Exp Brain Res (2006) 168: 88–97 DOI 10.1007/s00221-005-0070-6 R ES E AR C H A RT I C L E Slobodan Jaric Æ Jeffrey J. Collins Æ Rahul Marwaha Elizabeth Russell Interlimb and within limb force coordination in static bimanual manipulation task Received: 20 March 2005 / Accepted: 13 May 2005 / Published online: 3 August 2005 Springer-Verlag 2005 Abstract The aim of the study was to compare the coordination of hand grip (G) and load force (a force that tends to cause slippage of a grasped object; L) in static bimanual manipulation tasks with the same data obtained from the similar dynamic tasks. Based on the previous findings obtained from dynamic tasks, it was hypothesized that an increase in the rate of L change would be predominantly associated with a decrease in the coordination of the within limb forces (coordination of G and L of each hand as assessed through the correlation coefficients), while a decrease in coordination of interlimb forces (between two G and two L) will be less pronounced. Regarding the pattern of modulation of G, the same increase in L frequency was also expected to be associated with a decrease in G gain and an increase in G offset (as assessed by slope and intercept of the regression lines obtained from G to L diagrams, respectively), as well as with an increase in average G/L ratio. Subjects exerted oscillatory isometric L profiles by simultaneous pulling out two handles of an externally fixed device under an exceptionally wide range of L frequencies (0.67–3.33 Hz). The results demonstrated relatively high correlation coefficients between both the interlimb and within limb forces that were only moderately affected under sub-maximal L frequencies. Furthermore, the hypothesized changes in G gain and offset appeared only under the highest L frequency, while the G/L ratio remained unaffected. We conclude that, when compared with the dynamic tasks based on the unconstrained movements of hand-held objects that produce similar pattern of L change, the static manipulation tasks demonstrate a consistent and highly coordinated pattern of bilateral G and L under a wide range of frequencies. S. Jaric (&) Æ J. J. Collins Æ R. Marwaha Æ E. Russell Department of Health, Nutrition, and Exercise Sciences, Human Performance Lab, University of Delaware, 547 S. College Av., Newark, DE 19716, USA E-mail: jaric@udel.edu Tel.: +1-302-8316174 Fax: +1-302-8313693 However, the neural mechanisms that play a role in the revealed differences need further elucidation. Keywords Hand Æ Grip force Æ Load force Æ Correlation Æ Frequency Æ Oscillatory pattern Introduction \A number of daily activities involve bimanual manipulation, such as repositioning hand-held objects, operating tools, or using external supports. This manipulation requires coordination of both the interlimb and within limb forces for successful task performance. The interlimb coordination refers to the coordination of forces applied by two hands against the load imposed through a handheld object or objects, which is required for successful manipulation (Kelso et al. 1979; Scholz and Latash 1998). The within limb coordination refers to the requirement of keeping the grip force (G) of each hand within a certain range relative to the acting load (in further text, load force; L) that not only prevents slippage, but also avoids excessive G that can crush the object or cause rapid fatigue (Johansson and Westling 1988; Flanagan and Wing 1995). The following three paragraphs will specifically address the basic properties of the within limb force coordination, the interlimb force coordination, and the pattern of G modulation regarding changes in L. Particular attention will be given to the effects associated with an increase in frequency of L change. If G is limited to the normal force component acting upon the surface of a hand-held object, while L is the tangential force tending to cause slippage, a continuous adjustment of G–L variations caused by vertical shaking and point-to-point movements (Flanagan and Wing 1995, 1997; Nowak et al. 2002; Zatsiorsky et al. 2004), or gravity changes due to parabolic flight (White et al. 2005) has been reported with virtually no time lag between them (Flanagan and Wing 1995; Scholz and Latash 1998; Blank et al. 2001; Gysin et al. 2003; 89 Zatsiorsky et al. 2004). Based on these observations, it has been concluded that the coordination of G and L is controlled by predictive, feed-forward mechanisms (Johansson and Westling 1984; Flanagan and Wing 1995) providing a relatively stable grip-to-load (G/L) ratio with a small safety margin that prevents slippage (Johansson and Westling 1984; Serrien and Wiesendanger 2001c, d). Both the low level of the G/L ratio and a high level of modulation of G with respect to L proved to be difficult to maintain under increased difficulty of the task performed, such as an increase in frequency of vertical point-to-point movements or vertical shaking of handheld objects or dissimilar actions of two hands (Flanagan and Wing 1995; Blank et al. 2001; Serrien and Wiesendanger 2001a, b, c, d; Bracewell et al. 2003; McDonnell et al. 2004; Zatsiorsky et al. 2004). Nevertheless, it has been suggested that G/L ratio could be a controlled variable that is adjusted to the friction of the contact surface (Flanagan and Wing 1995; Burstedt et al. 1999; Serrien and Wiesendanger 2001c). There are several under-explored aspects of the within limb force coordination regarding potential differences between