Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 100 (2013) 77–87 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Neurobiology of Learning and Memory journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynlme Pre-learning stress that is temporally removed from acquisition exerts sex-specific effects on long-term memory Phillip R. Zoladz a,⇑, Ashlee J. Warnecke a, Sarah A. Woelke a, Hanna M. Burke a, Rachael M. Frigo a, Julia M. Pisansky a, Sarah M. Lyle a, Jeffery N. Talbot b a b Department of Psychology, Sociology, & Criminal Justice, Ohio Northern University, Ada, OH 45810, USA Department of Pharmaceutical & Biomedical Sciences, Raabe College of Pharmacy, Ohio Northern University, Ada, OH 45810, USA a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 8 October 2012 Revised 13 December 2012 Accepted 15 December 2012 Available online 22 December 2012 Keywords: Amygdala Cortisol Hippocampus Learning Memory Stress a b s t r a c t We have examined the influence of sex and the perceived emotional nature of learned information on pre-learning stress-induced alterations of long-term memory. Participants submerged their dominant hand in ice cold (stress) or warm (no stress) water for 3 min. Thirty minutes later, they studied 30 words, rated the words for their levels of emotional valence and arousal and were then given an immediate free recall test. Twenty-four hours later, participants’ memory for the word list was assessed via delayed free recall and recognition assessments. The resulting memory data were analyzed after categorizing the studied words (i.e., distributing them to ‘‘positive-arousing’’, ‘‘positive-non-arousing’’, ‘‘negative-arousing’’, etc. categories) according to participants’ valence and arousal ratings of the words. The results revealed that participants exhibiting a robust cortisol response to stress exhibited significantly impaired recognition memory for neutral words. More interestingly, however, males displaying a robust cortisol response to stress demonstrated significantly impaired recall, overall, and a marginally significant impairment of overall recognition memory, while females exhibiting a blunted cortisol response to stress demonstrated a marginally significant impairment of overall recognition memory. These findings support the notion that a brief stressor that is temporally separated from learning can exert deleterious effects on long-term memory. However, they also suggest that such effects depend on the sex of the organism, the emotional salience of the learned information and the degree to which stress increases corticosteroid levels. Ó 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Stress can enhance, impair or have no effect on learning and memory. One factor that plays an important role in dictating whether stress enhances or impairs learning is the stage of learning and memory that is affected by the stress. Perhaps the most consistent finding in this area of work is that stress administered after learning enhances consolidation (Beckner, Tucker, Delville, & Mohr, 2006; Cahill, Gorski, & Le, 2003; Hui, Hui, Roozendaal, McGaugh, & Weinberger, 2006; Preuss & Wolf, 2009; Smeets, Otgaar, Candel, & Wolf, 2008). The effects of stress on encoding and retrieval, however, have been less clear. Although a great deal of work has shown that stress impairs retrieval (Buchanan & Tranel, 2008; Buchanan, Tranel, & Adolphs, 2006; Kuhlmann, Piel, & Wolf, 2005; Park, Zoladz, Conrad, Fleshner, & Diamond, 2008; Smeets et al., 2008; Tollenaar, Elzinga, Spinhoven, & Everaerd, 2008), there ⇑ Corresponding author. Address: Department of Psychology, Sociology, & Criminal Justice, Ohio Northern University, 525 S. Main St. Hill 013, Ada, OH 45810, USA. Fax: +1 419 772 2746. E-mail address: p-zoladz@onu.edu (P.R. Zoladz). 1074-7427/$ - see front matter Ó 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2012.12.012 are discrepancies in the literature, with some studies reporting stress enhancing or having no effect on such processes (Beckner et al., 2006; Buchanan & Tranel, 2008; Schwabe et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2003). Investigations examining pre-learning stress have also been inconclusive and have revealed enhancements, impairments or no effects at all (Diamond et al., 2006; Duncko, Johnson, Merikangas, & Grillon, 2009; Elzinga, Bakker, & Bremner, 2005; Jelicic, Geraerts, Merckelbach, & Guerrieri, 2004; Kim, Koo, Lee, & Han, 2005; Kim, Lee, Han, & Packard, 2001; Nater et al., 2007; Park et al., 2008; Payne et al., 2006, 2007; Schwabe, Bohringer, Chatterjee, & Schachinger, 2008a; Smeets et al., 2008; Zoladz et al., 2011a). Researchers have claimed that the timing of stress plays a critical role in the stress-induced modulation of learning and memory (Diamond, Campbell, Park, Halonen, & Zoladz, 2007; Joels, Fernandez, & Roozendaal, 2011; Joels, Pu, Wiegert, Oitzl, & Krugers, 2006; Schwabe, Joels, Roozendaal, Wolf, & Oitzl, 2012). Diamond and colleagues suggested that stress exerts a biphasic effect on hippocampal function, which consequentially results in differential effects on long-term memory depending on when the stress is administered relative to learning (Diamond et al., 2007). According to these investigators, stress activates the amygdala, which produces a ra- 78 P.R. Zoladz et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 100 (2013) 77–87 pid enhancement of hippocampal synaptic plasticity that initially facilitates the storage of information; however, as times passes, the hippocampus descends into a refractory state for producing new plasticity, and the storage of information is impaired. Part of the basis for this theory was electrophysiological work indicating that glucocorticoids, as well as electrical stimulation of the amygdala, could exert immediate excitatory, but delayed inhibitory, effects on hippocampal synaptic plasticity (Akirav & Richter-Levin, 1999; Frey, Bergado-Rosado, Seidenbecher, Pape, & Frey, 2001; Karst et al., 2005; Wiegert, Joels, & Krugers, 2006). Indeed, a general consensus has begun to emerge suggesting that if the stress converges in time with the learning experience and activates neurobiological mechanisms that are in common with the learning experience, then long-term memory can be enhanced (Joels et al., 2006; Sandi, 2011). Such stress-induced enhancements are thought to be associated with the rapid effects of noradrenergic activity, non-genomic actions of corticosteroids and increased glutamatergic transmission. Stress-induced impairments of hippocampal function, on the other hand, are thought to be associated with delayed, gene-dependent activity of corticosteroids and NMDA receptor desensitization (Akirav & Richter-Levin, 2002; Diamond et al., 2007; Halonen, Zoladz, Park, & Diamond, 2007; Joels, Velzing, Nair, Verkuyl, & Karst, 2003). Much of the support for this ‘‘temporal dynamics model’’ has come from non-human animal work. For instance, Diamond et al. (2007) found that brief stress administered immediately prior to learning enhanced long-term spatial memory in rats; however, if the same stress was administered 30 min before learning, no enhancement was observed. The stress-induced enhancement of long-term spatial memory was mimicked by epinephrine and blocked by propranolol, a b-adrenergic receptor antagonist (Halonen et al., 2007). In contrast, the pre-learning stress-induced impairment of long-term spatial memory, which was induced by prolonged (i.e., 30 min) stress prior to training, was not prevented by propranolol. The findings of human work in this area have been mixed. Some studies examining pre-learning stress effects on longterm memory have been consistent with the temporal dynamics model (e.g., Schwabe et al., 2008a), while others have observed stress-induced enhancements or impairments of memory in situations that do not coincide with predictions that would arise from this theory (e.g., Smeets et al., 2009). Recently, we found support for the temporal dynamics model by showing that brief stress administered immediately before learning enhanced long-term memory in healthy young adults, while the same brief stressor administered 30 min before learning impaired long-term memory (Zoladz et al., 2011a). Importantly, the stress-induced enhancement of memory was associated with participants’ heart rate response to the stress, an indication of autonomic nervous system and noradrenergic activity, while the stress-induced impairment of memory was associated with participants’ corticosteroid and blood pressure responses to the stress. Combined with the above evidence from rodent research, these findings supported the possibility that noradrenergic mechanisms are responsible for prelearning stress-induced enhancements of long-term memory, but they are eventually suppressed by the delayed increase in corticosteroids, which results in memory impairment (Schwabe et al., 2012). Most, but not all, of the research examining stress effects on learning and memory has reported greater effects for information that is emotional in nature (Schwabe, Wolf, & Oitzl, 2010; Wolf, 2009). This finding makes sense considering the amygdala, an area of the brain that is highly involved in emotional memory formation (McGaugh, 2004), is significantly activated by emotional material (Dolcos, LaBar, & Cabeza, 2004; Strange & Dolan, 2004). One limitation of studies of this type, however, is that investigators have frequently confounded the emotional valence (e.g., positive, nega- tive, and neutral) of the words with the arousal level (arousing, non-arousing) of the words. That is to say, few, if any, studies have systematically manipulated both the emotional valence and arousal level of the learned information in order to examine how such manipulations influence stress-induced alterations of learning and memory (see Schwabe et al., 2008a for a consideration of this point). In the present experiment, we examined the influence of pre-learning stress on memory for information that varied in both emotional valence and arousal by systematically varying the levels of each based on subjective ratings provided by participants. Sex differences also abound in the stress–memory literature, and a clear agreement on how sex mediates such effects has not been reached. Investigators have contended that sex differences depend on the type of learning that occurs (Conrad et al., 2004), the stage of learning that is affected by the stress (Andreano & Cahill, 2009) and the hormonal response of the participant to the stress (Andreano & Cahill, 2006; Andreano & Cahill, 2009). Perhaps one relatively consistent finding in this area is that corticosteroidmediated modulation of learning and memory has been observed more in males than in females. Several studies have reported cortisol- or stress-induced alterations of learning and memory or significant relationships between cortisol and memory in males, but not females (Andreano & Cahill, 2006; Jackson, Payne, Nadel, & Jacobs, 2006; Wolf, Schommer, Hellhammer, McEwen, & Kirschbaum, 2001; Wood, Beylin, & Shors, 2001; Zorawski, Blanding, Kuhn, & LaBar, 2006). Ultimately, additional work is needed to assess the influence of sex on stress–memory interactions, especially in humans. The purpose of the present study was to examine the influence of sex and the perceived emotional nature of the learned information on pre-learning stress effects on long-term memory. Given the aforementioned findings, we predicted that a brief stressor administered 30 min before learning would impair long-term memory, particularly for emotional information (Zoladz et al., 2011a). We also expected that, since pre-learning stress-induced impairments of learning may be more dependent on corticosteroid mechanisms, males might be more adversely affected by the stress than females. 2. Method 2.1. Participants Ninety-seven healthy men and women (38 men, 59 women; age: M = 19.18, SD = 1.15) from Ohio Northern University volunteered to participate in the present experiment. Individuals were excluded from participating if they met any of the following conditions: diagnosis of Raynaud’s disease or peripheral vascular disease; presence of skin diseases, such as severe psoriasis, eczema or scleroderma; history of severe head injury; current treatment with psychotropic medications, narcotics, beta-blockers, steroids or any other medication that was deemed to significantly affect central nervous or endocrine system function; mental or substance use disorder; regular nightshift work. Individuals who smoked were allowed to participate in the study; information regarding individuals’ smoking habits was collected prior to the experiments via a short demographic survey. There was only one participant who reported smoking on a regular basis, and inclusion of the data from this participant in the statistical analyses did not alter the results. Females who took birth control on a regular basis were also allowed to participate in the study; prior to participation, we asked female participants if they took birth control via a short demographic survey. Females who reportedly took birth control were not significantly different from naturally cycling females on any physiological or behavioral measure, nor did stress significantly interact with birth control in these analyses. Therefore, we treated P.R. Zoladz et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 100 (2013) 77–87 all females as a single group in the statistical analyses for this study. Participants were asked to refrain from using recreational drugs (e.g., marijuana) for 3 days prior to the experimental sessions; to refrain from drinking alcohol or exercising extensively for 24 h prior to the experimental sessions; and, to refrain from eating or drinking anything but water for 2 h prior to the experimental sessions. Participants were awarded class credit upon completion of the study. All of the methods for the experiment were approved by the Institutional Review Board at Ohio Northern University. 2.2. Experimental procedures The experimental timeline for the present experiment is presented in Fig. 1. To control for diurnal variations in cortisol levels, all testing was carried out between 1200 and 1800 h. 2.2.1. Socially Evaluated Cold Pressor Test (SECPT) Participants were asked to submerge their dominant hand, up to and including the wrist, in a bath of water for 3 min. Those participants who had been randomly assigned to the stress condition (N = 49; 20 males and 29 females) placed their hand in a bath of ice cold (0–2 °C) water, while participants who had been randomly assigned to the control condition (N = 48; 18 males and 30 females) placed their hand in a bath of warm (35–37 °C) water. The water was maintained at the appropriate temperature by a Lauda Brinkmann RMT6 circulating water bath. To maximize the stress response, participants in each experiment were encouraged to keep their hand in the water bath for the entire 3-min period. However, if a participant found the water bath too painful, he or she was allowed to remove his or her hand from the water and continue with the experiment. Only five participants from the stress condition removed their hand from the water prior to 3 min elapsing (M water time = 166.37 s, SD = 40.99), and all participants from the no stress condition kept their hand in the water for the entire 3-min period. Based on previous work (Schwabe, Haddad, & Schachinger, 2008b), a social evaluative component was added to the cold pressor manipulation. Participants in the stress condition were misleadingly informed that they were being videotaped during the procedure for subsequent evaluation of their facial expressions, and throughout the water bath manipulation, they were asked to keep their eyes on a camera that was located on the wall of the laboratory. 79 2.2.2. Subjective pain and stress ratings All participants were asked to rate the painfulness and stressfulness of the water bath manipulation at 1-min intervals on 11point scales ranging from 0 to 10, with 0 indicating a complete lack of pain or stress and 10 indicating unbearable pain or stress. If a participant removed his or her hand from the water before 3 min had elapsed, the remaining data points were automatically scored as 10 s for each measure. 2.2.3. Word presentation Thirty minutes following exposure to the water bath manipulation, participants were presented with a list of 30 words, which were selected from the Affective Norms for English Words (Bradley & Lang, 1999). Based on standardized valence and arousal ratings, we chose 10 neutral (5 arousing and 5 non-arousing), 10 positive (5 arousing and 5 non-arousing) and 10 negative (5 arousing and 5 non-arousing) words, which, across emotional valence and arousal categories, were balanced for word length and word frequency. As per the methods employed by Schwabe et al. (2008a), participants were instructed to read each word aloud and rate its emotional valence on a scale from 3 (very negative) to +3 (very positive) and its arousal level on a scale of 1 (not arousing) to 7 (very highly arousing) on a sheet of paper containing the list of words. These manipulations were performed to promote encoding of the words, and they allowed us to analyze the final memory data based on participants’ own ratings of the words (see Section 2.3). Two versions of the word list were used in the experiments. According to the Affective Norms for English Words (Bradley & Lang, 1999), the mean (±SEM) standardized valence and arousal ratings for the words that made up these lists were as follows: word list 1 (positive arousing words (e.g., erotic): valence = 7.77 ± 0.16, arousal = 6.84 ± 0.29; positive non-arousing words (e.g., butterfly): valence = 7.41 ± 0.13, arousal = 3.23 ± 0.08; negative arousing words (e.g., poison): valence = 2.32 ± 0.20, arousal = 6.71 ± 0.37; negative non-arousing words (e.g., bored): valence = 2.50 ± 0.20, arousal = 3.75 ± 0.24; neutral arousing words (e.g., lightning): valence = 4.61 ± 0.25, arousal = 6.53 ± 0.12; neutral non-arousing words (e.g., hairpin): valence = 5.00 ± 0.18, arousal = 3.54 ± 0.13) and word list 2 (positive arousing words (e.g., lust): valence = 7.69 ± 0.26, arousal = 6.86 ± 0.22; positive non-arousing words (e.g., secure): valence = 7.40 ± 0.09, arousal = 3.46 ± 0.21; negative arousing words (e.g., bloody): valence = 2.30 ± 0.20, arousal = 6.74 ± 0.17; negative non-arousing words (e.g., trash): va- Fig. 1. Timeline for the methodology employed in the present study. Participants were exposed to the water bath manipulation at time point 0. Participants in the stress condition placed their dominant hand in ice cold (0–2 °C) water while believing that they were being videotaped [Socially Evaluated Cold Pressor Test (SECPT)]; participants in the no stress condition placed their dominant hand in warm (35–37 °C) water. Thirty minutes following the initiation of the water bath manipulation, participants were presented with a list of 30 words. They were asked to read each word aloud and to rate each word’s emotional valence and arousal level. Immediately following word list encoding, participants were given a free recall assessment. To verify the induction of a stress response, saliva samples (S in the figure) and cardiovascular measurements (C in the figure) were obtained from participants throughout the experimental session. Twenty-four hours later, participants returned to the laboratory to complete free recall and recognition tests regarding the word list that was studied on the previous day. Recognition memory was assessed 15 min following the free recall assessment. 