National Competitions Help Eradicate Gender Inequities in the Gifted and Talented

advertisement

ECHA 2008 1

National Competitions Help Eradicate Gender

Inequities in the Gifted and Talented

James Reed Campbell

St. John’s University

Jamaica, NY

Sharon Ann O’Connor-Petruso

Brooklyn College

Brooklyn, NY

Paper Presented at the Annual Meeting of the European Council for High Ability, Prague,

Czech Republic (September, 2008)

ECHA 2008 2

Abstract

Throughout much of the twentieth century Terman’s longitudinal study collected data from more that 1,000 gifted individuals. In the 1950s Terman rated the “life success” of the 150 most successful men (A Group) and compared them to the 150 least successful men (Group C) at the midpoint of their careers (at age 30). The objective of this

Olympiad study is to replicate the original Terman work with a modern sample of the most successful (A Group) and least successful (Group C) Academic Olympians from the

United States ( N =190). The results of the study support many of Terman’s findings. The most successful adult Olympians were found to have parents that supplied a Conducive

Home Atmosphere when they were growing up. This competition is very successful in nurturing STEM careers. Fifty percent of the Olympians earned doctorates. Finally, we found that the gender gap between male and female Olympians has diminished over the

14 years we have studied them.

Introduction

Throughout much of the twentieth century Terman’s longitudinal study collected data from more that 1,000 gifted individuals proceeded (Terman, 1926, 1954a, 1954b; Vialle,

1994). In the 1950s Terman (Terman, 1954b) rated the “life success” of 730 men at the midpoint of their careers. He labeled the 150 most successful the “A” group and the 150 least successful the “C” group. After isolating these groups, comparisons were made about the attitudes, values, and developmental experiences that affected their success in life. The criterion of success was the extent to which a subject had made use of his superior intellectual ability. Terman doubted Lange-Eichbaum’s theory (1932) that claimed great achievement usually stems from emotional tensions that border on abnormal. Instead, Terman found that success is associated with stability and absence of disturbing conflicts. Other key findings of the Terman study are as follows: average grades between the A and C groups were equal until high school; 97% of the As entered college and 90% graduated; 68% of the Cs entered college and only 37% graduated; half of the A group fathers were college graduates, but only 15% of the C group fathers had degrees; the estimated number of books in the A group homes was nearly 50% greater than in the C group homes.

Perspectives

The commonly held belief is that genius is born, not made. Michael Howe is one of the researchers who argues that the exceptional talents of those we call geniuses are the result of a unique set of circumstances and opportunities. Furthermore, in every case these talents are pursued and exploited with a characteristic drive, determination, and focus that the rest of us rarely show. All the geniuses in his study (Charles Darwin, George Eliot,

George Stevenson, the Bronte sisters, Michael Faraday and Albert Einstein) share human

ECHA 2008 3 qualities that set them apart from other people. These qualities are ones of temperament and personality rather than being narrowly intellectual ones (Howe, 1999). The finding that assessments of a child’s capacity to resist distractions and avoid impulsive actions are better predictors of later success than measures of early intelligence is consistent with this view (Goleman 1995). Terman’s finding that individuals who were identified as being unusually intelligent in childhood often went on to have highly successful adult careers. The result seems to confirm the predictive value of intelligence testing. However,

Howe argues that it was later demonstrated that had the children been selected on the basis of their family backgrounds and school records, without paying any attention to their intelligence test scores, equally accurate predictions could have been made about their attainments in later life (Howe 1999, 199-200).

The A and C groups in Terman’s study shared equal intellectual capacities.

However, the A group accomplished more than the C group in their adulthood. The group that accomplished more shares some personality qualities that are typical of geniuses.

These qualities include doggedness, persistence, the capacity for fierce and sustained concentration, as well as intense curiosity. A number of geniuses, including Darwin and

Einstein, disclaimed having superior inherent intelligence, but no genius has ever denied either possessing or relying upon a capacity for diligence or a healthy curiosity. In

Howe’s study all the geniuses had reached high standards by frequently and regularly practicing their talents over a period of years. Practice and preparation are shown to be vital in all fields of achievement. For example, around ten years of sustained training are needed for a chess player to reach international levels, and it takes comparable periods of time to reach the highest standards in mathematics and the sciences. Even in music, although it is widely believed that certain gifted individuals can excel without doing the lengthy practicing that ordinary people have to engage in, the evidence contradicts that view (Howe 1999, 5).

