Journal of Health Communication, 11:75–91, 2006 Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 1081-0730 print/1087-0415 online DOI: 10.1080/10810730500461091 Defining Moments in Risk Communication Research: 1996–2005 KATHERINE A. MCCOMAS Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, USA Ten years ago, scholars suggested that risk communication was embarking on a new phase that would give increased attention to the social contexts that surround and encroach on public responses to risk information. A decade later, many researchers have answered the call, with several defining studies examining the social and psychological influences on risk communication. This article reviews risk communication research appearing in the published literature since 1996. Among studies, social trust, the social amplification of risk framework, and the affect heuristic figured prominently. Also common were studies examining the influence of risk in the mass media. Among these were content analyses of media coverage of risk, as well as investigations of possible effects resulting from coverage. The use of mental models was a dominant method for developing risk message content. Other studies examined the use of risk comparisons, narratives, and visuals in the production of risk messages. Research also examined how providing information about a risk’s severity, social norms, and efficacy influenced communication behaviors and intentions to follow risk reduction measures. Methods for conducting public outreach in health risk communication rounded out the literature. For observers of risk communication, the last decade contained many defining moments: critical times when events or circumstances—in some cases tragic— resulted in opportunities to pause and reflect, initiate much-needed dialogue, redress past mistakes, and, with any luck, develop a more sophisticated understanding of the issue at hand. For risk communication, these moments included efforts to curb the spread of communicable diseases, such as AIDS in sub-Saharan Africa, foot-andmouth disease among livestock in Europe, and avian influenza in Asia. Natural disasters necessitating risk communication included the 9.0 magnitude earthquake off the coast of Sumatra that caused a tsunami killing more than 280,000 people in 10 countries, and Hurricane Katrina, which devastated the U.S. Gulf Coast along Louisiana and Mississippi and cost an as-yet-unknown number of lives. Due to risk communication efforts, previously unknown terms, acronyms, and exotic-sounding diseases became part of the popular lexicon, including acrylamides, the West Nile virus, Y2K, rBGH (recombinant bovine growth hormone), SARS (severe acute respiratory syndrome), GMOs (genetically modified organisms), EMFs (electromagnetic fields), and BSE (‘‘mad cow’’ disease). Terrorist attacks on the U.S. World Trade Centers and the Pentagon on September 11, 2001; the bombing of Madrid’s commuter train network on March 11, 2004; and two strikes on London’s Address correspondence to Katherine A. McComas, Assistant Professor, 313 Kennedy Hall, Department of Communication, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA. E-mail: kam19@cornell.edu 75 76 K. A. McComas Underground and bus system in July 2005 exacted a heavy toll on public confidence in transportation safety while calling yet again for concerted risk communication efforts. More mundane but just as demanding of risk communicators’ attention were ongoing day-to-day challenges, among them periodic outbreaks of food borne illness, unexplained cases of community ‘‘cancer clusters,’’ the continued prevalence of smoking and increasing evidence of the dangers of second-hand smoke, in-home threats from radon gas and flaking lead paint, backyard threats of Lyme disease, the ever-present impasses surrounding the siting of local landfills and hazardous waste sites, and a heightened awareness of and concern about an ‘‘epidemic’’ of obesity in America. Rather than being exhaustive, this list is meant to be evocative of the risks we as individuals and members of society encountered over the last 10 years. In response to the risks, we witnessed a constant stream of messages warning of dangers to public health and safety and suggesting safer courses of action. Whereas some of these messages achieved their intended effects, other messages elicited little if any public response. In turn, efforts to improve risk communication efforts spawned a growing literature investigating a practice that traditionally took place without much thought of research (Bostrom & Lofstedt, 2003). Risk has been defined as the ‘‘things, forces, or circumstances that pose danger to people or to what they value’’ (p. 215) and typically is described in terms of a likelihood or probability of loss occurring (Stern & Fineberg, 1996). In turn, risk communication can be broadly understood as an iterative exchange of information among individuals, groups, and institutions related to the assessment, characterization, and management of risk. Inherent to the understanding of risk, and the practice of risk communication, is an awareness that risk encompasses both objective and subjective qualities and that risk judgments are, to some degree, a by-product of social, cultural, and psychological