This article was downloaded by: [Hoffman, Lindsay] On: 12 March 2009

advertisement
This article was downloaded by: [Hoffman, Lindsay]
On: 12 March 2009
Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 909494005]
Publisher Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t775648091
The Effect of Television Viewing on Adolescents' Civic Participation: Political
Efficacy as a Mediating Mechanism
Lindsay H. Hoffman a; Tiffany L. Thomson
a
Department of Communication, University of Delaware,
Online Publication Date: 01 January 2009
To cite this Article Hoffman, Lindsay H. and Thomson, Tiffany L.(2009)'The Effect of Television Viewing on Adolescents' Civic
Participation: Political Efficacy as a Mediating Mechanism',Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media,53:1,3 — 21
To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/08838150802643415
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838150802643415
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf
This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or
systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or
distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.
The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents
will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses
should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss,
actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly
or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media/March 2009
The Effect of Television Viewing
on Adolescents’ Civic Participation:
Political Efficacy as a
Mediating Mechanism
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
Lindsay H. Hoffman and Tiffany L. Thomson
Pundits, parents, and scholars express concern about youth attention to latenight political comedy shows, such as The Daily Show, suggesting that such
viewing is deleterious for an active, efficacious citizenry. Yet as civic participation declines among adults, it appears to be growing among adolescents.
This study assessed the effects of television viewing on high school students’
civic participation. Results demonstrate that viewing late-night TV and local
TV news had a positive, significant effect on civic participation, and this
relationship was mediated by political efficacy. Implications for the study
of late-night TV and applications to research on political socialization are
discussed.
A democracy assumes that its citizens have motivation and agency to affect society through established political processes (Youniss & Yates, 1999). Social scientists
in the last half-century have become increasingly interested in what motivates and
socializes people into becoming active citizens of a democracy. Much of this interest
stems from observations about the decline in key indicators of an active citizenry,
including behaviors such as voting (Teixeira, 1987) and other forms of political
participation (Putnam, 2000), as well as affective dimensions such as confidence in
government (Cappella & Jamieson, 1996), and trust (Damico, Conway, & Damico,
2000). Meanwhile, numerous scholars of political socialization of youth attempt
Lindsay H. Hoffman (Ph.D., The Ohio State University) is an Assistant Professor in the Department
of Communication at University of Delaware. Her research interests include media effects, political
communication, and public opinion.
Tiffany L. Thomson (Ph.D., The Ohio State University). Her research interests include news use, political
discussion, and political knowledge.
This research was funded primarily by The Columbus Foundation through Kids Voting, Central Ohio.
Supplementary funding was provided by the School of Communication and the College of Social and
Behavioral Sciences at The Ohio State University. The authors would like to thank Suzanne Helmick of
Kids Voting, Central Ohio and Dwight Groce, Keith Bossard, Saundra Brennan, and Susan Martin of the
Columbus Public School District, as well as Dr. William P. Eveland, Jr. and Jessica Flanders for assistance
in the gathering of these data. This report is a product solely of the authors; the funding sources and those
who assisted in data gathering bear no responsibility for the contents of this manuscript.
This article was submitted and accepted under the editorship of Donald G. Godfrey.
© 2009 Broadcast Education Association
DOI: 10.1080/08838150802643415
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media 53(1), 2009, pp. 3–21
ISSN: 0883-8151 print/1550-6878 online
3
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
4
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media/March 2009
to answer the same question as some of the same variables are declining among
younger adults as well (Rahn & Transue, 1998). Contrasting reports suggest that
youth are participating at increased rates (Lopez, 2004). This suggests an unclear
outlook for the active citizenship of young Americans in the future, and encourages
scholars to closely analyze those characteristics of youth associated with democratic
citizenship. This study assesses youth attention to television news and late-night
comedy shows as one such characteristic that could provide key insights into this
area of research.
Popular media and polling reports have emphasized the increased participation among youth in the last two presidential elections. Organizations such as
the Corporation for National & Community Service, the Bureau of Labor Statistics,
and the Center for Information and Research on Civic Learning and Engagement
(CIRCLE) have reported that adolescents and college-age students are participating
in volunteer and civic activities in increasing numbers. For instance, over 75%
of high school seniors reported volunteering in 2001 compared to 62% in 1976
(Lopez, 2004). Importantly, such participation has been shown to influence activity
at a later age; for example, 20% of high school sophomores reported volunteering
in 1990, and of this same class, over twice that amount reported volunteering at age
20 (Lopez). Understanding that such participation might influence civic activity at a
later age, it becomes of great consequence to understand the mechanisms that drive
this activity among adolescents. The questions for mass communication researchers
become, does media use play a role in predicting this civic participation? If so, how?
Defining Political Socialization
There has been great attention to political socialization in the mass communication literature, even though children must learn many facets of life in order to
become socialized members of society. One reason for this focus is because scholars
understand that democracy isn’t exactly a natural state of being; instead, it consists
of habits and competencies that require skills that must be acquired (Barber, 1984;
Parker, 1996). These skills are obtained (or, arguably, not obtained in many cases)
through the primary socializing agents of school, family, and the media (Kelly &
Donohew, 1999).
Literature on political socialization proliferated in the 1950s and 1960s. Since
then, however, many scholars claim that the conceptualization of this process was
narrowly focused or even biased (McLeod, 2000; Youniss & Yates, 1999). Earlier
definitions emphasized an internalization of the norms existing in the current political system (e.g., Sigel, 1965), yet recent research has demonstrated that socialization
is not merely a ‘‘top-down transmission model’’ (McLeod, p. 46); rather the child
serves as an active participant in his or her socialization (McDevitt, 2006; McDevitt
& Chaffee, 2002).
One of the main scholarly foci of political socialization is the development of
citizenship or civic duty in young people (McLeod, 2000). As the more traditional
Hoffman and Thomson/TV, ADOLESCENTS, AND PARTICIPATION
5
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
model of political socialization has transitioned into the contemporary perspective
that a child is an active participant in his or her socialization, scholars have begun to
think of citizenship as commitment to actions that benefit others and the common
good (e.g., Sherrod, Flanagan, & Youniss, 2002). Media are considered to be an
important socializing agent in this process. That is, they can instigate pluralistic
communication, just as certain family communication patterns can (Chaffee & Yang,
1990). However, the role of media use in citizenship development among youth
has not been fully explored, particularly as it relates to civic or political participation (McLeod, 2000; Sherrod et al., 2002). In fact, Chaffee and Kanihan (1997)
remind readers that television and newspapers change rapidly, and that their roles
in political socialization need to be continually reassessed.