Heller’s Munich Models (Heller & Perleth, 2004; Ziegler & Heller, 2000) for talent development provide a theoretical underpinning for this study. These models fit the Olympians need to acquire in-depth knowledge of their subject early in their school careers because the examinations that are used to identify them are steeped in subject matter that is almost never presented in any high school. Howe’s finding that sustained practice applied over considerable periods of time fit nicely into this innovative model.

Objectives

Our study was designed to answer the following research questions:

1.

How do the 2006-2007 academic Olympians compare with the previous cohorts?

(Does the new cohort match the cohorts from the previous two rounds? Is there consistency?)

2.

Do the US academic Olympians fulfill their potential? Do these academic competitions supply US with science, technology, engineering and math

(

STEM) personnel?

3.

How do the 2006-2007 US adult Olympians compare to the 1954 Terman sample of gifted adults?

ECHA 2008 4

4.

What factors differentiate between the most productive and underproductive adult academic Olympians?

Data Sources

In each country separate programs have evolved to identify the Mathematics, Physics and

Chemistry Olympians. In some countries these programs are run by governments, while in the US they are conducted by independent professional associations. The Academic

Olympians are identified by administering a series of increasingly difficult and demanding tests. Thousands of talented high school students in each country take these subject matter tests. In the China Math Olympiad program, over one million students participate. The end result of this process culminates with the identification of 6-20 national finalists (Olympians). These individuals are then trained to compete in regional and or International Olympiad competitions. The process is the same as the one used to identify the national sports Olympic teams.

Our Olympiad teams started collecting retrospective data from academic

Olympians in 1994. The first international study was limited to the Math Olympians from the US, Taiwan and Mainland China (Campbell, 1996a, 1996b; Wu, 1996). In 1998 we collected another round of data that included the Math, Chemistry and Physics

Olympians in the US, Germany and Finland. The latest round of data collection was done from 2006-2007. Our teams have data from 814 Olympians and from many of their parents. We have data from 374 American, 369 European, and 71 Asian Olympians.

There is also data in the pipeline from another 100 international Olympians including a cohort of Korean Olympians. By the end of 2008 we will have international data from more than 900 academic Olympians.

The data utilized for this paper is limited to the 190 US Math, Physics, and

Chemistry Olympians who responded in 2006-2007. The subjects in this study represent several age cohorts (1970s, 1980s, 1990s, and 2000s).

Methods

The international Olympiad studies have been underway for 14 years in six countries.

Initially, the Olympians were mailed a multipaged Questionnaire and the Self-confidence

Attitude Attribute Scales (SaaS) (Campbell, 1996b). Their parents were mailed a shorter version of the same Questionnaire and the Inventory of Parental Influence (IPI)

(Campbell, 1996b). The duplication of questions from both sources were employed to assure validity in the responses and also to compare the varying perceptions of parents and their children. Dillman's (1978, 1991) methodology was employed for the design of the questionnaires and the mailings. The latest version of these studies now uses web instruments with the same methodology. Olympians are requested to visit a specially designed website (olympiadprojects.com) where instruments are provided. One on-line instrument contains items used to construct the many scales used in most analyses (full survey); while a shorter version was designed for Olympians that participated in previous rounds of data collection (follow-up). Both quantitative and qualitative data were derived in all of our studies.

ECHA 2008 5

Most of the data from these studies is derived from Principal Component

Analyses (PCA). Our approach is to first do PCAs at the national level and later to do

PCAs with the combined data from all of the countries (Campbell, Tirri, Ruohotie, &

Walberg, 2004).

For the current American Olympian data, the PCAs produced the following latent component/factors: Conducive Home Atmosphere during the school years (SA)(Alpha r=.87); Olympian’s reports of School Shortcomings (C_Short) (Alpha r=.90); Olympian’s reports of Negative Affect at school (C_AFF) (Alpha r=.73); Lack of Motivation

(C_FAIL) (Alpha r=.66); Individual’s Effort needed to Perform Well (CC_PE) (Alpha r=.67); Global degree of Effort needed for Success (CC_ES) (Alpha r=.70). Our analyses also include: Gender (SEX); family structure (one- and two-parent families) (LW), and immigration status. We also developed two composite variables, one that isolates

Computer Literacy (COMP) and the other incorporates all of the achievement and standardized test results into an Ability composite (Ab_final). Finally, we collected data about the Olympian’s college/university careers, their degrees, their occupations and the number of publications (Pubs) they produce. We used the 17 categories currently being used by the National Study of Postsecondary Faculty (NSOPF) to quantify data about the publications produced by the Olympians.