80 P.R. Zoladz et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 100 (2013) 77–87 lence = 2.39 ± 0.25, arousal = 4.35 ± 0.24; neutral arousing words (e.g., volcano): valence = 4.71 ± 0.14, arousal = 6.58 ± 0.33; neutral non-arousing words (e.g., bland): valence = 5.01 ± 0.24, arousal = 3.48 ± 0.13). 2.2.4. Memory testing Immediately following word list encoding, participants were given 5 min to write down as many words as they could remember from the list of words they just studied (i.e., immediate free recall). One day following the first experimental session, participants returned to the laboratory to have their memory for the list of words assessed. Participants were again given 5 min to write down as many words as they could remember from the list of words that they studied on the previous day (i.e., delayed free recall). Then, participants sat quietly and completed school work that they had brought to the laboratory. After 15 min had elapsed, participants were given a recognition test. They were presented with a list of words containing 30 ‘‘old’’ words (i.e., words that were presented on the previous day) and 30 ‘‘new’’ words (i.e., words that were not presented on the previous day) and were instructed to label each word as ‘‘old’’ or ‘‘new’’. The ‘‘new’’ words were matched to the ‘‘old’’ words on emotional valence, arousal level, word length and word frequency, according to the ratings obtained from the Affective Norms for English Words (Bradley & Lang, 1999). To assess participants’ ability to discriminate between ‘‘old’’ and ‘‘new’’ words, we calculated a sensitivity index (d0 = z[p(hit) p(false alarm)]) for each category of word (i.e., positive-arousing, positive-non-arousing, negative-arousing, etc.) to be used for statistical analysis (Wickens, 2002). 2.2.5. Cardiovascular analysis Heart rate (HR) and blood pressure (BP) measurements were taken 2 min before (baseline), halfway through and 10 min after the water bath manipulation. Cardiovascular activity was measured with a vital signs monitor (Mark of Fitness WS-820 Automatic Wrist Blood Pressure Monitor) placed on the wrist of each participant’s non-dominant hand. 2.2.6. Cortisol analysis Saliva samples were collected from participants 2 min before (baseline) and 28 min following exposure to the water bath manipulation to analyze salivary cortisol concentrations. The samples were collected in a Salivette saliva collection device (Sarstedt, Inc., Newton, NC). Participants were asked to place a swab of cotton in their mouths and chew on it so that it would easily absorb their saliva. Following 1 min of chewing, the swab was collected and placed in the Salivette conical tube and kept at room temperature until the experimental session was completed. The samples were subsequently stored at 20 °C until assayed for cortisol. Saliva samples were thawed and extracted by low-speed centrifugation. Salivary cortisol levels were determined by enzyme immuno assay (Caymen Chemical Co., Ann Arbor, MI) according to the manufacturer’s protocol. The minimum detectable concentration of cortisol was approximately 8 pg/ml, and the average interand intra-assay percent coefficients of variation were less than 6.9% and 6.8%, respectively. 2.3. Statistical analyses Prior to performing any statistical analyses, we conducted correlations to verify that participants’ valence and arousal ratings of the studied words were not significantly correlated; our analysis confirmed this prediction. Therefore, mixed-model ANOVAs were used to analyze all data; the between-subjects factors utilized in these analyses were stress and sex, and the within-subjects factors were word valence and arousal (for recall and recognition analy- ses) or time (for physiological and pain/stress ratings analyses). Participants in the stress condition were divided into ‘‘Responders’’ and ‘‘Non-Responders’’ based on their cortisol responses to the SECPT. Those participants exhibiting a cortisol increase of at least 1.5 nmol/l following the SECPT were considered Responders; all other participants were considered Non-Responders. The cutoff for dividing participants into Responder and Non-Responder groups was based on previous work using a similar criterion (Schwabe et al., 2008a; Zoladz et al., 2011a). The analyses of participants’ valence and arousal ratings and memory for the words (i.e., immediate free recall, delayed free recall, and recognition) were performed based on categorizing the words (i.e., distributing the words to ‘‘positive-arousing’’, ‘‘positive-non-arousing’’, ‘‘negative-arousing’’, etc. groups) according to participants’ subjective valence and arousal ratings that were obtained during the study (see Section 2.2.3). For each analysis of memory data, we conducted a mixed-model ANOVA analyzing the effects of stress, overall (i.e., stress vs. no stress), on memory performance, and then we conducted a subsequent mixed-model ANOVA to assess the influence of cortisol response to stress (i.e., Responders vs. Non-Responders) on memory performance. For memory measures affected by stress, we conducted additional ANCOVAs to control for the influence of participants’ valence and arousal ratings of the studied words on such effects. We also conducted bivariate correlations (Pearson’s r) to explore possible relationships between participants’ physiological responses to stress and cognitive performance. To limit the inflation of Type I error rates in these analyses, the correlations were performed only for memory measures affected by stress. Alpha was set at 0.05 for all analyses, and Bonferroni-corrected post hoc tests were employed when necessary. Outlier data points that were at least 3 standard deviation units beyond the exclusive group means were eliminated from the analyses; less than 1% of all data were outliers. SPSS (version 18.0; SPSS, Inc.) was used to perform all statistical analyses. 3. Results 3.1. Physiological responses 3.1.1. Heart rate (see Table 1) The stress manipulation had no effect on HR (no significant effect of condition: F(2,88) = 1.43, p > 0.05, g2 = 0.03). However, females exhibited an overall greater HR than males (significant effect of sex: F(1,88) = 4.53, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.05), and HR decreased over time across all participants (significant effect of time: F(2,176) = 6.92, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.07). No other significant effects were observed. 3.1.2. Systolic blood pressure (see Table 1) Stressed participants, independent of cortisol response to the stressor, exhibited greater systolic BP than non-stressed participants during the water bath manipulation (significant effect of condition: F(2,88) = 8.36, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.16; significant effect of time: F(2,176) = 145.68, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.62; significant Condition Time interaction: F(4,176) = 8.54, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.16). Male participants also exhibited greater systolic BP than female participants, particularly in response to the water bath manipulation (significant effect of sex: F(1,88) = 31.16, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.26; significant Sex Time interaction: F(2,176) = 4.43, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.05). No other significant effects were observed. 3.1.3. Diastolic blood pressure (see Table 1) Stressed participants, independent of cortisol response to the stressor, exhibited greater diastolic BP than non-stressed partici- 81 P.R. Zoladz et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 100 (2013) 77–87 sample sizes for each group: 20 Responders (8 male, 12 female) and 29 Non-Responders (12 male, 17 female). As expected, Responders exhibited greater salivary cortisol levels than nonstressed participants after the water bath manipulation (significant effect of condition: F(2,91) = 4.71, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.09; significant effect of time: F(1,91) = 41.07, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.31; significant Condition Time interaction: F(2,91) = 26.09, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.36). No other significant effects were observed. Table 1 Cardiovascular activity before, during and after the water bath manipulation. DV/condition Heart rate (bpm) Responders Male Female Non-Responders Male Female No stress Male Female Pre During Post 75.38 (5.14) 74.75 (4.29) 71.88 (3.86) 73.67 (4.94) 64.50 (3.04) 70.42 (2.90) 74.58 (5.80) 77.94 (3.52) 72.45 (4.40) 76.53 (2.70) 66.45 (5.44) 76.53 (3.18) 72.56 (1.86) 81.10 (2.50) 75.28 (2.65) 78.38 (1.96) 71.94 (2.20) 80.37 (1.80) 166.13 (8.54)* 148.33 (4.40)* 132.50 (5.48) 116.42 (3.38) 161.25 (5.90)* 137.71 (4.47)* 129.08 (2.50) 118.12 (2.57) 140.94 (3.11) 128.62 (1.80) 123.59 (3.51) 115.30 (1.93) 112.38 (3.04)* 99.33 (3.44)* 78.50 (3.44) 74.08 (2.41) 107.75 (5.19)* 95.35 (4.09)* 80.00 (2.01) 76.41 (1.95) 88.47 (2.55) 83.60 (1.40) 73.82 (2.69) 73.97 (1.17) Systolic blood pressure (mm Hg) Responders Male 134.38 (4.33) Female 123.50 (2.63) Non-Responders Male 135.75 (3.08) Female 127.47 (4.16) No stress Male 129.83 (2.86) Female 119.21 (2.06) Diastolic blood pressure (mm Hg) Responders Male 83.25 (2.93) Female 78.33 (1.90) Non-Responders Male 82.75 (2.53) Female 80.63 (2.26) No stress Male 82.41 (2.31) Female 77.59 (1.09) 3.2. Subjective ratings of water bath manipulation (see Table 2) 3.2.1. Pain ratings Stressed participants, independent of cortisol response to the stressor, reported greater pain ratings than non-stressed participants throughout the water bath manipulation, and these ratings increased over time (significant effect of condition: F(2,88) = 232.05, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.84; significant effect of time: F(2,176) = 8.00, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.08; Condition Time interaction trending toward significance: F(4,176) = 2.26, p = 0.06, g2 = 0.05). No other significant effects were observed. 