the static and dynamic tasks. For example, most of the mentioned studies have been based on unrestricted movements of a hand-held object, while there are virtually no data on static tasks that allow for better control of the experimental conditions. Although the important role of glabrous skin receptors in assessment of sufficient G (Johansson and Westling 1984; Monzee et al. 2003) may be unaffected by switching from static to dynamic task conditions, Zatsiorsky et al. (2004) suggested partly independent mechanisms for the adjustment of G to static and dynamic components of L. In addition to the within limb coordination of G and L, bimanual manipulations also require interlimb coordination of two G and two L exerted by each hand. The interlimb force coordination has been demonstrated in both similar and dissimilar tasks performed with two hands (Scholz and Latash 1998; Ohki and Johansson 1999; Perrig et al. 1999; Li et al. 2001; Serrien and Wiesendanger 2001a, b, c, d; Bracewell et al. 2003). When applied on the dissimilar bimanual tasks, an increase in the frequency is also believed to lead to ‘bimanual assimilation’ causing either gradual or abrupt switching to similar bimanual actions (Kelso et al. 1979; Kelso 1984; Spijkers and Heuer 1995; Serrien and Wiesendanger 2001d; Bracewell et al. 2003). A number of authors explained these phenomena by neural contralateral interactions that particularly affect dissimilar tasks performed by two hands (Swinnen et al. 1991). As a consequence, one could expect that the coupling of two lateral G or L could be relatively stronger at higher frequencies. However, the interlimb coordination of two lateral G and L has not been compared with the within limb coordination of the same forces exerted in a bimanual manipulation task. Importance of this comparison has been either explicitly or implicitly stressed in a number of recent studies. For example, the level of interference between motor commands for one and both arms is generally unknown (de Oliveira et al. in press). Although it could be task specific (Bracewell et al. 2003), and characterized by various transient phenomena (Ohki and Johansson 1999; Steglich et al. 1999), the question remains whether the G/L ratio is a ‘local’ (i.e. specified for each hand separately) or ‘global parameter’ (specified for both hands; Serrien and Wiesendanger 2001c, d). Regarding the pattern of G modulation with respect to change in L, an increase in frequency of vertical shaking of an object leads to a gradual decrease in both the consistency and range of G modulation with respect to L, while the overall G/L ratio increases (Flanagan and Wing 1993, 1995; Zatsiorsky et al. 2004). As a result, the frequency associated changes in the regression lines calculated from the G–L diagrams demonstrate a decrease in slope (G gain) associated with an increase in intercepts (G offset) of G modulation with respect to changes in L, as well as an increase in the average G/L ratio (increase in G offset). This phenomenon has been explained in different ways. Flanagan and Wing (Flanagan and Wing 1993, 1995) suggested that the G gain and the G/L ratio vary independently across the conditions. In particular, to economize muscular effort, the CNS keeps a high G gain in low frequency tasks, which results in a low and stable G/L ratio (Johansson and Westling 1984). Due to a high cost of G modulation in rapid (e.g. high frequency) movements, the system reduces G gain but, therefore, increases the G offset and G/L ratio to prevent slippage. Note that the hypothesized behavior could apply to both the static and dynamic manipulation conditions. Conversely, Zatsiorsky et al. (2004) decomposed the grip force into a ‘static fraction’ that is responsible for counteracting L solely originating from the object’s weight (and, thus, assessed from static conditions), a ‘stato-dynamic fraction’ that reflects a steady increase in static fraction due to shaking the object, and a ‘dynamic fraction’ originating solely from modulation of G with respect to the inertial component of L. They proposed these fractions to be controlled partly independently, presuming that the CNS was able to distinguish among them. However, the hypothesized behavior applies only to the tasks performed under dynamic conditions since static manipulation does not produce the inertial component of L. Within the present study, a bimanual static manipulation task was tested, while both G and L of two hands were recorded. Specifically, the subjects exerted laterally symmetric pulling L in an oscillatory fashion under a wide range of frequencies paced by a metronome. The first aim was to explore the interlimb and within limb coordination of G and L in the tested task. We hypothesized that the within limb coordination (as assessed through the correlation coefficients between G and L of each hand) will be more affected by an increase in L frequency than the interlimb coordination (i.e. the correlation between two G and two L). The second aim of the study was to explore the pattern of G modulation, in particular, the effect of the L frequency on the G gain, G offset and the average G/L ratio. Specifically, results in line to those observed under 90 dynamic conditions would support the suggested frequency related trade-off between the G gain and G offset, and the G/L ratio as a general strategy of the CNS applicable to both dynamic and static manipulation tasks. Alternatively, a difference from the previous