Adolescents and Political Socialization Through Media
Because the majority of people are not personally involved in politics, most
of what individuals know about the political environment comes via the mass
media (Becker, McCombs, & McLeod, 1975). In this way, the political world comes
to people as a ‘‘second-hand reality,’’ helping to form not only individuals’ first
political cognitions, but changing them over time (Becker et al., 1975).
Perhaps more importantly, even though political socialization begins in preschool
years, understanding of abstract political issues is not likely to occur until adolescence, when young people are more cognitively developed (Austin & Pinkleton,
2001). Because adolescents are able to think more abstractly and less egocentrically,
their potential for understanding politics and news programming is greatly increased
over that of younger children.
Chaffee, Jackson-Beeck, Durall, and Wilson (1977) concluded that mass media
constitute the principal source of political information for young people. This influential study also found that newspapers and television are the dominant tools
for learning political information, and that young people attribute considerable
influence on their opinions to the media. Finally, these authors asserted that children
do not necessarily adopt the public affairs media use of their parents.
Media have become a more powerful agent of socialization as a result of a decline
in the role of the family as a socializing agent (Arnett, 1995). This is particularly
true for adolescents who seek independence and look for sources of socialization
outside the family (Kelly & Donohew, 1999). Thus, understanding how adolescents
use media becomes an important method for identifying the ways in which they are
socialized into a culture.
A typology of the most common media uses by adolescents cannot cover all media
uses, but it provides some insight into the uses and gratifications sought by adolescents. Arnett (1995) suggested that adolescents use media for 1) entertainment; 2)
identity formation; 3) high sensation; 4) coping; and 5) youth culture identification.
One will notice that keeping up with news or public affairs does not make this
list. Yet public affairs media use could be said to fulfill the socialization process
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
6
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media/March 2009
in which adolescents engage to prepare for future roles (Arnett, 1995; Rogers,
1985). The focus of the present study is television, because the programming on this
medium has been identified as the predominant channel by which young people
first encounter political information and public affairs news (Chaffee & Kanihan,
1997), although future work should also assess the comparative role of the Internet
in socialization.
Beyond traditional public affairs media use, scholars have become increasingly
interested in late-night comedy television. Specifically, research has examined the
audiences of these programs, how the programs compare to other types of television
news, and what effects watching such shows might have. Although much of this
research does not examine adolescents, studies do show that the general audience
for late-night television is younger than for public affairs programs (e.g., Hollander,
2005; Young & Tisinger, 2006).
Watching late-night comedy shows has been associated with a number of both
positive and negative effects, such as attention to traditional news sources, political
learning, candidate evaluations, political efficacy, and political cynicism. Young and
Tisinger (2006) found that watching late-night comedy television is associated with
other forms of news exposure, including local and national television news. These
authors also found that an increase in perceived learning from late-night comedy
television was associated with an increase in learning from other forms of television
news. However, Hollander (2005) found that there is a curvilinear effect of viewing
late-night television on political learning, such that those who watch a moderate
amount tend to recall the most campaign information, while those who watched
either low or high amounts tend to recall less.
In another study, Young (2004) found that late-night comedy viewing did not
have a main effect on attitudes, but the impact of these ratings varied by level of
political knowledge. According to Baumgartner and Morris (2006), exposure to The
Daily Show with Jon Stewart was associated with decreased candidate evaluations,
decreased external political efficacy, and increased cynicism with the news media.
Hollander (2005), who assessed solely political learning as an outcome of viewing
such programming, concluded that ‘‘young people are capable of gleaning at least
modest amounts of campaign information from such content, but how competent
it leaves them to participate in a meaningful manner remains an open question’’
(p. 412). This study tests whether attention to various types of televised content
influences perceived competence—or efficacy—and actual participation in politics
and the community.
Civic Participation
Participation in extracurricular activities as an adolescent has been found to
influence political participation later in the life cycle (McFarland & Thomas, 2006;
Smith, Denton, Faris & Regnerus, 2002). Current polls show that adolescents and
college-age students are participating in volunteer and civic activities at an increased
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
Hoffman and Thomson/TV, ADOLESCENTS, AND PARTICIPATION
7
rate compared with their same age group in previous generations (Corporation for
National & Community Service, 2006). Volunteer rates among youth aged 16–24
has increased steadily over the last three years, from 21.9% to 24.4% (Bureau of
Labor Statistics, 2005). The report from the Corporation for National & Community
Service found that high school students who participated in school-based service
were more likely to participate in other volunteering activities, and to feel that they
can make a difference in their communities. But students do not participate solely
through school; some participate through religious groups, civic organizations, or
neighborhood groups.
Although many studies have established the link between media use and political
participation (e.g., Becker, Sobawale, & Casey, 1979; Lee, 2005; Moy, McCluskey,
McCoy, & Spratt, 2004; Ostroff & Sandell, 1989), it is important to acknowledge
that with adolescents, true ‘‘political’’ participation is not necessarily a feasible
option because of their age. Researchers must consider comparable activities that
have been shown to lead to participation later in life, such as volunteering for
a youth group or religious organization, participating in student government or a
debate club, or volunteering for a neighborhood or school program (McFarland
& Thomas, 2006; Smith et al., 2002). These extracurricular activities ‘‘play an
important role as a socializer, developing in young people the norms, attitudes, and
skills conducive to later adult political and civic participation’’ (Smith, 1999). This
study will examine whether a relationship exists between certain types of television
use—namely, national TV news viewing, local TV news viewing, and late-night
comedy TV viewing—and civic participation in youth-applicable extracurricular
activities.
Assessing Mechanisms of Media Effects
Although these studies have revealed the positive effects of adolescent civic
participation—such as their effect on adult political and civic behavior—it is still
essential to uncover the mechanisms by which these effects occur. Frisco, Muller,
and Dodson (2004) concluded that, although certain participatory activities among
adolescents do indeed foster moral and civic responsibility, ‘‘future research should
further explore the mechanisms behind relationships : : : such as an organization’s
ability to foster social capital, citizenship, and/or self-efficacy’’ (p. 673). For communication scholars, it becomes particularly relevant to unravel the mechanisms
by which the ‘‘organization’’—or socializing agent—of media might encourage or
diminish such civic activity, and how those effects occur.
