This article was originally published in the Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the author's benefit and for the benefit of the author's institution, for noncommercial research and educational use including without limitation use in instruction at your institution, sending it to specific colleagues who you know, and providing a copy to your institution’s administrator. All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including without limitation commercial reprints, selling or licensing copies or access, or posting on open internet sites, your personal or institution’s website or repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission may be sought for such use through Elsevier's permissions site at: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial Toth A.L. (2010) Development, Evolution and Behavior. In: Breed M.D. and Moore J., (eds.) Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior, volume 1, pp. 500-506 Oxford: Academic Press. © 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Author's personal copy Development, Evolution and Behavior A. L. Toth, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA ã 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Introduction The goal of this article is to explore the relationships between evolution, development, and behavior. There are two main reasons why such considerations are important to the field of animal behavior – one is the important mechanistic links between behavior and development, and the second relates to conceptual insights that can be gained from the field of evolutionary developmental biology. With respect to the first topic, it is abundantly clear that behavior and development are intimately linked. The brain develops both during embryological and adult stages, and this development can be a link between the environment and plasticity in individual behavioral responses. In addition, some forms of behavior develop and change over the lifetime of an individual, and thus can be considered developmental processes themselves. Second, studies of the molecular genetic basis of morphological development have progressed further than those of behavior. Molecular developmental biology paired with a comparative, evolutionary perspective has given rise to the field of ‘evo-devo,’ which can provide several lessons that can be applied to the study of behavior. These include the importance of conserved genes and changes in gene regulation in generating novel phenotypes and the utility of breaking down (behavioral or morphological) phenotypes into constituent parts, or ‘modules.’ In the sections that follow, both mechanistic and conceptual links between behavior and development are discussed. The first section explores the various ways in which behavior and development are interrelated and also how behavioral and morphological phenotypes differ but must be considered simultaneously. Then, the field of evolutionary developmental biology and some of the major tenets of evo-devo that can be applied to the study of behavior are described. Next, specific examples from across different animal taxa are reviewed that illustrate how considerations of the principles of evo-devo and the behavior-development relationship can advance our understanding of how behavior evolves. Finally, the article concludes by suggesting future directions for a more comprehensive integration of development and behavior that could lead to further insights into animal behavior. Relationships Between Behavior and Development In many ways, the study of behavior is the study of development. Like any other organ, the nervous system 500 develops during both embryological and adult stages (though certainly to varying degrees) and its development is highly responsive to external and internal environmental factors. Interestingly, some of the same genes can affect both nervous system development during embryonic stages and neural function and behavior in adult animals. One example is the gene fruitless in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. This gene is important for the development of male-specific neuronal projections into abdominal muscles used in mating, but also affects courtship behavior in mature animals. In fact, fruitless derives its name from a particular mutation that causes males to court other males. In addition, there are many known instances of mechanistic links between certain forms of behavior and morphological or physiological development – this can be the result of pleiotropic effects of specific hormones or genes on both the nervous system and other organs. For example, in many vertebrates, testosterone is important in sex determination of the gonads and brain during embryological development, but can also have effects on male aggressive, territorial behavior during adulthood. In female insects, juvenile hormone levels during development influence ovary size and can also affect various forms of adult behavior, including egg-laying and foraging. A prominent example of the pleiotropic effects of insect hormones on both reproduction and behavior involves ‘oogenesis-flight