Results

Comparing the 1998 and 2006-2007 American Olympians

1. How do the 2006-2007 academic Olympians compare with the previous cohorts?

(Does the new cohort match the cohorts from the previous two rounds? Is there consistency?)

There is only one major change in the data since 1995 and that concerns the number of females that win Olympiad contests. The international gender gap ratios (number of males : number of females) tell a dramatic increase in female particicpation over the three rounds of data collection. In 1995 the gender gap ratio was 102:1 signifying more than

100 males for every female winner; by 1998 the ratio was down to 15.6:1 indicating 15 males for every female winner; and by 2006 it was down to 7.2:1. These ratios clearly show that more females are getting involved in these technical STEM areas. The United

States Phyics and Chemistry Olympiads are responsible for the most female winners.

The 1998 and 2006-2007 data are summarized by a series of comparative tables.

The first set of tables illustrates the colleges and universities that the Olympians attend.

Both sets contain the same set of elite institutions with the top five continuing to be

Harvard, MIT, Princeton, Stanford, and U.C. Berkley. The next set of tables describes the doctorate degrees earned by the Olympians. Combining the 1998 and 2006 degree data, we find that the overall percentages of Olympians holding doctoral degrees are 57% for math, 39% for physics, and 43% for chemistry. The reason for the disparity for the different subjects is that the Physics and Chemistry Olympians are younger.

The next set of tables concerns the occupations of the Olympians. Both tables reveal that the Olympians gravitate toward jobs in academia, the scientific community, and the computer industry. There are also a small number of Olympians in positions

ECHA 2008 6 outside the technical areas. The 2006-2007 table indicates that one of the Olympians is studying to be a priest, another is an art director, and five are lawyers.

The last set of comparative tables concerns the Olympians’ publications. In our analysis of the 1998 data, we speculated that as the Olympians age they have the potential to publish to a much greater degree. In 1998 the total publications produced by 229

Olympians amounted to 2,921 publications. The 2006-2007 total is 40% greater with 203

Olympians publishing 4,877 items.

Researchers using US national data about the publication rate of college faculty

(NSOPF) frequently limit their reports to only refereed publications, but we included all publications because Terman listed all the publications published by his sample. Forty percent of the Olympians’ publications are refereed, and they include 57 patents and the production of 101 software products. For the publication data we subdivided the sample into three age cohorts:

1. Young Olympians (ages 15-22)

2.

3.

Early Career Olympians (ages 23-29)

Mature Olympians (ages 30+)

The 1998 Olympians were 10 years younger than the current sample, and the mature cohort (ages 30+) Mathematicians produced 1,622 (32/person) publications, the

Physicists produced 14 (4/person) publications, and the Chemists produced 280

(23.3/person) publications. The mature 2006-2007 Olympians did much more publishing. For Math they published 2,775 publications (68/person); the Physics

Olympians turned out 206 publications (13.7/person); and the Chemistry Olympians accumulated 1,112 publications (38.34/person).

The shear size of the increase in publications surprised us because the NSOPF data show (Cataidi Fahimi & Zimbler, 2005) that the publication rate for US college faculty is 1.05 refereed publication/year. Assuming the Olympians college career started at age 30, their rate of publications is impressive. The Math competition started in 1972, and the oldest Math Olympian is currently 53 years old. The Chemistry Olympiad program began in 1984, and the Physics program began in 1986. Consequently, the oldest Chemistry Olympian is 42, and the oldest Physics Olympian is 39. Furthermore, the senior Olympians may still be publishing for another 10-30 years and in doing so will continue to assemble substantial numbers of publications.

Does the new cohort match the cohorts from the previous two rounds (except for gender)? Is there consistency? Comparing the 1998 and 2006-2007 tables, it is obvious that the data from this round is consistent with that from the two earlier rounds. This conclusion holds up despite the addition of many Olympians who have never participated before as well as receiving confirmation from other Olympians who supply the same information that they gave us in 1995 or 1998. These individuals provide a measure of reliability to the data.

Actualizing Potential

ECHA 2008 7

2. Do the US academic Olympians fulfill their potential? Do these academic competitions supply US with science, technology, engineering and math

(

STEM) personnel?