3.2.2. Stress ratings Stressed participants, independent of cortisol response to the stressor, reported greater stress ratings than non-stressed participants throughout the water bath manipulation (significant effect of condition: F(2,88) = 89.49, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.67; significant effect of time: F(2,176) = 3.12, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.03). No other significant effects were observed. Data are presented as means ± SEM. p < 0.05 relative to the no stress group. 3.3. Word list ratings * pants during the water bath manipulation (significant effect of condition: F(2,87) = 10.49, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.19; significant effect of time: F(2,174) = 219.76, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.72; significant Condition Time interaction: F(4,174) = 18.43, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.30). Male participants also exhibited greater diastolic BP than female participants, particularly in response to the water bath manipulation (significant effect of sex: F(1,87) = 9.21, p < 0.01, g2 = 0.10; significant Sex Time interaction: F(2,174) = 7.38, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.08). No other significant effects were observed. 3.3.1. Valence ratings Positive words were given more positive ratings than neutral words, which were given more positive ratings than negative words (significant effect of valence: F(2,180) = 847.38, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.90). Non-arousing words were also given more positive ratings than arousing words (significant effect of arousal: F(1,90) = 5.61, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.06). The latter effect seemed to be driven by neutral words being rated more negatively if they were arousing (significant Valence Arousal interaction: F(2,180) = 14.80, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.14). No other significant effects were observed. 3.1.4. Salivary cortisol (see Fig. 2) Based on the criteria employed to divide participants into Responders and Non-Responders, we ended up with the following 3.3.2. Arousal ratings Arousing words were rated as more arousing than non-arousing words (significant effect of arousal: F(1,90) = 190.49, p < 0.001, Males Females 12 12 Responders Non-Responders No Stress * 8 6 4 -10 0 10 Time (min) 20 30 6 4 2 Learning 0 8 Water Bath Learning Water Bath 2 * Responders Non-Responders No Stress 10 Cortisol (nmol/l) Cortisol (nmol/l) 10 0 -10 0 10 20 30 Time (min) Fig. 2. Salivary cortisol concentrations before and after the water bath manipulation in males (left) and females (right). In both sexes, Responders were the only participants to exhibit significantly increased cortisol concentrations following the water bath manipulation. Data are presented as means ± SEM; = p < 0.05 relative to the no stress group. 82 P.R. Zoladz et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 100 (2013) 77–87 Table 2 Pain and stress ratings of the water bath manipulation. DV/Condition Minute 1 Painfulness (scale of 0–10) Responders Male 6.88 Female 5.75 Non-Responders Male 6.42 Female 6.18 No stress Male 0.28 Female 0.10 Minute 2 Minute 3 (0.64)* (0.60)* 6.75 (0.88)* 6.25 (0.66)* 7.25 (0.88)* 6.75 (0.88)* (0.65)* (0.60)* 6.67 (0.62)* 6.65 (0.49)* 6.92 (0.56)* 7.18 (0.46)* (0.14) (0.08) 0.17 (0.09) 0.11 (0.06) 0.18 (0.10) 0.10 (0.06) 5.75 (1.01)* 5.33 (0.70)* 5.75 (1.03)* 5.67 (0.71)* 5.42 (0.92)* 5.76 (0.63)* 5.67 (0.85)* 6.06 (0.58)* 0.53 (0.19) 0.27 (0.10) 0.41 (0.15) 0.30 (0.10) Stressfulness (scale of 0–10) Responders Male 6.00 (0.94)* Female 5.25 (0.72)* Non-Responders Male 5.00 (0.84)* Female 5.35 (0.70)* No stress Male 0.24 (0.11) Female 0.10 (0.06) Data are presented as means ± SEM. p < 0.05 relative to the no stress group. * g2 = 0.68). Interestingly, however, males rated the words, overall, as less arousing than females (significant effect of sex: F(1,90) = 4.82, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.05). In addition, positive words were rated as more arousing than negative words, which were rated as more arousing than neutral words (significant effect of valence: F(2,180) = 103.16, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.53). This effect was dependent on both the sex of the participant and the arousal level of the words, as males, in general, rated negative, arousing words as less arousing than females did (significant Valence Arousal interaction: F(2,180) = 7.63, p < 0.01, g2 = 0.08; significant Sex Valence Arousal interaction: F(2,180) = 3.52, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.04). No other significant effects were observed. 3.4. Memory testing 3.4.1. Immediate free recall (see Fig. 3) The analysis of stress effects, overall, revealed that the stress manipulation had no effect on short-term memory (no significant effect of stress: F(1,92) = 0.01, p > 0.05, g2 = 0.00). We then analyzed for the involvement of cortisol response in the effects of stress on immediate free recall; this analysis again revealed that the stress manipulation had no effect on short-term memory (no significant effect of condition: F(2,90) = 1.36, p > 0.05, g2 = 0.03). However, participants recalled more arousing words than nonarousing words (significant effect of arousal: F(1,90) = 50.93, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.36). This effect was dependent on word valence, as participants recalled more arousing words than non-arousing words only when the words were neutral or positive in valence (significant Valence Arousal interaction: F(2,180) = 9.61, p < 0.01, g2 = 0.10). No other significant effects were observed. 3.4.2. Delayed free recall (see Fig. 4) The analysis of the effects of stress, overall, revealed that the stress manipulation had no effect on delayed free recall (no significant effect of stress: F(1,90) = 0.05, p > 0.05, g2 = 0.00). We then analyzed for the involvement of cortisol response in the effects of stress on delayed free recall; this analysis revealed that male Responders recalled fewer words than all other groups (significant Condition Sex interaction: F(2,88) = 3.30, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.07). Also, arousing words were better recalled than non-arousing words, particularly when they were neutral or positive words (sig- nificant effect of arousal: F(1,88) = 75.06, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.46; significant Valence Arousal interaction: F(2,176) = 5.25, p < 0.01, g2 = 0.06). No other significant effects were observed. We then conducted an ANCOVA on the delayed free recall data, using the valence and arousal ratings as covariates. This analysis revealed that when controlling for such word ratings, the aforementioned stress effects on memory remained significant (significant Condition Sex interaction: F(2,86) = 3.16, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.07). 3.4.3. Delayed recognition (see Fig. 5) The analysis of the effects of stress, overall, on recognition revealed that stressed participants recognized fewer words than non-stressed participants, particularly when they were positive and non-arousing (effect of condition trending toward significance: F(1,86) = 3.89, p = 0.052, g2 = 0.04; significant Stress Valence Arousal interaction: F(2,172) = 3.10, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.04). We then analyzed for the involvement of cortisol response in the effects of stress on recognition; this analysis revealed that neutral words were better recognized than negative words (significant effect of valence: F(2,168) = 3.92, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.05). This effect may have been more evident in Non-Responders and non-stressed participants because Responders recognized fewer neutral words than all other groups (significant Condition Valence interaction: F(4,168) = 2.55, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.06). Male Responders and female Non-Responders also tended to recognize fewer words, overall, than their respective no stress group, but this effect was only approaching significance (Condition Sex interaction trending toward significance: F(2,84) = 2.70, p = 0.073, g2 = 0.06). Arousing words were also better recognized than nonarousing words, particularly when they were positive in valence (significant effect of arousal interaction: F(1,84) = 4.65, p < 0.05, g2 = 0.05; significant Valence Arousal interaction: F(2,168) = 9.55, p < 0.001, g2 = 0.10). No other significant effects were observed. We then conducted an ANCOVA on the recognition data, using the valence and arousal ratings as covariates. This analysis revealed that when controlling for such word ratings, the aforementioned stress effects on memory were only approaching significance (Condition Valence interaction trending toward significance: F(4,164) = 2.03, p = 0.093, g2 = 0.05; Condition Sex interaction trending toward significance: F(2,82) = 2.82, p = 0.065, g2 = 0.06). 