Louise Rönnqvist Erik Domellöf Department of Psychology, Umeå University, SE-901 87 Umeå, Sweden E-mail: louise.ronnqvist@psy.umu.se Quantitative Assessment of Right and Left Reaching Movements in Infants: A Longitudinal Study from 6 to 36 Months ABSTRACT: This longitudinal study aimed to explore the early presence and developmental pattern of laterality in reaching kinematics and its relationship to side use. In order to do so, 3-D kinematic measurements as well as 2-D video recordings of right-left reaching movements were successively carried out for 17 infants at the ages of 6, 9, 12, and 36 months. Additional investigations of hand preference were made at 36 months. As four infants were prematurely born, their outcomes were compared to those of the fullterm participants. While most of the infants in the early ages showed a rather inconsistent preference in terms of frequency and distributions of right-left side use, the analyses of reaching kinematics revealed a more consistent pattern of fewer movements units (MUs) and straighter right-sided reaching for the majority of infants at all tested ages. However, reaching kinematics from the preterm infants were generally more variable and less side consistent. It is proposed that the development of human handedness originates from an early right arm rather than hand preference in that representations of asymmetry in bilateral projections (involved in arm movements) developmentally precede contralateral projections (involved in refined hand/finger movements). ß 2006 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Dev Psychobiol 48: 444–459, 2006. Keywords: laterality; handedness; arm preference; kinematics; infants; motor development; reaching; preterm Human handedness is associated with differences in specialization and anatomical structure between the hemispheres of the brain, and thought to originate from evolutionary, genetic, environmental, and experiential factors. However, even though a large number of studies and theories during the last decades have been trying to identify and describe the handedness phenomenon and its origin (see e.g., Corballis, 2003; Hopkins & Rönnqvist, Received 5 May 2006; Accepted 20 May 2006 Correspondence to: L. Rönnqvist Contract grant sponsor: Swedish Research Council Contract grant number: 421-2001-4589 Contract grant sponsor: Norrbacka-Eugenia foundation Contract grant number: 239/02 Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI 10.1002/dev.20160 ß 2006 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 1998; Previc, 1991; Provins, 1997; Toga & Thompson, 2003, for reviews), we still lack substantive understanding of the underlying processes for how hand preference actually develops during the first years of life. An answer to this question would provide valuable clues as to how the brain develops and becomes organized the way it is. To date, few longitudinal studies have been made with a focus on side differences related to the developmental organization and changes of early arm/hand movements. This is especially true for more specific computer-aided investigations of kinematics involved in both right and left reaching activity of young infants as well as of infants born at-risk for developmental delays. Thus, the main purpose of the present study was to longitudinally investigate reaching kinematics in young infants (both fullterm and moderately preterm) in order to determine to what extent right-left side differences in kinematic parameters are present early, consistent, or develop during Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev the infants’ first years to become more evident and stable. The infants were first investigated at 6 months, an age when the first fast developmental phase of reaching has ended and most infants perform relatively stable prehension movements (von Hofsten, 1991), and additionally at 9 and 12 months of age. The final investigation was carried out when the infants had reached 36 months of age, when a stable hand preference can be expected (Corbetta & Thelen, 1999; McManus et al., 1988). In one of the few longitudinal studies carried out by means of kinematic registrations of both right and left arm movements (Corbetta & Thelen, 1996, 1999), it was found that lateral biases in both reaching and spontaneous arm movements were unstable and fluctuated in four infants studied throughout the first year. In these investigations, differences regarding right-left hand activity and the average speed performed by each hand were analyzed and further computed by means of a laterality index. Although no stable pattern of side preference was found by means of kinematic parameters, either for the spontaneous arm movements or for the reaching performances, all infants showed a short period of a right-sided preference at some point during the first year. These temporary right-sided biases were later found to be matched with the infants’ right-handed preference at 3 years of age (Corbetta & Thelen, 1999). In another study, based on 2-D video scorings of reaching movements performed by infants from 20 to 32 weeks of age, a shorter movement time, a more direct hand trajectory, and fewer corrections of the right hand in comparison to the left were found on a group level (Morange-Majoux, Peze, & Bloch, 2000). Most studies so far, however, have been made by simply observing the frequency of young infants’ right and left hand use during different tasks in cross-sectional age groups, and with a lack of congruent outcomes. For instance, in one study investigating infants from 3 to 8 weeks, the majority (14 of 15) showed a consistent leftsided bias for number of reaches toward targets presented at different positions from the body midline (McDonnell, Anderson, & Abraham, 1983). In another, a stable rightsided preference for reaching during the first 6 months of life was found (Michel & Harkins, 1986). Moreover, infants with a hand-use preference (independent of side) for reaching and grasping an object at 7, 9, and 11 months have been found to display a corresponding side bias for unimanual manipulation activities (Hinojosa, Sheu, & Michel, 2003). For closely spaced time intervals around 6 months of age, others have reported large changes in the classification of hand preference (McCormick & Maurer, 1988). The same picture of fluctuating asymmetries was found in a longitudinal study from 6 to 12 months (Carlson & Harris, 1985). Fluctuating arm preferences have also been noticed in terms of disappearing lateral preferences Side Differences in Infant Reaching Kinematics 445 during time periods when infants repeatedly use both arms to attain a target (Fagard & Pezé, 1997). However, beyond the first year both cross-sectional and longitudinal outcomes point to the majority of healthy infants attaining a relatively consistent right-hand preference across a range of tasks (Archer, Campbell, & Segalowitz, 1988; Gottfried & Bathurst, 1983). This inconsistency in findings during the first year of life may be a consequence of the difficulty in assessing hand preference within this early time period, in part due to the varying level of infants’ manual ability over the first year of age. Furthermore, it is not clear how well such information describes actual hand-use preference as measured by frequency or pattern of use (Steenhuis & Bryden, 1999). The lack of consistency between studies and fluctuations within longitudinal studies may also be a consequence of the different assessments and/or ageadjusted criteria used. In addition, a lack of sufficient sensitivity in the measurement techniques used, and the choice of parameters to be analyzed could be a part of the explanation. It has also been suggested that individual inconsistency and fluctuation in arm and hand preference during the first year may simply be a result of a poor arm control that might interfere with an actual preferred lateral bias during the infants’ first year (Corbetta & Thelen, 1999). A related explanation is that there is an interaction between hand use and early postural and locomotor reorganizations during this particular time period of infant development (Corbetta & Bojczyk, 2002; Corbetta, Williams, & Snapp-Childs, 2006). Several investigations of infants’ reaching (Berthier, Clifton, McCall, & Robin, 1999; Fetters & Todd, 1987; von Hofsten, 1979, 1991) and grasping movements (Kuhtz-Buschbeck, Boczek-Funcke, Illert, Joehnk, & Stolze, 1999; von Hofsten & Rönnqvist, 1988) have shown that kinematic recordings are sensitive for investigating age-related changes in healthy, typically developing infants and children. Findings include that the sequential structuring of the reaching trajectory becomes more systematic, faster, and smoother over time, and that infants’ ability to anticipate the finger grip in relation to the object size becomes evident around 9 months of age. Kinematic parameters have also been found to be sensitive for identifying deviations in reaching movements of preterm infants and children with neurological dysfunctions (e.g., Chang, Wu, Wu, & Su, 2005; van der Heide, Fock, Otten, Stremmelaar, & Hadders-Algra, 2005). However, none of these studies have been concerned with right-left side differences related to the kinematic parameters