Any analysis of the tables presented above affirmatively answer both of these questions.

Certainly the small number of academic Olympians take up technical STEM positions and contribute a substantial number of publications. Over 50% of the national winners of these competitions earn doctorate degrees (see Tables 2.1 & 2.2), and a careful examination of their occupations (Tables 3.1 & 3.2) demonstrates the different ways they contribute to America’s STEM fields.

Comparing Terman & Olympian Samples

3. How do the 2006-2007 US adult Olympians compare to the 1954 Terman sample of gifted adults?

Before analyzing the differences between the Terman and the Olympiad samples some mention of the differences between the instruments and methods employed in these two studies needs to be emphasized. Studies conducted in the 1920s-1950s utilized many univariate variables and some of the earliest developed psychometric instruments. In

Terman’s case his team collected extensive data over decades. A large body of qualitative data was collected in addition to medical and even dental exams. Terman’s sample spent considerable periods of their time supplying information, whereas the

Olympiad samples supplied only one hour of their time for any of the rounds of data collection. However, the Olympiad studies were able to use sophisticated methods to produce refined factors/components that are much more reliable and valid than the instrumentation that was possible 75-100 years ago.

Comparing the two samples, Terman’s gifted students completed some version of the Stanford-Binet IQ test. This test purports to measure the “g-factor” that includes four cognitive abilities: verbal reasoning, quantitative reasoning, abstract/visual reasoning, and short-term memory (Freed, Hess & Ryan, 1989). There is no dispute that Terman’s sample was intellectually gifted. We measured the ability of the Olympians by constructing a composit that uses standardized test scores (SAT, PSAT and GRE exams), grade point averages, and the Olympian’s high school ranking at graduation. Many of the

Olympians also possess high levels of cognitive ability.

The major difference that sets the Olympians apart is the level of subject matter acquired. In order to win an academic Olympiad competition, the student must be able to read the technical literature while still in high school. The required expertise goes far beyond the normal subject matter covered in high school courses (even advanced placement courses). Most high school science and math courses attempt to make the students good consumers of knowledge. The same analysis applies to introductory courses taught at the college level. Even the upper level college courses do not expect the students to be up to date on the advanced literature in the technical journals.

Establishing Productive vs. Underproductive Groups (A vs. C)

ECHA 2008 8

4. What factors differentiate between the most productive and underproductive adult academic Olympians?

In the 1950s Terman compared his subjects at age 30. His team wanted to find out how the lives of these gifted individuals were turning out. He wanted to ascertain the nonintellectual factors that were responsible for their productivity. He isolated the 150 most productive men (A group) and the 150 least productive (C group). He left no explanation of how he subdivided these two groups.

We searched for ways to subdivide the most and least productive Olympians and do the same type of analysis as Terman did in the 1950s. But how do you separate the most successful Olympians (A Group) from those that do not do well (C Group)?

We realize that there are many ways that can be used to separate the high performers form the underperformers. One member of our research team suggested separating them in terms of their annual income. In terms of making money, the one

Chemistry Olympian that joined Goldman Sacks as an associate has made more money that the many professor Olympians. There are two Talmud scholars and one priest in our samples; certainly these Olympians must be considered successful. There is also one art director, one independent film maker, and a number of career musicians. Aren’t they successful?

In exploring ways to subdivide the sample into A and C groups, we used some of the Terman findings to work backwards. For example, Terman found that his A Group have more college graduates than his C Group. In terms of advanced degrees, the

Terman sample included 78 PhDs (9.75%); 48 MDs (6%); and 85 Lawyers (11%). But the number of Olympians with undergraduate and advanced degrees is so large that this factor could not be used to subdivide the productive from the underproductive.

We used the same reasoning with several of the Terman findings until we came to the publications. Overall, the Terman sample published 67 books (.084/person), 1,400 scientific/technical articles (1.75/person), and 150 patents (.18/person). The number of publications can be used to separate the two extremes among the Olympians. We separated the A and the C groups by the total number of publications produced, but we struggled to find a cut-off number of publications that would isolate each group. Some of the Olympians have produced more than 300 publications, while some of the younger ones have already turned out more than 100 publications.

Terman’s gifted sample was virtually the same age, but the ages of our Olympians range from 16 to 53. How could we find a way to find the Olympians who turned out the most work at any age?

