M.I.T. LIBRARIES - DEWEY Dewey HD28 .M414 ) ALFRED P. WORKING PAPER SLOAN SCHOOL OF MANAGEMENT Comparisons and Cooperative R&D Ventures: The Double-Edged Sword of Communication Social Christopher Maureen June 1994 Tucci Lojo L. P. WP # 109-94 MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY 50 MEMORIAL DRIVE CAMBRIDGE, MASSACHUSETTS 02139 The International Center for Research on the Management of Technology Comparisons and Cooperative R&D Ventures: The Double-Edged Sword of Communication Social Christopher Maureen Tucci Lojo L. P. WP June 1994 Sloan © WP# # 109-94 3699-94 1994 Massachusetts Institute of Technology Sloan School of Management Massachusetts Institute of Technology 38 Memorial Drive, E56-390 02139-4307 Cambridge, MA :.•; A3SAC u vS?: ns U'isn tute OF TECHNOLOGY i ', SEP 21 1994 LIBRARIES Acknowledgments The authors wish to acknowledge Cusumano, Anthony Frost, the helpful comments and suggestions of Michael A. Varghese George, Michael Rappa, Maureen Scully, T. Michael Toole, Mary Tripsas, Marcie Tyre, and four anonymous referees. Abstract In this paper, we examine the communication-performance link in the R&D laboratory, with an eye toward the effects of task- and equity-related communications on the social environment. We argue that social comparisons between themselves and their (in which employees make comparisons coworkers) can have deleterious effects on performance under certain conditions, possibly negating the positive effects of increased communication. We performance and then propose mechanisms link, discuss the theory and evidence behind the social comparisonto comparisons, while minimizing the negative. organization structure and human resource promote the positive effects of social These mechanisms take the form of management policies. The conclusion is that while other factors, such as characteristics of the partners, technology, and market, are important in structuring the cooperative theory's consequences Keywords: social may R&D venture, attention to social comparison help the venture realize its full potential. comparison theory, cooperative research and development. 1. Introduction R&D ventures (CRDVs) are becoming a popular complement to in- Cooperative house R&D activities for many For example, Hladik firms. 1985) finds that the ( R&D jumped from in their book Cooperative R&D percentage of joint ventures formed to collaborate on & between 1974 and 1982. Link (1989, p. 13), Bauer, find in a survey of executives and technology managers 10% in that to 20% Manufacturing perceived R&D has risen dramatically in the 1980s. Concomitant with this rising interest in cooperative R&D has come a profusion of importance of cooperative analyses of CRDVs, which have taken several analytical forms. The through case studies from them (for how individual companies succeeded in forming example, Hamel, Doz, examines organizational incentives to & Prahalad work on returns to cooperative 1990; Friedman, Berg & Duncan 1979). study, highlighting the differences disadvantages (Katz The & Ordover 1989). conduct joint outcomes (see Katz 1986; Combs 1990; Sinha empirical first CRDVs and R&D in terms of the profitability of the & Cusumano The 1991). third describes R&D investments (for example. Link & Bauer The fourth approach is 1990; Ouchi & Bolton the comparative case their advantages and 1985).' number of important factors that lead to the success of CRDVs, including lack of competition between partners, complementarity of between partners, reciprocity, profited The second type of analysis between current CRDVs and studies cited above reveal a of these shows skills, trust and lack of opportunistic behavior. Managers must take these factors into account, along with history (e.g., previous relationships between partners, changing conditions of partners) and environmental concerns characteristics, specific technology involved) in structuring the market CRDV. R&D labs from the communications studies pioneered by Allen (1977) and Pelz & Andrews Another important stream of literature emanates (e.g., relating to performance in (1978). In very general terms, ^ these and other authors found that increased communication among researchers related to higher "performance," including higher quality technical results. By applying social psychological analysis to the study of technology strategy, paper explores a little-examined among themselves the paper, issue: the effects ("social comparisons") we argue that while it 'These classifications are somewhat is of the comparisons researchers on motivation and performance in this make CRDVs. In tempting to generalize the lessons of traditional arbitrary', since some of the papers ^Exceptions are discussed below. 1 — fail into more than one category. communication studies of scientists and engineers social comparisons made among that are indirect relevance to managerial decision relevant case that is CRDVs, those lessons may not hold R&D venture as they do for the single strongly for the cooperative The to making in setting (HRM) more policies thn it likely, indirectly-relevant case is that may be modified to take social To develop an CRDVs, we we would by evaluating according first may is directly- lead to different partner human to the once a venmre both direct and up a CRDV. The an understanding of social comparisons choice, location decisions, structures, staffing policies, or firm's research labs. may have researchers resource management above-mentioned entered into, the factors. HRM how social we how and why social comparisons affect performance in Section in 2, in general. reevaluate the connection between communications, social comparisons, and performance managers can use policies comparisons affect performance review the communications-performance connection. Then, Finally, in Section 3, The comparisons into consideration. understanding of discuss the theory of as to create in light of the theory, and propose mechanisms that and modify CRDVs so they reach Section 4 their full potential. presents the conclusions of the paper. 