investigated, nor presenting data of the infants’ frequency of right-left arm/hand use. To this date, being perhaps the only study to have compared in detail the 3-D kinematics of both arms during reaching in 6-month-old infants, Hopkins and Rönnqvist 446 Rönnqvist and Domellöf (2002) found an expression of a lateral bias that consisted of less segmented and smoother right arm reaching movements (in terms of fewer MUs) in comparison to left arm. In conjunction with the fact that no hand preference for grasping or contacting the object was found, this raises an interesting point about the nature of the early development of handedness. One important fact is the proximodistal nature of neural development. In mammals, the ventromedial pathways develop before the direct corticospinal system (Kuypers, 1981, 1985; Martin, 2005). These pathways contain the vestibulospinal tract that projects bilaterally to the spinal cord and controls the proximal muscles of the arm. Thus, the finding of an early lateral bias in right reaching movements rather than a hand preference for grasping suggests that the initial manifestations of lateral structuring in infants’ reaching should be regarded as primarily indicative of an (proximal) arm rather than a (distal) hand preference (Hopkins & Rönnqvist, 1998, 2002; Rönnqvist, 2003). It is well known that in the great majority of human adults, the left hemisphere controls predominantly the movements of the right hand. The developmental question then is when a lateralized bias in the functioning of the ventromedial system (i.e., in control of the arm movements) initiates a bias in the corticospinal system (i.e., in control of the more distal hand movements)? The aim of the present study was to longitudinally explore the kinematic characteristics of both right- and left-sided reaching movements made by young infants. To do so, we studied 17 infants over the ages 6, 9, 12, and 36 months. Four of the infants were moderately preterm and 13 were born within a normal gestational age. At the first three ages, kinematics of right and left successful reaching movements (i.e., only reaching ending up with grasping the object) were recorded and a number of kinematic parameters analyzed. In addition to the kinematics, the frequency and the distribution of each infant’s hand use were calculated. Taken together, these measurements allowed for an investigation of the relationship between right-left differences in reaching kinematics and a developmental change in the amount of right or left hand use/preference to be made. Based on the theory that arm/hand preference is a biologically rooted proximal to distal developmental process, and on the previous finding by Hopkins and Rönnqvist (2002) of fewer MUs in the right than the left arm during reaching movements in 6-month-old infants, we predicted that the spatiotemporal reaching pattern of the right arm/hand would differ from the left. Thus, we expected to find less segmented and straighter right-sided reaching patterns compared to the left in the majority of infants when successively tested during the second half of their first year of life. In addition, to further our understanding of the developmental changes we Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev continued to explore side differences in the same infants at 36 months. At this age, both the kinematics of their reaching movements with regard to side differences in kinematic outcome parameters as well as their arm/hand preference on three items were investigated. In keeping with previous reports of established hand preference in children of this age, we expected to find a stable arm/hand preference in the majority of the infants, especially for more demanding tasks. As the present study includes a sample of prematurely born infants, an additional objective was to explore whether dissimilarities in patterns of laterality during reaching exist depending on birth condition. Preterm children are a population known to be at-risk for developing cerebral palsy (CP) and other motor and/or cognitive problems. Moreover, it is not uncommon that problems such as CP in preterm children with no apparent insult to the brain are not detected until later ages (Myklebust & Gottlieb, 1997). It is further known that there is an overrepresentation of left- and nonrighthandedness in ex-preterm children (e.g., Marlow, Roberts, & Cooke, 1989; O’Callaghan, Burn, Mohay, Rogers, & Tudehope, 1993). Thus, longitudinal studies by means of kinematic measurements involving both fullterm and preterm infants could be important in providing more knowledge about the salient features that might be causing deviant motor behavior, including deviations in siderelated behavior. Based on previous findings and reviewed studies, the following questions were addressed: (1) Does a lateral preference exist in terms of the frequency of arm/hand use during successful reaching-grasping in young infants? (2) How do young infants’ right and left reaching movements differ in terms of spatio-temporal organization and structuring? (3) If a side difference exists already at 6 months of age in terms of less segmented and smoother reaches by the right arm, how consistent is this side difference over the second half of the first year? (4) How are the kinematic characteristics of right-left reaching movements related to the individual infants’ frequency and distribution of actual arm/hand use? (5) How is hand preference for different arm/hand activities at 36 months of age related to the kinematic characteristics of right and left reaches during the second half of the first year? Finally, an additional question was posed in relation to the inclusion of both fullterm and preterm infants in the present study: (6) Do kinematic characteristics and side differences in reaching movements diverge depending on birth condition? Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev METHODS Participants Seventeen infants (14 boys and 3 girls) participated in the study. Thirteen of these infants were fullterm (mean gestational age ¼ 40.2 weeks, range 38–42) and four were moderately preterm (mean ¼ 35.1 weeks, range 33–36). All infants were healthy, with no known sensory, motor, or neurological impairments. The study was approved by the Ethical Review Board of the Swedish Council for Research in Humanities and Social Sciences, and all parents gave their informed consent. The infants were observed longitudinally at the age of 6 (M ¼ 6.2), 9 (M ¼ 8.9), 12 (M ¼ 12.1), and 36 months (M ¼ 35.7). At the first three observations (6, 9, and 12 months), the four preterm infants were observed at their corrected age (1 week). One family moved within the time period of this study, hence data from 16 children were collected at 36 months. Experimental Set-Up and Procedure Prior to each recording session the infants were familiarized with the laboratory setting. Before the recordings started, a 10 mm3 reflective marker was attached to the right and the left wrist. At the ages 6, 9, and 12 months, the reaching targets used consisted of a set of six differently colored, easy graspable spheres (4 cm in diameter), each with a small bell inside. All spherical objects were mounted on a base construction (5 cm long, 2 cm in diameter) equipped with reflective tape (1 cm wide) to enable identification of the exact time of hand-object contact. Each spherical object could be attached to a specially constructed displayer by means of a magnet fixated to the base, and was presented at the infant’s shoulder height in three possible positions: right, left, or midline. The distance between the object centers of these three positions was 12 cm (36 cm in total). To make the reaching and grasping task more challenging when the infants were tested at 36 months of age, the targets consisted of six differently colored pegs (1.2 cm3, 10 cm long), placed in a pegboard at right, left, or midline position (12 cm between position centers). The pegboard was securely fastened to a table in front of the infant. On top of each peg, a reflective half-spherical marker (1 cm3) was fastened to enable identification of the exact time of hand-object contact. The seating conditions were age adjusted by means of using three different, specially designed infant chairs (when tested at 6 and 9 months of age the same chair was used). The distance between the infant and the placement of the object was individually adjusted enabling the object to be reachable at an extended arm length when the infant was appropriately seated in the chair. When the infant was judged to be in an alert and optimal state for testing, the experimenter attached the object on to the displayer (at 6, 9, and 12 months) or to the pegboard (at 36 months) in front of the infant. The object to be presented was hidden from the infant until it was positioned. At least four presentations in each of the three positions (right, left, midline) were given, until the infant no longer was interested in reaching and grasping. The presentation order in relation to the three positions was randomized but counterbalanced in terms of number of object presentations per position (block of 12 trials; 4 trials 3 positions). If the infant’s Side Differences in Infant Reaching Kinematics 