influences (Slovic, 1999). According to Leiss (1996), recognizing that risk is, in part, a social construct marked an evolution in thought about risk communication.1 Cvetovich and Lofstedt (1999) similarly remarked that current research in risk assessment and management indicates an understanding that ‘‘judgments of risk are not limited to assessments of physical processes . . . [but] are also reflections of the understanding of social systems and the actors playing roles within them’’ (p. 3). This evolution in thought is reflected in the last 10 years of risk communication research, which this article seeks to synthesize with some important caveats. First, because of the wealth of research, a 10-year literature review must, by necessity, exclude some noteworthy articles in an effort to include others.2 Readers seeking additional perspectives are referred to other excellent reviews (Bier, 2001; Boholm, 1 According to Leiss (1996) and others (Fischhoff, 1995; Plough & Krimsky, 1987), technical assessments dominated early approaches to risk communication. Persuasive approaches geared at increasing audience acceptance of technical assessments characterized the next phase of risk communication, which also saw a growing recognition of the importance of trust in risk communication efforts. The pervasive lack of trust in many risk management institutions ushered in the next phase, roughly since 1995, which included a greater acceptance that risk occurs in a social context (Leiss, 1996). 2 In some cases, this article refers to seminal works published prior to 1996 for background purposes. Risk Communication: 1996–2005 77 1998; Bostrom & Lofstedt, 2003; Gurabardhi et al., 2004; Renn, 1998b).3 This article further delimits its subject matter by focusing on risk communication related to public health rather than environmental health; however, it maintains that the origins of public health risks can be environmental, technological, or medical.4 Finally, although much risk communication takes place as part of risk reduction interventions or campaigns, this review excludes campaign research, which is the focus of another article in this anniversary issue. Rather, this review focuses on research examining risk communication that occurs intentionally or unintentionally, often in response to new or emerging health risks, between or among government agencies, industries, scientists or technical experts, and members of the public.5 The first part of the review is structured around research investigating public reactions to health risks, including the influence of unintentional risk messages appearing in the mass media. The next part of the review considers research on strategic risk communication, including the design and delivery of health risk messages. Public Reaction to Health Risks In the months preceding the terrorist strikes of September 11, 2001, Americans paid witness to what Time magazine called in its July 30 cover story, the ‘‘Summer of the Shark’’ (McCarthy, 2001). Although isolated incidents, shark attacks occurring off the coasts of Florida, Virginia, and North Carolina, including two fatal attacks around Labor Day weekend, resulted in a media ‘‘feeding frenzy’’ that some argued amplified the risk of shark attacks, frightened vacationers away, and dampened tourism along the East Coast. Calling the timing of the attacks coincidental, statisticians tried to assuage concerns by pointing out that there were actually fewer shark attacks reported in 2001 than 2000; they also noted that the greater number of people in the water during the summer holidays increased the likelihood of shark attacks. Others noted that people were more at risk from dying on the highway driving to the beach than from a shark attack (Minor, 2001; Moran, 2002). An underlying risk message was that fear of shark attacks was unwarranted, if not downright irrational. 3 Readers also are referred to journals that devoted special issues to risk communication, among them the Journal of the National Cancer Institute Monographs, 1999, Number 25; theJournal of Health Communication, volume 8 supplement; Risk Analysis, volume 23, issue 3; and Risk: Health, Safety & Environment, volume 10, number 3. 4 Although some scholars prefer to differentiate public health risk communication from environmental risk communication, drawing a hard line between these two subfields may be unwarranted. Many of the most prolific scholars in what may be considered environmental risk communication are also frequent contributors to the literature on health risk communication (Gurabardhi et al., 2004), suggesting an implicit if not explicit link between these two areas. This link arguably resides not in the origin of the risk but in its potential endpoint, the injured party. For example, whether the risk of cancer originated with the voluntary use of tobacco or an involuntary exposure to toxic waste, both circumstances ultimately pose a public health risk and could be considered under the rubric of public health risk communication. So, too, do potential risks related to biotechnology, water quality, and global climate change, making their exclusion from a review of public health risk communication unnecessary. 