1.1. Evidence of the relationship between communication and performance For at least thirty years, scholars have studied the relationship between communication among researchers the individual and project levels. effective and efficient (scientists The and engineers) and Rc&D productivity literature suggests oral communication method of transferring information (March Marquis 1969). One implication of & Simon is at both a highly 1958; Myers & research depends upon this finding is that effective information processing capability or information "flow" within a lab (Allen & Cohen 1969). Pelz eleven & Andrews (1976) studied the communication patterns of 1300 researchers in R&D labs. They asked questions about number of colleagues consulted. performance for each scientist (e.g., They the frequency of also gathered subjective and objective measures of judgments of publication frequency). Their analysis communication and the showed scientific contribution, patents that, at researchers had higher scientific contribution the an individual more frequent level, and PhD-level their contact with colleagues. Similar relationships were found between communication and other performance measures. Performance also increased with the number of close colleagues within the same group (up to nine), and for the group. In other words, Pelz correlated with the & Andrews found number of close colleagues that scientists' number of close colleagues with outside the performance was positively whom they consulted. Whereas Pelz & Andrews provide strong evidence communications on performance at the extend the findings to the project level. scientists' oral of an individual level, other scholars have sought to Tushman communication and subjective R&D laboratory employing 345 for the positive effect of (1978), for example, collected data on project-level performance rankings in a study Distinguishing between tasks focused researchers. on research, development, and technical service, Tushman found high-performing that research-oriented teams communicated significantly more within their own project and division of the lab than low-performing teams. Allen (1977) found additional evidence confirming the Pelz project-level performance. Technical solutions generated & Andrews results for by researchers in matched pairs of parallel projects were rated for quality, and the frequency and level of their oral were recorded. Allen's analysis shows communications within and outside their projects that the so-called "high-performers" communicated far more with researchers within project than "low-performers" did, even controlling for different levels of effort the (i.e., communications per hour of effort). There is general agreement correlation exists between There are, all of these studies that a strong positive communication and performance both the individual and group level. among increase communication among R&D organization, at This has led to recommendations for management to technical staff in order to increase performance. however, some inconsistencies In addition to the findings cited above, Allen external contacts (by in the in the found that data that merit further examination. more frequent communication with which he means colleagues outside the organization, not including vendors and customers) was associated with poorer performing groups. Furthermore, Tushman's data indicate that high performance less project-level on technical service tasks communication than low performance. is associated with In the next section, we discuss a potential explanation for these empirical puzzles. 1.2. Two types of information exchange R&D is a highly complex endeavor that invariably involves the exchange of very task-specific information between researchers. In addition to communicating information of a scientific or technical nature ("task-related" information), researchers also communicate more general information about themselves and their coworkers relative standing of individuals in terms of skill, pay, status, etc. of information as "equity-related." The argument assumption that communicating it is increase performance in We in this We on paper hinges on the propose that task-specific information exchange does R&D projects (as demonstrated by the studies cited above). — — 3 the will refer to this type communicate one kind of information without difficult to the other. made that provides data however the exchange of equity-related information may have either beneficial or detrimental effects on performance, as illustrated in Figure equity-related communication can potentially 1. The negative effects of outweigh the benefits of exchanging task- related information. Performance Task-Related Information + /- C oramu nic alio n Equity-Related Information Social Comparisons Figure Effects of two kinds of information on performance 1. Allen's and Tushman's findings the theory that , in some communication and performance instances apparently inconsistent with are positively correlated, can be reinterpreted in light of social comparison theory. Social comparison theory provides an appropriate vocabulary and framework for analyzing outcomes level (Wood 1989) at the individual between researchers having extensive contact with associates who may be different on various dimensions — ladders, for example. more or less competent, or subject to different pay scales and career Such comparisons, if not managed properly, may lead to conflict, decreased motivation, and poor performance. What happens when we more firms by necessity? Such ventures provide an interactions and comparisons between researchers. actually be detrimental to project performance, can be a particularly challenging task 2. R&D venture that involves two or consider the cooperative in institutional structure for If ongoing more frequent communication may managing the interaction making a venture between partners successful. Social comparison theory People have a strong tendency to organizations are no exception. Theories comparisons were developed in the compare themselves with others, and scientists in conceming how and why people make 1950s by Festinger —4— ( 1954), among others. Early social comparison theory held as its basic tenets that people make comparisons objective information about themselves, in an environment that since been extended to include goals beyond in order to collect The theory has neutral. is the collection of objective information address the influence of potentially active social environments (Wood and to 1989). Here we use the term "social comparison theory" as a general theory describing why people compare themselves with others. 