findings based on the unconstrained movements would suggest that the within limb force coordination of manipulative tasks could be task specific regarding the static and dynamic manipulation conditions. Methods Participants The study was performed on ten healthy human volunteers (six women and four men) aged between 19 and 29 years. Subjects completed the ten-point Edinburgh Inventory to quantify their handedness (Oldfield 1971). Nine subjects appeared to be right handed and one left handed, all showing the maximum scores on the applied scale. The experimental procedure was conducted in accordance to Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Human Subjects Review Board of the University of Delaware. Experimental device The experimental device is shown schematically in Fig. 1. It consists of two coupled handles covered by nylon and four force transducers (miniature strain gauge Fig. 1 Schematic representation of the experimental device. The circles illustrate the positions of four fingers and the thumbs applying a precision grip on two lateral handles of the device. The transducers recording both the right and left grip force, as well as the right and left load force (L indicated with arrows) exerted horizontally in the direction of compression/ tension are depicted with squares and while the direction of the recorded force is indicated with arrows load cells WMC-50, Interface Inc., Atlanta, GA, USA) allowing simultaneous recording of the two independent contralateral compression/tension forces (in further text ‘load force’ (L)) exerted along the long axis of the device and the grip forces (G) of each hand. The device was fixed in a horizontal position, symmetrically with respect to the subject’s sagittal plane, while the height of the device was individually adjusted for each subject to provide a ‘comfortably position’. Subjects sat comfortably in a chair and held the device in front of them with the tips of all five fingers (‘pinch grip’) with the elbows supported by pads positioned on the top of the table in front of the subject and forearms oriented vertically. The same handle aperture of 4.5 cm was applied in all subjects. Note that we were interested in the overall L frequency effects rather than the across the subject comparisons. Therefore, we adjusted the aperture to a ‘comfortable size’ for our subject instead of recording the hand sizes and adjusting the device aperture to each subject individually. Experimental procedure The experimental procedure consisted of three consecutive steps. First, the minimum G/L ratio that prevents slippage (‘slip point’) was assessed according to methods applied in previous studies (Flanagan and Wing 1995). In short, subjects held the a distal end of the device’s handle stationary in vertical position with one hand while gradually reducing their grip force. The ratio between G at the moment slippage began (recorded as an 91 abrupt decrease in L) and the weight of the device (corresponding to L) was calculated as the slip point. Second, the maximum pinch force of each hand was recorded by a pinch dynamometer (JAMAR, range 200 N). Finally, the subjects were tested on a bimanual manipulation task performed under isometric conditions. They were asked to hold the device with the tips of their fingers and pull the handles laterally equally with both hands (i.e. to produce L in the direction of tension) and, thereafter, to relax, which was expected to provide an oscillatory L pattern. To provide a feedback, the current average of the two lateral L exerted by the subject’s right and left hand was shown on a computer screen, as well as two horizontal lines depicting the target levels for the peaks of L. The target level for the minimum peak of L was zero, while the target level of the maximum peak of L corresponded to 25% of the tested maximum pinch G since a pilot experiment suggested that exerting L below 30% of the maximum pinch G would not cause fatigue. The rationale of this approach was the findings that for the given body position the subjects’ ability to exert high L was limited by their G, as well as the presumption of proportionality of the subjects’ ability to exert G and L. To familiarize with the task, the subjects were allowed to practice for 15 min prior to the experiment. The main experimental factor applied was the frequency of the oscillatory L profile paced by a metronome. Preliminary experiments revealed that most of the subjects start switching from feed-forward to feedback control of the exerted L at the frequencies below 0.6 Hz, which presumably causes a change in involved neural mechanisms. Specifically, the subjects reported that they were able to ‘‘draw entire cycle of the force’’ using online corrections based on the current visual feedback, instead on just relying on previous L peaks for correcting the forthcoming ones. In addition, the highest L frequency that the subjects were able to follow was between 3.33 and 4 Hz. Therefore, five frequencies were selected to cover the interval between these two limits (specifically, 0.66, 1.33, 2, 2.67 and 3.33 Hz) presumably providing feed-forward control mechanisms of the tested trials. The order of the corresponding five experimental trials (i.e. one trial per each frequency) was randomized. Data processing The duration of each trial was 12 s. However, to avoid analyzing the initial adjustment to the instructed L level, the final 9 s were taken for further analysis. The signals from all four