447 mood and interest allowed, a second block of 12 trials was presented directly following the first when tested in the second half of the first year (thus, to optimize the number of reliable recordings). In addition to the kinematic recording session when tested at the age of 36 months, the hand preference of each infant was also assessed in terms of frequency of hand use for three different items: precision throwing (picking up a ball and throwing it at a specific goal), drawing (picking up a pencil and drawing on a paper), and hammering (picking up a hammer and nailing in eight plastic spikes to a child toy hammering plate). Five trials of each item were performed and hand use for each trial identified. The object to be used (ball, pencil, hammer) was always presented in midline. Kinematic Recordings Kinematic data were recorded by means of a six-camera ProReflex system (Qualisys, Inc., Gothenburg, Sweden). Each camera monitored the reflective wrist markers at a frame rate of 240/s. Recordings were triggered by pressing a hand-held external trigger, always occurring when the infant’s hand(s) contacted the object. The pretrigger function allowed the cameras to continuously fill their buffer memory with data until the recording was triggered. The frames from the predefined pretrigger time (2 s) were then added to the captured frames for the measurement. Thus, recording onset started 2 s prior to hand-object contact, ensuring that the entire reaching movement was registered. Only kinematic data from successful reaching movements (ending up with grasping of the object) were collected. Although the starting position and the choice of hand was unrestricted when tested at the ages of 6, 9, and 12 months, the infant’s hands had to be at rest when the object was placed in the position to be reached and grasped for. When tested at 36 months, the infant’s hands were initially situated in a right-left start position (approximately 24 cm between each hand, with the finger tips 18 cm from the target object) indicated by colored tape on the table. Prior to each trial, the experimenter both verbally informed the infant about, as well as actually pointed at, the hand to be used. At all tested ages the total recording time for each trial was set to 4 s. The 2-D data from each camera were stored on a computer, with the third dimension being reconstructed at a later stage using a direct linear transform algorithm (MacReflex Software). All measurement sessions were also simultaneously video recorded. These recordings served three functions. Firstly, if questions concerning the reaching movement arose during the analysis of the ProReflex data, they made it possible to double check. Secondly, they were used in combination with the kinematic recordings (velocity profile and spatial displacement of the wrist marker) to identify the precise onset of a reaching movement in relation to the hand positions at start. Thirdly, they enabled the scorings of the amount of both successful (i.e., grasping the object) and nonsuccessful (i.e., not grasping the object) reaches. Data Scorings First, the scorings based on the video recordings and the 3-D kinematics from each testing session at the ages of 6, 9, and 448 Rönnqvist and Domellöf 12 months were made and the number of successful and nonsuccessful unimanual right, unimanual left, and bimanual reaches and grasps identified. Following a bilateral reach, the hand first contacting the object was identified. A bilateral reach was coded when both hands started to move within maximally 500 ms of each other toward the object, with one hand contacting or grasping the object and the other hand within <5 cm distance from the object. All other arm movements culminating in object contact/grasp were treated as unilateral reaches. When tested at the age of 36 months, the infants were able to follow instructions about which hand to use during the kinematic recording sessions and therefore these scorings were redundant. Scorings of arm/hand preference on the three items (throwing, drawing, hammering) tested at 36 months were done by means of direct observation and then noting which hand the child employed for picking up and using the object. Only two children used different hands for pick up and use, and this only occurred during trials 1 and 3, respectively for one item (drawing). The analyses of kinematic parameters from the first three testing sessions (at the ages 6, 9, and 12 months) are based on 588 reliable recordings of successful reaching and grasping trials (128 at 6, 228 at 9, and 232 at 12 months) out of a total of 824 trials. An additional 325 nonsuccessful reaching trials were identified from the video recordings, that is, trials when the infants’ reaches were incomplete, too short, and/or when they missed the object and consequently did not end up in grasping it. The majority of these nonsuccessful reaching trials were performed at the age of 6 months (195, 37.2% with the right, 48.6% with left, and 14.2% bimanual), 96 at 9 months (42.4% with right, 46.4% with left, and 11.2% bimanual), and 34 at 12 months (51.4% with right, 46.3% with left, and 2.3% bimanual). At 36 months, the number of reliable kinematic recordings of successful reaching and grasping trials was 166 out of a total of 192. No nonsuccessful reaches were identified at this age. In total 132 trials (106 at 6, 9, and 12 months, and 26 at 36 months) had to be excluded from further analysis due to marker(s) being out of the view of the cameras, the reach onset too early to capture in relation to the predefined pretrigger time of the measurement system, and in a few cases when an infant was reaching/grasping without visually focusing on the object (i.e., looking in another direction than at the object). Kinematic Outcome Parameters and Criteria All kinematic-derived parameters were smoothed by means of a second-order 10 Hz dual pass Butterworth filter, and analyzed in MATLAB (The MathWorks, Inc., Boston, MA). For every reach, onset time, the time of hand-object touch, and the time of hand offset of the reach phase (time between first hand-object touch and the time to establishing the grasp) were identified. These measures were derived from the 3-D spatial plots (Fig. 1A) and the 3-D tangential velocity and acceleration profiles (Fig. 1B and C). The variables describing absolute values of the infants’ rightleft reaches were: total duration (TD), cumulative (total 3-D) distance of the reaching trajectory (CD), the time between the first hand-object touch and the offset time of the hand (TTO), Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev FIGURE 1 (A) 3-D image of a right-sided reaching movement by a 6-month-old boy, with corresponding (B) velocity and (C) acceleration profiles. The lines in (B) mark the movement onset, peak velocity, object touch, and movement offset. The lines in (C) mark the onset, peak deceleration, touch, and offset. peak velocity (PV), time-to-peak velocity (TPV), the placement of the peak velocity by means of percentage of the reach duration (PPV), and the velocity at hand-object touch (VT). From the same markers, the number of movements units (MUs) was computed according to an algorithm devised by von Hofsten (1991)1, examples of which the typical progression, from a multisegmented velocity profile at 6 months to a more adultlike, bell-shaped profile at 36 months are shown in Figure 2A–D. A final parameter concerned the straightness of a reaching movement, computed as the ratio between the actual distance 1 A movement unit consists of one acceleration phase and one deceleration phase. The beginning of these respective phases are defined as an accumulated increase or decrease in velocity of at least 20 mm/s and an acceleration or deceleration exceeding 5 mm/s2. Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev Side Differences in Infant Reaching Kinematics 449 Analysis The frequency and distributions of successful right, left, or bimanual reaching movements in relation to the three object positions were analyzed by means of a 3 (age: 6, 9, 12 months) 3 (object position: right, left, midline) 3 (hand-use: right, left, bimanual) ANOVAs, with repeated measures for the last two factors. All kinematic parameters from when tested at 6, 9, and 12 months of age were subjected to separate 3 (age) 2 (birth condition: fullterm, preterm) 2 (side: right, left) ANOVAs. Note: in this analysis, the trials consisting of bimanual reaches were included by dividing them into right or left hand use with regard to which of the hands that first touched the object. Side could not be treated as a repeated measure for these data due to the fact that the individual number of reliable recordings of reaches for one or the other side were too few (< 3) for calculating individual means, or nonexisting (as was the case for two infants at 6, and one at 12 months). At 36 months, separate one-way ANOVAs for testing side differences were employed for all kinematic parameters. For all analyses, where appropriate, significant main and interaction effects were further analyzed using the Scheffé post hoc test. The majority of analyses were performed on an individual level, based on the infant’s mean values. It should be noted that we are aware that the