syndrome,’ in which ovarian development is associated with sedentary behavior, and a shut-down of ovarian development is associated with sustained flight. In two species of migratory locusts, Locusta migratoria and Schistocerca gregaria, this syndrome is exhibited in the extreme. Two entirely different locust morphs exist – larger solitary locusts that have narrow foraging ranges, cryptic coloration, and high ovarian development; and smaller gregarious locusts that fly hundreds of miles in search of food, are brightly colored, and have lower ovarian development. In both species, two hormones (juvenile hormone and corazonin) stimulate various aspects of the solitary phase including green coloration, reproductive physiology, and solitary-like behavior. Although behavior and development are in many ways intimately linked, research in the fields of ethology and development have proceeded quite separately. Several reasons likely account for this separation. First, there is a general perception by biologists that behavioral phenotypes are farther removed from genes than developmental phenotypes; behavior is generated by neurons in real time at a rate that is much more rapid than even the fastest known changes in gene expression, whereas development proceeds gradually over hours, weeks, or even years. Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior (2010), vol. 1, pp. 500-506 Author's personal copy Development, Evolution and Behavior In addition, whereas morphological phenotypes are stable or slowly changing, behavioral phenotypes are more unpredictable and fleeting, often making them harder to measure. For these reasons, in-depth studies of the genetic basis for behavior began later and have progressed more slowly than such studies of development. Although some behaviors consist of stereotyped action patterns, many behaviors are more complex and require long-term maturation or a process of development to come to fruition. Such behavioral development may or may not require learning and/or restructuring of the nervous system. Notable examples of behavioral development include song learning and development by songbirds, the transition from hive work to foraging in honeybees, and juvenile play behavior (e.g., play fighting and play hunting) by a wide range of vertebrate animals, especially mammals. By recognizing behavioral phenotypes as developmental phenotypes themselves, it may be fruitful to apply a similar approach to the study of behavior as has been historically used to study development. Basics of ‘Evo-Devo’ During the early history of evolutionary thought, evolution and development were considered to be inseparable. Studies of embryology were used to infer evolutionary relationships among organisms, typified by Ernst Haeckel’s famous insight that ‘ontogeny recapitulates phylogeny.’ Although such comparisons can be useful, subsequent studies in embryology showed that this view is an oversimplification. The adoption of a population-level focus on evolution with the rise of the modern synthesis of genetics and evolution led to a formal separation of the fields of evolution and development until fairly recently. With the application of molecular genetics to developmental biology, the fields of evolution and development were eventually reintegrated in the ‘evo-devo synthesis,’ and numerous important findings have already emerged from this relatively new hybrid field. One of the major discoveries in developmental biology was the revelation that something so complex as multicellular development is genetically orchestrated via precise changes in the timing and location of gene expression. This insight stemmed from the elucidation of a hierarchical cascade of transcription factor genes that lead to the differentiation of segments during embryological development. This developmental cascade was first studied in the fruit fly D. melanogaster and the set of genes controlling early embryological development were elucidated in remarkable detail. This groundbreaking work by Lewis, Nusslein-Volhard, and Wieschaus was awarded with the Nobel Prize in Physiology and Medicine in 1995. By using a comparative approach and studying the molecular genetics of development in other species, the pioneers of evo-devo made a startling discovery. It turns out that 501 many of the same genes regulating early development in insects, specifically, the homeotic or Hox genes which determine the identity of segments, also control the development of segmentation in vertebrates, even in a similar anterior-to-posterior pattern (Figure 1). Cross-species studies of the molecular basis for development have fueled the evo-devo synthesis, and in some cases, the findings have even caused biologists to rethink how new structures arise during evolution. A case in point involves the evolution of image-forming eyes in animals. The compound eyes of arthropods and the camera-like eyes of vertebrates differ hugely in their basic structure, and were long considered to be a classic example of convergent evolution. However, this interpretation was called into question with the discovery of the primary role of Pax6 