3.5. Associations between physiological stress response and memory There were significant negative correlations between cortisol and (1) overall delayed free recall, r(19) = 0.46, and (2) emotional word (i.e., positive, negative, neutral) delayed free recall, r(19) = 0.48, which were observed in stressed males (i.e., Responders and Non-Responders combined) only (p’s < 0.05). 4. Discussion We have found that brief stress administered 30 min before learning exerts deleterious effects on long-term (24-h) memory in human participants. Importantly, however, the magnitude of these effects and the type of influence that corticosteroids had on such effects appeared to differ across the sexes. We did find that Responders, independent of sex, exhibited impaired recognition of emotionally neutral information, but this effect seemed to be visibly more pronounced in males. In addition, males exhibiting a robust stress-induced increase in cortisol displayed significantly impaired recall and a marginally significant impairment of recognition memory, while females exhibiting a blunted stress-induced 83 P.R. Zoladz et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 100 (2013) 77–87 Males Responders Non-Responders No Stress 80 Free Recall (% of total) 80 Free Recall (% of total) Females Responders Non-Responders No Stress 60 40 20 0 60 40 20 0 Arousing NonArousing NonArousing NonArousing Arousing Arousing Positive Negative Neutral Arousing Total NonArousing NonArousing NonArousing Arousing Arousing Positive Negative Neutral Total Fig. 3. Immediate free recall performance in males (left) and females (right). No statistically significant group differences were observed, suggesting that stress exposure did not impact short-term memory. Data are presented as means ± SEM. Males Females Responders Non-Responders No Stress 60 50 40 30 * 20 10 * 0 Free Recall (% of total) 50 Free Recall (% of total) Responders Non-Responders No Stress 60 40 30 20 10 0 Arousing NonArousing NonArousing NonArousing Arousing Arousing Positive Negative Neutral Arousing Total NonArousing NonArousing NonArousing Arousing Arousing Positive Negative Neutral Total Fig. 4. Long-term (24-h) free recall performance in males (left) and females (right). Male Responders recalled fewer words, overall, than all other groups. This effect seemed to be driven by a large reduction in their recall of non-arousing words (verified via planned comparisons; see Section 4); however, the Condition Sex Arousal interaction was not significant. Data are presented as means ± SEM; = p < 0.05 relative to the Non-Responder and no stress groups. increase in cortisol exhibited a marginally significant impairment of recognition memory. In general, our findings support the temporal dynamics model of stress–memory interactions by revealing that even a brief stressor that is temporally separated from the learning experience can impair long-term memory, hypothetically via amygdala-induced inhibition of hippocampal processing. However, the differences observed across male and female participants draw attention to the fact that sex differences must be integrated into this model for it to be a more comprehensive approach to understanding emotional memory processing. 4.1. Neurobiological mechanisms Extensive work has shown that stress exerts differential effects on various stages of learning and memory. For instance, stress can enhance, impair or have no effect on encoding/acquisition (e.g., Diamond et al., 2006; Payne, Nadel, Allen, Thomas, & Jacobs, 2002; Schwabe et al., 2008a). On the other hand, stress generally facilitates consolidation (Beckner et al., 2006; Cahill et al., 2003; Hui et al., 2006; Preuss & Wolf, 2009; Smeets et al., 2008), while impairing retrieval (Buchanan & Tranel, 2008; Buchanan et al., 2006; de Quervain, Roozendaal, & McGaugh, 1998; Diamond et al., 2006; Kuhlmann et al., 2005; Smeets et al., 2008; Tollenaar et al., 2008), effects that have both been associated with stress-induced increases in glucocorticoids. In the present study, we observed significant effects of stress on delayed, but not immediate, memory testing, suggesting that stress largely affected the consolidation of information. Since we observed significant stress-induced impairments of long-term memory in both males and females, it might appear that our results are in conflict with the prevailing view that stress enhances consolidation. However, it is important to emphasize that a significant portion of studies demonstrating stress-induced enhancements of memory consolidation have administered the stress after learning (Beckner et al., 2006; Cahill et al., 2003; Hui et al., 2006; Preuss & Wolf, 2009; Smeets et al., 2008). The effects of pre-learning stress on memory consolidation are less clear and often depend on the timing of the stress relative to learning (Diamond et al., 2007; Zoladz et al., 2011a). For 84 P.R. Zoladz et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 100 (2013) 77–87 Males 3.0 2.5 Responders Non-Responders No Stress 3.5 β 2.0 1.5 1.0 0.5 0.0 Discrimination Index (d' ) 3.5 Discrimination Index (d' ) Females Responders Non-Responders No Stress 3.0 β 2.5 2.0 1.5 1.0 0.5 0.0 Arousing NonArousing NonArousing NonArousing Arousing Arousing Positive Negative Neutral Arousing Total NonArousing NonArousing NonArousing Arousing Arousing Positive Negative Neutral Total Fig. 5. Long-term (24-h) recognition performance in males (left) and females (right). Responders recognized fewer neutral words than all other groups. The Condition Sex interaction was approaching significance, suggesting that male Responders and female Non-Responders tended to recognize fewer words, overall, than their respective no stress group. The observed effect in male Responders seemed to be driven by a large reduction in their recognition of arousing words, while the observed effect in female NonResponders seemed to be driven by a large reduction in their recognition of non-arousing words (verified via planned comparisons; see Section 4); however, the Condition Sex Arousal interaction was not significant. Data are presented as means ± SEM; b = p = 0.073 relative to the no stress group. instance, according to the temporal dynamics model, pre-learning stress would enhance consolidation only if the stressor was brief and presented in close temporal proximity to the learning experience. If, on the other hand, the stressor was long-lasting or was a brief stressor that was temporally separated from the learning experience, then consolidation would hypothetically be impaired (see Diamond et al., 2006 for an example). Research has also shown that pre-learning stress effects on long-term memory depend on the sex of the organism and the type of information being acquired. For instance, studies have reported that pre-learning stress often facilitates memory for emotional information, while impairing memory for neutral information (Jelicic et al., 2004; Payne et al., 2006; Payne et al., 2007). Moreover, the stress-induced enhancement of emotional memory has been reported to be stronger in females, while the stress-induced impairment of neutral memory has been reported to be stronger in males (Payne et al., 2006). Along similar lines, we found that stress impaired the recognition of neutral words in Responders, an effect that seemed to be more pronounced in males, and that the stress-induced impairment of recall in males and recognition in females was largely driven by reduced memory for non-arousing information (discussed further below). Thus, how stress affects consolidation depends on several factors related to the timing of the stressor, the type of information being learned and the sex of the organism. Research has accumulated over the past decade to suggest that corticosteroids act as ‘‘two-stage rockets’’, exerting immediate excitatory, but delayed inhibitory, effects on cellular function and synaptic plasticity (Joels et al., 2006; Joels et al., 2011). In relation to the temporal dynamics model of emotional memory processing, the initial excitatory phase of hippocampal function that immediately follows stress onset is thought to depend, at least in part, on noradrenergic activity coupled with non-genomic activity of corticosteroids and an ensuing increase in glutamatergic transmission (Diamond et al., 2007; Karst et al., 2005). The inhibitory phase that follows shortly thereafter is thought to depend on NMDA receptor desensitization and delayed, gene-dependent activity of corticosteroids (Diamond et al., 2007; Joels et al., 2003). The present findings support this model, at least to a degree, in that Responders, overall, exhibited impaired recognition memory for neutral words, while male Responders exhibited impaired recall and a marginally signif- icant impairment of recognition memory. However, the predictions based on the temporal dynamics model were less evident in female participants. That is to say, despite the finding of impaired recognition memory for neutral information in Responders, this effect seemed to be driven largely by deficits in males. Moreover, stressed females demonstrated no significant alteration of recall performance, and the marginally significant impairment that was