force transducers were digitized on line at the rate of 200 Hz, low-pass filtered (10 Hz) and stored on a computer disk for further analysis. To assess the task performance, constant and variable errors were calculated from the differences between the consecutive peaks of the averaged L and the aimed maximum level of force depicted at the screen. To assess the coordination of the recorded forces, the cross cor- relations between the pairs of both the interlimb (G versus L of each hand) and within limb (G versus G and L versus L) forces were calculated. The cross correlation between two lateral G/L ratios was also calculated. However, the time lags proved to be small relative to the durations of particular L cycles. In particular, when averaged across the subjects and hands, the L frequencies 0.67, 1.33, 2, 2.67 and 3.33 Hz revealed the time lags (Mean ± SD) 3.6±7.3, 1.6±7.1, 2.8±3.9, 4.4±5.5 and 6.4±5.2 ms (positive sign meaning that G precedes L), respectively. Therefore, we decided to present only the Pearson’s correlation coefficients instead. In addition to the G versus L correlation coefficients, we also assessed the pattern of G modulation by calculating G gain and offset relative to L changes, as well as the average G/L ratio. The G gain and offset were obtained from the slope and intercept, respectively, of the linear relationship between G and L assessed from a linear regression model (see (Flanagan and Wing 1995) for similar approach; see also right hand panels of Fig. 2 for illustration). A standard software package STATISTICA for Windows (SoftStat) was used for statistical analysis. In addition to descriptive statistics, repeated measures ANOVAs were applied to test the effects of force pair (G versus L of each hand, G versus G and L versus L), hand dominance, and frequency on the Z-transformed correlation coefficients, while the Schefee test was applied as a post hoc test. The effect of frequency on variable errors, slope, intercept and G/L ratio was assessed by repeated measures one-way ANOVA. Lateral differences between two G and two L, as well as between two lateral G/L ratios were assessed by paired t test. Results Force profiles and task performance Maximum pinch force (averaged across the subjects and hands) was (Mean ± SD) 52±13 N demonstrating no effect of hand dominance. The recorded G and L forces obtained from the dominant hand of a representative subject under the lowest (0.67 Hz), medium (2 Hz) and highest (3.33 Hz) frequency are depicted in Fig. 2 (left panels). Regarding the L profiles, higher frequencies demonstrate the expected sinusoidal pattern, while the 0.67 Hz trial profile appears to be somewhat irregular. However, a more important finding is that the depicted force profiles suggest a high level of coordination of both the interlimb and within limb forces. In general, the modulation of G seems to be both comparable in size and temporally synchronized with the changes of L, although the depicted force patterns also suggest a moderate decrease in G modulation associated with an increase in L frequency. Note also that instead of the expected relaxation of pulling force (i.e. zero L at the end of each cycle), this particular subject demonstrated a moderate pushing 92 Fig. 2 The data obtained from a representative subject at the lowest (top panels), medium (middle panels) and highest frequency trial (bottom panels). Left-hand side panels show the recorded grip and load forces. Right-hand side panels depict the corresponding G versus L diagrams of the dominant arm together with the corresponding regression lines, regression equations and correlation coefficients. For illustrative purposes, the left-hand side graphs show the data only for the middle 4 s of the each trial, while the grip-load diagrams, as well as the regression data are based on all 9 s force. According to our recent results (Jaric et al. 2005), switching from uni-directional to bi-directional exerting of L could be associated with both an increase in G/L ratio and a decrease in the correlation between G and L, which could affect the important findings of the study (see further text for details). However, we also found a similar number of the trials/cycles that demonstrated somewhat insufficient relaxation. The most important finding is that this phenomenon was mainly a subject specific and also not affected by the change in frequency. Regarding the interlimb differences in the recorded forces and force ratios, the results (averaged across the subjects and trials) revealed that the dominant hand exerted both an 8% stronger G and 9% stronger L than the non-dominant hand (P<0.001; paired t test). However, note that these two differences provided a similar offset of G (i.e. averaged G/L ratio) for the dominant and non-dominant hand. Since the remaining data showed neither the main effect of hand dominance nor the interactions with other factors tested, all of the data throughout this and the following section will be presented as averaged across two hands. The constant errors (averaged across the subjects and trials) were 0.3±0.9 N suggesting no effect of frequency. The variable errors recorded from 0.66, 1.33, 2, 2.67 and 3.33 Hz trials were 0.69±0.33, 0.64±0.23, 0.89±0.33, 1.02±0.36 and 1.13±0.45, respectively, revealing a significant effect of frequency [F(4,36)=5.3; P<0.01]. In 93 particular, the variable error recorded in the 3.33 Hz trial was higher than the same error recorded in 0.66 and 1.33 Hz. 1 