3 In compare themscives with to we discuss first the dimensions along we show how the effects of the comparisons. Finally, why whom they by the choice of people with others, followed compare themselves. Then we examine made and this section, people select which comparisons empirical work are is consistent with the theory. 2. 1. Reasons for social comparisons Wood discusses three principal motivations individuals have for comparing themselves with others: (1) self-evaluation, (2) self-improvement, and (3) enhancement. Self-evaluation refers to self- people seeking accurate and unbiased estimates of their own may not always receive accurate and unbiased estimates. In social settings, individuals' abilities. only estimates may This was the basis for Festinger's original theory. In be derived from comparing themselves with others — people reality, a clearly inaccurate and biased estimate, but perhaps the only one readily available. People in research organizations may be especially prone to compare themselves to others for the purpose of self-evaluation because the uniqueness and complexity of their tasks makes determine difficult to how they are performing. Self-enhancement refers to people using comparisons to protect their egos or esteem and it In this case, individuals to build feelings of self-worth. unbiased estimates of their abilities. With this goal, may self- not even want people tend to ignore evidence detrimental to their egos and accept evidence that bolsters them. This effect has found broad empirical support (1985) find that almost in the literature. all As an example, Wood, breast cancer patients interviewed Taylor, and Lichtman compared themselves spontaneously to less fortunate patients and not to healthy people. To place this in the context of a research organization, scientific communities can be highly competitive (for resources, recognition, in etc.), and researchers may find themselves conditions that promote comparisons of this type. Self-enhancement, however, generally not regarded as a constructive goal; thus, research managers discourage such comparisons if ^For a more detailed discussion, refer may want to they possibly can. to Masters & Smith (1987) and Nagata & Crosby (1991). is fe^imeroyenM refers ut,l,zes self-evaluation to .he over utne (with evaluation) and combtnes it companson of oneself ,o chers all with learning ,o better oneself of the biases and inaccuracies inherent in such antJ supenor ability. As an example. Nosanchuk who wanted rat to improve deriving insp.ratton from people with & Enckson (1985) show that bridge players almost always chose to look er than equal or infenor players' strategies. self-tmprovement 2.2. thetr gan,e is clearly the I, From most destrable goal of at better players' strateg.es a research social managers point of v,ew compansons. Dimensions of social comparisons Social compadsons as a targe. ^ along these ,s are made between the individual "dimens.ons" of imponance to the tndividu^. the dimension under consideratton, which .nd,v,dual is most concented. At the same time, ocustng on a "related" dtmenston. dimensions and how difficult and one or more people chosen is The most impotrant of the dimension about which the the individu^ must choose the target In this section, we discuss both of these by Wnds of they are to assess. According to theoty, social comparisons are made along dimensions that are relevant to the self-esteem of the comparer. Esteem-relevance .s based on how similar the arget (or companson-other") is, and how imponant the dimension under consideration is to the subject. Examples of possible dimensions of interest for researchers may be technical skill and knowledge, recognition from peers, autonomy in setting research agendae. and perceived value to the organization. The grea.er ,he esteem-relevance of,he d,mens,on. the stronger the effects of the social comparison mil be Related or surrounding dimensions determine how similar the comparer is of the comparison. Examples of related dimensions are age, educational background, organizational tenure, and other demographic characteristics These target characteristics are usually apparent to individuals even numbered their identification ,o the working together Some companies have badges by hire date, producing the unintended result of announcing an individu^'s organizational tenure. Related dimensions may be predictive of performance on the dimension of interest, or may simply indicate that the comparer and comparison-other are similar in some way. For example, gender does not predict skill but women nught tend to compare themselves with other women on the dimension of slall because they view other women as similar to themselves. Related dimens.ons an> no, esteem-relevant, at least not in relation to the companson being made. 77,. ,...,.. the stmtlanty of,he comparison-other. the stronger the effects of the social comparison .ill be 'Targets are discussed in the next section. —6 Information on comparison-others' standing on the primary dimension of interest is not always readily available. For example, scientists do not have access to complete data on their peers. collect; the Even public information, such more closely guarded which formal measures dimension of facts may such as salary be the all more may costly to In cases in so. are unavailable, people will generally use informal In the case of salary this interest. number of publications, may be as measures of the involve seeing what neighborhood they live in or what kind of car they drive. Performance along dimensions of others' performance interest are generally on the same dimension. Although measurable on absolute scales, the absolute value relative deprivation has consistently is meaningful only relative somewhat & Watts unhappy and who were making more money resentful be literature on found that beyond a very low stipend, people almost 1983; Crosby 1976). For example, Messe with ten years' tenure The irrelevant. exclusively care about their salaries only relative to what others Messe may salaries, skill, etc. to when confronted make (Olson 1986; et al. & Watts found that executives became than they had ever anticipated with the circumstance of a newly-hired employee with a higher salary. 