observed on recognition memory in females was observed in Non-Responders, not Responders. There was also a significant negative correlation between cortisol and memory in stressed males, but no such association was observed in females. In fact, if anything, our findings would suggest that cortisol had some sort of ‘‘protective’’ effect on memory, at least for that of recognition memory overall, in females. Ultimately, these results support the temporal dynamics model by showing that brief stress that was temporally separated from the learning experience impaired long-term memory in both male and female participants. However, the sex-dependent influence of corticosteroids on these effects indicate that there is a need to update the model, which was based largely on research conducted in males, by considering how sex differences influence the relationship between timing, stress– memory interactions and their neurobiological basis. Another, fairly complementary, approach to the biological basis of stress–memory interactions takes into account general differences in the effects of stress on amygdala and hippocampus function. Extensive work has shown that an intact amygdala is essential for enhanced memory of emotional information (Adolphs, Cahill, Schul, & Babinsky, 1997; Cahill et al., 1996; Canli, Zhao, Brewer, Gabrieli, & Cahill, 2000), as well as for the stress-induced effects on hippocampus-dependent learning and synaptic plasticity (Kim et al., 2001, 2005; Zoladz, Park, & Diamond, 2011b). There is also considerable evidence for the direct modulation of hippocampal function via amygdala activation (Akirav & Richter-Levin, 1999, 2002; Richter-Levin & Akirav, 2003; Vouimba, Yaniv, & Richter-Levin, 2007), which provides a potential mechanism to explain stress-induced effects on information storage. It has been speculated that amygdala activation underlies the stress-induced enhancement of emotional memory, while hippocampal inhibition underlies the stress-induced impairment of neutral memory (Diamond et al., 2007; Jelicic et al., 2004; Payne et al., 2007). Theoret- P.R. Zoladz et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 100 (2013) 77–87 ically, in the face of stress, the processing of information with survival value (e.g., arousing) would take priority over the storage of other, less important (e.g., non-arousing) information (Diamond et al., 2007). In the present study, it is possible that stress enhanced amygdala function while at the same time inhibited hippocampal function, which manifested as impaired memory, particularly for emotionally neutral information. Indeed, a close examination of the effects of stress on memory in the present study revealed that the stress-induced impairment of recall in males and the stress-induced marginally significant impairment of recognition in females seemed to be largely driven by reduced memory for non-arousing information. These speculations were corroborated by planned comparison analyses (p’s < 0.05), despite the lack of statistically significant Condition Sex Arousal interactions. Also, Responders, independent of sex, recognized significantly fewer neutral words than all other groups, even though this effect also seemed to be more pronounced in males. On the other hand, the marginally significant impairment of recognition memory observed in males seemed to be largely driven by reduced memory for arousing information, which according to the aforementioned perspective, might be expected to be enhanced, if anything, as a result of a stress-induced increase in amygdala activity. It is possible that stress does not always enhance one’s memory for arousing information. Indeed, some research has shown that whether or not the information is related to the stressor is important in determining if memory is affected by the stress. Smeets et al. (2009) found that stress enhanced participants’ memory for words only if the words were highly arousing and directly related to the stressor that was employed in the study. Thus, the traditional stress-induced enhancement of arousing information may not have taken place in the present study because the studied words were not related to the cold pressor stress. 4.2. Sex differences Our finding of greater deleterious effects of pre-learning stress on long-term memory in males, relative to females, is not uncommon. Others have reported significant effects of stress on memory in males, while finding no differences (e.g., Andreano & Cahill, 2006) or an enhancement of memory (e.g., Payne et al., 2006) in females. Comparable findings have also been reported in studies examining stress effects on working memory (e.g., Cornelisse, van Stegeren, & Joels, 2011). It is possible that participants’ perceived arousal of the studied words influenced the observed sex differences in the present study. We found that males rated the studied words as less arousing than females did. Thus, it is possible that stress exerted greater deleterious effects on memory in males because the studied information, overall, was not perceived as emotionally arousing as it was in females. This speculation would relate to previous work that has shown greater stress-induced impairments of memory for neutral information in males (Payne et al., 2006). The present findings are also markedly similar to those from investigators who have reported negative or curvilinear relationships between cortisol and memory in males, but not females (Andreano & Cahill, 2006; Wolf et al., 2001). We found that male Responders exhibited impaired long-term free recall and a marginally significant impairment of long-term recognition memory. Female Non-Responders, on the other hand, displayed a marginally significant impairment of long-term recognition memory. Thus, as indicated above, cortisol was potentially associated with better memory, at least for that of recognition memory overall, in females, in contrast to males. A closer examination of the female recognition memory data, however, suggests that a strong cortisol response to the stress might have simply ‘‘preserved’’ their memory (Payne et al., 2006), rather than facilitating it, since female 85 Responders performed better relative to female Non-Responders only, and female Non-Responders performed more poorly than the no stress group. Since we did not observe any significant sex differences in cortisol response to the stressor, these differential effects that we observed on measures of memory are not likely attributable to variations in hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis response. Studies have shown that differences in the menstrual cycle play an important role in the stress-induced alteration of cognition. For instance, Andreano, Arjomandi, and Cahill (2008) observed a positive correlation between stress-induced salivary cortisol and memory only when female participants were in the mid-luteal phase of the menstrual cycle, a time when progesterone levels are significantly elevated. Subsequent work substantiated these findings by reporting that females exhibiting high levels of progesterone (i.e., mid-luteal phase) demonstrate better memory for emotional information, greater stress-induced elevations of salivary cortisol and stronger stress-induced enhancements of emotional memory than women with low levels of progesterone (i.e., non-luteal phase) (Ertman, Andreano, & Cahill, 2011; Felmingham, Fong, & Bryant, 2012). Females also exhibit stronger responses of the amygdalahippocampus neural network to emotional stimuli when they are in the luteal phase (Andreano & Cahill, 2010). Although we did not control for the menstrual cycle in the present study, it is possible that the differential effects observed in males vs. females could be attributable to such a factor. If female Responders and Non-Responders were in different stages of the menstrual cycle, they could have exhibited differential sensitivity to stress hormones, which led to different effects on memory. Indeed, research has shown that the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle is associated with altered glucocorticoid sensitivity (Rohleder, Schommer, Hellhammer, Engel, & Kirschbaum, 2001; Rohleder, Wolf, Kirschbaum, & Wolf, 2009) and elevated norepinephrine levels (Minson, Halliwill, Young, & Joyner, 2000). In order to verify this speculation, however, future work will need to control for this factor when examining pre-learning stress effects on memory. It is also possible that the female brain does not respond to emotion in the same manner that the male brain does. Some research has shown that the brain responses of males and females to emotional stimuli are hemisphere-specific. For instance, Cahill and others have reported that, when presented with emotional stimuli, males tend to exhibit greater activity in the right amygdala, while females tend to exhibit greater activity in the left amygdala (Cahill, Uncapher, Kilpatrick, Alkire, & Turner, 2004; Cahill et al., 2001; Canli, Desmond, Zhao, & Gabrieli, 2002; Mackiewicz, Sarinopoulos, Cleven, & Nitschke, 2006). Interestingly, the right amygdala has been associated with one’s memory for gist, while the left amygdala has been associated with one’s memory for details (Fink, Marshall, Halligan, & Dolan, 1999; Fink et al., 1996). Thus, it may not be surprising to note that, under emotionally arousing conditions, males exhibit greater memory for the central aspects of a scene, while females exhibit greater memory for the details (Cahill & van Stegeren, 2003). This differential activation of brain areas in response to emotional stimuli could play an important role in sex influences on stress–memory interactions. It could also be related to stress-induced switches in learning style that appear to differ across the sexes (Beck & Luine, 2010). 