B6 ..
Penny Hauser-Cram and Darcy B. Mitchell
Two 4-year-olds, Keisha and Kamilah, are each attempt ing to put together a complex puzzle. Keisha tries to ori ent a piece but can't locate the place it belongs; she then chooses another piece, tries to orient it, then re-orients it and persistently searches for its place. Kamilah se lects a different piece, tries to orient it and then throws it down, running to join another activity in the classroom.
W hy does Keisha persist in trying to complete the puzzle while
Kamilah abandons it?
Keisha and Kamilah illustrate how children the same age can display different levels of what is called mastery motivation.
One child will persist in the face of a chal lenging task l another will give up. What do early childhood professionals know about mastery motivation? How does it vary by age, caregiving processes, and dis abiJity? How can early childhood teachers promote it?
Mastery motivation is defined as a "psy chological force that stimulates an individ ual to attempt independently, in a focused and persistent manner, to solve a problem or master a skill or task which is at least moderately challenging for him or her"
(Morgan, Harmon, & Maslin-Cole 1990
1
319). Two key components of this defini tion include attempts to master a task independent of explicit adult instruction and perSistence in attempting to master the task even when solutions aren't im mediately obvious. The child who exhibits high levels of mastery motivation will seek out a challenging task and derive pleasure from the outcome of having persevered with it.
Mastery motivation is considered to be universal and intrinsic to every child, as indicated by developmental theorists including Piaget (1952), White (1959), and Hunt (1965), who all maintained that a child is bom motivated to explore the world. White contended that children have a need to produce an effect nn the environment and that they achj,>' ihis through exploration and play co,,; ) (xl with selection, direction, and pen.,[ence.
White defined the process of eng.1"{'[nent in such activities as effectance or ';.'lellce
motivation and proposed that su6 ',1gage ment helps children acquire a "feeling of
defined effectance motivation as a "desire to solve cognitively challenging problems for gratification inherent in discovering the solution" (370). Harter emphasized several key components: curiosity, preference for challenge, internal criteria of success, and working for one's satisfaction.
A construct similar to mastery moti vation and its relative, effectance motiva tion, was proposed by Dweck (1986) in studies of school-age children. Based on children's approach to challenging tasks, she described some children as "mastery oriented" and others as "helpless." The mastery-oriented children tended to persist on difficult tasks, attribute their failures to factors they could control (e.g.~ effort), have positive expectations for fu ture success even after failure, and display positive or neutral affect while working on challenging tasks.
In contrast, the helpless children tended to give up quickly, attribute fail ures to events out of their control (e.g., ability), have low expectations for success on future similar challenges, and show negative affect during challenges. Dweck
(1986) proposed that some children de velop "learned helplessness" because they believe, based on past experiences, that they have little control over the events that affect them.
More recent work indicates that these two dimensions of mastery motivation can be identified in early childhood. Results from several studies indicate that about 40 percent of kindergartners show the help less pattern in response to achievement based challenges (Zi~gert et al. 2001). This finding indicates that preschool might be a critical time to intervene to prevent the formation of the helpless pattern and promote the development of a mastery oriented one.
Thus, the preschool years are an important time to help children get on a path of being motivated to master tasks as opposed to feeling helpless in the face of challenge.
During the first two years of life, children begin to develop a "self-system" (Kelley,
Brownell, & Campbell 2000); that is, they begin to select activities to engage in, and they begin to evaluate their own perfor mance. Both selection of activities and self evaluation are aspects of motivation.
Developmentally, children progress through several shifts in mastery motiva tion, so that the motivated child behaves differently at different life phases. The mo tivated infant, younger than 6 months, ex plores objects through reaching, mouthing, and visual exploration. Around 9 months, however, infants begin to understand ru dimentary aspects of cause and effect, and the motivated infant begins to engage in goal-directed activity with unfamiliar tasks
(Jennings 1993).
Goal-directed behavior becomes more complex as toddlers gradually learn to seek more distant goals that involve a chain of actions for goal attainment (e.g., filling a bucket with sand to be used in building a castle) (Jennings 2004). At around 18 months, children begin to be able to compare their behavior with a standard (Jennings 1993), and the moti vated toddler attempts to approximate the standard. Moreover, toddlers who are highly motivated often show great pride in self-agency, as displayed in enthusias tic comments and hand claps. During the preschool years, motivated children begin to self-select challenging tasks and prefer tasks that "make them think" to those that are easy to accomplish (Stipek 1996).
Although motivation is considered to be intrinsic, children show individual differ
138
COGNITIVE ANO LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT
ences in such motivation over their early childhood years. The transactions that occur between children and caregivers appear to be critical to children's mastery motivation. Bornstein (1989) delineated two general modes of interaction between children and caregivers that have conse quences for cognitive development: social and didactic. Both appear to be important in the development of mastery motivation.