Coordination between the interlimb and within limb forces Figure 3 shows indices of coordination as assessed through median correlation coefficients obtained from the interlimb (G versus G and L versus L) and within limb forces (G versus L, averaged for two hands) recorded under five L frequencies. The statistical analysis performed on the Fisher-Z transformed correlation coefficients revealed the effect of force pair [F(2,18)=30, P<0.001], as well as of frequency [F(4,36)=31, P<0.001]. In particular, L versus L provided higher correlation coefficients than the remaining two force pairs, while the 0.67 Hz trial provided a higher correlation coefficient and the 3.33 Hz trial provided a lower correlation coefficients than the remaining three trials performed at intermediate frequencies. Note that the non-significant ‘force’ · ‘frequency’ interaction [F(8,72)=0.7; P>0.05] suggested that the frequency did not affect coordination of interlimb and within limb forces differently. Also note a lack of difference in the within limb coordination between the dominant and non-dominant hand. The same figure also shows the correlation coefficients obtained from two lateral G/L ratios that were not included in the presented statistical analysis (see previous paragraph). When compared with the same coefficients obtained from the pairs of interlimb and within limb forces, the correlation coefficients obtained between two lateral G/L ratios appear to be considerably lower and also not affected by the change in frequency [F(4,45)=0.2, P>0.05]. Pattern of G modulation: offset, gain and G/L ratio Of particular importance for the study is the frequency associated changes in the pattern of the G modulation obtained from G versus L diagrams (for illustration see right hand panels of Fig. 2). The pattern specifically refers to the G gain and offset illustrated by the regression lines depicted in the same figures, as well as to the average G/L ratio. The G gain and offset were assessed through the linear regressions calculated from G versus L diagrams (also illustrated in Fig. 2). Specifically, a high gain (i.e. a high level of modulation of G with respect to change in L) and low offset were expected to be revealed through a high slope and low intercept, respectively. Since no effect of hand on the tested variables was recorded (see previous text for details), the results were averaged across both the subjects and hands (see top panels of Fig. 4). The one-way ANOVA revealed a significant decrease in slopes [F(4,36)=11, P<0.001] and an increase in intercepts [F(4,36)=16, P<0.001] associated with an increase in frequency. The Correlation Coefficient 0.9 G/L G/G L/L G/L-G/L 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.66 0.67 1.32 1.33 1.98 2.00 2.64 2.67 3.3 3.33 Frequency (Hz) Fig. 3 Median correlation coefficients calculated from the interlimb (between two load (L) and between two grip (G) forces), as well as from the within limb (between G and L, averaged across two hands). The correlation coefficient between two lateral G/L ratios is also depicted post hoc analysis revealed that both findings were based on the difference between the highest (i.e. 3.33 Hz) and four lower frequencies (see Fig. 4 for details). When averaged across the hands, subjects and frequencies, the average G/L ratio was 0.92±0.21 (mean ± SD). Since the slip point (averaged across the subjects and hands) was 0.64±0.06 (mean ± SD), this finding suggests a safety margin of 44% (i.e. the G was on average 44% higher than the minimum G needed to prevent slippage due to the action of L). However, of particular importance is the finding that the G offset suggested no effect of frequency [F(4,36)=0.5, P>0.05; see bottom panel of Fig. 4 for illustration]. The data presented in Fig. 4 (i.e. slope, intercept and G/L ratio, averaged across the subjects and hands) were used to illustrate the effect of frequency on the pattern of the G modulation (Fig. 5). The depicted regression lines reveal the results in line with the findings based on the statistical analysis (see previous paragraph). In particular, except a moderate decrease in G gain and an increase in G offset obtained under the highest L frequency, the pattern of G modulation as assessed through G offset, gain and G/L ratio remained unaffected by change of L frequency within the most of the tested range. Discussion General considerations Regarding the specific aims of the present study, the results obtained revealed two major findings. First, the 94 Fig. 4 Indices of the pattern of G modulation (averaged across the subjects and hands; error bars represent standard errors). The top panels illustrate the slope and intercept (in N) of the regression lines obtained from the G versus L diagrams, while the bottom panel illustrates the average G/L ratio (* different from lower frequencies; + different from 0.67 Hz). Note that the slope and intercepts depict the G gain and offset, respectively Intercept Slope 1 3.5 * 3 0.8 2.5 * 0.6 2 + 1.5 0.4 1 0.2 0.5 0 0.67 1.33 2.00 2.67 0 3.33 0.67 Frequency (Hz) 1.33 2.00 2.67 3.33 Frequency (Hz) G/L Ratio 1.2 1 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 0.67 1.33 2.00 2.67 3.33 Frequency (Hz) coordination of both the interlimb and within limb forces, as revealed through the correlations calculated among them, proved to be high, particularly for two L. In addition, all correlation coefficients decreased at a similar rate with an increase in L frequency. Second, the