4.3. Limitations and caveats Although the present findings provide important insight into the factors that influence stress–memory interactions, there were some limitations that deserve attention. First, when we divided the stressed participants into Responders and Non-Responders, we ended up with a relatively small sample size of male Responders (N = 8). Since a significant portion of our effects were evident in 86 P.R. Zoladz et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 100 (2013) 77–87 this group, caution should be employed when interpreting the data, and future work should be conducted to replicate the results. Second, we did allow females who were regularly taking birth control to participate in the study. Even though we detected no statistically significant differences between birth control users and naturally cycling females, the presence of both groups could have added noise to the data, influencing the resulting stress effects on cognition. Third, it is unknown as to how the menstrual cycle influenced the results for female participants in the present study, since we did not control for this factor. Fourth, we have interpreted some of the findings based on marginally significant effects, and in order to verify our speculations, future work, perhaps with larger sample sizes and greater statistical power, will need to be conducted. Finally, additional research should address the involvement of neuromodulators other than cortisol in the pre-learning stress-induced alteration of learning and memory. For instance, it is well known that noradrenergic mechanisms interact with corticosteroids to influence cognition, and recent work has shown that endocannabinoids play an important part in this process. 4.4. Conclusions In sum, we have shown that brief stress administered 30 min prior to learning exerts differential effects on long-term memory in males and females, effects that depend largely on the cortisol response of participants and the emotional salience of the learned information. Responders, overall, exhibited impaired recognition of neutral information. Additionally, male Responders exhibited impaired recall and a marginally significant impairment of recognition, while female Non-Responders exhibited a marginally significant impairment of recognition. From these findings, it can be hypothesized that there is a sex-dependent involvement of corticosteroids in pre-learning stress effects on long-term memory. Thus, the temporal dynamics model, in its current form, perhaps better summarizes stress–memory interactions in males, and it is necessary to revise this approach to produce a more comprehensive understanding of how stress affects cognition. On the other hand, general stress effects on amygdala vs. hippocampus function and how they interact with sex provides for an equally appealing, complementary explanation of the results. References Adolphs, R., Cahill, L., Schul, R., & Babinsky, R. (1997). Impaired declarative memory for emotional material following bilateral amygdala damage in humans. Learning and Memory, 4(3), 291–300. Akirav, I., & Richter-Levin, G. (1999). Biphasic modulation of hippocampal plasticity by behavioral stress and basolateral amygdala stimulation in the rat. Journal of Neuroscience, 19(23), 10530–10535. Akirav, I., & Richter-Levin, G. (2002). Mechanisms of amygdala modulation of hippocampal plasticity. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(22), 9912–9921. Andreano, J. M., Arjomandi, H., & Cahill, L. (2008). Menstrual cycle modulation of the relationship between cortisol and long-term memory. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33(6), 874–882. Andreano, J. M., & Cahill, L. (2006). Glucocorticoid release and memory consolidation in men and women. Psychological Science, 17(6), 466–470. Andreano, J. M., & Cahill, L. (2009). Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learning and Memory, 16(4), 248–266. Andreano, J. M., & Cahill, L. (2010). Menstrual cycle modulation of medial temporal activity evoked by negative emotion. Neuroimage, 53(4), 1286–1293. Beck, K. D., & Luine, V. N. (2010). Evidence for sex-specific shifting of neural processes underlying learning and memory following stress. Physiology and Behavior, 99(2), 204–211. Beckner, V. E., Tucker, D. M., Delville, Y., & Mohr, D. C. (2006). Stress facilitates consolidation of verbal memory for a film but does not affect retrieval. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120(3), 518–527. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical report C-1. The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida. Buchanan, T. W., & Tranel, D. (2008). Stress and emotional memory retrieval: Effects of sex and cortisol response. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 89(2), 134–141. Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., & Adolphs, R. (2006). Impaired memory retrieval correlates with individual differences in cortisol response but not autonomic response. Learning and Memory, 13(3), 382–387. Cahill, L., Gorski, L., & Le, K. (2003). Enhanced human memory consolidation with post-learning stress: Interaction with the degree of arousal at encoding. Learning and Memory, 10(4), 270–274. Cahill, L., Haier, R. J., Fallon, J., Alkire, M. T., Tang, C., Keator, D., et al. (1996). Amygdala activity at encoding correlated with long-term, free recall of emotional information. Proceedings of the National Academic Science USA, 93(15), 8016–8021. Cahill, L., Haier, R. J., White, N. S., Fallon, J., Kilpatrick, L., Lawrence, C., et al. (2001). Sex-related difference in amygdala activity during emotionally influenced memory storage. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 75(1), 1–9. Cahill, L., Uncapher, M., Kilpatrick, L., Alkire, M. T., & Turner, J. (2004). Sex-related hemispheric lateralization of amygdala function in emotionally influenced memory: An FMRI investigation. Learning and Memory, 11(3), 261–266. Cahill, L., & van Stegeren, A. (2003). Sex-related impairment of memory for emotional events with beta-adrenergic blockade. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 79(1), 81–88. Canli, T., Desmond, J. E., Zhao, Z., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Sex differences in the neural basis of emotional memories. Proceedings of the National Academic Science USA, 99(16), 10789–10794. Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Brewer, J., Gabrieli, J. D., & Cahill, L. (2000). Event-related activation in the human amygdala associates with later memory for individual emotional experience. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(19), RC99. Conrad, C. D., Jackson, J. L., Wieczorek, L., Baran, S. E., Harman, J. S., Wright, R. L., et al. (2004). Acute stress impairs spatial memory in male but not female rats: Influence of estrous cycle. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 78(3), 569–579. Cornelisse, S., van Stegeren, A. H., & Joels, M. (2011). Implications of psychosocial stress on memory formation in a typical male versus female student sample. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36(4), 569–578. de Quervain, D. J., Roozendaal, B., & McGaugh, J. L. (1998). Stress and glucocorticoids impair retrieval of long-term spatial memory. Nature, 394(6695), 787–790. Diamond, D. M., Campbell, A. M., Park, C. R., Halonen, J., & Zoladz, P. R. (2007). The temporal dynamics model of emotional memory processing: A synthesis on the neurobiological basis of stress-induced amnesia, flashbulb and traumatic memories, and the Yerkes-Dodson law. Neural Plasticity, 2007, 60803. Diamond, D. M., Campbell, A. M., Park, C. R., Woodson, J. C., Conrad, C. D., Bachstetter, A. D., et al. (2006). Influence of predator stress on the consolidation versus retrieval of long-term spatial memory and hippocampal spinogenesis. Hippocampus, 16(7), 571–576. Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2004). Interaction between the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system predicts better memory for emotional events. Neuron, 42(5), 855–863. Duncko, R., Johnson, L., Merikangas, K., & Grillon, C. (2009). Working memory performance after acute exposure to the cold pressor stress in healthy volunteers. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 91(4), 377–381. Elzinga, B. M., Bakker, A., & Bremner, J. D. (2005). Stress-induced cortisol elevations are associated with impaired delayed, but not immediate recall. Psychiatry Research, 134(3), 211–223. Ertman, N., Andreano, J. M., & Cahill, L. (2011). Progesterone at encoding predicts subsequent emotional memory. Learning and Memory, 18(12), 759–763. Felmingham, K. L., Fong, W. C., & Bryant, R. A. (2012). The impact of progesterone on memory consolidation of threatening images in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(11), 1896–1900. Fink, G. R., Halligan, P. W., Marshall, J. C., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S., & Dolan, R. J. (1996). Where in the brain does visual attention select the forest and the trees? Nature, 382(6592), 626–628. Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., Halligan, P. W., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). Hemispheric asymmetries in global/local processing are modulated by perceptual salience. Neuropsychologia, 37(1), 31–40. Frey, S., Bergado-Rosado, J., Seidenbecher, T., Pape, H. C., & Frey, J. U. (2001). Reinforcement of early long-term potentiation (early-LTP) in dentate gyrus by stimulation of the basolateral amygdala: Heterosynaptic induction mechanisms of late-LTP. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(10), 3697–3703. Halonen, J. D., Zoladz, P. R., Park, C. R., & Diamond, D. M. (2007). Propranolol blocks the stress-induced enhancement, but not impairment, of long-term spatial memory in adult rats. Paper presented at the thirty-seventh annual meeting of the society for neuroscience, San Diego, CA. Hui, I. R., Hui, G. K., Roozendaal, B., McGaugh, J. L., & Weinberger, N. M. (2006). Posttraining handling facilitates memory for auditory-cue fear conditioning in rats. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 86(2), 160–163. Jackson, E. D., Payne, J. D., Nadel, L., & Jacobs, W. J. (2006). Stress differentially modulates fear conditioning in healthy men and women. Biological Psychiatry, 59(6), 516–522. Jelicic, M., Geraerts, E., Merckelbach, H., & Guerrieri, R. (2004). Acute stress enhances memory for emotional words, but impairs memory for neutral words. International Journal of Neuroscience, 114(10), 1343–1351. Joels, M., Fernandez, G., & Roozendaal, B. (2011). Stress and emotional memory: A matter of timing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(6), 280–288. Joels, M., Pu, Z., Wiegert, O., Oitzl, M. S., & Krugers, H. J. (2006). Learning under stress: How does it work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(4), 152–158. Joels, M., Velzing, E., Nair, S., Verkuyl, J. M., & Karst, H. (2003). Acute stress increases calcium current amplitude in rat hippocampus: Temporal changes in physiology and gene expression. European Journal of Neuroscience, 18(5), 1315–1324. P.R. Zoladz et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 100 (2013) 77–87 Karst, H., Berger, S., Turiault, M., Tronche, F., Schutz, G., & Joels, M. (2005). Mineralocorticoid receptors are indispensable for nongenomic modulation of hippocampal glutamate transmission by corticosterone. Proceedings of the National Academic Science USA, 102(52), 19204–19207. Kim, J. J., Koo, J. W., Lee, H. J., & Han, J. S. (2005). Amygdalar inactivation blocks stress-induced impairments in hippocampal long-term potentiation and spatial memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(6), 1532–1539. Kim, J. J., Lee, H. J., Han, J. S., & Packard, M. G. (2001). Amygdala is critical for stressinduced modulation of hippocampal long-term potentiation and learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(14), 5222–5228. Kuhlmann, S., Piel, M., & Wolf, O. T. (2005). Impaired memory retrieval after psychosocial stress in healthy young men. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(11), 2977–2982. Mackiewicz, K. L., Sarinopoulos, I., Cleven, K. L., & Nitschke, J. B. (2006). The effect of anticipation and the specificity of sex differences for amygdala and hippocampus function in emotional memory. Proceedings of the National Academic Science USA, 103(38), 14200–14205. McGaugh, J. L. (2004). The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 1–28. Minson, C. T., Halliwill, J. R., Young, T. M., & Joyner, M. J. (2000). Influence of the menstrual cycle on sympathetic activity, baroreflex sensitivity, and vascular transduction in young women. Circulation, 101(8), 862–868. Nater, U. M., Moor, C., Okere, U., Stallkamp, R., Martin, M., Ehlert, U., et al. (2007). Performance on a declarative memory task is better in high than low cortisol responders to psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32(6), 758–763. Park, C. R., Zoladz, P. R., Conrad, C. D., Fleshner, M., & Diamond, D. M. (2008). Acute predator stress impairs the consolidation and retrieval of hippocampusdependent memory in male and female rats. Learning and Memory, 15(4), 271–280. Payne, J. D., Jackson, E. D., Hoscheidt, S., Ryan, L., Jacobs, W. J., & Nadel, L. (2007). Stress administered prior to encoding impairs neutral but enhances emotional long-term episodic memories. Learning and Memory, 14(12), 861–868. Payne, J. D., Jackson, E. D., Ryan, L., Hoscheidt, S., Jacobs, J. W., & Nadel, L. (2006). The impact of stress on neutral and emotional aspects of episodic memory. Memory, 14(1), 1–16. Payne, J. D., Nadel, L., Allen, J. J., Thomas, K. G., & Jacobs, W. J. (2002). The effects of experimentally induced stress on false recognition. Memory, 10(1), 1–6. Preuss, D., & Wolf, O. T. (2009). Post-learning psychosocial stress enhances consolidation of neutral stimuli. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92(3), 318–326. Richter-Levin, G., & Akirav, I. (2003). Emotional tagging of memory formation–in the search for neural mechanisms. Brain Research. Brain Research Reviews, 43(3), 247–256. Rohleder, N., Schommer, N. C., Hellhammer, D. H., Engel, R., & Kirschbaum, C. (2001). Sex differences in glucocorticoid sensitivity of proinflammatory cytokine production after psychosocial stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63(6), 966–972. Rohleder, N., Wolf, J. M., Kirschbaum, C., & Wolf, O. T. (2009). Effects of cortisol on emotional but not on neutral memory are correlated with peripheral glucocorticoid sensitivity of inflammatory cytokine production. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72(1), 74–80. Sandi, C. (2011). Glucocorticoids act on glutamatergic pathways to affect memory processes. Trends in Neurosciences, 34(4), 165–176. Schwabe, L., Bohringer, A., Chatterjee, M., & Schachinger, H. (2008a). Effects of prelearning stress on memory for neutral, positive and negative words: Different 87 roles of cortisol and autonomic arousal. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 90(1), 44–53. Schwabe, L., Haddad, L., & Schachinger, H. (2008b). HPA axis activation by a socially evaluated cold-pressor test. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33(6), 890–895. Schwabe, L., Joels, M., Roozendaal, B., Wolf, O. T., & Oitzl, M. S. (2012). Stress effects on memory: An update and integration. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(7), 1740–1749. Schwabe, L., Romer, S., Richter, S., Dockendorf, S., Bilak, B., & Schachinger, H. (2009). Stress effects on declarative memory retrieval are blocked by a betaadrenoceptor antagonist in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(3), 446–454. Schwabe, L., Wolf, O. T., & Oitzl, M. S. (2010). Memory formation under stress: Quantity and quality. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(4), 584–591. Smeets, T., Otgaar, H., Candel, I., & Wolf, O. T. (2008). True or false? Memory is differentially affected by stress-induced cortisol elevations and sympathetic activity at consolidation and retrieval. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33(10), 1378–1386. Smeets, T., Wolf, O. T., Giesbrecht, T., Sijstermans, K., Telgen, S., & Joels, M. (2009). Stress selectively and lastingly promotes learning of context-related high arousing information. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(8), 1152–1161. Strange, B. A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Beta-adrenergic modulation of emotional memory-evoked human amygdala and hippocampal responses. Proceedings of the National Academic Science USA, 101(31), 11454–11458. Tollenaar, M. S., Elzinga, B. M., Spinhoven, P., & Everaerd, W. (2008). Long-term outcomes of memory retrieval under stress. Behavioral Neuroscience, 122(3), 697–703. Vouimba, R. M., Yaniv, D., & Richter-Levin, G. (2007). Glucocorticoid receptors and beta-adrenoceptors in basolateral amygdala modulate synaptic plasticity in hippocampal dentate gyrus, but not in area CA1. Neuropharmacology, 52(1), 244–252. Wickens, T. D. (2002). Elementary signal detection theory. Oxford: University Press. Wiegert, O., Joels, M., & Krugers, H. (2006). Timing is essential for rapid effects of corticosterone on synaptic potentiation in the mouse hippocampus. Learning and Memory, 13(2), 110–113. Wolf, O. T. (2009). Stress and memory in humans: Twelve years of progress? Brain Research, 1293, 142–154. Wolf, O. T., Schommer, N. C., Hellhammer, D. H., McEwen, B. S., & Kirschbaum, C. (2001). The relationship between stress induced cortisol levels and memory differs between men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26(7), 711–720. Wood, G. E., Beylin, A. V., & Shors, T. J. (2001). The contribution of adrenal and reproductive hormones to the opposing effects of stress on trace conditioning in males versus females. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115(1), 175–187. Yang, Y., Cao, J., Xiong, W., Zhang, J., Zhou, Q., Wei, H., et al. (2003). Both stress experience and age determine the impairment or enhancement effect of stress on spatial memory retrieval. Journal of Endocrinology, 178(1), 45–54. Zoladz, P. R., Clark, B., Warnecke, A., Smith, L., Tabar, J., & Talbot, J. N. (2011a). Prelearning stress differentially affects long-term memory for emotional words, depending on temporal proximity to the learning experience. Physiology & Behavior, 103(5), 467–476. Zoladz, P. R., Park, C. R., & Diamond, D. M. (2011b). Neurobiological basis of the complex effects of stress on memory and synaptic plasticity. In C. D. Conrad (Ed.), The handbook of stress: Neuropsychological effects on the brain (pp. 157– 178). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. Zorawski, M., Blanding, N. Q., Kuhn, C. M., & LaBar, K. S. (2006). Effects of stress and sex on acquisition and consolidation of human fear conditioning. Learning and Memory, 13(4), 441–450.