2.3. Targets of social comparisons According to social comparison-other, with comparison theory, an individual whom to make a comparison. This target will vary the goal of the comparison, as discussed in Section make comparisons with others who selects a target, or 2. 1 The theory . some way. For are similar in motive, the theory would predict selection of comparison-others in some related dimension. Individuals can then dimension of interest relative to the comparison-other. motive, the target will probably be someone dimension of will be interest, someone who Another in an gauge who is their depending on calls for individuals to the self-evaluation who presumed similar are own performance on In the case of the presumed superior the self-improvement in terms of the and can therefore serve as a model. For self-enhancement, the is assumed scientist or to be worse off or inferior on the dimension of manager of similar responsibility R&D lab, since responsibility is highly observable. of reporting employees, and total is target interest. a likely comparison target number Position in the hierarchy, number of employees under a manager are readily apparent in an organization. Likewise, the relative authority of individuals to make capital acquisitions or decide on research directions, and even their personal influence, are well- known among members of an organization. 7 — 2.4. Effects of social comparisons Majorat (1991) examine the conditions under which social comparisons have al. positive and negative effects on individuals in terms of self-esteem, motivation, stress, and satisfaction. The authors and downward. dimension of distinguish between An upward interest, i.e., downward comparison is comparison the one is comparison in which two basic types of comparisons one is which the in target is the target is inferior, downward comparisons have will be in the future. Perceived control feeling of the stability of the dimension as a measure and to is The is potential critical how manipulable perceived control, the subject's perception of outcome has been and the and thus the comparison effects that are either positive or negative, as illustrated in Figure 2. is on unfavorable for the comparer. Similarly, a favorable for the comparer. Both upward and determining factor suf)erior — upward the based on the general on the subject's perceived ability maintain or change the relative standing. High Morale Favorable Stress or Complacency Social Comparisons Competitiveness Unfavorable Low Figure In the first 2. Positive and negative effects of social case, individuals feel that they compare favorably self-relevani dimensions. If they feel that their relative standing control, they experience satisfaction and believe that their superior standing they may feel is is Morale comparisons to similar others stable and under enhanced self-esteem. Alternatively, if on their they subject to change, and they have httle control over vulnerable and experience symptoms of stress or depression. Another possible effect not addressed by Major et al. is complacency. If relative standing is it, perceived to be stable, but not a direct consequence of personal respond by reducing effort and resting on their undeserved In the second case, individuals on self-relevant dimensions. If their control, increased striving and competitiveness compare unfavorably will result. change regardless of may laurels. they believe their relative standing feel that their standing is unlikely to dissatisfaction, depression, feel that they effort, individuals is to similar others changeable and under However, if individuals their efforts, they will experience and jealousy of those with higher relative standing. Empirical support 2.5. Empirical research on social comparison theory has largely confirmed the effects of interpersonal comparisons on motivation and morale. In this section, the major studies showing the comparison-performance we examine some link. A classic example of the deleterious effects of interpersonal comparisons provided by Stouffer et al. (1949), who examined respect to their pay and promotions. policemen, who were the satisfaction of They described of a situation in Army is units with which military almost never promoted, were far more satisfied with their promotion system than servicemen in the "air corps," in which advancement was often rapid. The authors argued that since individuals in the air corps compared themselves to their friends whom they saw promoted,^ such comparisons led to low morale. In contrast, military policemen were almost never promoted, so there were no negative comparisons to be made. Paradoxically, even though the comparisons hurt More air corps was absolutely better off, social their morale. recent studies of the effect of social comparisons on motivation and performance manipulated both the goals of completing a task and the perceived subjects in an experimental setting (Elliott & Dweck positive evaluation of performance, subjects with 1988). When low perceived the goal ability was of to receive a ability^ exhibited "helpless" behavior by avoiding challenges and not persisting with solutions. This can be interpreted as a comparison avoid negative feedback. high (i.e., on an esteem-relevant dimension, because the goal was On subjects initially the other hand, when the manipulated perceived ability was learning (i.e., was compared favorably with comparison-others), subjects exhibited "mastery-oriented" behavior with high motivation and persistence. goal to When the lower esteem-relevance), the performance of subjects with both high and low perceived ability was not significantly affected. ^Even though promotion in general was rapid compared to the MPs, at any given time only number of people were promoted, leaving the