McLeod and Reeves (1980), among other communication scholars, have called for
more research aimed at uncovering the mechanisms of media effects. That is, how or
by what means do media effects occur? Methods employed in assessing such means
are often moderation or mediation—that is, variables that interact with or come
between the effect of one variable on another. Several media-effects studies have
followed this line of research, such as Shah, Cho, Eveland, and Kwak (2005), who
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
8
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media/March 2009
concluded that informational media use fostered interpersonal political discussion,
which in turn contributed to increased civic participation.
Given that both media-use patterns and civic participation in youth tend to
influence later adult behaviors (McFarland & Thomas, 2006; Smith et al., 2002;
St. Peters, Fitch, Huston, Wright & Eakins, 1991), it is important to discover if
media use negatively influences civic participation among youth as it has been
argued to do in adults (Putnam, 2000). That is, does this relationship begin when
adolescents are learning about politics and government, or does it only settle in
during adulthood?
Moreover, as Moy et al. (2004) and others have argued, it is not simply the time
spent with a medium that accounts for media effects. Rather, the content attended to
provides greater insight into the relationship. Baumgartner and Morris (2006) classify
late-night comedies as ‘‘soft’’ news, while traditional newscasts can be considered
‘‘hard news.’’ The former is often classified as emotional and immediate, while the
latter consists of ‘‘what an informed person in society should know,’’ (Bennett, 2007,
p. 21). Although Graber (2001) argues that traditional television news is becoming
more ‘‘soft’’ in its focus, we follow the existing precedent and categorize national
and local TV news as traditional hard news, and late-night comedy programming
as soft news.
A recent report from the Pew Research Center for the People and the Press (2007)
shows evidence that the audiences of these programs vary somewhat demographically. Late-night comedy audiences are more likely to be male (54% of viewers),
and 31% are college graduates, while 26% are aged 18–29. More women and
fewer younger people tend to be the audiences for national and local TV news.
Specifically, while 26% of these viewers are college graduates, only 15% and 18%,
respectively, are between 18 and 29 years old; both types of hard news programming
have between 2 and 5% more female than male viewers.
The way people process information in these types of programs also differs. For
instance, Young (2004) argues that humor requires more active processing. Nabi,
Moyer-Guse, and Byrne (2007) corroborated this claim in their finding that humor
in messages motivated central processing. If such information is processed centrally,
then viewers may be able to recall and recognize political information from those
programs more readily. Late-night shows like The Daily Show have been found to air
the same amount of substance and actually contain a larger proportion of campaign
coverage than TV news (Fox, Koloen, & Sahin, 2007). It is possible that the amount
of time given to politics, combined with the inclusion of humor, on these programs
gives young viewers feelings of competence about their understanding of politics.
Traditional or ‘‘hard’’ news can be argued to have similar effects. Graber’s (2001)
extensive work on the role of audiovisual content in news programming suggests
that those stories that generate strong emotions are more likely to be embedded
in long-term memory and retained even when they are not regularly rehearsed.
Because hard news often follows guidelines for inclusion of dramatic and personal
imagery (Bennett, 2007), one could argue that hard news could also provide viewers
with increased efficacy about their capacity for political understanding.
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
Hoffman and Thomson/TV, ADOLESCENTS, AND PARTICIPATION
9
However, a further distinction between national and local TV news might also be
made. The format of these programs tends to differ in the magnitude of arousing content, with local news containing more sensational stories than national network news
(Grabe, Zhou, & Barnett, 2001). This format difference has been found to influence
information processing. For instance research has demonstrated that arousing news
content increases attention, interest, and recall (e.g., Lang, Newhagen, & Reeves,
1996). On the other hand, such content can decrease credibility and perceived
informativeness that audiences lend to the programming (Grabe, Lang, & Zhao,
2003).
In the political arena, the effects of viewing different types of content are not
exactly clear. Perhaps the most debated of these effects is whether ‘‘soft’’ news aids
political knowledge gain or not. Although several studies in the 1990s found that
soft news did not significantly predict knowledge (e.g., Hollander, 1995; McLeod
et al., 1996), recent research suggests that such programming can at least positively influence recognition of campaign information (Hollander, 2005) and add
to incidental learning among politically inactive individuals (e.g., Baum, 2003).
Interestingly, although causal claims cannot be made, the Pew Research Center (2007) results reveal that audiences of late-night comedy shows are consistently in the highest-knowledge group, along with newspaper web sites. Young
(2004) also found that late-night TV viewers are more likely to view other types
of news content, which might explain why they tend to be in the high-knowledge
group.
The present study aims to uncover the mechanisms by which certain activities—
particularly, the use of different media programming—promote civic participation
among adolescents. Two such mechanisms, as outlined in recent research (Baumgartner & Morris, 2006) are political efficacy and political cynicism.
Political Efficacy and Cynicism
Political efficacy has been defined broadly as ‘‘the belief that one has the skills
to influence the political system’’ (Zimmerman, 1989, p. 554). The two dimensions
of this concept are internal political efficacy—which focuses primarily on the role
that an individual plays in a democracy—and external political efficacy—which
is associated more with beliefs about one’s government and its competence to
be responsive and effective (Niemi, Craig, & Mattei, 1991; Zimmerman, 1989).
Relevant to the present research, those high on internal efficacy display a belief that
they are competent enough to participate in political activities (Niemi et al., 1991;
Zimmerman, 1989). The difference between these two dimensions is simply one of
self-perception (internal) versus perceptions of one’s environment (external).
Among youth, there is evidence that viewing certain television shows can either increase or decrease political efficacy. For example, Baumgartner and Morris
(2006) found that viewing the late-night comedy, The Daily Show with Jon Stewart,
had a negative effect on young people’s perceptions of political candidates and
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
10
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media/March 2009
decreased their feelings of external efficacy. Yet they called for further research on
the behavioral effects of such late-night TV shows, because viewing such shows has
also been shown to actually increase internal efficacy—the confidence in one’s own
competence to understand and participate in politics (adapted from Niemi et al.,
1991). Thus, although Baumgartner and Morris’s subjects may have felt less external
efficacy—operationalized as distrust in government institutions—from viewing The
Daily Show with Jon Stewart, they seem to have felt more competent in their ability
to do something about it.