use of parametric ANOVA analysis is not optimal as the data on the level of individual infants showed nonnormal distributions. Yet, these ANOVAs offered the only solution to analyzing the combined effect of multiple factors on the kinematic outcome parameters. A Spearman correlation was conducted to investigate the relationship between the right-left frequencies of successful reaches and the outcome from kinematic parameters based on individual means. Any other test employed is indicated in the subsequent text. A preset alpha level of p < .05 was used for all analyses. RESULTS Side Distribution of Successful Reaching Table 1 presents the frequency and distributions of hand use during successful reaching trials included in the analyses made based on the outcomes from the video scorings. FIGURE 2 Examples of four tangential velocity profiles, with movement units indicated, from the right-sided reaching movements of a boy aged (A) 6 months, (B) 9 months, (C) 12 months, and (D) 36 months. Note: The circles on the respective reaching trajectory mark the start/stop of a movement unit, and the small vertical lines indicate the velocity peaks within respective MU. The number of MUs are five, two, one, and one, for (A), (B), (C), and (D), respectively. the wrist marker was transported and the shortest distance connecting the hand at movement onset to the center of the object. Accordingly, the closer this ratio is to 1.00, the straighter the reach. Right-Left Side Differences. A significant main effect of side was found, F(2,90) ¼ 26.2, p < .0001, and a significant interaction between age and side, F(2,90) ¼ 7.6, p < .0001 (Fig. 3). As can be seen from Figure 3, there was no evident side difference regarding the distribution of hand use found at 6 months, although more unimanual left than both unimanual right and bimanual reaching were performed. However, the post hoc test showed that at 9 months of age the infants used significantly more unimanual than bimanual reaching (p < .01), but no significant right-left difference was found. At 12 months, the infants showed 450 Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev Rönnqvist and Domellöf Table 1. Total Number of Successful Reach-Grasp and Distribution of Relative Arm/Hand Use (Unimanual Right, Unimanual Left, Bimanual) for Each Infant Up To 12 Months Successful Reach-Grasp Arm/Hand Use (%) Infants ALa ISJa JEa KLa AD AN ED EL EM IS JI JO KR MI NI OL WI Total (n) Right Left Bimanual 52 43 26 27 58 48 43 57 51 42 65 46 55 62 60 46 43 76.9 11.8 66.6 40.1 57.1 59.5 12.5 20.8 60.5 34.5 46.9 46.6 60.4 62.2 41.2 45.9 55.8 23.1 52.9 33.4 48.6 28.6 28.6 68.7 66.6 32.6 51.7 51.0 53.3 34.8 28.8 35.3 29.8 44.2 0 35.3 0 13.3 14.3 11.9 21.8 14.6 6.9 13.8 2.1 0 4.6 8.8 23.5 24.3 0 A Right Hand Left Hand 35 30 25 % 20 15 10 5 0 6 months Left B Mid Right 35 30 25 9 months % 20 15 10 5 0 Left C Mid Right 35 30 25 12 months % 20 15 10 5 0 Left Note: n, number. a Prematurely born infants. significantly more right reaching than left (p < .05), and significantly more unimanual (independent of side) than bimanual reaching (p < .01). On the whole, the majority of infants showed an increasing number of unimanual right reaching and a decreasing number of bimanual reaching over the first three ages tested (i.e., over the second half of the first year). A significant effect of object position, F(2,90) ¼ 3.5, p < .05, and an interaction between object position and side of arm/hand use, F(4,180) ¼ 53.1, p < .0001, were further revealed (Fig. 4A–C). Although a few contral70 Right Left Bimanual 60 50 (%) 40 30 20 10 0 6-months 9-months Mid Right Object position 12-months AGE FIGURE 3 Distribution of the relative frequencies of successful arm/hand use (unimanual right, unimanual left, bimanual) over the first three testing ages (6, 9, and 12 months). FIGURE 4 Distribution of the relative frequencies of rightleft arm/hand use in relation to the object position at (A) 6 months, (B) 9 months, and (C) 12 months. ateral reaches (i.e., crossing the midline) were performed already when tested at the age of 6 and 9 months, the infants mainly reached and grasped with the ipsilateral arm/hand, thus, at the same side as the placement of the object (Fig. 4A and B). When the object was placed in the midline position, no clear side preference could be observed at these ages (6 and 9 months). However, when tested at 12 months of age, significantly more right reaching-grasping of an object placed to the right than left reaching-grasping when the object was placed to the left was found (Fig. 4C). Additionally, at 12 months there was significantly more right arm/hand crossing the midline to reach for and grasp an object placed on the left side than left arm/hand contralateral reaching-grasping. In line with these side differences, more right reachinggrasping was also found when the object was placed in the midline when tested at 12 months of age, compared to the younger ages of 6 and 9 months. Furthermore, the results from the additional arm/hand preference tests carried out when the infants were 36 months of age showed that all of the participating infants displayed a significant and consistent preference Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev for using their right arm/hand in all the three tasks tested. The distribution of the frequencies of the right-left arm/ hand usage for the three tasks was: 98.7% right and 1.3% left for precision throwing; 89.7% right and 10.3% left for drawing; and 94.4% right and 5.6% left for hammering. Differences Related to Preterm Infants. Regarding the number of successful reaching-grasping trials during the first three testing ages (6, 9, and 12 months), no significant differences between the fullterm and the preterm infants were found with regard to the total number of trials (p ¼ .07), or the frequency of right and left hand use distribution (p ¼ .12). Furthermore, no significant differences between fullterm and preterm infants for any of the three tasks tested at 36 months of age could be found. Kinematic Findings Figure 5 illustrates the typical developmental pattern in the kinematics for the movement distance of the wrist marker trajectories over time and the corresponding velocity profiles from the right- and left-sided reaching movements made by one infant at the four different test occasions (6, 9, 12, and 36 months). Table 2 presents the means and standard deviations for all kinematic parameters investigated. Note: as no significant differences were found between 36-monthold fullterm and preterm infants, data from the kinematic parameters of all infants are pooled together at this age. Right-Left Side Differences. Analysis of side effects on kinematic outcome parameters associated with the infants’ reaching movements at the ages of 6, 9, and 12 months revealed a significant overall effect of side for number of MUs, F(1,74) ¼ 8.26, p < .01. As judged by post hoc testing, there were significantly fewer MUs (p < .05) during right arm reaching movements compared to left at all of the ages tested during the second half of the first year (Tab. 2). In addition, a similar side difference by means of fewer MUs was also found when the infants were tested at the age of 36 months, F(1,32) ¼ 7.6, p < .01. A significant overall effect of side was also found for the straightness of the reaching movements, F(1,74) ¼ 4.51, p < .05. The post hoc test revealed significantly (p < .05) straighter right-sided reaching trajectories in comparison to left when tested at 6 and 12 months of age. However, with the four preterm infants excluded from this analysis, the significantly straighter right-sided reaching trajectories are found at 9 and 12 months of age (Tab. 2). When the infants were tested at 36 months of age, no significant side difference was found related to the straightness of the reach (Tab. 2). For the other kinematic parameters analyzed in this study, no Side Differences in Infant Reaching Kinematics 451 significant overall effects of side were found at 6, 9, and 12 months, nor when tested at 36 months of age. Differences Related to Preterm Infants. Several differences between fullterm and prematurely born infants were revealed with regard to side differences in the kinematic findings. At the age of 12 months, the pattern of fewer MUs for right-sided reaching, as found for the fullterm infants, was not evident for the prematurely born infants. On the contrary, at this age they showed more MUs for the right reaching movements in comparison to the left (Tab. 2). In terms of straightness of the reaching, the prematurely born infants had significantly straighter rightsided reaching trajectories than left at 6 and 9 months, but not at 12 months, in contrast to 9 and 12 months for the fullterm (Tab. 2). This result is also in line with the finding of more MUs in the right reaching movements compared to the left for the 12-month-old prematurely born but not for the fullterm infants. At 36 months, no difference between the fullterm and the prematurely born infants was found in terms of straightness of the reaching. Furthermore, even if no overall side differences for the remaining kinematic parameters were found, significant right-left differences related to interaction effects between side