In a study of twins who were in kin dergarten or first grade, Deater-Deckard and colleagues (2006) demonstrated that although task persistence has genetic influ ences, changes in such persistence could be explained by differences in maternal behavior, with more positive changes occurring when mothers expressed both warmth and support during interaction with their child. Maternal affect, especially attempts to soothe and show empathy when a child demonstrates distress, also appears to predict mastery motivation in preschool-age children with disabilities who were born preterm (Young & Hauser
Cram 2006).
In relation to caregivers' didactic behaviors, studies indicate that parental interference with children's mastery at tempts has deleterious effects on children's mastery motivation. This has been found for both typically developing children
(Frodi, Bridges, & Grolnick 1985; Wachs
1987) <}fld for those with developmental diSabilities (Hauser-Cram 1993). Research ers (e.g., Morgan et a1. 1991) contend that parents who are highly directive may encourage children to be efficient respond ers but not efficient initiators. In contrast, parents who provide "gentle guidance" may assist in children's development of mastery.
Kelley, Brownell, and Campbell (2000) found that mothers who scaffolded their toddler's efforts in attempting challenging tasks (e.g., who held the toy so the child could work on it without it slipping; who asked questions such as "Where do you think that piece goes?") had children who demonstrated greater task persistence in the preschool years. They speculated that maternal support through scaffolding might help children understand that they are able to do the task.
Research findings are mixed as to whether children with disabilities, especially those with cognitive disabilities, show lower lev els of mastery motivation. Several studies now indicate that such toddlers and young preschoolers show levels of motivation equal to those of their typically develop ing peers (Hauser-Cram 1996; Glenn et a1.
2001; Gilmore, Cuskelly, & Hayes 2003).
By the end of preschool, however, lower mastery motivation has been noted for children with cognitive disabilities, at least based on parent reports (Nicols, Atkinson,
& Pepler 2003).
In a longitudinal study of children with developmental disabilities, one study found that mastery motivation measured at age 3 predicted growth in cognitive and daily living skills over a to-year period
(Hauser-Cram et al. 2001), with greater growth for children with higher levels of mastery motivation. This suggests that preschool, again, could be a critical time of intervention for children with disabilities, as it is for typically developing children, in relation to their trajectories of mastery motivation.
Given the importance of children's pre school years in the divergence of pathways regarding mastery motivation, what are
specific ways that preschool teachers can support and enhance mastery motivation?
Most research has focused on the relation between teachers' behaviors and children's mastery motivation. But such investigations give clues as to ways to improve children's mastery motivation in the classroom. In addition, some interven tion studies have been conducted looking at how to increase the task performance of children with disabilities in the preschool classroom (e.g., Karnes, Johnson, & Beau champ 2005).
Several important suggestions emerge from such work:
1. Provide children a choice of activities.
A modest number of choices, rather than no choice or a large number of choices, is optimal in enhancing motivation (Stipek
1996).
2. Provide children with activities that of fer an opportunity to learn rather than only opportunities to be correct or incor rect. For example, provide problem solving tasks, games, or other activities in which there are several possible ways to solve the problems posed.
3. Support children in ways that do not in terfere with their autonomy. Sometimes this requires adults to let children expe rience dead ends when having difficulty rather than the adults anticipating a way to prevent such outcomes. To avoid children's frustration this may need to be coupled with "gentle support," such as a well-timed suggestion (e.g., "Maybe if you tum that piece around ... "), rather than a direct command (e.g., "That piece fits he,re").
4. Use puppets as a way to demonstrate productive cognitive strategies (Kames,
Johnson, & Beauchamp 2005). For example, a puppet can model self-talk in working on a task (e.g., "Maybe if I turn this"), when confronting difficulties
(e.g., "Maybe I could try it a different way"), and after success (e.g., "Hooray,
I did it!").
Despite the growing amount of research on mastery motivation, together with ways of measuring it through parent and teacher reports and direct observation (Morgan,
Harmon, & Maslin-Cole 1990), knowledge about what kinds of interventions might be able to enhance this important behavior is lacking. Given the social organization of classrooms, more information is needed on how children support or undermine each other in mastery motivation, as well as on how classroom structure and rou tines might relate to mastery motivation.
Research on which types of interventions might work best for children with certain attributes or interests is also needed.
Although there are many future avenues to explore in understanding and encouraging mastery motivation in young children, we do know that it is a critical area of development. Given that research ers have found that mastery motivation relates to later academic achievement
(Shiner 2000), a more thorough focus on this construct during the early childhood years seems to be imperative.
Updated from the Research in Review article in the
July 1998 issue of Young Children.