majority dissatisfied. ^Perceived ability was manipulated by assigning the subjects a bogus score on a pretest. —9 a small Ames (1984) shows markedly similar compared with significant increase in helpless behavior "individualistic" (i.e., less ? esteem- & Leggett (1988) report a series of quasi-experiments Dweck relevant) environments. "competitive" environments lead to results: that confirm previous studies: performance goals and comparisons are associated positively with avoiding challenges, perceptions of low negative attitudes, and low motivation ability, and persistence. Although the works cited above assume performance, it that comparisons affect motivation and also true that performance affects comparisons, thus forming a feedback is loop. Farris (1969) noted that performance codetermines environment, and actually precedes some aspects of and performance strictly one-way? Even though it. Farris many aspects of the social Is the link found between comparisons that "contact" (communication) was an antecedent of performance and not a consequent, social comparison theory provides some reasons why performance may also drive comparisons a feedback loop. From two ways: through the perception that there Figure 2, we can see that performance outcomes can play a in role in development of a similar comparison-other and through the is little control over the outcome. Through performance, a formerly dissimilar comparison-other may be developed. For example, a successful employee may be promoted into a position of higher responsibility or salary, thereby bringing that person into the sphere of similarity to other managers. Likewise, an underperforming employee may be held back from responsibility, thereby becoming more similar to a cohort with lower organizational tenure. The change rapidly advancing due in status employee (or to group of employees) may lead others to believe that there no chance of competing well along a certain dimension, leading We can performance. see, therefore, that the potential for a to and so on. The and consequences could snowball performance differentials, to poor motivation and feedback loop which may lead comparisons may lead effects A performance may also affect perceived control. to to exists. Social more comparisons, unforeseen sizes if not anticipated and planned for in advance. 2.6. Summary Thus, as illustrated in Figure 3, theory and empirical evidence suggest that highly esteem-relevant comparisons combine with low perceived control to produce negative effects, regardless of apparent when the subject that the subject will the subject is whether the comparison is remain is worse-off than the inferior. favorable or unfavorable. This target, since However, perhaps is more low perceived control means surprisingly, it is also true when superior to the target, since the subject will either feel unable to maintain superiority, or will expect to remain superior effortlessly. — 10 is Similarity of Comparison-Other EsteemRelevance Importance of Dimension Strength of Effect Behavior Positive or Negative Effect Stability of Measure Perceived Control Ability to Change Relative Standing Figure 3. Summary of important factors in the effects of social comparisons In what form do the effects take the effects of social form of actions and comparisons manifest themselves? Negative feelings regarding the self ("self-directed"), the dimension ("dimension-directed"), and the target ("comparison-other-directed"). Self- directed responses include lower self-esteem, depression, stress, and dissatisfaction, while dimension-directed responses include decreased motivation, playing down the importance of the dimension, and claims of other more relevant dimensions. Comparison-otherdirected responses include jealousy, avoidance, envy, derogation, and, occasionally, even violence (Salovey 1991). Such behavior in the workplace would obviously not be conducive Do to high levels of productivity. dissatisfaction performance because at many the and lack of motivation at the individual level translate into group level? The case of CRDVs is backed by the evidence cited above, of the comparisons are group-based; that is, the comparisons are precisely by employees of different groups and have similar effects on group. The bipolar nature of two distinct cultures interacting in the consider social comparisons' effect on performance — 11 poor at the group all CRDV level. made members of the allows us to 3. Influencing social comparisons now examine Let us Employees of two more or the relevance of this theory to structuring the CRDV. working together may be subject distinct organizations to different policies regarding compensation, mcentives, career advancement, research autonomy, and so on. They will naturally tend to these dimensions and will experience some compare themselves with each other on of the effects described above. Will these set be constructive or destructive to the overall mission of the CRDV? How can mitigate the negative effects and create an organization that effects? Recent work invite or force social important — and in social comparison theory holds is n.anagers conducive to the positive may that the social enviroimient comparisons (Wood 1989). In other words, the social environment the individual's perceptions and actions are affected by assumption of this paper holds that it. is A major managers can and do have some influence on the social environment through various mechanisms. In light of social comparison theory, CRDVs require attention to interpersonal CRDVs may be communication among structured in such a way comparisons, and performance. While manipulate organization structure and in CRDVs, these own in Even if the problems before they social we human resource policies^ (Nelson While the partners in a CRDV CRDV & Winter 1982), are each internally up their cannot be freely structured, however, an may help focus management's attention on fully develop. We first show how the structure of the CRDV kinds of comparisons that can be important for a venture's success. some human resource management we ability to discuss various options or mechanisms that affect the level of comparisons