pattern of G modulation appeared to be relatively stable since the G offset and gain remained only moderately affected by an increase in frequency, while the average G/L ratio remained unchanged. Prior to interpreting these data within the following subsections, two potentially important methodological points need to be stressed. First, due to the nature of the task, the recorded G and L mainly demonstrated a sinusoidal pattern, although some visible irregularities appeared at the lowest frequency (see Fig. 3). This pattern remains preserved when the rate of force change is calculated (i.e. the derivative of sinusoidal function is also sinusoidal). Therefore, the findings of the present study could be compared not only with the studies based on the G and L forces, but also with those based on the force derivatives (Ohki and Johansson 1999; Serrien and Wiesendanger 2001a, b, c; Bracewell et al. 2003; Gysin et al. 2003). Second, the tested frequency interval 0.66– 3.33 Hz applied in the present study was considerably broader than the intervals in other studies (e.g. 1.43– Fig. 5 Illustration of the regression lines depicting G gain, offset and the average G/L ratio (data averaged across subjects and hands; based on the data from Fig. 4) obtained from trials performed under five different frequencies 95 3.13 Hz in (Flanagan and Wing 1995), 1–2 Hz in (Zatsiorsky et al. 2004) and 1.33–2.67 Hz in (Jaric et al. 2005). Since the size of the frequency effect should increase with the range of the applied L frequencies, this difference needs to be taken into account in further discussion. Comparison of the interlimb and within limb coordination The coordination of the interlimb forces as assessed by correlation coefficients was comparable with other data obtained from static manipulation conditions (Diedrichsen et al. 2003; Jaric et al. 2005), but remarkably high when compared with a discrete dynamical task (Bracewell et al. 2003). Regarding the coordination of the within limb forces, the available data are inconsistent, partly due to differences in the task tested and/or variables evaluated. For example, most of the experiments have been performed on discrete vertical movements while both the forces and their rates of change were correlated. In addition, the recorded forces were correlated through both their time series and their peaks to assess the within limb force coordination. Taken together, the results revealed relatively low correlation coefficients between the within limb forces (Flanagan and Wing 1997; Augurelle et al. 2003; Bracewell et al. 2003; Gilles and Wing 2003; McDonnell et al. 2004) when compared with the present study. However, the within limb force control in short-lasting discrete tasks and continuous oscillation tasks could be different due to various transient neural processes that particularly characterize force initiation (Ohki and Johansson 1999; Steglich et al. 1999). Therefore, of more importance could be the comparison of the present data with studies based on the vertical shaking of hand-held objects or static tasks that provided force profiles similar to those recorded in the present study. These studies revealed either a comparable (Flanagan et al. 1993; Zatsiorsky et al. 2004; Jaric et al. 2005) or a lower level of the within limb coordination of G and L (Flanagan and Wing 1995; Gysin et al. 2003). In addition to high correlation coefficients observed between both the interlimb and within limb forces, a relatively weak effect of the task frequency on these coefficients could also deserve attention. Even the frequency 3.33 Hz revealed the correlation coefficients regarding both the interlimb and within limb coordination above 0.8, which is well above the same coefficients observed from discrete movements with lower rate of force changes (Bracewell et al. 2003; McDonnell et al. 2004). In addition, the frequency-associated effect was similar for the interlimb and within limb coordination, although an increase in task difficulty caused by increased frequency (also supported by an increase in variable errors) is generally expected to lead to a relatively stronger interlimb coordination (see Introduction for details). One could speculate that both the high level of force coordination and a relatively weak effect of frequency suggest that the tested isometric task could be easier to control than the tasks based on unconstrained vertical hand movements. In particular, the high correlation coefficients observed among the recorded forces could suggest that all four forces may share the same single central command. Strong ‘bimanual assimilation’ of two G/L ratios in a dissimilar bimanual task obtained in a number of previous studies (Kelso et al. 1979; Perrig et al. 1999; Serrien and Wiesendanger 2001b, c) also speaks in favor of this assumption. However, the correlation coefficients observed between the two lateral G/ L ratios appear to be relatively low when compared with the same coefficients observed between two lateral G and L, as well as with the correlation coefficients between G and L of each hand that provide the lateral G/L ratios [see Jaric et al. (2005) for similar findings). Therefore, despite a high level of coordination of both the interlimb and within limb forces, the results suggest that G/L ratios could be partly independently controlled. As a result, the G/L ratio could not be a ‘global parameter’ (i.e., specified for two hands; Serrien and Wiesendanger 2001c, d) in