Penny Hauser-Cram is a professor at Boston College.
Darcy 8. Mitchell is the project director and senior research associate of a longitudinal study at Boston
College.
Bornstein, M.H. 1989. Between caretakers and their young: Two modes of interaction and their consequences for cognitive growth. [n [nterac~ tion in human deCleiopment, eds. M.H. Bomstein
& JS
Bruner, 197-214. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Deater-Deckard, K., S.A. PetTi II, LA. Thompson, &
LS. Dethome. 2006. A longitudinal behavioral genetic analysiS of task persistence. De1)eiopmen~ tal Science 9: 498--504.
Dweck, e.S. 1986. Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist 41: 1040--48.
140 COGNITIYE AND LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT
Frodi, A., L. Bridges, & W. Grolnick. 1985. Corre lates of mastery-related behavior: A short-term longitudinal study of infants in their second year. Child Development 56: 1291-98.
Gilmore, L., M. Cuskelly, & A. Hayes. 2003. A comparative study of mastery motivation in young chiLdren with Down's syndrome: Similar outcomes, different proce&<;es? Joumal of Intel
lectual Disability Research 47: 181-90.
Glenn, S., B. Dayus, C Cunningham, & M. Hor gan. 2001. Mastery motivation in children with
Down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and
Practice 7: 52-59.
Harter, S. 1975. DevelopmentaL differences in the manifestation of mastery motivation on problem-solving tasks. Child Development 45:
370--78.
Hauser-Cram, P. 1993. Mastery motivation in
3-year-old children with Down syndrome. In
Mastery motivation in early I;hildhood: Development,
measurement and social processes, ed. D. Messer,
230-50. London: Routledge.
Hauser-Cram, P. 1996. Mastery motivation in toddlers with developmental disabilities. Child
Dewlopment 67: 236-48.
Hauser-Cram, P., ME Warfield, J.P. Shonkoff, &
M.W. Krauss. 2001. Children with disabilities:
A longitudinal study of child development and parent well-being. Monographs of the Society for
Re~earch in Child Development 66, serial no. 266.
Hunt J.McV. 1965. Intrinsic motivation and its role in psychological development. In Nebraska symposium on motivation, ed. n
Levine, 189-282.
Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Jennings, J.D. 1993. Mastery motivation and the formation of self-concept from infancy through childhood. In Mastery motivation in early child hood: Development, measurement and social pro
cesses, ed. D. Messer, 36-·54. London: Routledge.
Jennings, J. D. 2004. Development of goal-directed behaviour and related self-processes in toddlers. lntemational Journal of Behavioural Development
28: 319-27.
Karnes, M.B., LJ Johnson, & K.D.F. Beauchamp.
2005. Reprise: Developing problem solving skills to enhance task persistence of handicapped preschool children. Joumal of Early Intervention
27: 236-46.
Kelley, SA, CA. Brownell, & S.B. CampbeLL 2000.
Mastery motivation and self-evaluative affect in toddLers: Longitudinal relations with maternal behavior. Child Development 71: 1061-71.
Morgan, G.A., N.A. Busch-Rossnagel, CA. Maslin
Cole, & RJ. Harmon. 1991. Mastery motivation tasks: Manual for 15 to 36 month old children.
Bronx, NY: Fordham University Psychology
Department.
Morgan, G.A., RJ. Harmon, & CA. Maslin-Cole.
1990. Mastery motivation: Definition and measurement. Early Education and Development
1: 318-39.
Nicols, A., L Atkinson, & D. Pepler. 2003. Mastery motivation in young children with Down's syndrome: Relations with cognitive and adap tive competence. Journal of Intellectual Disability
Research 47: 121-33.
Piaget,
1952. The origins ofintelligence in children.
New York: International Universities Press.
Shiner, RL. 2000. Linking childhood personality with adaptation: Evidence for continuity and change across time into late adolescence. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychologlj 78: 310--25.
Stipek, DJ 1996. Motivation and instruction. In
Handbook ofeducational psychology, eds. D.C
Berliner & RC Calfee, 85--113. New York: Mac millan Library Reference.
Wachs, TO. 1987. Specificity of environmental action as manifest in environmental correlates of infant's mastery motivation. Developmental
Psychology 23: 782-90.
White, RW. 1959. Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review 66:
297-333.
Young, J.M., & P. Hauser-Cram. 2006. Mother child interaction as a predictor of mastery moti vation in children with disabilities born preterm.
TournaI of Early inten·ention 28: 252-63.
Ziegert, D., J.A. Kistner, R Castro, & B. Robertson.
2001. Longitudinal study of young children's re sponses to challenging achievement situations.
Child Development 72: 609-24.
L.
National Association for the Education of Young Children
Washington, DC