made. environment. Finally, special particular, most cases, history constrains a firm's understanding of social comparison theory In this section, and engineers. In histories, they are generally less constrained in setting joint research endeavors. potential management pay as to optimize the effects of communication, social are subject to negotiation. constrained by their scientists that policies and their influence We influences the then examine on the comparison provide recommendations for minimizing negative social comparisons and maximizing positive ones. 3.1. Organization structure Crt^Vs can take many organizational forms. For example, partners can take an equity stake in an entirely new enterprise or form a general partnership; partners can be competitors, suppliers, or customers; partners can be from different countries or the ^We are calling the judicious planning and implementation of the three supposed to be different from "organization structure," which — 12 is same mechanisms "structuring." This one of the mechanisms. is country; the term of the cooperation can be limited or unlimited in duration; panners can share a lab or perform research solo; and so on. The subsequent discussion concentrate on those aspects of organization structure that most partners in the and collocation. Internationalization CRDV most interesting and find comparisons: employee motivation, and organizational incentives: critical to social internationalization we will refers to the degree to which the are based in different countries. Collocation refers to whether the collaborative research conducted jointly is same in the laboratory. Internationalization Mowery (1992) identifies four types of collaboration most amenable to intemational partners:^ (1) research alone; (2) exchanging proven technologies across a single product line; (3) joint functions shows development of one or more products; and (e.g., (4) collaboration across different marketing and research). Using examples from these four types, the conditions under which international collaboration direct foreign investment (DFI). Intemational collaboration the technology in question is preferred efficient scale, when preferred to licensing or is preferred to licensing when complex, new, or has weak patent protection. Intemational alliances are preferred to DFI in cases with minimum is Mowery and high weak appropriability, weak control, large site-specific learning. Intemational collaboration contracts are limited or difficult to enforce, and when it is difficult is also to ascertain the value of participants' contribution. Comparing intemational and domestic CRDVs, Tucci (1994) proposes three main differences between them: (1) the level of competition (degree of substitutability of products) between firms in an international collaboration can often be lower than between domestic partners; (2) it is more intemational CRDV; and (3) results in it is difficult to coordinate more and transfer difficult for firms outside an intemational collaboration. The competition effect three, with the benefits results in an of the is the CRDV to obtain the most important of the of lower competition often outweighing the costs of linguistic and cultural differences. Given the above, how does the internationalization of the venture affect the social comparison environment? The intemational CRDV provides by working through esteem-relevance in Figure 3. far fewer social comparisons The intemational venture makes the comparison-others less similar because language and cultural differences are barriers development of a shared identity. Internationalization also makes comparable. For example, using pay as a comparison yardstick ^Although structuring the firm is CRDV often occurs after the partner searching for a partner with whom is to cooperate. 13 — is the measures to the less a problem in the chosen, there are occasions wherein the international case since buying power can be pay differentials rates fluctuate. Therefore different in different countries CRDV in the international and exchange are less likely to generate dissatisfaction than they would in the domestic case.^ Collocation To decide whether or not to collocate researchers incentives for truth-telling and coordination / promising results are lower than they would be scientists often "trade own if tell the research not just an organization-level is between On When the other participants about were conducted jointly phenomenon, since individual knowledge" and withhold recent information firm (von Hippel 1987; Schrader 1991). the tradeoff administration must be understood. performing research separately, the incentives for parties to (Ciborra 199 1).'^ This CRDV, in a that would hurt their the other hand, joint research involves higher setup and administrative costs, such as procuring property at a "neutral" location and monitoring the venture to ensure that results are transferred back to the participating companies. It is clear that joint research provides a greater opportunity for interaction, communication, development of a shared maximizing communication would lead identity, to joint within the venture would call for separate and social comparisons. While R&D, highly unequal treatment of groups R&D due to negative social comparison effects. Social comparison theory cannot and should not be considered in isolation, but putting diverse researchers in the same room may negate some of the communication, particularly there if comparison and/or individuals' 3.2. Human beneficial effects of frequent low perceived control over the dimensions of is relative standings on it. resource policies If partners in CRDVs have different organizational policies concerning esteem- relevant dimensions, social comparisons between researchers associated with the different companies have the potential to create negative effects on motivation and performance. Compensation, resources, autonomy, and control of research results are four areas that are highly esteem-relevant for researchers. Compensation . Although wages may be difficult to compare across national boundaries, as discussed above, other forms of compensation are readily comparable. Benefits, holidays, vacation, and