5 Although risk communication frequently concerns risks where a large degree of scientific consensus exists, it also is often less clear what the appropriate public response should be. Thus, whereas persuasive strategies typify the former risk communication efforts, the latter are better illustrated by efforts to provide balanced, accurate information so that people can, in a sense, make up their own minds about what constitutes acceptable risk. 78 K. A. McComas This example illustrates the divide that can exist between scientific and nonscientific assessments of risk—namely, public perceptions of risk frequently do not align with scientific assessments. Indeed, several decades’ worth of risk perception research supports the finding that nonexpert audiences bring other factors to bear on risk judgments, such as a risk’s voluntariness, controllabilty, catastrophic potential, scientific understanding, effects on future generations, and dread (Slovic, 1987, 1999, 2000). While sometimes perceiving greater risk, individuals also are prone to optimistic biases or illusions of invulnerability, meaning the tendency to believe that negative events are more likely to happen to other people than to oneself (Gurmankin et al., 2004; Salmon et al., 2003; Weinstein, 1989). The last decade saw continued study of risk perceptions. Particularly promising were advances made with respect to the affective dimensions of risk-related behaviors (Loewenstein et al., 2001; Peters et al., 2004; Poortinga & Pidgeon, 2005; Savadori et al., 2004; Sinaceur et al., 2005; Slovic, 1999, 2001; Slovic et al., 2004). In describing what they term the affect heuristic, Slovic and colleagues (2004) explained that people base their risk judgments not only on what they think about the risk but also on what they feel about it. If people have positive feelings about an activity, they tend to judge the risks as lower than if they have negative feelings about the activity and vice versa. Feelings also can override analytical reasoning. Research on young adults’ smoking behavior found, for example, that feelings more than risk perceptions drove decisions to start smoking (Romer & Jamieson, 2001). Similarly, Loewenstein and colleagues (2001) suggested a ‘‘riskas-feelings’’ hypothesis, which posits that an individual’s feelings can at times supersede what that individual considers the wisest course of action. Research on the affect heuristic is still developing, and its implications for risk communication research and practice remain largely uncharted (Slovic et al., 2004). Loewenstein and colleagues (2001) suggested that the risk-as-feelings hypothesis can explain gender and age-related differences in responses to risk, as well as why vivid language, narratives, or affective-laden imagery, often accompanying news stories, strongly influence people’s reactions to risk. One study found, for instance, that using the term ‘‘mad cow’’ rather than the scientific label influenced individuals to react more on emotion than on personal risk estimates (Sinaceur et al., 2005). Understanding the role of affect also may lead to a greater ability to explain and predict the development of stigma and its impact on risk perceptions and communication (Peters et al., 2004). Other research, reviewed in further detail below, has noted the influence of affect, such as worry, on individuals’ risk-information-seeking behaviors (Griffin, Neuwirth, Dunwoody, & Giese, 2004). The public’s reaction to the shark attacks off the East Coast offers an example of how affect can guide behavior (e.g., images of shark attacks, perhaps even primed by the movie Jaws, provoke a visceral aversion to the water). It also provides an example of what social scientists have termed the social amplification of risk, which is said to occur when risk events interact with psychological, social, institutional, and cultural processes in ways that amplify or attenuate people’s risk perceptions and subsequently their risk behavior (Kasperson & Kasperson, 2005; Pidgeon, Kasperson, & Slovic, 2003). An integrative framework, it aims to describe why some risks that scientists consider to pose relatively little threat to society receive a disproportionate amount of attention, whereas other risks considered to pose more serious risk receive scant notice (Kasperson et al., 2003). Potential consequences of risk amplification or attenuation include not only harm to public health but also Risk Communication: 1996–2005 79 increased liability, economic costs, loss of trust in institutions, and stigmatization. In essence, the framework represents an effort to capture the dualistic nature of risk as both an objective event as well as a social construct. First introduced in the late 1980s, the social amplification of risk framework was used over the last decade to investigate public reactions to several real or potential health risks, including Y2K (MacGregor, 2003), measles, mumps, rubella (MMR) vaccines (Petts & Niemeyer, 2004), cancer clusters (Trumbo, 1996), acrylamides (Lofstedt, 2003), genetically modified foods (Frewer et al., 2002), and nuclear power (Flynn et al., 1998). Although its strength to date lies primarily in its explanatory power, some have suggested that risk managers can use the framework to better anticipate and respond to risk controversies (Leiss, 2003). Risk in the Media Memorial