genes, first identified as affecting vertebrate eye development and subsequently found in Drosophila. Further studies revealed another member of the Pax gene family to have a role in complex eye development in a jellyfish, suggesting Pax involvement in eye development may predate the evolution of the common ancestor of both insects and vertebrates. This finding suggested that vertebrate and insect eyes could have arisen from a proto-eye shared by a common ancestor. Yet another (less likely) possibility is that the Pax genes were coopted to regulate eye development multiple times during animal evolution, and represent a remarkable example of convergent evolution on both genetic and phenotypic levels. Although these issues have not yet been completely resolved, the realization of these complexities of conservation and convergence would not have been possible without molecular genetic studies. A similar depth of study will be necessary to untangle these issues for behavior that are the apparent result of convergent evolution. Evo-devo studies across a diversity of animals including butterflies, ants, and stickleback fish have provided additional insights into how evolution occurs. In particular, it has been fruitful to study convergent morphologies that have evolved repeatedly in several relatively closely related species. Studies of stickleback fish have shown that a convergent phenotype (the reduction of bony armor) can be attained via evolutionary changes in the same molecular pathways, but by altering different individual genes within the pathway. On the other hand, studies of winglessness in worker ants have shown that the genetic pathways that maintain a particular phenotype over evolutionary time can change. Although wing loss evolved only once early in ant evolutionary history, the network of wing development genes is interrupted at different points in different species of modern ants. Insights to Be Gained from an Evo-Devo Approach to Behavior As described earlier, comparative studies of the genetic basis for development have uncovered the deep extent of Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior (2010), vol. 1, pp. 500-506 Author's personal copy 502 Development, Evolution and Behavior Figure 1 Diagram mapping hox genes to specific segments in both a Drosophila and mouse embryo. Adapted from Carroll SB (1995) Homeotic genes and the evolution of arthropods and chordates. Nature 376: 479–485. conservation of gene function across organisms and led to a more careful consideration of the roles of conservation versus convergence in evolution. The field of evo-devo is also beginning to provide broadly generalizable principles about the evolutionary process itself, making it all the more important for behaviorists to consider an evo-devo approach. For example, some authors have suggested a shift in the focus of the levels of selection in evolution from the gene to the phenotype. In addition, evo-devo also forces one to consider the importance of nongenetic, or epigenetic, influences on phenotypes, including the external environment and social (e.g., maternal) effects. Some have suggested that epigenetic effects can lead to the evolution of novel phenotypes even before such changes are fixed by mutations in the gene sequence. The field of evo-devo has led to several main insights, each of which can provide useful lessons for the study of animal behavior: (1) the importance of changes in gene regulation (in addition to changes in coding regions of genes) in generating morphological diversity, (2) the idea of a shared ‘genetic toolkit’ for development consisting of a core set of deeply conserved genes that are used repeatedly across taxa to generate diversity in form, and (3) the idea of modularity, that is, that morphology can be broken down into several distinct components that tend to be repeated in series and can be added, deleted, or shuffled, to create novel morphologies. The Importance of Gene Regulation Mutations in the coding regions of genes can disrupt the basic function of a protein, which can have severe if not lethal effects on the organism. Alterations in gene regulation involving the timing and location of the expression of genes, on the other hand, can result in more subtle changes in phenotype. Thus, regulatory changes have been proposed to be more likely targets for natural selection, which could result in more gradual evolutionary changes. There is a growing base of examples from evo-devo showing the importance of regulatory changes in generating morphological diversity. For example, Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior (2010), vol. 1, pp. 500-506 Author's personal copy Development, Evolution and Behavior changing the localization of transcripts of specific Hox genes can result in a variety of morphological novelties ranging from the patterns on butterfly wings to the shape and number of appendages. Studies of postdevelopmental changes in brain gene expression suggest that this may also be generalizable to behavior. Soon after the shocking discovery that humans and chimpanzees have 98% of their DNA sequences in