Bjørghild Kjelsvik Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies, Faculty of Humanities University of Oslo Schemas of emergent speech genres in teaching and learning interaction The theoretical background Production: Department of Linguistics, University of Oslo. Photos: Bjørghild Kjelsvik © Design: Alv Reidar Dale. Print: Allkopi, 07/2008 In my PhD-project I have analysed speech genres used in teaching and learning interaction, in two settings in Cameroon. The theoretical frameworks used are Cognitive linguistics (CL) and Communities of Practice theory (Wenger 1998). The main analytical concept of speech genre is taken from (Bakhtin 1986 [1979]). Bakhtin postulated a rigid distinction between language as a formal system and speech communication as always growing chains of communication. Speech genres are the typical forms utterances are cast in, but not part of the language system. From the CL point of view, however, the language system is usage-based and emergent (Langacker 1987) (Hopper 1998)). CL thus alleviates the dichotomy of language and speech communication. The category of speech genres can in CL be seen as schematic linguistic units alongside other such units, all shaped by language usage (Langacker 2001). The advantage of introducing speech genres as a category of linguistic units is the opportunity this offers for handling interaction in a principled way within a CL framework. Analysing teaching interaction in Cameroon – data and research question I videotaped teaching interaction in Cameroonian villages and classrooms in order to look for the speech genres of these communication situations. Speech genres would be “recurrent commonalities of discourse”, schemas creating discourse expectations as interaction proceeds. My main question was: How are speech genres used as social and cognitive resources by the particpants in teaching and learning interaction? It is mainly the school interaction which is presented in this poster. Applying the CDS construct to teaching and learning interaction A teacher is normally more knowledgeable than a learner, and both parties are aware of this. The CDS will thus contain the Teacher’s additional knowledge besides the shared knowledge, and the utterances will focus on T’s knowledge in his turns of the interaction, striving to make this knowledge available to the Learner, see Fig. 4 for a depiction of this discourse situation. Working on actual discourse data: The CDS construct Importantly, in CL all linguistic units are seen as abstracted from usage events. Linguistic units are schematized patterns of action imminent in their usage events. This view makes the study of actual utterances imperative. Langacker (2001) introduces the notion of the Current Discourse Space (CDS) to show discourse as a continuous flow of Fig. 1: Usage event captured in a CDS, grounded by language use through time, each CDS upa Speaker and a Hearer, with shared knowledge. dating the discourse. Fig. 1 thus captures a given moment of an unfolding discourse as a bounded utterance or usage event. The CDS is grounded in its participants (minimally Speaker and Hearer) and their location and circumstances. The utterance may focus on any aspect of the world; it indexes the Ground by using pronouns and deictics, and other linguistic devices relating to the actual situation, together with intonation, gaze, gestures and body positions. Through the usage event the participants pay joint attention to some aspect of the world, as if looking through a viewing frame. This viewing frame contains both conceptualisation and vocalisation channels, see Fig. 2. Any aspect of a usage event can in principle be abstracted as a linguistic unit, emerging by way of being a recurrent commonality over many usage events. This applies to both verbal and nonverbal means of communication. The CDS schema shown in figs. 1 and 2 is a way of depicting the elements taking part in the process. Fig. 2: An utterance creates a frame for joint attention to some aspects of the world. Fig. 3: A classroom teaching situation in Cameroon: A boy stands to answer the teacher’s question in class. Fig. 4: CDS schema showing a sequence of utterances in the interaction of a Teacher and a Learner, the current utterance being the T’s utterance appearing in the middle frame. Sentence completion and complete sentences in classroom interaction A much used speech genre in the classes I observed was to start a sentence or word and let it hang unfinished to get the class to supply the rest in a choral response. Another common characteristic of school talk in Cameroon is the emphasis on using complete sentences. In the following transcript from a lesson on the qualities of drinking water, the preference for complete sentences meshes with the sentence completion genre in an unfortunate way for Oumarou (8–9 years old) as he strives to understand what the teacher is after. He first answers the teacher’s question of How is the water of the