Day Weekend traditionally marks the start of the summer movie season in the United States, and in 2004, the American film industry was banking on the global climate change thriller, The Day After Tomorrow, to attract large audiences. Critics aside, the movie did not disappoint, becoming a commercial success. For observers of risk communication, the film was most notable for the debate it sparked about possible effects on public perceptions of health risks from global climate change (Leiserowitz, 2004). A comparison of movie viewers’ and nonviewers’ attitudes before and after its debut suggested the film may have had short-term impacts on viewers’ risk perceptions; however, the study cautiously noted that, despite its commercial success, only a small proportion of the U.S. population saw the movie (Leiserowitz, 2004). The controversy surrounding The Day After Tomorrow illustrates how people often ascribe great powers to the mass media to influence public attitudes and behaviors. Because of their ability to increase the ‘‘availability’’ of risk images and therefore contribute to possible overestimation of risks (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974), mass media often are pointed to as having great influence over public reactions to risk. In fact, the literature investigating media’s influence on risk perceptions is much less certain (af Wahlberg & Sjoberg, 2000). Rather than showing strong effects, studies over the last decade generally were consistent with Tyler and Cook’s (1984) impersonal impact hypothesis, which posited that media coverage generates societal level, not personal level, judgments of risk. For example, Morton and Duck (2001) found that individuals exposed to risk information in the mass media tended to believe others were more likely to be victims of risk than themselves. The study did find, however, that when respondents were more dependent on media as important sources of health information, they were more likely to view risk information as relating to their own health risk. Others also have examined the influence of television viewing on individuals’ fear of being a crime victim and found that, although television exposure may have some limited effects, direct experience with a crime is a stronger predictor (Gross & Aday, 2003; Shrum, 2001). Whereas mass media may alert the public about health risks and help them form societal level judgments, research has shown that people more often rely on interpersonal channels of communication, such as social networks, to assess their personal health risks (Petts & Niemeyer, 2004; Scherer & Cho, 2003). Still, some research suggests that mass media can sometimes influence risk perceptions more than 80 K. A. McComas interpersonal sources, especially during times of heightened media coverage (Verbeke et al., 1999). During these periods of constant media attention, researchers have pointed to media coverage as a contributing factor in the social amplification of risk (Flynn et al., 1998; Frewer et al., 2002; Hill, 2001; Lofstedt, 2003; McInerney et al., 2004), as well as the development of stigma (Flynn et al., 1998; Flynn et al., 2001). In turn, the last decade saw several studies examining whether mass media disproportionately sensationalize risk or emphasize negative impacts. Rather than showing a consistent bias toward sensationalism or negativism, however, a review of the available research suggests more moderate or neutral reporting of risks (af Wahlberg & Sjoberg, 2000; Freudenburg et al., 1996; Sachsman et al., 2004). When mass media do include risk information, research suggests that the stories often lack important information for people to evaluate risks. One crossnational study of newspaper content appearing 10 years after the Chernobyl accident found that coverage included little use of statistics and provided few comparisons to help people understand the risks (Rowe et al., 2000). Another study of women’s magazines found that articles discussing breast cancer risks tended to report incorrect or incomplete information, leading the authors to question whether the reports could contribute to women’s misunderstanding about breast cancer risks (Marino & Gerlach, 1999). Other research found that media tended to under-represent certain causes of death, like tobacco use and heart disease, while over-representing others, like deaths caused by motor vehicles or toxic agents (Frost et al., 1997). In addition to unintentional risk messages, media often are used intentionally for risk communication. For example, after the September 11, 2001, terrorist strikes and the subsequent anthrax attacks, the U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) monitored the media to help shape its communication responses for the public (Prue et al., 2003). In another study of CDC risk communication efforts, Mebane and colleagues (2003) compared print media coverage to CDC press releases during the anthrax attacks of 2001 and found that, although the media generally followed CDC releases, they gave less weight to covering who was exposed, how they were exposed, and the role of antibiotics in preventing anthrax. As noted above, even were people to rely on media for personal risk information, media frequently did not include relevant risk