common, biologists hypothesized that it is differences in gene regulation, rather than differences in coding sequence, that must explain the huge differences in behavior and intelligence between us and them. The application of global gene expression analysis to this question has indeed uncovered largescale changes in brain gene regulation between chimps and humans. Another example of the importance of gene regulation comes from rodents. The localization of vasopressin receptors (V1aR) in a brain region (ventral striatum) in several species of voles makes all the difference between promiscuous, absentee fathers and monogamous, paternal males. There are different levels at which changes in gene regulation can affect a phenotype – at the transcriptional or translational levels. In addition, a distinction has been made between two different types of transcriptional gene regulation: (1) cis regulatory change – a change in gene regulatory sequences that affects transcription of a given gene nearby, and (2) trans regulatory change – a change in one gene that regulates the expression of other genes that may be in a different part of the genome. Mounting evidence from developmental biology suggests that cis regulatory changes appear to be extremely important in the evolution of morphological changes across species. More detailed studies of the gene regulatory networks that affect variation in behavior both within and across species will be needed before an assessment of the importanc of cis versus trans regulation can be made for the field of behavior. Genetic Toolkits for Behavior? Studies of the genetic basis of development across a wide variety of taxa suggest that the existence of a ‘genetic toolkit’ for development, that is, a core set of genes or pathways that underlie morphological development and that are used repeatedly during evolution to generate diversity in body form. Prominent examples from development are homeotic (Hox) genes in segmentation and Pax genes in eye development across both vertebrate and invertebrate animals. Does a similar ‘genetic toolkit’ for behavior exist as well? Or do behavioral phenotypes rely more on new genes to generate behavioral novelty? Studies across both vertebrate and invertebrate animals suggest that such toolkits may indeed underlie several basic forms of behavior (aggression, reward, and sociality), as discussed below. 503 Many animals exhibit aggressive behavior, which can vary widely in the form of expression (from biting to stinging to highly ritualized aggressive displays) and in the context in which it is used (i.e., to defend a territory, to gain access to mates, to establish a position in a dominance hierarchy). Nonetheless, research on the mechanistic basis of aggressive behavior in both vertebrate and invertebrate animals suggests that these behaviors may share common molecular underpinnings. For example, low levels of the neuromodulator serotonin affect aggressive behavior in mice and have also been associated with impulsive aggression in humans. In lobsters, both extremely elevated and depressed levels of serotonin are associated with increased aggression. This is one example of the same molecule being associated with aggression across taxa in which opposite patterns of regulation can affect similar behaviors across species. Thus, the serotonin pathway may be evolutionarily labile, that is, easily changed during evolution to regulate behavioral differences, though the exact pattern of regulation may vary across taxa. An important aspect of motivation is that the performance of some behaviors produce a self-reinforcing sensation of ‘reward.’ The reward system has long been known in mammals, typified by drug addictions in humans and mice that seek electric stimulation of the ‘pleasure center’ of the brain in preference to food. In vertebrates, the main neurotransmitter that has been associated with reward is dopamine. Dopamine is released in a certain brain region (nucleus accumbens) in response to eating and sexual activity. Elegant work on the molecular basis of pair bonding in voles has demonstrated a connection between expression of the vasopressin V1aR receptor in the ventral pallidum, but pair bonding can only occur when dopamine is actively present in the same brain region, suggesting that pair bonding has evolved to become a rewarding stimulus. Recent studies with insects suggest that invertebrates possess a reward system not so different from that of vertebrates. Research with crickets, flies, and honeybees suggest that dopamine instead mediates the learning of negative, aversive stimuli whereas a related neurochemical, octopamine, can affect learning and perception of rewarding stimuli such as food. Eusociality, the complex form of social behavior that is defined by the presence of reproductive queens and workers that forgo their own reproduction to aid the reproduction of others has evolved multiple times in animals from termites to bees to naked mole rats. With striking convergent evolution of social form across such a wide variety of animal taxa, the study of the evolution of eusociality provides a good