and birth condition for a range of additional variables were present, all associated with the prematurely born infants (Tab. 2). Based on post hoc tests, the following significant right-left side differences were found (all results compared to right-sided reaching and related only to the prematurely born infants): for TD by means of a shorter TD for left-sided reaching movements at 6 months; for TTO by means of a shorter TTO for leftsided reaching at 6 months; for PV by means of a lower PV for left-sided reaching at 9 months; for TPV by means of earlier TPV for left-sided reaching at 6 months, a later TPV for left reaching at 9 months, and an earlier TPV for left-sided reaching at 12 months; for PPV by means of an earlier PPV for left-sided reaching at 6 months. In general, differences between preterm and fullterm infants were evident already from the visual inspections of the kinematic outcome parameters, where the prematurely born infants seemed to be less consistent (in regards to both intra- and inter-variability) than the fullterm infants. The statistical analyses also confirmed these differences. It was found that, independent of side, the straightness of the reaching trajectory differed between the preterm and the fullterm infants, F(1,74) ¼ 16.68, p < .0001. Prematurely born infants in general showed less straight reaching trajectories in comparison to fullterm infants, particularly when tested at 6 and 9 months of age (Tab. 1). Additionally, a significant difference was found for the number of MUs within the reach, F(1,74) ¼ 9.56, p < .01, characterized by reaches of preterm infants containing significantly more MUs at 6, 9, and 12 months than 452 Rönnqvist and Domellöf Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev FIGURE 5 Overlay plots of the corresponding profiles for distance from reaching onset to hand object contact (distance against time) and tangential velocity in the right-sided (bold line) and leftsided (thin line) reaching movements of a boy aged (A) 6 months, (B) 9 months, (C) 12 months, and (D) 36 months. L R L R L R L R L R L R L R La R La R TD: total duration (s) 1.144 (.54) 1.248 (.55) 151 (77) 155 (53) 131 (114) 121 (86) 355 (163) 352 (144) 415 (322) 381 (398) 43.7 (23.8) 36.3 (27.3) 151 (104) 111 (84) 2.07 (.67) 2.15 (.72) 3.74 (1.76)b 3.22 (.99) 1.021 (.42) 1.024 (.40) 172 (81) 185 (61) 96 (116) 107 (73) 398 (165) 427 (169) 359 (226) 340 (205) 41.7 (23.1) 38.2 (19.2) 131 (99) 121 (88) 1.62 (.77)b 1.47 (.36) 3.18 (1.39)b 2.87 (1.21) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) .910 (.35) .932 (.35) 213 (127) 189 (88) 109 (102) 108 (74) 471 (234) 446 (235) 345 (214) 393 (277) 39.0 (22.4) 47.4 (27.0) 122 (94) 134 (122) 1.46 (.74)b 1.39 (.35) 3.18 (1.48)b 2.55 (1.33) Mean (SD) 12 Note: L, left; R, right; SD, standard deviation; n, number; s, seconds; mm, millimeters; ms, milliseconds. a Overall effect of side. b Significant right-left difference. MU: movement units (n) STR: straightness TPV: time to peak velocity (ms) PPV: peak placement of the reach (%) VT: velocity at touch (mm/s) CD: cumulative distance (mm) TTO: touch-offset difference (ms) PV: peak velocity (mm/s) Side Kinematic Parameters 9 1.200 (.52) 1.050 (.31) 167 (89) 196 (62) 55 (47)b 119 (81) 459 (295) 493 (133) 285 (196)b 513 (446) 26.4 (18.8)b 51.4 (29.1) 153 (226) 252 (132) 3.64 (2.49)b 2.98 (1.97) 4.88 (1.81)b 4.14 (1.95) b Mean (SD) 6 1.211 (.34) 1.208 (.74) 154 (50) 194 (107) 111 (81) 109 (82) 301 (80)b 419 (145) 535 (263)b 412 (202) 49.0 (20.7) 43.9 (24.4) 118 (110) 80 (79) 1.82 (.18)b 1.60 (.52) 3.89 (1.88)b 3.11 (1.41) Mean (SD) 9 Age (months) Age (months) 6 Preterm Born (n ¼ 4) Fullterm Born (n ¼ 13) 1.100 (.70) 1.186 (.76) 218 (139) 190 (94) 79 (56) 84 (55) 513 (358) 415 (234) 212 (219)b 248 (195) 24.7 (21.4) 28.0 (23.6) 103 (74) 102 (107) 1.47 (.42) 1.53 (.51) 3.13 (1.68)b 3.89 (2.47) Mean (SD) 12 .729 (.22) .728 (.20) 246 (35) 253 (46) 106 (06) 114 (05) 681 (170) 690 (172) 376 (138) 396 (159) 52.2 (17.1) 54.3 (19.8) 54 (67) 57 (63) 1.32 (.14) 1.31 (.16) 2.13 (1.08)b 1.74 (.74) Mean (SD) 36 Age (months) All (n ¼ 16) Table 2. Means and Standard Deviations for Kinematic Parameters During Reaching and Grasping as a Function of Birth Condition (Fullterm, Preterm) and Age (6, 9, 12, and 36 Months) Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev Side Differences in Infant Reaching Kinematics 453 454 Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev Rönnqvist and Domellöf fullterm infants (Tab. 1). Thus, the reaching movements of preterm infants were less smooth in terms of a more segmented velocity profile. A significant difference was also found for the time spent between hand-object touch and offset of the reaching (TTO), F(1,74) ¼ 4.08, p < .05. The prematurely born infants were found to have a shorter TTO time in comparison to the fullterm infants, but the post hoc test showed that this was only significant when tested at 12 months. Although not significant, F(1,74) ¼ 2.77, p ¼ .09, the preterm infants also tended to have a longer reaching duration in comparison to the fullterm, most apparent when tested at 12 months of age (Tab. 1). No other analysis of the investigated kinematic parameters showed a significant difference between the fullterm and the preterm infants, nor were there any significant interactions found between birth condition and testing age. Furthermore, when the infants were tested at 36 months of age there were no significant differences found between infants born fullterm and prematurely for any of the parameters analyzed. distributions of successful reaching movements when tested in the early ages. Thus, the reaching was not marked by an overall side preference at 6 and 9 months, although an increasing amount of right-sided reaching could be observed in most infants at 12 months of age. In terms of the amount of successful reaching for individual infants at 6, 9, and 12 months of age, 10 of the infants showed more right-sided reaching and 7 more left-sided. However, for four of these infants the side differences seen were more or less negligible at all of the first three testing ages. Looking at the individual infants’ outcomes as revealed by kinematics, it was found that, in agreement with the significant overall right-left side difference for number of MUs, the great majority of infants showed significantly (t-tests, p < .05 on individual infants outcomes) fewer MUs in their right reaching in comparison to their left (Tab. 3). Consistent over the first three ages tested, fewer MUs in the right reaches in comparison to left reaches were found in 10 of the 17 infants. Three infants showed a pattern of fewer MUs at two out of three test occasions (2/3), and four at one out of three (1/3). Two of the latter four infants were prematurely born. Adding the outcomes from the analyses of MUs at 36 months (including 16 of the children), a consistent right-left difference (i.e., at 4/4 or 3/4 test sessions) in terms of fewer MUs in the right reaches Side Consistency The majority of infants did not show any clear side preference with regard to the frequency and side Table 3. Total Number of Kinematic Recordings and Distribution of Number of MUs (Right, Left) for Each Infant at 6, 9, 12, and 36 Months MUs (n) Kinematic Recordings 6 Months 9 Months 12 Months 36 Months Trial Side Side Side Side Infants Total (n) Right (M) Left (M) Right (M) Left (M) Right (M) Left (M) Right (M) Left (M) a 46 45 32 38 53 34 43 41 54 26 51 58 54 43 40 38 58 2.28 4.10 3.40 1.99 2.70 3.20 2.33 3.20 2.00 3.20 3.00 3.60 3.45 — 4.00 3.20 3.07 3.66 4.80 5.80 3.72 3.71 3.70 4.00 3.80 2.77 3.74 4.00 4.50 — 5.00 4.30 3.80 5.80 3.05 4.69 5.30 3.60 3.21 3.10 3.25 2.83 2.57 2.70 3.10 2.33 3.00 3.80 2.00 2.33 2.41 3.26 4.00 4.33 3.30 4.00 3.50 4.10 3.43 2.60 3.10 2.75 3.25 4.00 3.20 3.12 3.70 2.75 2.45 3.25 5.50 3.20 2.10 2.83 3.50 2.66 1.64 2.50 3.20 1.87 2.84 3.16 2.50 2.30 2.67 2.71 3.23 4.00 1.60 3.43 3.50 — 3.69 2.00 3.20 3.25 3.60 3.25 3.28 2.33 3.00 2.76 — 1.80 2.13 1.67 1.50 1.00 1.79 1.60 1.60 1.97 1.31 1.34 2.00 1.86 1.75 1.35 1.50 — 3.02 2.43 1.50 1.72 2.00 2.24 1.10 1.67 2.66 1.74 1.73 2.50 1.87 3.00 2.00 2.28 AL ISJa JEa KLa AD AN ED EL EM IS JI JO KR MI NI OL WI Note: Significant fewer MUs in right reaching movements for respective age tested are highlighted in bold. M, mean. a Prematurely born infants. Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev was found for 11 of the infants. Five of the infants showed no clear side consistency, of whom three were prematurely born (Tab. 3). None of the infants tested showed consistently fewer MUs in their left reaching movements. In agreement with the findings of fewer MUs in the right reaching, the majority (11) of the infants also showed a significant (t-tests, p < .05) and consistently straighter reaching trajectory for their right-sided reaching movements in comparison to their left over all ages tested (6, 9, 12, 36 months). The remaining six infants showed a more inconsistent right-left side difference in terms of reaching trajectory straightness. However, this inconsistency was mainly related to when the infants were tested at 6 months of age, and when a higher variability of the reaching trajectory in general was found. None of the infants showed a consistently straighter reaching trajectory for left-sided reaching over the tested ages. In addition, Spearman correlation showed that the individual frequencies and distributions of successful right-left reaches over the first three testing ages were not significantly correlated with the individual infant’s reaching kinematics in terms of number of MUs for the respective right-left reaches (r ¼ .206, p > .05). Thus, independent of the frequency of successful right-left reaching distributions in infants tested during the second half of the first year (6, 9, and 12 months), the pattern of less segmented right-sided reaching movements in comparison to left was evident in the findings. Age Effects Independent of birth condition and side, the ANOVAs performed on all kinematic variables signaled a significant effect with regard to the infants’ testing age for the following variables: total reaching duration (p < .01), cumulative distance of the reaching (p < .001), actual PV (p < .01), straightness of the reaching trajectory (p < .0001), and number of MUs (p < .001) as illustrated in Figure 2A–D. It was found that the reaching duration progressively decreased over the first three ages tested, from a mean duration of 1.17 s at 6 months to a mean duration of .98 s at 12 months, with the largest decrement between 6 and 9 months. In contrast to this finding, the cumulative distance of the reach trajectory increased over the ages tested, from an average distance of 168 mm at 6 months to 211 mm at 12 months of age. The most plausible explanation for this increasing length of the reach trajectory over age is the parallel increasing growth in length of the infants’ arms. The age effect for PV is mainly explained by an increasing PV between 6 and 12 months. However, as can be seen in Table 2, the PV, as well as the TPV and the placement of the PV, were rather variable over the first Side Differences in Infant Reaching Kinematics 455 three ages tested. This finding was particularly true for the preterm infants. The most evident overall effects of age were found for the straightness of the reach and the number of MUs per reach. It was found that, with an increasing age, the distal reach trajectory became progressively both straighter and smoother (less segmented). This is also confirmed by the results from when the infants were tested at 36 months of age (Tab. 2). DISCUSSION The present study aimed at investigating whether there are right-left side differences in the reaching kinematics of developing infants, the consistency of such side differences, and the relationship between reaching kinematics and frequency of arm/hand use. By carrying out successive quantitative analyses of infants’ reaching movements at the ages 6, 9, 12, and 36 months, we expected to find side differences that may not otherwise be visible and/or detected by simply focusing on the frequency of the infants’ choice of right or left arm/hand for reaching-grasping. Overall, it was found that the majority of the infants in the current study showed a relatively inconsistent side distribution of reachinggrasping over the ages tested; this was especially true in the early ages of 6 and 9 months. However, the kinematics in terms of the spatiotemporal structuring of the infants’ reaching trajectories indicated a more consistent and stable right-left difference by means of straighter and less segmented right-sided reaching movements in most infants/children. This finding was true for most of the fullterm infants, whereas the prematurely born infants included in this study generally showed a more inconsistent and fluctuating outcome in terms of right-left reaching kinematics and side consistency. When investigated at the ages of 6 and 9 months, most of the infants did not show any evident side preference with regard to the frequency of right- or left-sided reaching and grasping. At 12 months, however, a rather evident right-sided preference for reaching and grasping was found in most of the infants tested. This finding is in keeping with the distal progress in anticipatory preparation and adjustment of hand shape when grasping differently sized object in infants (Fagard, 2000; von Hofsten & Rönnqvist, 1988; White, Castle, & Held, 1964), which are abilities dependent on direct corticospinal connections providing contralateral control of the fingers. Hence, hand preference seems to develop in synchrony with a substantial maturation of the corticospinal system in control of more refined hand/finger movements. In addition, when investigated at 36 months of age, all participating children in the present study additionally showed a clear-cut right-sided bias for 456 Rönnqvist and Domellöf different complex items involving performances with both arm and hand (throwing, drawing, hammering as well as for grasping the ‘‘tool’’). No stable pattern of side preference was found with respect to the frequency and side distribution of individual infants’ reaching and grasping before 12 months of age. This implicates that measurement by only focusing on the frequency and distribution of right-left reaching and grasping in infants less than 1 year of age may not provide a valid index of hand preference and/or prediction for later handedness. However, it is notable that although the infants in the present study were free to choose whatever hand to reach and grasp the object with, the experimental set-up was deliberately constructed to optimize reaching and grasping performance with both the right and the left sides. This was done in order to increase our ability to make kinematic recordings of both the right and left reaching movements in individual infants, allowing side comparisons to be made. In most studies reviewed, the target object for the infant to reach and grasp for have been positioned in a midline position. In the present study, target objects were presented in a right, left, and a midline position. Thus, we are aware that this method may have affected the frequency of right-left reaches found in this study. Still, the method may have a better ecological validity than if the objects always are presented in a midline position, which is not the case in infants’ natural settings. Moreover, it is well known from previous studies that young infants prefer to reach and grasp with the hand at the same side as the placement of a stationary object (e.g., Fagard, 1998), a result that was also confirmed in the present study. However, the notion that the ability to cross the midline during reaching-grasping is associated with the maturation of the corpus callosum, with spontaneous midline crossing first occurring after 1 year of age (Bishop, 1990), seems to be overly stressed. In the present study, reaching across the midline was present (although not so frequently occurring) already at 6 and 9 months of age. At 12 months of age, reaching across the midline was a more frequent observation, and mainly in terms of more right reaches crossing the midline toward an object to the left in the majority of infants. The majority of infants also showed a later midline crossing with the left arm/hand in comparison to the right, and not as frequent, in line with findings from both typically developing 12-month-old infants as well as infants with agenesis of the corpus callosum (see Sacco, Moutard, & Fagard, this issue). Taken together, these findings suggest that reaches across midline might be a more sensitive indicator of early side preference than reaching and grasping frequency per se. Turning to the kinematic findings, the results from the present study verify the findings of Hopkins and Rönnqvist (2002), and are in line with Morange-Majoux Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev et al. (2000), with regard to significant side differences in terms of less segmented and straighter right-sided reaching trajectories in comparison to left-sided in 6-month-old infants. As judged by the present study, we can now add to these findings that such side differences are also evident when infants are tested at 9, 12, and 36 months of age. Furthermore, on an individual basis, these side differences were found to be consistent over time in most of the infants investigated. Still, some infants showed a relative inconsistency in terms of right-left differences regarding the straightness and segmentation of the reaching movement, although this was mainly the case for the few preterm infants included in the study. However, none of the participating infants tested showed an over age consistent pattern of fewer MUs and/or a straighter trajectory for left-sided reaching. Furthermore, in line with previous studies of hand preference in children at about 3 years of age, all of the children in the present study showed an evident right-hand preference at 36 months of age. In agreement with Corbetta and Thelen (1996, 1999), a fluctuating and inconsistent velocity profile in infant reaching movements during the second half of the first year was confirmed also in the present study. It was found that PV, as well as TPV and the placement of the PV, were rather variable and inconsistent over the first three ages tested. One explanation for the incoherent velocity profiles in the present study, particularly evident in the younger ages