Political cynicism reflects low external efficacy (Baumgartner & Morris, 2006;
Cappella & Jamieson, 1996), yet it differs in its specificity to feelings of trust in
politicians and government. External political efficacy, on the other hand, focuses
on an environmental evaluation of competence. Baumgartner and Morris operationalized political cynicism as a single survey item that measured ‘‘faith’’ in the
U.S. electoral system. However, cynicism is a multifaceted—and often disagreed
upon—concept that cannot be reduced to one component. In the literature, political
cynicism is often defined in terms of its opposition to efficacy (e.g., Craig, Niemi, &
Silver, 1990; Niemi et al., 1991), and trust (Mishler & Rose, 2001). Here, cynicism
is defined as the belief that politicians and the government do not work in the best
interests of citizens and cannot be trusted.
The present study’s hypotheses build upon the suggestions for future research in
Baumgartner and Morris (2006) by adding a measure of internal political efficacy
and a more robust measure of political cynicism. Although there has been some
discussion of the relationship between efficacy and viewing late-night programs,
researchers have not vigorously explored the effects of national and local TV news
use on efficacy or cynicism. The literature shows that national TV news use has
positive effects on efficacy (Delli Carpini, 2004; Kenski & Stroud, 2006), and local
TV news use has moderately positive effects on efficacy (McLeod, Scheufele, & Moy,
1999). Although the majority of this research employs adult samples, we hypothesize
that these programs should also have positive effects on adolescent efficacy (and,
as such, negative effects on cynicism).
Because television is the predominant channel by which young people first encounter political information (Chaffee & Kanihan, 1997), these hypotheses focus
solely on television programming. Mediating relationships between three types of
media use as the independent variable, efficacy and cynicism as the mediating
variables, and civic participation as the outcome variable are predicted.
H1 : Political efficacy will be a positive mediator between (a) late-night TV viewing, (b) national TV news viewing, and (c) local TV news viewing and civic
participation.
H2 : Political cynicism will be a negative mediator between (a) late-night TV
viewing, (b) national TV news viewing, and (c) local TV news viewing and
civic participation.
Hoffman and Thomson/TV, ADOLESCENTS, AND PARTICIPATION
11
Method
Procedure
A series of paper-and-pencil surveys were distributed to social studies departments
in each of 18 public high schools in an urban Midwestern school district. The
response rate from teachers was approximately 50%. Student data collection took
place in two waves: a survey distributed in their social studies class, and a follow-up
survey mailed to their homes.
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
Obtaining Parental Consent
The first step in obtaining child survey data is obtaining consent from their
parents. This is seen as an obstacle by many researchers because response
rates can be very low, as they are in the present study. Certain methods can
greatly increase both the response rates and the consent rates among parents,
including some incentive for parents (Thompson, 1984), so we employed a lotterytype incentive for participation.1 Although 6,238 consent forms were mailed to
parents, only 517 were returned. Of the total sample, the response rate was
201/6,238, or 3.2%. However, this small response rate is a function of the
parental permission required to survey the students. Among the parents who gave
permission (8.2% of the original sample), the response rate among students was
39%.
Sample
A total of 201 students completed the survey. Although this response rate
is disappointingly small, the sample does not appear to be drastically different
from the school district’s student population. Of these students, there was at
least one student represented from all but 3 of the 18 schools. Nearly 50%
of the student sample was female, with an average age of 16 years, and
students reported a B average in their grades.2 Of all students responding,
54.8% reported being Caucasian and 36% reported being African American
(with other categories selected by no more than 4.8% of respondents). Although
our sample nearly exactly represents the data provided by the school district
in terms of gender, the data on race revealed the opposite balance; 62.4% of
students in all grades were African American, while 30.6% were White during
the year of our data collection (Columbus Public Schools, 2005). However,
an independent samples t test revealed that there were no significant differences in either the independent or dependent variables between Blacks and
Whites.
12
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media/March 2009
Measurement
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
Independent Variables. Respondents were asked three separate questions to gauge
their TV viewing habits. Late-night comedy TV viewing was measured by asking
respondents to indicate on a 0–7 scale the number of days per week they watched
late-night comedy shows. Examples of ‘‘The Tonight Show or The Daily Show
with Jon Stewart’’ were provided so that respondents understood what was meant
by ‘‘late-night comedy shows.’’ Local and national TV news viewing were each
measured on the same 0–7 scale. Respondents reported the highest levels of local
news viewing (M D 4.42 days, SD D 2.02), followed by national news (M D 2.68
days, SD D 2.26) and late-night comedy (M D 1.93 days, SD D 2.35).
Mediating Variables. Political efficacy (specifically ‘‘internal’’ political efficacy)
and political cynicism were hypothesized to mediate the relationship between
television viewing and civic participation. In order to assess political efficacy, respondents were asked to rate the degree to which they had confidence in being able
to meaningfully participate in politics. This was measured by three items that were
rated on a scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree): ‘‘I consider myself
well qualified to participate in politics when I turn 18’’ (M D 4.13, SD D .85),
‘‘I think that I am better informed about politics than most people my age’’ (M D
3.07, SD D 1.07), and ‘‘I feel I have a pretty good understanding of the important
political issues facing our country’’ (M D 3.40, SD D .98). The scale was reliable,
Cronbach’s ˛ D .70 (M D 3.53, SD D .77).
Political cynicism was measured as respondents’ ratings on each of three items
on a scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree): ‘‘Elected officials almost
never keep campaign promises’’ (M D 3.42, SD D .91), ‘‘Politicians will say almost
anything to get elected’’ (M D 3.87, SD D .94), and ‘‘The government wastes a lot
of the taxpayer’s money’’ (M D 3.74, SD D .95). Items were combined into a scale
of political cynicism, Cronbach’s ˛ D .66 (M D 3.68, SD D .73).