system to test for the existence of a ‘genetic toolkit’ underlying the evolution of complex social behavior. It has long been known that nutritional asymmetries among individuals within a social insect colony relate to differences in reproductive capacity and body form, and contribute to the development of Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior (2010), vol. 1, pp. 500-506 Author's personal copy 504 Development, Evolution and Behavior different castes, including kings and queens, workers, and worker subcastes that specialize in particular colony tasks. Recent studies at the molecular level suggest that certain genes or pathways are associated with sociality across multiple taxa, many of which relate to nutritional and metabolic processes. For example, the storage protein Hexamerin is associated with caste differences in distantly related insects (termites and paper wasps). In addition, genome-wide studies of gene expression have repeatedly uncovered important differences in metabolic enzymes in numerous lineages (bees, wasps, and ants). Finally, differences in the regulation of deeply conserved genes that control feeding behavior (including the foraging gene and the insulin pathway) appear to regulate behavior across independently evolved lineages of ants, bees, and wasps. These studies suggest that eusociality, a complex form of social organization, evolved in part by changes in the regulation of deeply conserved genes regulating feeding and nutritional physiology. Further studies of the molecular genetic basis of eusociality in even more distantly related taxa, for example, mole rats, will provide a crucial test of this hypothesis, and will allow us to assess how broadly such a ‘genetic toolkit’ applies. The Evolution of Behavioral Modules Modules can be defined as distinct phenotypic units, developing more or less independently from each other, that make up part of a larger whole. It is intuitive that animal body plans are modular. Vertebrates have repeating series of vertebrae, and insect body plans are clearly organized into segments – just think of a caterpillar. Comparative anatomical studies and gene-level studies have shown that such modules can be reorganized to give rise to new body structures. Additional modules can be added, subtracted, or fused to form new structures. For example, in vertebrates, jaws evolved from modular series of gill arches in early fish, and skulls from fused elements of several vertebrae. In insects, repeated pairs of segmented appendages have evolved into specialized mouthparts and antennae, and the thorax has evolved from the fusion of three ancestral body segments. Although somewhat less obvious than for morphology, some behaviors are also modular in structure. Many behaviors can be broken down into constituent parts, which often occur sequentially over time. This is true for both short-term sequences of behavior and behavioral phases that occur over the course of a lifetime. Breaking down complex behaviors into smaller component behaviors (or behavioral modules) can be a useful entree into detailed studies of the mechanisms underlying these behaviors. In the following paragraph, two examples of modular behaviors – one describing a set of behavioral modules expressed on a short time scale, and the other involving more long-term behavioral phases – are described. In each case, modules appear to have been reorganized to generate novel forms of behavior during evolution. Courtship behavior in Drosophila fruit flies is a complex affair. The general sequence consists of several stages (or behavioral modules), as follows: first, the male orients toward the female; then he taps her with his antennae; then he begins singing a courtship song by buzzing his wings; then he licks her genitals; then he mounts, and finally, if successful, he copulates. This is the general series of steps, but the sequence varies across species, with various elements that are either prolonged, shortened, or elaborated. The courtship song itself consists of modules of different forms of sound that vary widely across species. Elements of this courtship behavior vary across Drosophila spp., and there is evidence that in some cases, differences in courtship sequence, especially song, can act as speciesisolating mechanisms. In the same way in which Drosophila courtship songs may help to isolate species, bird songs may do the same, facilitated by reorganizing different combinations of trills and whistles, which in some cases appear to be behavioral modules of song. With regard to eusocial insects, a great mystery that has intrigued biologists since Darwin relates to the evolution of queens and workers. Given the fact that in most species, any female egg can become a worker or a queen, how can such extreme differences in morphology and behavior arise from the same genome? One hypothesis utilizes the idea of behavioral modules. If we imagine a solitary maternal insect, its behavior can be broken down into two distinct behavioral modules: (1) egg-laying and (2) maternal provisioning of brood with food collected during foraging. It has been suggested