stream? without standing up, then must repeat three times, saying the correct answer Dirty four times before our excerpt. But the teacher is still not satisfied, he wants his complete sentence repeated by Oumarou in a model-and-imitation sequence (translated from French): the other hand, the school rule for students to stand up straight while answering questions in class goes against the normal Nizaa body posture in similar situations outside school, where they will crouch in a respectful stance to adults. In some cases this translates into the hybrid stance seen in Fig. 5. Such nonverbal practices are part of the speech genres of the class. Fig. 5: Standing up and bowing forward – showing respect in two different ways simultaneously. A speech genre of negative comparison Below is an example of a speech genre schema with two different conceptualisations from the same lesson on drinking water. Lessons use to have a little text formulated by the teacher to be learned by heart by the students. At the end of the lesson they are supposed to be able to quote this target text back when asked. In this session, the teacher several times used negative comparisons. He explained that drinking water was uncoloured and tasteless by way of comparing it to maize gruel and sugar with a sentence frame of “<y> is not like <x>”. These explanations were not meant as target text to be learned, but as side text enlarging the students’ notion of the topic. However, some of the students were not able to tell side texts from target texts and gave “<y> is not like <x>” answers back when asked about what clean water is like. This situation is schematically shown in Fig. 6 and 7 below. The only difference between the two schemas are in the conceptualisation channel of Information structure. Transcript 1: Practical Hygiene, Mipom Public School 23rd Feb 2006, turns (27)–(34) (27) Teacher Teacher: (28) Oumarou: (29) Teacher: (30) Somebody: (31) Oumarou: (32) Teacher: (33) Oumarou: (34) Issa: 02.48.6 Repeat! [1.4] The water of the stream is– [0.3] 02.51.4 (standing, looks forward, glances at camera) Dirty. [0.2] 02.52.4 (stronger, with handbeat) Repeat, (overlapping low talk in class) The water of the stream is– = 02.54.3 (not very loud) = The water of the strea– 02.55.5 The water of the stream is– [0.7] 02.57.9 –is how then? [0.1] 02.58.7 (looks around) Dirty. (glances at teacher) 03.02.9 (stands up besides Oumarou) The water of the stream is dirty. [0.2] In turn (27) the teacher tells Oumarou to repeat, and starts off the the sentence he wants him to say. However, he lets it hang incompleted, and Oumarou takes his cue from this rather than the repeat order, thus mixing the speech genres of sentence completion and student imitation of modeled language. Other students pick up what the teacher wants, and finally another boy submits the correct answer. The sequence illustrates how differing conceptualisations of the speech genres can lead to misunderstanding and frustration for both teacher and students. The transcript also gives glimpses of nonverbal communication: Oumarou’s avoids gazing directly at the teacher to show respect, as strongly emphasized by his ethnic group, the Nizaa. On References: - Bakhtin, M. (1986 [1979]). Speech genres and other late essays. Austin, University of Texas Press. - Hopper, P. J. (1998). Emergent Grammar. The new psychololgy of language. Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure. M. Tomasello. Mahwah, New Jersey/London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: 155–176. - Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar. Vol. 1, Theoretical prerequisites, Stanford University Press. - Langacker, R. W. (2001). “Discourse in Cognitive Grammar.” Cognitive Linguistics 12(2): 143–188. - Wenger, Etienne (1998). Communities of practice. Learning, meaning and identity. Cambridge University Press. Fig. 6: The teacher uses the sentence frame of ”<Y> is not like <X>” to broaden the students’ notion of the main theme. It is conceptualised as a side text, extra information. Fig. 7: The students understand the information to be central new information about the topic and part of the main text to be learned. The target text and side text are clear to the teacher, but not to his students. In time, most of the class will probably come to learn this distinction. In the meanwhile they struggle along as the schemas slowly emerge from the repetitions in the unfolding discourse. My research results The microanalyses of interaction in schoolclasses show that speech genres are important orientational frameworks in interaction. They are emergent and become entrenched over many usage events, just as other linguistic units. Groups and individuals with different experiences will have differing conceptualisations of such frameworks for interaction. The Current Discourse Space concept can be used to capture schemas of these units, retaining both their base in actual discourse and their schematic quality.