expected have lower esteem-relevance than wages working hours are examples. While these may in domestic CRDVs, differences can nonetheless "Of course, these very factors that preclude social comparisons also make oral communication an and somewhat expensive medium (March & Simon 1958; Katz & Kahn 1966). '^Ouchi (1984) points out that this problem is especially acute 14 — among American firms. inefficient produce friction among researchers, especially in international ventures, in which they may be the most salient points of reference. Resources office and engineers require a great deal of resources: equipment, Scientists . and laboratory space, support well-funded likely to CRDVs have budgets to attend conferences, be an esteem-related dimension of comparison. . freedom ability to defines two kinds of autonomy: strategic to define the direction of one's choose how to own research; of)erational pursue research directions. First, natural for seems employees that strategic to compare autonomy is this level is less fairly rare that, publish and publish is some The opportunity the project. in fact, it is it to other to publish for) maturity or organizational trust. seems within the same research organization, are not," but is observable, but not invisible to employees. Both at first glance to be an unlikely dimension of comparison because most companies either allow It is is some subset of a of responsibility allowed, and, forms of autonomy can be seen as a sign of (or reward . autonomy one of the most easily observed variables available employees. Operational autonomy Control of results autonomy employees desire the responsibility to determine the course of the research project, or It is Even etc. resource constraints; thus, these items are highly prized and are Autonomv Bailyn (1984) the staff, travel some people it are allowed to CRDV. certainly a possibility within a or they do not. Ability to also highly observable, and so can be viewed as a potential dimension of comparison. Approval to present results at technical conferences similar to the opportunity to publish. Patent ownership is another example quite a third possibility related to is control over research results and can be a powerful motivator, especially if royalties are involved. 3.3. Recommendations What while at the actions can managers take to minimize negative effects of social comparisons same time maximizing managers should consider the the positive ones? In choosing partners for effects of internationalization on the CRDVs, comparison environment. Researchers from different countries will view each other as less similar than partners in domestic ventures, and so the strength of both positive and negative effects will be diminished. Furthermore, some of the measures used stable in international Once In general, in social CRDVs, thereby lowering perceived comparisons will be control. a partner has been chosen, managers can address the structure of the we recommend creating in CRDV. a shared identity to the greatest extent possible, by collocating the collaborating researchers and standardizing the policies concerning 'This does occur, for example, less defense-related aerospace companies. — 15 human resource management. Although this will increase the similarity of the researchers, and thus the strength of the effects of social comparisons between them, taking actions to improve perceived control will favor the desirable positive effects, and discourage negative effects. Human Many resource management policies are critical for enhancing perceived control. of the comparison dimensions reflect the organization's values and priorities. These can be conveyed through chosen methods of performance evaluation and managerial acknowledgments of individual effort. If employees have significant input into these choices, they will feel greater control over them. Furthermore, perceived control over on the dimensions of relative standings interest can be increased by providing organizational resources for self-improvement efforts. For example, allocating time and financial support for additional training or attendance at technical meetings should increase individuals' ability to increase their standing on dimensions that will gready benefit the organization, as well as the employees' self-esteem and morale. 4. Conclusions In this paper, we examined the communication-performance link laboratory, with an eye toward the effects of task- and equity-related the social environment. We argued that in cooperative R&D in the communications on R&D ventures, social comparisons can have effects on performance, possibly negating the positive effects of increased communication. performance We discussed the theory and evidence behind the social comparison- link, and then proposed mechanisms to promote the positive effects of social comparisons, while minimizing the negative. This conceptual paper has also provided a base from which future research can spring. In particular, one could seek empirical support for the effect of social on performance both within individual laboratories and CRDVs. In addition, empirical tests could be performed on the effect of organization structure, staffing, and social comparisons HRM policies comparisons and overall communication patterns. Finally, further work development could center on how unique aspects of different groups, such as engineers, In may in theory scientists and require unique organization structures and policies. summary, managers responsible cooperative on for deciding upon the structure of the R&D venture should take into consideration many factors, including characteristics of the partners communications and the environment. effects of organizational structure should be considered. If the partner In addition, both the direct and indirect and human resource policy mechanisms has been decided, the manager should concentrate on the structure of the venture. If the structure has been decided, the — 16 manager should pay careful attention to to social human resource comparisons allows the management mechanisms. Thus CRDV to reach its full potential. — 17 — it may be that attention References Allen, T. Managing 1977. J., the & Flow of Technology. MiT Information flow Cohen, S., 1969. Allen, T. J. Quarterly, 14: 12-19. Ames, C, 1984. Achievement attributions in R&D Press, labs. Autonomy 1984. Management Working in the pp. Administrative Science and self-instructions under competitive and individual..5tic goal structures. Journal of Educational Bailyn, L., Cambndge, 320 R&D Industrial Psychology, 76: 478^87. Lab. Mil Sloan School of Paper, No. 1592-84. C, 1991. Alliances as learning experiments: cooperation, competition, and in hightech [sic] industries. In: L. K. Mytelka (Ed.), Strategic Partnerships: States, Firms, and International Competition, Fairleigh Dickinson University Press, Rutherford: 51-77. Ciborra, change L., 1990. Cooperative research and innovative Micro-Economics, 5: 47-67. Combs, K. A Crosby, F., 1976. 