Dependent Variable. In order to assess civic participation, respondents were presented with a list of civic activities in which adolescents might participate. This has
been defined elsewhere as ‘‘extracurricular participation’’ (Smith, 1999), however,
the measure employed here excludes activities like participating in athletics, arts, or
hobby groups. Both school and non-school activities were included, because both
have been shown to influence adult political participation (Frisco et al., 2004). The
measures here were based in part on those used in Frisco et al., but participation
in student government or debate was added to the list, because this activity tends
to predict civic participation better than other extracurricular activities (Glanville,
1999). Also included were religious volunteer activities, because they are often
associated with participation in other civic activities (Smith et al., 2002).
Respondents were asked to indicate which of the following activities they had
participated in since the beginning of the current school year (1 D yes, 0 D no):
‘‘participated in student government or debate club’’ (M D .10, SD D .30), ‘‘was
Hoffman and Thomson/TV, ADOLESCENTS, AND PARTICIPATION
13
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
involved in any youth organizations (sports leagues, scouts, Boys/Girls Club)’’ (M D
.69, SD D .46), ‘‘volunteered for your place of worship’’ (M D .40, SD D .49),
‘‘volunteered for school or youth programs’’ (M D .59, SD D .49), or ‘‘volunteered
for any neighborhood or civic group’’ (M D .23, SD D .42). The responses were
summed to create an additive measure of civic participation. Of the 5 activities,
students reported participating in an average of 2 (SD D 1.30).
Control Variables. Ideology was entered into the analyses as a control variable,
because identification with a certain ideology tends to be related to participation, at
least in adult samples (Abramson & Aldrich, 1982; Gershtenson, 2002). Respondents
reported a mean ideology of 3.77 (SD D 1.44) with 1 as very liberal, and 7
as very conservative. This measure was folded in half to represent ideological
extremity, with ‘‘somewhat conservative, somewhat liberal’’ and ‘‘moderate’’ in
one category, and ‘‘very liberal, very conservative, liberal,’’ and ‘‘conservative’’ in
another category.
Grade in school was also entered as a control variable, since it is possible that the
amount of television viewing, especially of late-night programming, might vary as a
function of grade level. The sample contained 36.9% freshman, 30% sophomores,
9.7% juniors, and 15.2% seniors.3
Analyses
Testing for Nonlinearity. Several studies have found that the relationship between
media use and civic participation is nonlinear, such that the variables are positively
associated at low levels of use, but negatively related as use increases (Hollander, 2005; Pasek, Kenski, Romer, & Jamieson, 2006). This is particularly true for
television use. For this reason, a test for nonlinearity was employed between the
three television use variables and civic participation using the nominalization test for
nonlinearity. The test was nonsignificant for both national TV news viewing and latenight TV viewing, so all further tests treat the relationships between these variables
and civic participation as linear. The relationship between local TV news use and
civic participation, however, was nonlinear and marginally significant (F-change
from linear to nonlinear model D 1.86, p < .10). This finding was further explored
by conducting hierarchical polynomial regression to test whether a quadratic model
fits better than a linear model, and found that the quadratic model did not fit better
than the linear model (F-change D .366, p D .546), so all media use variables and
their relationships with civic participation were treated as linear.
Mediation Analyses. An important direction for scientific research is assessing
how and by what means an independent variable influences another variable through
some mediating variable(s) (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). The hypotheses suggest that
certain types of media use will influence civic participation by means of individual
political efficacy and cynicism. One way of testing these relationships is through
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
14
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media/March 2009
mediation analyses. Barron and Kenny (1986) outlined a causal-steps approach
to detecting mediation. However, others have criticized this approach because
it requires running several regression analyses, can be inconsistent, and often is
not powerful enough to detect indirect effects (Preacher & Hayes). The Sobel test
examines mediation more directly by computing the product of the direct effect
from some independent variable to its mediator, and from that mediator to the
dependent variable, which is then compared to the normal-theory standard error.
But the distribution is often skewed, so this test is also not ideal. Because Preacher
and Hayes and others have suggested that the bootstrapping technique accounts
for some of these limitations, this was the method employed. Bootstrapping is
a nonparametric resampling procedure that accounts for the skewed distribution
that occurs in the Sobel test. It is also well-suited for testing hypotheses about
mediation given the nonnormal nature of the sampling distribution of an indirect
effect (Preacher & Hayes).
Results
Hypothesis 1a proposed that political efficacy would positively mediate the relationship between late-night TV viewing and civic participation. Using the bootstrapping macro for estimating indirect effects in SPSS,4 results showed that the point
estimate for the total indirect effect of late-night TV viewing on civic participation
with political efficacy as a mediator was .0247 (se D .0128).5 In order to test the
significance of this mediator, one examines the 95% confidence interval and in
this case, it does not contain zero (.0034, .0574), therefore political efficacy is a
significant mediator between late-night TV viewing and civic participation.
As presented in Table 1, this relationship persisted even when political ideology
and grade in school were added into the model as controls, such that the 95%
confidence interval remained significant for political efficacy. The results suggest
Table 1
Mediation of Three Types of TV Viewing on Civic Participation Through
Political Efficacy, Controlling for Political Ideology and Grade in School
Product of Coefficients
Late-Night TV a
National TV Newsb
Local TV Newsb
Point Estimate
SE
.0247
.0199
.0233
.0137
.0145
.0131
Bootstrapping
95% CI
.0038, .0601
.0053, .0520
.0035, .0571
Note. a sample size D 187; b sample size D 188. Confidence intervals are bias-corrected and
accelerated. 1,000 bootstrap samples.
Hoffman and Thomson/TV, ADOLESCENTS, AND PARTICIPATION
15
Table 2
Mediation of Three Types of TV Viewing on Civic Participation Through
Cynicism, Controlling for Political Ideology and Grade in School
Product of Coefficients
Late-Night TV a
National TV Newsb
Local TV Newsb
Point Estimate
SE
.0010
.0007
.0005
.0039
.0037
.0039
Bootstrapping
95% CI
.0040, .0134
.0041, .0154
.0048, .0136
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
Note. a sample size D 187; b sample size D 188. Confidence intervals are bias-corrected and
accelerated. 1,000 bootstrap samples.
that Hypothesis 1a was supported and political efficacy mediates the relationship
between late-night TV viewing and civic participation.
Hypotheses 1b and 1c predicted that efficacy would also mediate the relationship
between national and local TV news viewing and civic participation. The relationship was not significant for national TV news use, with a point estimate for the
indirect effect of .0214 (se D .0132, CI: .002, .0529). Hypothesis 1b was not
supported. The indirect effect was significant, however, for local news (.0239, se D
.0135, CI: .0031, .0553), with an adjusted R2 D .032 [F(3, 194) D 4.20, p D .05].