of 6 and 9 months, is that the reaching movements are structured into several segments of velocity-based MUs, whereas the within-reach velocity peaks sometimes varies with regard to its corresponding MU placement (exemplified in Fig. 2A). In more mature reaches, the first transport unit is generally the largest, containing the highest PV and transporting the hand the longest distance (e.g., Jeannerod, 1984). In the present study, the velocity pattern was more variable and inconsistent when the infants were investigated at the younger ages (especially at 6 and 9 months). Thus, it was found that both spatial and/or temporal correction sometimes occurred later in the reaching path, resulting in a corresponding MU with a higher velocity peak than in the first unit. Nevertheless, despite the fact that the PV profile within reaching movements was not found to have a fixed relationship, the number of velocity-based MUs and the corresponding right-left side differences turned out to be more stable. It is not possible from the current findings of consistent smoother and straighter right-sided reaching to determine whether these are expressions of differences in a biologically rooted neural circuitry that give rise to asymmetries in the structuring of infants’ early arm movements, or the other way around, as a consequence of early arm use and activity. Nevertheless, our current findings strongly support Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev the notion that the right-left differences found with regard to spatiotemporal organization and structuring of early reaching movements are associated with a biologically based developmental process. Furthermore, our findings suggest that this developmental process shows a similar and rather stable timetable in the vast majority of typically developing infants. Thus, in line with the suggested proximal-distal trend in motor development, the neural systems controlling the proximal arm movements develop before the systems controlling the distal hand movements involved in more refined finger movements (i.e., pincer grip). Hence, the initial manifestations of hemispheric dominance related to a side preference should be regarded as primarily the development of a trunk, head, and arm preference rather than a hemispheric dominance for handedness. Arm preference by means of activity seems to appear several months before term age (McCartney & Hepper, 1999). Thus, in terms of developmental origins, the present findings of side differences in the spatio-temporal structuring of infant reaching movements may be determined by physiological asymmetries that are present long before the possibility for experience-related movement patterns to develop. According to this prospect, infants’ arm and hand preference would emerge from distinctive neural circuits in each hemisphere that are specialized for controlling different aspects of arm/hand movements, in keeping with a view of handedness as innate and genetically rooted (Annett, 1985). In support for this view is the outcome from the study by McCartney and Hepper, who investigated fetal arm movements from 12 to 27 weeks gestational age. They found that throughout all periods of observation, the great majority of fetuses showed more right than left arm movements. This early presence of lateralized arm movements was suggested possibly generating later asymmetries in the brain, and thought to have a genetic origin. Furthermore, it has been suggested that newborns’ head position and head turning preference can predict the arm used in infants’ initial attempts to reach, as well as later hand preference (e.g., Coryell & Michel, 1978; Michel, 1981). In this view, head positioning preference to the right or left in the newborn may induce a lateral asymmetry in looking at and activating one hand or the other (i.e., a greater visual experience of one hand leads to it being preferred for later reaching). Still, before birth, intrauterine position and/or early postural bias might have an effect on newborns’ head positioning preference, in line with Previc (1991)’s leftotolith dominance hypothesis. Even if the development of human hand preference seems to be biologically rooted, a number of recent studies have showed that the early developmental process of structural-functional motor asymmetries can change as a result of even minor interruptions (e.g., influences of Side Differences in Infant Reaching Kinematics 457 intrauterine exposure to teratogens and preterm birth), especially during the early establishment of brain connectivity. Thus, a polygenetic explanation, which takes these early intrauterine, perinatal, and postnatal environmental influences on the early neuronal development into consideration, is probably called for. A consistent finding is that left- and nonright-handers are overrepresented in ex-preterm children (e.g., Marlow et al., 1989; O’Callaghan et al., 1993). This atypical preference has also been suggested associated with poor cognitive performances at school age, and consequently reflecting subtle disturbances in brain organization (Bracewell & Marlow, 2002). However, the relationship between nonright handedness in preterm children and specific deviations in sensory-motor development, as well as how deviations in the developing nervous system may be expressed in atypical functional asymmetries, is still largely unknown. Kinematic analysis has proved to be promising for investigating, for example, reaching in children with neurological dysfunctions (e.g., Chang et al., 2005), or born at-risk for such dysfunctions (e.g., Fallang, Saugstad, Grøgaard, & Hadders-Algra, 2003). Longitudinal kinematic studies of lateralized differences in early movement patterns of fullterm and preterm infants could thus add further to the understanding of the processes that may lead to deviant developmental outcomes in ex-preterm children. The prematurely born infants in our study displayed a developmental delay in terms of reaching kinematics, together with an alternative developmental pattern with regard to expressions of hand preference. However, at 36 months both the fullterm and preterm children showed a dominant right-sided preference in terms of hand use and differences in terms of kinematic characteristics could no longer be found. With regard to underlying neural mechanisms, the development of the corticospinal system has been suggested susceptible to functional divergence (Martin, 2005), which may also apply to deviations in side-related behavior. For example, complications following preterm birth may impair the functional state of the corticospinal system, which could be reflected in deviant expressions of functional asymmetries. In terms of motor functioning, brain damage associated with preterm birth implicates a modulation of functional neural connections during CNS maturation that may result in irregularities in motor output. Repeated uncorrelated movements may then reinforce these disordered or inappropriate neural connections later in development (Myklebust & Gottlieb, 1997). One explanation for the temporary deviation from the typical developmental pattern at 6–12 months found in the present study could thus be that premature birth (in our study, 33–36 weeks of gestational age) may only result in very mild complications that, following increased activity and motor 458 Rönnqvist and Domellöf experiences in a dextrally biased world, progressively are compensated for. Alternatively, this could be an expression of a general, slower motor development and as a consequence a later developing arm/hand preference, thus, not necessarily involving a functional neural deviation. Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev tion (239/02). We are especially grateful to the parents and their infants for their participation over the years. We also thank Thomas Rudolfsson for the MATLAB programming. REFERENCES CONCLUSION Based on our present results and findings from previous relevant studies, we propose that human hand preference originates from an early dominant arm advantage (mainly involving bilateral projections), as it is not until later in development that the infant gains increased precision and accuracy of the hand (mainly involving contralateral projections). This is in agreement with the suggestion that the initial manifestations of lateral structuring in infants’ reaching should be regarded as primarily indicative of an arm rather than a hand preference (Hopkins & Rönnqvist, 1998, 2002; Rönnqvist, 2003). In addition, such a proposal is in keeping with the notion of the developmental course of motor control functions of the corticospinal system progressing from initially mainly representing proximal muscles toward including the distal muscles representing the hand and finger movements (Martin, 2005). This developmental progression in the corticospinal system, not fully completed until 2–3 years of age, is also similar to the proximal to distal development of a control strategy for arm movements in human infants (Berthier et al., 1999). Hence, we suggest that the evolution of hand preference has occurred within an early developmental context of a lateralized asymmetry of hemisphere organization, prompted by an early bilaterally controlled arm. It should be noted that our findings originate from a limited number of infants, who all later became right handed. Thus, there is a need for additional longitudinal studies, involving a larger sample, including preterm infants with a wider range of gestational ages and with infants developing both a right and a left hand preference. Doing so would help to clarify whether the developmental patterns found in the present study are consistent and typical also for infants who develop a preference for the left arm/hand and/or infants born at-risk for developmental delays. In addition, incorporating analyses of the kinematics properties of intra-joint dynamics in such studies would further elucidate the relationship between asymmetries in the movements of the shoulder, arm, and hand. NOTES This study was supported by grants from the Swedish Research Council (421-2001-4589) and by the Norrbacka-Eugenia founda- Annett, M. (1985). Left, right, hand and brain: The right shift theory. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd. Archer, L. A., Campbell, D., & Segalowitz, S. J. (1988). A prospective study of hand preference and language development in 18-month-olds to 30-month-olds. 1. Hand preference. Developmental Neuropsychology, 4, 85–92. Berthier, N. E., Clifton, R. K., McCall, D. D., & Robin, D. J. (1999). Proximodistal structure of early reaching in human infants. Experimental Brain Research, 127, 259– 269. Bishop, D. V. M. (1990). Handedness and developmental disorder. Clinics in developmental medicine no. 110. London: Mac Keith Press. Bracewell, M., & Marlow, N. (2002). Patterns of motor disability in very preterm children. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 8, 241–248. Carlson, D. F., & Harris, L. J. (1985). Development of the infant’s hand preference for visually directed reaching: Preliminary report of a longitudinal study. Infant Mental Health Journal, 6, 158–174. Chang, J.-J., Wu, T.-I., Wu, W.-L., & Su, F.-C. (2005). Kinematical measure for spastic reaching in children with cerebral palsy. Clinical Biomechanics, 20, 381–388. Corballis, M. C. (2003). From mouth to hand: Gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 199–260. Corbetta, D., & Bojczyk, K. E. (2002). Infants return to twohanded reaching when they are learning to walk. Journal of Motor Behavior, 34, 83–95. Corbetta, D., & Thelen, E. (1996). The developmental origins of bimanual coordination: A dynamic perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 502–522. Corbetta, D., & Thelen, E. (1999). Lateral biases and fluctuations in infants’ spontaneous arm movements and reaching. Developmental Psychobiology, 34, 237–255. Corbetta, D., Williams, J., & Snapp-Childs, W. (2006). Plasticity in the development of handedness: Evidence from normal development and early asymmetric brain injury. Developmental Psychobiology, 48, 460–471. Coryell, J. F., & Michel, G. F. (1978). How supine postural preferences of infants can contribute toward the development of handedness. Infant Behavior and Development, 1, 245– 257. Fagard, J. (1998). Changes in grasping skills and the emergence of bimanual coordination during the first year of life. In K. J. Connolly (Ed.), The Psychobiology of the hand (Vol. Clinics in Developmental Medicine, pp. 123–143). Londres: Mac Keith Press. Fagard, J. (2000). Linked proximal and distal changes in the reaching behaviour of 5- to 12-month-old human infants Developmental Psychobiology. DOI 10.1002/dev grasping object of different sizes. Infant Behavior & Development, 23, 317–329. Fagard, J., & Pezé, A. (1997). Age changes in interlimb coupling and the development of bimanual coordination. Journal of Motor Behavior, 29, 199–208. Fallang, B., Saugstad, O. G., Grøgaard, J., & Hadders-Algra, M. (2003). Kinematic quality of reaching movements in preterm infants. Pediatric Research, 53, 836–842. Fetters, L., & Todd, J. (1987). Quantitative assessment of infant reaching movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 19, 147– 166. Gottfried, A. W., & Bathurst, K. (1983). Hand preference across time is related to intelligence in young girls, not boys. Science, 221, 1074–1076. Hinojosa, T., Sheu, C. F., & Michel, G. F. (2003). Infant hand-use preferences for grasping objects contributes to the development of a hand-use preference for manipulating objects. Developmental Psychobiology, 43, 328– 334. Hopkins, B., & Rönnqvist, L. (1998). Human handedness: Development and evolutionary perspectives. In F. Simion, & G. E. Butterworth (Eds.), The development of sensory, motor and cognitive capacities in early infancy: From sensation to cognition (pp. 191–236). Hove: Psychology Press. Hopkins, B., & Rönnqvist, L. (2002). Facilitating postural control: Effects on the reaching behavior of 6-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 168–182. Jeannerod, M. (1984). The timing of natural prehension movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 16, 235–254. Kuhtz-Buschbeck, J. P., Boczek-Funcke, A., Illert, M., Joehnk, K., & Stolze, H. (1999). Prehension movements and motor development in children. Experimental Brain Research, 128, 65–68. Kuypers, H. G. J. M. (1981). Anatomy of the descending pathways. In Vernon B. Brooks (Ed.), Handbook of physiology II (pp. 597–666). Bethesda, MD: American Physiology Society. Kuypers, H. G. J. M. (1985). The anatomical and functional organization of the motor system. In M. Swash, & C. Kennard (Eds.), Scientific basis of clinical neurology (pp. 3– 18). Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone. Marlow, N., Roberts, B. L., & Cooke, R. W. I. (1989). Laterality and prematurity. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 64, 1713–1716. Martin, J. H. (2005). The corticospinal system: From development to motor control. Neuroscientist, 2, 161–173. McCartney, G., & Hepper, P. (1999). Development of lateralized behaviour in the human fetus from 12 to 27 weeks’ gestation. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 41, 83–86. McCormick, C. M., & Maurer, D. M. (1988). Unimanual hand preferences in 6-month-olds: Consistency and relation to familial-handedness. Infant Behavior and Development, 11, 21–29. McDonnell, P. M., Anderson, V. E., & Abraham, W. C. (1983). Asymmetry and orientation of arm movements in three- to eight-week-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 287–298. Side Differences in Infant Reaching Kinematics 459 McManus, I. C., Sik, G., Cole, D. R., Mellon, A. F., Wong, J., & Kloss, J. (1988). The development of handedness in children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 257–273. Michel, G. F. (1981). Right-handedness: A consequence of infant supine head-orientation prefererence? Science, 212, 685–687. Michel, G. F., & Harkins, D. A. (1986). Postural and lateral asymmetries in the ontogeny of handedness during infancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 19, 247–258. Morange-Majoux, F., Peze, A., & Bloch, H. (2000). Organisation of left and right hand movement in a prehension task: A longitudinal study from 20 to 32 weeks. Laterality, 5, 351– 362. Myklebust, B. M., & Gottlieb, G. L. (1997). Spinal reflex organization in early development: Electrophysiological measures and proposed motor pathways. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 3, 175–183. O’Callaghan, M. J., Burn, Y. R., Mohay, H. A., Rogers, Y., & Tudehope, D. I. (1993). The prevalence and origins of left hand preference in high-risk infants, and its implications for intellectual, motor and behavioral performance at 4 and 6 years. Cortex, 29, 617–627. Previc, F. H. (1991). A general theory concerning the prenatal origins of cerebral lateralization in humans. Psychological Review, 98, 299–334. Provins, K. A. (1997). Handedness and speech: A critical reappraisal of the role of genetic and environmental factors in the cerebral lateralization of function. Psychological Review, 104, 554–571. Rönnqvist, L. (2003). Developmentally, the arm preference precedes handedness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 238–239. Sacco, S., Moutard, M. L., & Fagard, J. (2006). Agenesis of the corpus callosum and the establishment of handedness. Developmental Psychobiology (this issue). Steenhuis, R. E., & Bryden, M. P. (1999). The relation between hand preference and hand performance: What you get depends on what you measure. Laterality, 4, 3–26. Toga, A. W., & Thompson, P. M. (2003). Temporal dynamics of brain anatomy. Annual Review of Biomedical Engineering, 5, 119–145. van der Heide, J. C., Fock, J. M., Otten, B., Stremmelaar, E., & Hadders-Algra, M. (2005). Kinematic characteristics of reaching movements in preterm children with cerebral palsy. Pediatric Research, 57, 883–889. von Hofsten, C. (1979). Development of visually guided reaching: The approach phase. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 5, 160–178. von Hofsten, C. (1991). Structuring of early reaching movements: A longitudinal study. Journal of Motor Behavior, 23, 280–292. von Hofsten, C., & Rönnqvist, L. (1988). Preparation for grasping an object: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 610–621. White, B. L., Castle, P., & Held, R. (1964). Observations on the development of visually-directed reaching. Child Development, 35, 349–364.