85-113. Dweck, C. S. & model of egoistical activity. relative deprivation. Advances in Applied Psychological Review, 83: Leggett, E. L., 1988. A social-cognitive approach to motivation and Psychological Review, 95 (2): 256-273. personality. Agency theory: an assessment and review. Management Review, 14 (1): 57-74. Eisenhardt, K. M., 1989. & Academy of Dweck, C. S., 1988. Goals: an approach to motivation and achievement. Personality and Social Psychology, 54: 5-12. Journal of Elliott, E. S. FaTis, G. F., 1969. Some antecedents and consequences of scientific performance. Transactions on Engineering Management, 16 (1): 9-16. Festinger, L., 1954. A theory of social comparison processes. Human IEEE Relations, 7: 117- 140. Friedman, P., Berg, S. V., & Duncan, Business, 31 (2): 1979. J., acquisition: joint venture activity and R&D External vs. internal knowledge Journal of Economics and intensitv. 103-110. Hamel, G., Doz, Y. L., & Prahalad, C. K., 1989. Collaborate with your competitors and win. Harvard Business Review, January - February: 133-139. Hladik, K.J., 1985. International Joint Ventures. 134 pp. Katz, D. & Kahn, R., 1966. Lexington Books, Lexington, Mass., The Social Psychology of Organizations. York. — 1! — Wiley, New Katz, M. L., 1986. An analysis of cooperative research and development. of Economics, 17 Katz, (4): Journal R&D & M. Rand 527-543. L., Ordover, J. A., 1990. cooperation and competition. Brookings Papers: Microeconomics, Washington, DC. & Bauer, L. L., 1989. Cooperative Research Lexington Books, Lexington, Mass., 10 pp. Link, A. N. in US Manufacturmg. 1 Major, B., Testa, M., & Bylsma, W., 1991. Responses to upward and downward social comparisons: the impact of esteem-relevance and perceived control. In: J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social Comparison: Contemporary Theory and Research. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, March, & J. Hillsdale, NJ. Simon, H., 1958. Organizations. Wiley, New York. & J. C. Smith, W. P. (Eds.), 1987. Social Comparison, Social Justice, and Relative Deprivation: Theoretical, Empirical, and Policy Perspectives. Erlbaum. Hillsdale, NJ. Masters, & Watts, B. L., 1983. Complex nature of the sense of fairness: internal standards and social comparison as bases for reward evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46: 684—693. Messe, L. A. Mowery, D. C, International collaborative ventures. 1992. In: N. Rosenberg, R. Landau, and D. C. Mowery (Eds.). Technology and the Wealth of Nations. Stanford University Press, Palo Alto. Myers, Successful Industrial Innovation. S. & Marquis, D., 1969. Foundation, Washington, DC. National Science Nagata, D. & Crosby, F., 1991. Comparisons, justice, and the internment of JapaneseAmericans. In: J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social Comparison: Contemporary Theory and Research. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ. Nelson, R. R. & Winter, S. G., 1982. An Evolutionary Theory of Economic Change. Belknap Press, Cambridge, Mass., 437 pp. Nosanchuk, T. A. comparison 624-634. & Erickson, B. H., 1985. How high is up? Calibrating social in the real world. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48: Olson, M., Herman, C. P., & Zanna, M. P. (Eds.), 1986. Social Comparison. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ. Ouchi, W. Ouchi, W. J. G., 1984. The Reform Relative Deprivation and Society. Addison-Wesley, Reading, Ivlass. & Bolton, M. K., 1988. The logic of joint research and development. G., California Management Review, Spring: 9-33. & Andrews, F. M., 1976. Scientists in Organizations: Productive Climates for Research and Development (Revised Edition). Institute for Social Research, Ann Arbor. Pelz, D. C. — 19 — Salovey, P., 1991. Social comparison processes in envy and jealousy. In: J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social Comparison: Contemporary Theory and Research. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ. Schrader, S., 1991. Informal technology transfer between firms: cooperation through information trading. Research Policy, 20: 153-170. & Cusumano, M. A., 1991. Complementary resources and cooperative research: a model of research joint ventures among competitors. Management Sinha, D. K. Science, 37 (9): 1091-1106. & Williams, R. M., Stouffer, S. A., Suchman, E. A., DeVinney, L. C, Star, S. A., 1949. The American Soldier: Adjusting during Army Life, Vol. 1. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Tucci, Competition, spillovers, and international collaboration International Center for Research on the Management of Technology Working Paper Series, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. C. L., 1994. (forthcoming). Technical communication in R&D laboratories: the impact of project work characteristics. Academy of Management Journal, December: 624— 645. Tushman, M. von Hippel, L., E., 1978. 1987. Cooperation between rivals: informal know-how trading. Research Policy, 16: 291-302. Wood, J. Wood, Theory and research concerning social comparisons of personal Psychological Bulletm. 106 (2): 231-248. v., 1989. attributes. & R., 1985. Social comparison in adjustment Personality and Social Psychology, 49: 1 169-1 183. of to breast cancer. Journal J. v., Taylor, S. E., 6178 Lichtman, R. o'^"^ — 20 Mil 3 lOflO CIBHARICS Q0aPM07E g Date Due APR. \ JUH. 8l 08 Lib-26-67