This relationship persisted when ideology and grade in school were entered as a
control (see Table 1). Hypothesis 1c was supported.
Hypotheses 2a, 2b, and 2c predicted that cynicism would negatively mediate
the relationship between these three types of TV viewing and civic participation.
Bivariate correlations were not significant for any of the types of television viewing
and political cynicism. Using the same methods described above for Hypotheses
1a, 1b, and 1c, results showed that there was no significant relationship between
any of the three types of TV viewing and civic participation with cynicism as a
mediating variable (see Table 2).
Discussion
In an effort to extend the literature on political socialization, the research presented here examined the influence of various forms of media use on civic participation, utilizing a sample of urban high school students. The study answered the
call of previous research that stressed the importance of assessing the behavioral,
as well as attitudinal, effects of TV viewing on adolescents.
This research posited that political efficacy would positively mediate the relationship between TV viewing and civic participation, while political cynicism would
negatively mediate this relationship. Internal political efficacy did indeed mediate
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
16
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media/March 2009
the relationship between late-night TV comedy viewing and civic participation, as
well as local TV news viewing and civic participation. However, cynicism was
neither significantly correlated with any of the types of television viewing, nor did it
mediate the relationship between such viewing and civic participation. This counters
the findings from Baumgartner and Morris (2006), who found that not only were
cynicism and late-night TV viewing (in particular, The Daily Show with Jon Stewart)
related, but viewing this show predicted political cynicism. They called for further
analyses to test this effect on participation, and the findings of the present study
indicate that not only is this type of TV viewing unassociated with cynicism, but
cynicism does not mediate the effect of such viewing on civic participation. Thus,
the results demonstrate that viewing both traditional forms of news (local news)
and nontraditional sources of political information on television (late-night comedy)
increases adolescents’ internal political efficacy, which in turn positively predicts
their civic participation.
Limitations
While these results are stimulating, several limitations should be acknowledged.
First, although 517 parents signed consent forms for their children to participate
in the study, only 201 students actually filled out the surveys. A larger sample
of students would provide a stronger test of the hypotheses presented here. As
presented here, the results provide motivation for researchers to further examine
the relationship between especially late-night TV viewing and civic participation
by examining mediating factors, but the authors acknowledge that the results are
limited to the geographic area in which the study was conducted. In addition, issues
of self-selection into the sample also might have biased results to some extent.
Second, measures of television viewing and of civic participation might be improved in future studies. For instance, respondents were asked to indicate how many
days of the last week they watched certain types of television programming; the
civic participation measure, however, asked about participation in activities since
the beginning of the school year. It is often recommended to ask about difficultto-remember behaviors in terms of some recent time period in order to aid recall
(e.g., Tourangeau, Rips, & Rasinski, 2000). However, the mismatch between the
time periods in this survey might have been problematic for respondents, and the
wording about viewing in the past week might also introduce some error into the
results.
Moreover, TV shows such as The Daily Show with Jon Stewart or The Tonight
Show do not air seven days a week as national and local TV news regularly do.
The collapsed one-item measure of late-night comedy programming might also be
masking differential effects between types of late-night content. Future research
should parse out these programs into different survey items.
Because this survey was administered in the early months of the school year,
it is also possible that students might not yet have participated in civic activities.
Hoffman and Thomson/TV, ADOLESCENTS, AND PARTICIPATION
17
Thus, the results presented here are conservative tests of the hypotheses. Future
research might consider administering such a survey later in the year to account for
such participation. It should also be noted that the coefficients were relatively weak,
albeit significant. Finally, because this research is not longitudinal, causal inferences
cannot be made. Future research should continue to assess these relationships,
hopefully with a larger, longitudinal sample.
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
Implications for Future Research
These results beg the question: Why did political efficacy mediate the relationship
between certain television programming and civic participation, but cynicism did
not? Baumgartner and Morris (2006) found that late-night comedy programs, like
The Daily Show, increased cynicism among youth in their sample, but they also
found an increase in internal efficacy. In the present study, internal efficacy—and
not cynicism—was significantly predicted by late-night television comedy as well
as local news viewing among adolescents. Yet this study took it a step further
by assessing the behavioral outcomes of such viewing. It was found that efficacy
was a significant mediator between late-night comedy and local news viewing and
civic participation. This is true even though cynicism was relatively higher among
respondents in this sample (M D 3.68, SD D .73) than efficacy (M D 3.53, SD D
.77). Although these patterns generally reflect what was found in the Baumgartner
and Morris study, they also suggest that the outlook is perhaps not as grim as
these authors suggested. Not only did late-night television comedy and local news
programming encourage feelings of political efficacy, they indirectly influenced
adolescents’ patterns of civic participation. An interpretation of this is that such
programming empowers adolescents, even if at the same time it gives them negative
feelings toward government and politics.
The finding regarding late-night comedy programs is perhaps the most intriguing
in these analyses. One interpretation for why such programming positively predicted
efficacy, which in turn predicted civic participation, is that the format of these shows
differs greatly from news. For those citizens new to political and civic processes—
such as high school students—programming that appeals to adolescents’ desire
for certain features, such as humor, might be better received. Research in health
communication has found that humor increases liking and perceived attractiveness
of alcohol advertisements among both children and adolescents (Chen, Grube,
Bersamin, Waiters, & Keefe, 2005; Waiters, Treno, & Grube, 2001). Although research in political communication has not systematically examined the differing
effects of humor versus hard news on attitudinal or behavioral outcomes of youth,6
these results suggest this might be a fruitful area for future research.
In addition to the inclusion of humor, late-night TV shows like The Daily Show
have been found to show an equivalent—if not greater—proportion of campaign
coverage than TV news (Fox et al., 2007). On average, the stories devoted to
the 2004 presidential campaign tended to be longer on The Daily Show with Jon
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
18
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media/March 2009
Stewart than on network news. Might it be possible that the depth given to certain
topics on such programs gives young viewers feelings of competence about their
understanding of politics? Future research should continue to assess the differences
in both content and format of such programming to determine how late-night
comedy programs and news generate different effects, particularly among youth.