Finally, we realized that the rate of publications produced each year would allow us to compare the young and mature Olympians on an equal footing. The cut-off rate we used for including Olympians in the A group was that they have to publish at least one publication per year for their entire life span. Very few Olympians published anything before the age of 20, but as they precede into college, some start to publish. The top publication rate was 8 publications/year for a 36-year-old Chemistry Olympian (288 publications). To further illustrate, one A group Physics Olympian is only 21 years old and yet has turned out 32 publications (rate 1.57/year). A 29-year-old lawyer (Physics

Olympian) has a rate of 1.28/year (37 publications), and a 39-year-old software engineer

(Math Olympian) has a rate of 2.44/year (95 publications).

ECHA 2008 9

In assembling the C group we used three criteria: gender, age and occupation.

We started with the 24 highest producers in the A group and matched each member by age, gender and occupation. For example, we matched a 29-year-old high producing lawyer with a 30-year-old low producing lawyer, and the two A group females with less producing females with the same ages and occupations. Subsequently, we matched the seven scientists, the two college students, the one engineer, the three software engineers, the one MD, and the 10 professors/post doctorates. Each group has 24 Olympians including two females and 22 males. The C group is almost like a control group with the one exception being the number of publications produced.

Despite the very small size of the groups we calculated the mean differences ( t tests) for many variables. For both groups, 23 were reared within two-parent families ( t =

-1.00, p-.328). There were only two one-parent families among the 48 Olympians. One of these families had a deceased parent (Group C), and the parents in the other family were divorced (Group A). We also had immigrant information about the Olympians, but there were no significant differences in terms of immigration status ( t = -.731, p= .468).

Both groups were made up predominantly of third-generation Americans where the

Olympian and both parents were born in the US. However, there were two immigrants in the A group but only one in the C group. There were also no significant differences in terms of age between the A and C groups ( t =.816, p= .419).

The A group produced a mean of 92.88 publications, while the C group mean was only 6.63 publications ( t =4.8; p =.000). These tests revealed only two additional significant differences. One difference concerns a higher SES for the A group ( t =2.6; p =.014), and the other for the Olympians reporting higher levels of a Conducive Home

Atmosphere ( t =3.7; p =.004) during their school years.

The Cs have more motivation problems (t= -1.1940, p=.059), and this finding confirms one of Terman’s findings (1954a p17). Their lack of motivation was almost significant despite the small size of the sample. The As reported more school problems during their elementary and high school years; the C group had a slightly higher level of ability and reports a greater negative affect problems where some teachers and peers were insensitive or showed little respect to these gifted children.

Multivariate Analysis

We performed a Discriminant Function Analysis with these variables. Even with these small samples (see Table 6) the Wilks lambda is significant (Lambda=.567, x

2

=22.147, p<.014), which indicates that the model including these 10 variable/factors was able to significantly discriminate the two groups. The standardized function coefficients show the weights used to separate the groups. The classification results show that this analysis predicted the membership of 91.3% of the A group and 82.6% of the C group.

The resulting discriminant function positions all of the variables together. Those in the A group had a much more Conducive Home Atmosphere when they were growing up. Their homes had an abundance of books, and magazines that spurred their interests and the mother and father both recognized the Olympian’s talent and encouraged him/her to develop it. The SES mean score (X=88) indicates that the parents’ occupations were professional and technical (see Miller 1991). This function also shows that the A group

ECHA 2008 10

Olympians were not hampered by a lack of motivation. Their high level of motivation continues into their adulthood.

The C group is characterized by lower SES families (X=78) whose occupations can be classified as managers, officials and proprietors (Miller, 1991). These families were not able to develop such a positive Conducive Home Atmosphere. There is less stimulation, less recognition of their talent and encouragement to develop it. This group had more negative affective school experiences. Negative affect comes about because of insensitive teachers and peers and is especially lethal in the elementary school years where highly gifted children feel isolated and alone. Secondary school for such students is prison-like. However, our qualitative studies with the Olympians show that magnet schools and specialized schools for the gifted alleviate this dilemma (Bittman, 2007).