that an ancestral ovarian cycle consisting of these two basic modules of egglaying and foraging/provisioning could be uncoupled. Instead of being separated in time as in solitary maternal wasps, the two behaviors could become separated into different individuals – queens that focus on egg-laying and workers that specialize in foraging/provisioning. Thus, worker behavior, which involves caring for siblings, may have evolved from maternal foraging/provisioning. Recent evidence at the molecular level supports the idea that worker behavior evolved from maternal behavior; similar patterns of brain gene expression underlie both maternal and worker behavior in primitively social Polistes metricus paper wasps. Further expansions of an ancestral groundplan may have occurred among workers later in social insect evolution, in two contexts. First, colonies show a division of labor among nest workers and foragers; nest workers have higher reproductive capacity than foragers, and recent results suggest that the brain gene expression patterns of honeybee nest workers are indeed more queen-like than those of foragers. Second, we see a fine-tuned division of labor for foraging in honeybees; bees that forage for pollen have more well-developed ovaries and higher levels of Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior (2010), vol. 1, pp. 500-506 Author's personal copy Development, Evolution and Behavior expression of the egg-yolk protein Vitellogenin than bees that forage for nectar. Thus, these two ancestral modules of egg-laying and foraging may have been separated multiple times during social insect evolution to produce specialized individuals, giving rise to a division of labor. The Co-evolution of Behavior and Development Thus far, behavior and morphological development as separate phenotypic entities have been considered. However, in many cases, behavior and morphology coevolve. This may be due to similar selection pressures causing parallel evolution of the two or due to constraints imposed by pleiotropic effects of genes that affect behavior and morphology concurrently. As discussed earlier, there have been several studies of hormonal effects on both behavior and development suggesting the possibility of common mechanistic elements to the regulation of physiology, development, and behavior. However, to date, there have been few studies that have attempted to examine whether the same genes or pathways underlie both developmental and behavioral differences within and across species. This is an area ripe for study, and in the following section, two particularly promising models for addressing this question are summarized. Three-spined stickleback fish (Gasterosteus aculeatus) have been important model systems for studying the evolution of development. These fish have evolved from marine to freshwater forms multiple times in several widely separated geographical areas. They thus provide a perfect system to examine the roles of conservation and convergence in phenotypic (both morphological and behavioral) evolution. Each time sticklebacks have invaded freshwater habitats, and this has been accompanied by a reduction in the presence of armored plates along the lateral side of the body as well as shortened pelvic spines, which are protection against predators. In many freshwater populations, sticklebacks have further diversified into distinct benthic (bottom dwelling) and limnetic (surface dwelling) forms, which show differences in jaw morphology that are related to differences in their feeding habits. These benthic and limnetic forms show consistently different patterns of foraging behavior, courtship, and aggressive behavior. It remains to be seen whether some of the same genes that regulate morphological differences are also used to regulate behavioral differences, or whether different toolkits are employed for each. If different tookits exist, it is an intriguing question as to whether such toolkits coevolve via common regulatory elements that control numerous different pathways, or whether there are no such common regulatory elements to link pathways. 505 Horned scarab beetles are found worldwide, with striking variation in the presence/absence of horns and in their size and morphology. In some dung beetles, males take alternative forms: territorial, large-horned males, and nonterritorial small-horned males. The horns are used in combat between males for dung resources, and such contests help assure them possession of dung territories and access to females. Recent studies have begun to elucidate the molecular basis of horn development in dung beetles and suggests an important role for the insulin pathway in affecting energy allocation to horns (vs. other morphological features) resulting in allometric changes in horn size. There is also a correlation across species between horn size and behavior: beetle species that tunnel into dung have large horns, whereas those that roll dung on the surface do not. Given the role of various insulin pathway genes in regulating feeding and social behavior in insects, it will be intriguing to test whether the insulin pathway also