Overall, these results suggest that efficacy does indeed mediate the relationship
between certain types of television viewing and civic participation among adolescents, providing an initial answer to the question, how or by what means do
media effects occur? At least for the youth in this sample, the effects of television
viewing on civic participation occur by means of political efficacy. This counters
previous research and lay concerns that some programming, such as late-night
comedy shows, adversely affect youth by discouraging participation in a democracy.
Understanding the patterns of such behaviors at a young age is crucial, given that
civically active youth become civically active adults. Since democracy is founded
on the idea of participation among the citizenry, it is important to continue to assess
what makes individuals participate more or less in this system. This study echoes
previous work which concluded that the media are an important socializing agent
for youth into a political world. How and what types of programming produce these
effects is a fruitful, and fundamental avenue for continued research.
Notes
1 The
lottery-type incentive was a one-in-one-hundred chance at winning $100.
were no data available from the school district on average course grades for high
school students.
3
The breakdown of year-in-school percentages is in part influenced by the year in which
social studies courses were required.
4 Available for download at http://www.quantpsy.org/.
5 Bootstrapping D 1000 re-samples.
6 Although, see Young & Tisinger (2006) for an example of political communication research on the effects of such humor in adult samples.
2 There
References
Abramson, P. R., & Aldrich, J. H. (1982). The decline of electoral participation in America.
American Political Science Review, 76, 502–521.
Arnett, J. J. (1995). Adolescents’ uses of media for self-socialization. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 24, 519–533.
Austin, E. W., & Pinkleton, B. E. (2001). The role of parental mediation in the political
socialization process. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 45(2), 221–240.
Barber, B. R. (1984). Strong democracy. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Barron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. 1986. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social
psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.
Baum, M. A. (2003). Soft news and political knowledge: Evidence of absence or absence of
evidence? Political Communication, 20, 173–190.
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
Hoffman and Thomson/TV, ADOLESCENTS, AND PARTICIPATION
19
Baumgartner, J., & Morris, J. S. (2006). The Daily Show effect: Candidate evaluations, efficacy,
and American youth. American Politics Research, 34(3), 341–367.
Becker, L. B., McCombs, M. E., & McLeod, J. M. (1975). The development of political cognitions. In S. H. Chaffee (Ed.), Political communication: Issues and strategies for research.
Beverly Hills: Sage.
Becker, L. B., Sobawale, I. A., & Casey, W. E., Jr. (1979). Newspaper and television dependencies: Effects on evaluations of public officials. Journal of Broadcasting, 23, 465–475.
Bennett, W. L. (2007). News: The politics of illusion, 7th edition. New York: Pearson Longman.
Bureau of Labor Statistics (2005, December). Volunteering in the United States, 2005. Retrieved
October 17, 2006, from http://www.bls.gov/news.release/volun.nr0.htm.
Cappella, J. N., & Jamieson, K. H. (1996). News frames, political cynicism, and media cynicism. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 546, 71–84.
Chaffee, S. H., Jackson-Beeck, M., Durall, J., & Wilson, D. (1977). Mass communication in
political socialization. In S. A. Renshon (Ed.) Handbook of political socialization: Theory
and research (pp. 223–258). New York: The Free Press.
Chaffee, S. H., & Kanihan, S. F. (1997). Learning about politics from the mass media. Political
Communication, 14, 421–430.
Chaffee, S. H., & Yang, S. (1990). Communication and political socialization. In O. Ichilov
(Ed.) Political socialization, citizenship education, and democracy. New York: Teachers
College Press.
Chen, M., Grube, J. W., Bersamin, M., Waiters, E., & Keefe, D. B. (2005). Alcohol advertising:
What makes it attractive to youth? Journal of Health Communication, 10, 553–565.
Columbus Public Schools (2005). Student and Building Data. Retrieved October 29, 2007, from
http://cpsweb.columbus.k12.oh.us/website.nsf/A5C8E6E824C08D9085257346004B54D1/
$File/2007-08%20CPS%20Fact%20Sheet.pdf?OpenElement
Corporation for National & Community Service (2006, March). Educating for active citizenship:
Service-learning, school-based service, and civic engagement. Retrieved October 17, 2006,
from http://www.nationalservice.gov/pdf/06_0323_SL_briefing_factsheet.pdf.
Craig, S. C., Niemi, R. G., & Silver, G. E. (1990). Political efficacy and trust: A report on the
NES pilot study items. Political Behavior, 12, 289–314.
Damico, A. J., Conway, M. M., & Damico, S. B. (2000). Patterns of political trust and mistrust:
Three moments in the lives of democratic citizens. Polity, 32(3), 377–400.
Delli Carpini, M. X. (2004). Mediating democratic engagement: The impact of communications
on citizens’ involvement in political and civic life. In L. L. Kaid (Ed.) Handbook of political
communication research (pp. 395–429). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Fox, J., Koloen, G., & Sahin, V. (2007). No joke: A comparison of substance in The Daily
Show with Jon Stewart and broadcast network television coverage of the 2004 presidential
election campaign. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 51, 213–227.
Frisco, M. L., Muller, C., & Dodson, K. (2004). Participation in voluntary youth-serving organizations and early adult voting behavior. Social Science Quarterly, 85, 660–676.
Gershtenson, J. (2002). Partisanship and participation in political campaign activities. Political
Research Quarterly, 55, 687–714.
Glanville, J. (1999). Political socialization or selection? Adolescent extracurricular participation
and political activity in early adulthood. Social Science Quarterly, 80, 279–90.
Grabe, M. E., Lang, A., & Zhao, X. (2003). News content and form: Implications for memory
and audience evaluations. Communication Research, 30, 387–413.
Grabe, M. E., Zhou, S., & Barnett, B. (2001). Explicating sensationalism in television news:
Content and the bells and whistles of form. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media,
45, 635–656.
Graber, D. A. (2001). Processing politics: Learning from television in the Internet age. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Hollander, B. A. (1995). The new news and the 1992 presidential campaign: Perceived versus
actual political knowledge. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 72, 786–798.
Hollander, B. A. (2005). Late-night learning: Do entertainment programs increase political
campaign knowledge for young viewers? Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 49,
402–415.
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
20
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media/March 2009
Kelly, K., & Donohew, L. (1999). Media and primary socialization theory. Substance Use &
Misuse, 34, 1033–1045.