the tested task. Finally, note that it remains possible, that despite a hypothetically high coordination (i.e., high correlation coefficient) between two particular forces observed within each single cycle, the overall coordination obtained from the entire trial could still remain low. This outcome would be caused by an inconsistent overall G or L level recorded in consecutive cycles within the same trial (see right hand panels of Fig. 2 for illustration). Therefore, the obtained high correlation coefficients generally suggest that both the interlimb and within limb forces demonstrate not only a highly coordinated, but also a consistent pattern of the recorded forces over a number of consecutive cycles. Pattern of G modulation: gain, offset and G/L ratio In addition to the comparison of coordination of interlimb and within limb forces, we were particularly interested in the effect of frequency on the pattern of the G modulation. Specifically, the experimental results were expected to reveal whether the frequency associated changes in the modulation of G with respect to changes in L under the tested static conditions correspond to the same modulation previously observed in the tasks performed under dynamic conditions. We found this correspondence to be only partial. Specifically, an increase in L frequency was associated with a decrease G gain (reflected in both decrease in slope of G versus L regression lines) an increase in G offset. These findings appear to be generally in line with the findings observed from dynamic conditions, such as unconstrained vertical movements or shaking of a hand-held object (Flanagan and Wing 1995; Zatsiorsky et al. 2004). The recorded safety margin was also either close to the safety margin 96 obtained from the free movement tasks (Gilles and Wing 2003; Zatsiorsky et al. 2004), or somewhat lower (Serrien and Wiesendanger 2001d; Nowak et al. 2003). However, we also found a number of discrepancies between our findings and the findings obtained from dynamic tasks. First, note that the recorded changes in G gain and offset were relatively weak and mainly originated from the data obtained from the highest frequency. This frequency (i.e., 3.33 Hz) was not only exceptionally high when compared to other studies, but also close to the physiological limits of the subjects (see Methods for details). Second, G/L ratio did not demonstrate the expected frequency associated increase. Therefore, when compared to similar dynamic tasks, we could conclude that the tested static task revealed a high interlimb and within limb force coordination, while the pattern of the G modulation remained predominantly preserved within a broad range of the L frequency. Since a number of methodological factors can affect the studied coordination (e.g. force profiles, range of frequencies, hand conditions, etc.; see previous text for details), more research is needed to support the abovementioned conclusion regarding the differences in the control of static and dynamic manipulation tasks. Nevertheless, two implications of the discussed findings deserve to be mentioned. First, since the importance of the afferent sensory information from skin receptors for G and L coordination has been both extensively studied and strongly emphasized (Johansson and Westling 1984; Monzee et al. 2003), the L profiles acting through the contact between the skin and hand-held object recorded in the present study closely correspond to the same profiles obtained from unconstrained vertical movements. Therefore, it is unlikely that the skin receptors could play an important role in the observed differences between the static and dynamic manipulation tasks. Second, since it appears that the CNS distinguishes between the static and dynamic manipulation tasks, the suggested pattern of G modulation based on the tradeoff of between the G gain and offset and average G/L ratio adjusted for different rates of L change (Flanagan and Wing 1995) cannot be accepted as a control pattern that applies to all manipulation tasks. Therefore, the present data indirectly supports the concept of Zatsiorsky et al. (2004) of partly independent fractions of G modulation originating from coupling with the static and dynamic components of L. Concluding remarks As compared with the similar tasks performed under dynamic conditions, the present study revealed a relatively high and consistent coordination of both the interlimb and within limb forces in a symmetric bimanual manipulation task performed under static conditions. A potentially more important and a rather novel finding is that the pattern of G modulation with respect to L change remains predominantly unaffected within the most of the range of L frequencies. Taken together, the results of the present study suggest that the control mechanisms of the manipulation tasks performed under static and dynamic conditions could be partly different, even when these tasks are based on a similar pattern of G and L change. However, taking into account the specificity of the tested task, further research is needed to generalize these findings to other task conditions, as well as to explore specific neural control mechanisms that play a role in the observed phenomena. Comparisons of discrete and long lasting continuous tasks, as well as of unimanual and bimanual tasks performed under both static and dynamic conditions could deserve particular attention. Acknowledgments The study was supported in part by a grant from the