information to enable them to estimate personal health risks. In turn, one study sought to determine how the inclusion of population data along with frequency data would influence individuals’ apprehension about risk, as well as their perceived victimization (Berger, 1998). The study found that when population data were included before frequency data, male subjects exhibited less apprehension and less perceived victimization for high risks. The inclusion of population data did not have similar influences for males for low risks, and the data had no influence on female subjects’ risk perceptions. In another study of media effects, Neuwirth, Dunwoody, & Griffin (2000) examined the influence of health risk information in news media on individuals’ intention to seek additional information. The results found that when media included information about the risk’s severity, individuals were more influenced to seek additional information, as well as express willingness to take protective actions. Information about the level of risk and what actions individuals could take (i.e., efficacy) also influenced whether individuals expressed a willingness to take protective actions. Risk Communication: 1996–2005 81 Strategic Risk Communication To this point, much of the review has focused primarily on public reactions to unintentional messages about health risks communicated via mass media or other sources. Whereas risk communicators may have little control over the design, delivery, and impact of these unofficial risk messages, research also has examined ways to improve intentional or strategic risk communication over which communicators can exercise greater control. Some of this research focuses on the design of risk messages, whereas other research investigates the processes and outcomes of delivery. Developing Health Risk Messages As a method for identifying specific content for risk messages, the ‘‘mental models’’ approach continued to receive attention and support (Cox et al., 2003; Fischhoff, 1999; MacGregor et al., 1999; Morgan et al., 2002; Niewohner et al., 2004; Zaksek & Arvai, 2004). The mental models method uses interviews with expert groups and a representative sample of target audience members to determine how expert and lay understanding of risk differs. These interviews then help communicators choose content to include in risk communication materials. The language used in risk messages also received attention. Among studies, Jardine and Hrudey (1997) examined how several words commonly used by risk managers to describe risk, such as ‘‘significant’’ and ‘‘nonsignificant,’’ can confuse lay audiences. Research also found that the inclusion of information about risk benefits can influence reactions to risk messages, particularly when the risks already were considered low (Gaskell et al., 2004; Knuth et al., 2003). Other research examined public attitudes toward risk comparisons. Contrary to previous assumptions that counseled against risk comparisons, results found that comparisons can at times elicit positive reactions from the public (Johnson, 2003, 2004a, 2004b). Trust in the source of the information appeared key to whether individuals view the comparisons as valid (Johnson & Chess, 2003). In addition to examining public attitudes toward risk, research sought to examine how particular information about risk influenced risk behavior. Lipkus and colleagues tested the influence of information about risk factors and risk severity in messages informing individuals about colorectal cancer risks (Lipkus et al., 1999; Lipkus et al., 2003). The authors found that risk factor information increased participants’ knowledge of the risks but did not influence their affective responses (e.g., worry, anxiety, or fear) or intentions to get screened for colorectal cancer the following year (Lipkus et al., 1999). Results of a subsequent study did find, however, that increasing the perceived severity of the risks in the information materials influenced participants’ likelihood of having a follow-up screening (Lipkus et al., 2003). To examine what contributes toward long-term behavior changes in health risk behavior, Griffin and colleagues (1999, 2002) developed the risk information seeking and processing (RISP) model. This model integrates the heuristic-systematic processing model (Eagly & Chaiken, 1993) and the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1988) to examine how people differentially react to risk information. The RISP model posits that the gap between what people know and what they perceive they need to know (‘‘information sufficiency’’) will influence information processing (i.e., heuristic or systematic) and information seeking (i.e., active, routine, avoidance) behaviors. In turn, information sufficiency is predicted by affective responses 82 K. A. McComas to a risk and beliefs about what others think they should know about the risk (Griffin et al., 2004). When people have greater affective responses (e.g., more worry) and feel greater social pressure to learn more about the risk, they tend to perceive a greater need for information. Moreover, research has found that the larger the gap, the more likely individuals are to process the information systematically (Kahlor et al., 2003). Research also investigated