affects tunneling and aggressive behavior in beetles. Future Directions Evo-devo has been extremely successful in elucidating several important principles about how morphological evolution can occur (as described in ‘Basics of EvoDevo’). Notably, the major insights from evo-devo have resulted from pairing molecular genetics data with comparative methods by studying a wide variety of species. The mechanistic basis of behavior, while traditionally believed to be harder to dissect than that of development, has nonetheless already hinted at similar findings to evodevo – namely, that changes in the regulation of deeply conserved genes are likely to result in behavioral evolution and that a core set of genes, or ‘genetic toolkit,’ may be used repeatedly during the evolution of novel behaviors. The studies of the mechanisms responsible for the evolution of behavior have focused mainly on a handful of species (e.g., rodents, honeybees, and fruit flies). Reflecting on the history of evo-devo, it is clear that behavioral studies could also benefit greatly from a much expanded comparative analysis of behavior. This need may be fulfilled by a general broadening of the taxa considered for comparison to include distantly related species with similar patterns of behavior. Well-resolved phylogenies are needed in order to carefully choose species that are informative in a phylogenetic context (e.g., species in basal lineages or species within a branch of a phylogenetic tree that appear to have evolved similar behaviors independently). One of the main obstacles to such studies has been the lack of gene sequence information and genetic resources for nonmodel genetic species. New technologies are quickly getting around this roadblock. For example, it is now possible to manipulate gene expression patterns in a Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior (2010), vol. 1, pp. 500-506 Author's personal copy 506 Development, Evolution and Behavior number of model organisms through the use of pharmacological treatments or RNA interference (RNAi). In addition, next generation sequencing methods, which generate huge amounts of data at a fraction of the cost of traditional sequencing, are improving rapidly. Such methods are now being effectively used to generate large databases of expressed genes for a wide variety of ecologically and evolutionarily important species. Such technological improvements can help pave the way for new and creative ways to study the molecular genetic basis of behavior in a wide variety of species. These advances, when coupled with an evo-devo perspective on behavior, promise to yield major insights into behavioral evolution in the near future. See also: Caste in Social Insects: Genetic Influences Over Caste Determination; Drosophila Behavior Genetics; Evolution: Fundamentals; Genes and Genomic Searches; Honeybees; Integration of Proximate and Ultimate Causes; Play; Social Insects: Behavioral Genetics; Sociogenomics; Threespine Stickleback; Zebra Finches. Further Reading Abouheif E and Wray GA (2002) Evolution of the gene network underlying wing polyphenism in ants. Science 297: 249–252. Barron AB and Robinson GE (2008) The utility of behavioral models and modules in molecular analyses of social behavior. Genes, Brain, and Behavior 7: 257–265. Carroll SB (1995) Homeotic genes and the evolution of arthropods and chordates. Nature 376: 479–485. Carroll SB (2008) Evo-devo and an expanding evolutionary synthesis: A genetic theory of morphological evolution. Cell 134: 25–36. Carroll SB, Grenier J, and Weatherbee S (2004) From DNA to Diversity: Molecular Genetics and the Evolution of Animal Design, pp. 272. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. Cresko WA, Amores A, Wilson C, et al. (2004) Parallel genetic basis for repeated evolution of armor loss in Alaskan threespine stickleback populations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101: 6050–6055. Emlen DJ, Lavine LC, and Ewen-Campen B (2007) On the origin and evolutionary diversification of beetle horns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104(supplement 1): 8661–8668. Hudson ME (2008) Sequencing breakthroughs for genomic ecology and evolutionary biology. Molecular Ecology Resources 8: 3–17. Kozmik Z (2005) Pax genes in eye development and evolution. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development 15: 430–438. Love AC and Raff RA (2003) Knowing your ancestors: Themes in the history of evo-devo. Evolution & Development 5: 327–330. Nusslein-Volhard C and Wieschaus E (1980) Mutations affecting segment number and polarity in Drosophila. Nature 287: 795–801. Raff RA (2000) Evo-devo: The evolution of a new discipline. Nature Reviews Genetics 1: 74–79. Robinson GE and Ben-Shahar Y (2002) Social behavior and comparative genomics: New genes or new gene regulation? Genes, Brain, and Behavior 1: 197–203. Toth AL and Robinson GE (2007) Evo-devo and the evolution of social behavior. Trends in Genetics 23: 334–341. West-Eberhard MJ (2003) Developmental Plasticity and Evolution, pp. 794. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior (2010), vol. 1, pp. 500-506