Discussion

The goal of the discussion segment of a paper is to explain the results, and we found the Munich developmental models (Heller & Perleth, 2004; Perleth, 2001; Perleth

& Heller, 1994; Ziegler & Heller, 2000; Ziegler & Perleth, 1997) to be especially useful in explaining the results of this study. Figure 1 presents the Munich Process Model of

Giftedness. We view the triangle on its side as an example of how a student begins to prepare for the Olympiad tests by starting to master material. This student enters the triangle at the narrow top (left side of the tip of the triangle). He/she must then become enmeshed in the subject as time proceeds. As the student becomes more knowledgeable, he/she moves into the middle of the triangle by gaining a breath of information. As time goes on the student becomes an “expert” of sorts and now occupies the base of the triangle.

Insert Figure 1 about here

The makers of the challenging academic Olympiad tests expect these students to be able to possess the levels of expertise that only the producers of knowledge in that subject possess. These tests are designed to measure graduate level expertise in the different academic subjects. They expect the students to be knowledgeable in current research trends. What is really required is that these high ability students must leap frog not only their high school peers but also many college students.

What does it take to acquire such skills at the high school level? It certainly requires a high level of motivation and dedication to learn this technical material when it is really not required at the school level. Such individuals are more similar to the gifted individuals described by Howe (1999) than those included in the Terman sample. The screening process requires the Olympians to exhibit drive, determination, and the ability to concentrate on their subject at very young ages.

Insert Figure 2 about here

Figure 2 summarizes a more advanced Munich model that depicts the small triangle displayed under the school age label. Notice the larger shaded professional triangles below. The successful high school Olympiad participant must now move to one

ECHA 2008 11 of the professional triangles to further his/her development. The Olympian is now entering into the technical world of the scientist, engineer and mathematician. Some US

Olympians begin this process in the 9 th

grade, while others start the process a little later.

After more concentrated time acquiring additional technical knowledge, the student expands his/her expertise and begins to move along the triangle (deliberate practice).

Once the student has a solid base of expert information, he/she is ready to tackle the

Olympiad exam process.

After winning the Olympiad contest, the beginning scientist is or mathematician’s growth continues at the college/university level where the triangle assumes more professional dimensions. This process continues as the STEM career proceeds.

The one key finding from this study is that the learning environment established by parents during the school years was found to have long-lasting consequences years later in the child’s career. Once parents in the Olympians’ life make a major effort to recognize their innate talents and organize to develop it, this fact is just never forgotten.

Implications

This study carries on the work started a century ago by Louis Terman at the turn of the

20 th

Century. Our focus is on the development of talent. What factors help or hinder its development? By selecting the academic Olympians, we automatically isolate individuals with high levels of talent. Our focus on the Olympian’s development well into adulthood has implications both for school and for parents.

Implications for Parents

The one clear implication from this study is that parents need to supply a Conducive

Home Atmosphere when their children are growing up. In another study (Nokekainen,

Tirri, Campbell, & Walberg, 2007) of the International 1998 Olympians, we subdivided the the sample into three A and C cohorts (Young Olympians (ages 15-22); Early Career

Olympians (ages 23-29); Mature Olympians (ages 30+). We found that a Conducive

Home Atmosphere was found to be a significant predictor of productivity for all of the cohorts. Such an atmosphere promotes academic achievement (Campbell & Verna,

2007) and has long-term effects. The fact that this factor emerges again and again in our

International and American studies is striking. Consequently, parents that are able to encourage the Olympians to develop functioning attitudes and work habits during the developing years might be surprised to learn that these instilled qualities are still in operation 20 or 30 years later. The implications for every parent are sobering.

We believe that this finding has implications for the gifted and talented students at many levels. When a parent recognizes their child’s natural talents and organizes activities to actively develop these talents, the effects can be long-lasting. Some of these activities might involve parents dealing with schools that do not recognize their child’s potential. Other’s might be supportive of the sacrifices that a student must make to develop a high level of expertise. Still others might involve seeking out special training to develop the child’s unique talents. Still, when parents go the extra mile, our studies

ECHA 2008 12 show that this extra commitment is not lost on the child. They continue to recognize it decades later.

Implications for Schools

Similarly, the long-term effects of insensitive teachers and peers (negative affect) when the highly gifted child is growing up is also important to consider. Our qualitative studies of the Olympians show the pain they experience when they feel the sting of being labeled the class “freak.” Our qualitative work also shows that teachers need to become more sensitive to what is happening to such children. When teachers ignore the bulling and taunting they do not realize the potential long-term negative consequences of these behaviors on the exceptional child. Teachers also need to appreciate the fact that some of these very young Olympians know more in certain areas than their teachers. Rather than be threatened by such a child, teachers need to help the child consolidate his/her advanced knowledge and to spur him/her on to even greater growth.