National Multiple Sclerosis Society (PP1018) and a grant from the University of Delaware Research Foundation to the first author. References Augurelle AS, Smith AM, Lejeune T, Thonnard JL (2003) Importance of cutaneous feedback in maintaining a secure grip during manipulation of hand-held objects. J Neurophysiol 89:665–671 Blank R, Breitenbach A, Nitschke M, Heizer W, Letzgus S, Hermsdorfer J (2001) Human development of grip force modulation relating to cyclic movement-induced inertial loads. Exp Brain Res 138:193–199 Bracewell RM, Wing AM, Soper HM, Clark KG (2003) Predictive and reactive co-ordination of grip and load forces in bimanual lifting in man. Eur J Neurosci 18:2396–2402 Burstedt MK, Flanagan JR, Johansson RS (1999) Control of grasp stability in humans under different frictional conditions during multidigit manipulation. J Neurophysiol 82:2393–2405 Diedrichsen J, Hazeltine E, Nurss WK, Ivry RB (2003) The role of the corpus callosum in the coupling of bimanual isometric force pulses. J Neurophysiol 90:2409–2418 Flanagan JR, Wing AM (1993) Modulation of grip force with load force during point-to-point arm movements. Exp Brain Res 95:131–143 Flanagan JR, Wing AM (1995) The stability of precision grip forces during cyclic arm movements with a hand-held load. Exp Brain Res 105:455–464 Flanagan JR, Wing AM (1997) The role of internal models in motion planning and control: evidence from grip force adjustments during movements of hand-held loads. J Neurosci 17:1519–1528 Flanagan JR, Tresilian J, Wing AM (1993) Coupling of grip force and load force during arm movements with grasped objects. Neurosci Lett 152:53–56 Gilles MA, Wing AM (2003) Age-related changes in grip force and dynamics of hand movement. J Mot Behav 35:79–85 Gysin P, Kaminski TR, Gordon AM (2003) Coordination of fingertip forces in object transport during locomotion. Exp Brain Res 149:371–379 Jaric S, Russell EM, Collins JJ, Marwaha R (2005) Coordination of hand grip and load forces in unidirectional and bidirectional static force production tasks. Neurosci Lett 381:51–56 Johansson RS, Westling G (1984) Roles of glabrous skin receptors and sensorimotor memory in automatic control of precision grip when lifting rougher or more slippery objects. Exp Brain Res 56:550–564 Johansson RS, Westling G (1988) Coordinated isometric muscle commands adequately and erroneously programmed for the weight during lifting task with precision grip. Exp Brain Res 71:59–71 97 Kelso JA (1984) Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. Am J Physiol 246:R1000–R1004 Kelso JA, Southard DL, Goodman D (1979) On the coordination of two-handed movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 5:229–238 Li S, Danion F, Latash ML, Li ZM, Zatsiorsky VM (2001) Bilateral deficit and symmetry in finger force production during two-hand multifinger tasks. Exp Brain Res 141:530–540 McDonnell MN, Ridding MC, Flavel SC, Miles TS (2004) Effect of human grip strategy on force control in precision tasks. Exp Brain Res 161(3):368–373 Monzee J, Lamarre Y, Smith AM (2003) The effects of digital anesthesia on force control using a precision grip. J Neurophysiol 89:672–683 Nowak DA, Hermsdorfer J, Marquardt C, Fuchs HH (2002) Grip and load force coupling during discrete vertical arm movements with a grasped object in cerebellar atrophy. Exp Brain Res 145:28–39 Nowak DA, Hermsdorfer J, Marquardt C, Topka H (2003) Moving objects with clumsy fingers: how predictive is grip force control in patients with impaired manual sensibility? Clin Neurophysiol 114:472–487 Ohki Y, Johansson RS (1999) Sensorimotor interactions between pairs of fingers in bimanual and unimanual manipulative tasks. Exp Brain Res 127:43–53 Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113 Perrig S, Kazennikov O, Wiesendanger M (1999) Time structure of a goal-directed bimanual skill and its dependence on task constraints. Behav Brain Res 103:95–104 Scholz JP, Latash ML (1998) A study of a bimanual synergy associated with holding an object. Hum Mov Sci 17:753–779 Serrien DJ, Wiesendanger M (2001a) Bimanual organization of manipulative forces: evidence from erroneous feedforward programming of precision grip. Eur J Neurosci 13:1825–1832 Serrien DJ, Wiesendanger M (2001b) Dissociation of grip/loadforce coupling during a bimanual manipulative assignment. Exp Brain Res 136:417–420 Serrien DJ, Wiesendanger M (2001c) A higher-order mechanism overrules the automatic grip-load force constraint during bimanual asymmetrical movements. Behav Brain Res 118:153– 160 Serrien DJ, Wiesendanger M (2001d) Regulation of grasping forces during bimanual in-phase and anti-phase coordination. Neuropsychologia 39:1379–1384 Spijkers W, Heuer H (1995) Structural constraints on the performance of symmetrical bimanual movements with different amplitudes. Quart J Exp Psychol A 48:716–740 Steglich C, Heuer H, Spijkers W, Kleinsorge T (1999) Bimanual coupling during the specification of isometric forces. Exp Brain Res 129:302–316 Swinnen SP, Young DE, Walter CB, Serrien DJ (1991) Control of asymmetrical bimanual movements. Exp Brain Res 85:163–173 White O, McIntyre J, Augurelle AS, Thonnard JL (2005) Do novel gravitational environments alter the grip-force/load-force coupling at the fingertips? Exp Brain Res 163(3):324–334 Zatsiorsky VM, Gao F, Latash ML (2004) Motor control goes beyond physics: differential effects of gravity and inertia on finger forces during manipulation of hand-held objects. Exp Brain Res 162(3):300–308