms of our faults and mistakes. 5. We like each other. 6. We respect each other. 7. Our lives are better because of each other. 8. We enjoy the relationship. 9. She cares about the way I feel. 10. We feel like we're a unit. 11. There's a great amount of unselfishness in our relationship. 12. She always thinks of my best interest. 13. I'm lucky to have her in my life. 14. She always makes me feel better. 15. She is important to me. 16. We love each other. 17. I'm sure of this relationship. 85 Appendix C Attachment Scale 1. We're dependent on each other. 2. We anticipate each other's mood. 3. We nurture each other. 4. I feel like I want to support her. 5. She is closer to me than others are. 6. We're emotionally dependent on each other. 7. When we anticipate being apart, our relationship intensifies. 8. We anticipate each other's needs. 9. Our best times are with each other.