We must stress that this study illustrates the potential that high ability students have in being able to leap frog ahead of their peers at any grade level in their area of expertise. Many of the Olympians bitterly complain about the lock-step one-size fits-all high school curriculum. They understand the futility of forcing every student to remain with their age mates during the school years when so much more can be accomplished.

In effect, we are holding them back while they are capable of zooming ahead at warpspeed. How much talent is lost by applying these academic straight jackets?

References

Bittman, A. (2007). Exploring factors that promote or hinder the career productivity of academically gifted female international Olympians in the disciplines of

Mathematics, Physics, and Chemistry . Unpublished doctoral dissertation, St.

John's University, Jamaica, NY.

Campbell, J. R. (1996a). Cross-national retrospective studies of Mathematics Olympians.

International Journal of Educational Research, 25 (6), 477-582.

Campbell, J. R. (1996b). Developing cross-cultural/cross-national instruments: using cross-national methods and procedures. International Journal of Educational

Research, 25 (6), 485-496.

Campbell, J. R., Tirri, K., Ruohotie, P., & Walberg, H. (2004). Cross-cultural research:

Basic issues, dilemmas, and strategies . Finland: Hame Polytechnic.

Campbell, J. R., & Verna, M. (2007). Effective parental influence: Academic home climate linked to children’s achievement. Educational Research and Evaluation ,

13 (6) 501-519.

Cataidi, E., Fahimi, M. & Zimbler, L. (2005). 2004 national study of postsecondary faculty (NSOPF:04). NCES, U.S. Deptartment of Education, Institute of

Educational Sciences 2006-176.

ECHA 2008 13

Conley, V., & Zimbler, L. (1997). Characteristics and attitudes of institutional faculty and staff in the humanities . Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office.

Dillman, D. A. (1978). Mail and telephone surveys: The total design . NY: Wiley.

Dillman, D. A. (1991). The design and administration of mail surveys. Annual Review of

Sociology, 17 , 225-249.

Freed, M., Hess, R., & Ryan, J. (1998). The educator's desk refenence (EDR): A sourcebook of educational information and research . NY: MacMillan Publishing

Company.

Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence . NY: Bantam Books.

Heller, K., & Perleth, C. (2004). Adapting conceptual models fpr cross-cultural applications. In J. R. Campbell, K. Tirri, P. Ruohotie & H. Walberg (Eds.), Crosscultural research: Basic issues, dilemmas, and strategies (pp. 81-101).

Hameenlinna, Finland: Hame Polytechnic.

Howe, M. (1999). Genius explained . NY: Cambridge University Press.

Lange-Eichbaum, W. (1932). The problem of genius . NY: MacMillan.

Miller, D. C. (1991). Handbook of research design and social measurement. (5th ed.) .

CA: Sage Publications.

Nokelainen, P., Tirri, K., Campbell, J. R., & Walberg, H. (2007). Effective parental influence: Academic home climate linked to children’s achievement. Educational

Research and Evaluation, 13 (6), 483-500.

Perleth, Ch. (2001). Follow-up-Untersuchungen zur Münchner Hochbegabungsstudie

[Follow-up studies of the Munich Giftedness Study]. In K. A. Heller (Ed.),

Hochbegabung im Kindes- und Jugendalter (2nd ed., pp. 357-446). Göttingen:

Hogrefe.

Perleth, Ch., & Heller, K. A. (1994). The Munich Longitudinal Study of Giftedness. In R.

Subotnik & K. Arnold (Eds.), Beyond Terman: Longitudinal studies in contemporary gifted education (pp. 77-114). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Terman, L. M. (1926). Mental and physical traits of a thousand gifted children . Stanford,

CA: Stanford University Press.

Terman, L. M. (1954a). Discovery and encouragement of exceptional talent. American

Psychologist, 9 (6), 221-230.

Terman, L. M. (1954b). Scientists and non-scientists in a group of 800 gifted men.

Psychological Monographs, 68 (7), 1-44.

Vialle, W. (1994). "Termanal" science? The work of Lewis Terman revisited. Roeper

Review, 17 (1), 32-38.

Wu, W. (1996). Growing up in Taiwan: The impact of environmental influences on the

Math Olympians. Journal of Educational Research, 25 (6), 523-534.