Kenski, K., & Stroud, N. J. (2006). Connections between internet use and political efficacy,
knowledge, and participation. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 50, 173–192.
Lang, A., Newhagen, J., & Reeves, B. (1996). Negative video as structure: Emotion, attention,
capacity, and memory. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 40, 460–477.
Lee, T. (2005). Media effects on political disengagement revisited: A multiple-media approach.
Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 82, 416–433.
Lopez, M. H. (2004, February). Volunteering among young people. Retrieved October 17,
2006, from http://www.civicyouth.org/PopUps/FactSheets/FS_Volunteering2.pdf.
McDevitt, M. (2006). The partisan child: Developmental provocation as a model of political
socialization. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 18, 67–88.
McDevitt, M., & Chaffee, S. H. (2002). From top-down to trickle-up influence: Revisiting
assumptions about the family in political socialization. Political Communication, 19, 281–
301.
McFarland, D. A., & Thomas, R. J. (2006). Bowling young: How youth voluntary associations
influence adult political participation. American Sociological Review, 71, 401–425.
McLeod, J. M. (2000). Media and civic socialization of youth. Journal of Adolescent Health,
27S, 45–51.
McLeod, J. M., Guo, Z., Daily, K., Steele, C. A., Horowitz, E., & Chen, H. (1996). The impact
of traditional and nontraditional media forms in the 1992 presidential election. Journalism
and Mass Communication Quarterly, 73, 401–416.
McLeod, J. M., & Reeves, B. (1980). On the nature of mass media effects. In S. B. Withey
& R. P Abeles (Eds.) Television and social behavior (pp. 17–53). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
McLeod, J. M., Scheufele, D. A., & Moy, P. (1999). Community, communication, and participation: The role of mass media and interpersonal discussion in local political participation.
Political Communication, 16, 315–336.
Mishler, W., & Rose, R. (2001). What are the origins of political trust? Testing institutional and
cultural theories in post-Communist societies. Comparative Political Studies, 34, 30–62.
Moy, P. McCluskey, M. R., McCoy, K., & Spratt, M. A. (2004). Political correlates of local
news media use. Journal of Communication, 54, 532–546.
Nabi, R. L., Moyer-Guse, E., & Byrne, S. (2007). All joking aside: A serious investigation into
the persuasive effect of funny social issue messages. Communication Monographs, 74, 29–
54.
Niemi, R. G., Craig, S. C., & Mattei, F. (1991). Measuring internal political efficacy in the
1998 national election study. American Political Science Review, 85, 1407–1413.
Ostroff, D. H., & Sandell, K. L. (1989). Campaign coverage by local TV news in Columbus,
Ohio, 1978–1986. Journalism Quarterly, 66, 114–120.
Parker, W. C. (1996). Introduction: Schools as laboratories of democracy. In W. C. Parker
(Ed.), Educating the Democratic Mind (pp. 1–22). New York: State University of New York
Press.
Pasek, J., Kenski, K., Romer, D., & Jamieson, K. H. (2006). America’s youth and community
engagement: How use of mass media is related to civic activity and political awareness in
14- to 22-year-olds. Communication Research, 33, 115–135.
Pew Research Center for the People and the Press (2007, April). Public knowledge of current
affairs little changed by news and information revolutions. Retrieved October 30, 2007,
from http://people-press.org/reports/pdf/319.pdf
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Contemporary approach to assessing mediation in
communication research. In A. F. Hayes, M. D. Slater, and L. B. Snyder (Eds), The Sage
sourcebook of advanced data analysis methods for communication research (pp. 13–54).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American community. New
York: Touchstone.
Downloaded By: [Hoffman, Lindsay] At: 23:32 12 March 2009
Hoffman and Thomson/TV, ADOLESCENTS, AND PARTICIPATION
21
Rahn, W. M., & Transue, J. E. (1998). Social trust and value change: The decline of social
capital in American youth, 1976–1995. Political Psychology, 19(3), 545–565.
Rogers, D. (1985). Adolescents and youth, 5th edition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall,
Inc.
Shah, D. V., Cho, J., Eveland, W. P., Jr., & Kwak, N. (2005). Information and expression in a
digital age: Modeling Internet effects on civic participation. Communication Research, 32,
531–565.
Sherrod, L. R., Flanagan, C., & Youniss, J. (2002). Dimensions of citizenship and opportunities
for youth development: The what, why, when, where, and who of citizenship development.
Applied Developmental Science, 6, 264–272.
Sigel, R. S. (1965). Political socialization: Its role in the political process. Annals of the
American Academy of Political Science, 361.
Smith, E. S. (1999). The effects of investments in the social capital of youth on political and
civic behavior in young adulthood: A longitudinal analysis. Political Psychology, 20, 553–
580.
Smith, C., Denton, M. L., Faris, R., & Regnerus, M. (2002). Mapping American adolescent
religious participation. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 41, 597–612.
St. Peters, M., Fitch, M., Huston, A. C., Wright, J. C., & Eakins, D. J. (1991). Television and
families: What do young children watch with their parents? Child Development, 62, 1409–
1423.
Teixeira, R. A. (1987). Why Americans don’t vote: Turnout decline in the United States, 1960–
1984. New York: Greenwood Press.
Thompson, T. L. (1984). A comparison of methods of increasing parental consent rates in
social research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 48, 779–787.
Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of survey response. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Waiters, E. D., Treno, A. J., & Grube, J. W. (2001). Alcohol advertising and youth: A focusgroup analysis of what young people find appealing in alcohol advertising. Contemporary
Drug Problem, 28, 695–718.
Young, D. G. (2004). Late-night comedy in election 2000: Its influence on candidate trait
ratings and the moderating effects of political knowledge and partisanship. Journal of
Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 48, 1–22.
Young, D. G., & Tisinger, R. A. (2006). Dispelling late-night myths: News consumption among
late-night comedy viewers and the predictors of exposure to various late-night shows.
Harvard International Journal of Press-Politics, 11, 113–134.
Youniss, J., & Yates, M. (1999). Introduction: International perspectives on the roots of civic
identity. In J. Youniss & M. Yates (Eds.), Roots of Civic Identity (pp. 1–15). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Zimmerman, M. A. (1989). The relationship between political efficacy and citizen participation: Construct validation studies. Journal of Personality Assessment, 53, 554–566.
Download