Ziegler, A., & Heller, K. (2000). Conceptions of giftedness from a meta-theoretical perspective. In K. Heller, F. Monks, R. Sternberg & R. Subotnik (Eds.),

International handbook of giftedness and talent (2nd Ed.) (pp. 3-21). Oxford, UK:

Elsevier.

ECHA 2008 14

Ziegler, A. & Perleth, Ch. (1997). Schafft es Sisyphos den Stein den Berg hinauszurollen? Eine kritische Bestandsaufnahme der Diagnose- und

Fördermöglichkeiten in der beruflichen Bildung vor dem Hintergrund des

Münchner Begabungs-Prozeß-Modells [Will Sisiphos succed in rolling the stone up the hill? A critical review of the possibilities of diagnosis and fostering in vocational training with respect to the Munich Giftedness-Process-Model].

Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht , 44 , 152-163

ECHA 2008

Colleges/

Table 1.1

Colleges and Universities Attended by Olympians

1998

Number enrolled Number enrolled Total

15

Stanford 24

22

17

16

Cambridge (UK)

U. Illinois

4

3

6

7

10

10

Duke

Rice

Carnegie Mellon

U. Michigan

Yale

7 10

6 2 8

4

4

3

2

2

3

6

6

6

6 Cornell 2

Northwestern

UCLA

3

1

2

3

5

4

4

Johns Hopkins

Columbia

4

1

2

2

1

3

3

ECHA 2008

Colleges/

Table 1.2

Colleges and Universities Attended by Olympians

2006-2007

Number enrolled Number enrolled Total

16

Rice 5 1 6

Cornell 4 4

UCLA 1 4 5

1

ECHA 2008

Advanced Degrees

Table 2.1

Doctoral Degrees Attained by Olympians

1998

M.D.

J.D. in Subject Areas

Degrees

2

1

2

17

9

1

ECHA 2008

Advanced Degrees

Table 2.2

Doctoral Degrees Attained by Olympians

2006-2007

J.D.

Degrees

4 2

18

ECHA 2008

Table 3.1

Olympians’ Occupations (not in Colleges and Universities)

1998

Computer Occupations

Computer Companies

19

Scientific Occupations

Nat. at Bell labs (ATT) 1 at IBM

Other Occupations

5

1

Lawyer

Executive Director of Nonprofit Corp.

ECHA 2008

Computer Occupations

Table 3.2

Olympians’ Occupations 2006-2007

20

1 Senior Program Manager

Scientific Occupations/Academia

Technology Corporations

Assistant Technical Staff at University Lab 1

Other Occupations

3 Lawyer

1 Data entry clerk

ECHA 2008 21

Table 4.1

Total Publications

1998

Olympiad

Ages 15-22 10 113 107

PreCollege/College (1.4/person) (6.3/person) (4.9/person)

Ages 23-29 231 141 401

Career (18.2/person)

Ages 30-46 1,622 14 280

Career (23.3/person)

Total Publications in 1,865 268

Subject N=51

788

ECHA 2008 22

Table 4.2

Total Publications

2006-2007

Olympiad

Ages 15-22 60 28

PreCollege/College (5.00/person) (1.87/person)

Ages 23-29 185 274

110

(3.3/person)

139

(6.04/person)

Ages 2,775 1,112

(68/person) (13.7/person) (38.34/person)

Subject N=72

1,361

ECHA 2008 23

Table 5

Comparison of Group A and C Olympians on selected factor/components (N=24

Group A and N=24 Group C)

Variable M t df

Conducive Home

Atmosphere

SES

Lack of

Motivation

School

Hindrances

Negative Affect at School

Effort Attribution

Individual

Effort Attribution

Global

Publications

ECHA 2008 24

Table 6

Standardized Function and Correlation Coefficients

Function

(Socio-Economic Status)

(Family Structure

One-Parent/Two-Parent Families)

(Conducive Home

Atmosphere)

(School Shortcomings)

(Negative School Affect)

(Individual’s Effort needed

to Perform Well)

(Lack of Motivation)

(Global Degree of Effort needed for Success)

(Computer Literacy)

ECHA 2008 25

Figure 1 The Munich Process Model of Giftedness by Ziegler and Perleth (1997).

ECHA 2008 26

Figure 2 : The Munich Dynamic Ability-Achievement Model according to Perleth

(2001, p.367).

Download