the influence of efficacy information on promoting healthy behaviors. Much of this research builds on the extended parallel process model (Witte, 1994), which posits that when people perceive a threat and become scared, they are motivated to reduce that fear. Whether individuals engage in danger control (e.g., seek additional information or adopt risk reduction behaviors) or fear control (e.g., ignore warnings or denigrate the message source) depends on their perceived efficacy, with higher levels of perceived efficacy leading to danger control. To help predict whether individuals will engage in danger or fear control, Witte and colleagues (1996) developed and tested a risk behavior diagnosis scale for use by practitioners. Expanding efficacy research, Rimal (2001) examined the relationship among self-efficacy, perceived risk, and information seeking behaviors, and found that when self-efficacy was high, individuals perceiving greater risks were more likely to seek information. Rimal and Real (2003) also found some support that when people are made more aware of the risks, as well as what they could do to reduce their exposure (i.e., efficacy), they are more likely to report intentions to increase risk reduction behaviors. Despite the apparent importance of enhancing efficacy beliefs to promote risk reduction behaviors, one study of government and nongovernment produced food safety materials found that self-efficacy statements appeared far less frequently than risk-stimulating statements (Gordon, 2003). For an alternative to traditional ‘‘fact sheets,’’ researchers also investigated the use of novel formats of message delivery, including the use of narratives or stories (Greene & Brinn, 2003; Johnson, 2004b; Scherer et al., 1999). One study that offered promising results found that narratives about the risks of tanning bed use were rated high on realism and promoted intentions not to use tanning beds (Greene & Brinn, 2003). Finally, research examined the use of visuals, such as diagrams, charts, and risk ladders, to aid in communication efforts (Connelly & Knuth, 1998; Gibson & Zillmann, 2000; Lipkus & Hollands, 1999). Communicating Health Risk Messages It may be a truism to say that even the most carefully developed risk messages are destined to fail if people do not trust the messengers; however, some would argue that it is also the case that, until quite recently, trust largely was unappreciated in risk management efforts (Slovic, 1993). Building on research trends that began in the early 1990s, the last decade saw sustained study of trust and its brethren concept, source credibility (Frewer et al., 1996; Frewer et al., 2003; Heath et al., 1998; Jungermann et al., 1996; Maeda & Miyahara, 2003; McComas & Trumbo, 2001; R. G. Peters et al., 1997; Poortinga & Pidgeon, 2005; Siegrist et al., 2003; Trumbo & McComas, 2003). Perhaps most prominent among risk communication research was the elaboration and testing of social trust related to risk management (Cvetkovich & Lofstedt, 1999; Earle & Cvetkovich, 1995; Lofstedt, 2005). Although a specific definition of social trust in risk management remains somewhat elusive, a general understanding considers it a process whereby individuals choose to trust or rely on risk management institutions, and the often unknown individuals operating Risk Communication: 1996–2005 83 them, to protect them and their interests from risk (Cvetkovich & Lofstedt, 1999). As such, social trust differs from what researchers term interpersonal trust, meaning trust that is placed in specific individuals based on the perceived presence or absence of certain traits (more akin to source credibility; Earle & Cvetkovich, 1995). Research conducted during the last decade suggests that social trust in risk management is based, in part, on perceived shared values, which are learned via stories or narratives that institutions tell (Earle & Cvetkovich, 1995, 1999). Perceiving shared values also can influence perceived risks and benefits (Siegrist et al., 2000; Siegrist et al., 2001). For example, Siegrist and colleagues (2001) found in a study of cancer cluster communication that individuals were more likely to believe that clusters could occur randomly when they believed risk managers shared their values. Other studies examining the relationship between trust or credibility and public acceptance of risk have generally found a correlation between trust or credibility and risk perceptions: namely, when trust or credibility is found wanting, people perceive greater risks and vice versa (Cvetkovich et al., 2002; Frewer et al., 2003; McComas & Trumbo, 2001; Poortinga & Pidgeon, 2005; Siegrist et al., 2005; Trumbo & McComas, 2003).6 Recent research has tried to disentangle the causal relationship, that is, whether risk acceptability encourages people to trust, or trust encourages people to be more accepting of risk (Poortinga & Pidgeon, 2005). Finally, supporting the axiom that trust is easier lost than regained, research found that even after receiving positive information about their actions, people were less likely to trust previously distrusted risk managers (Cvetkovich et al., 2002). Although much health risk communication involves providing information on an individual basis to allow people to make personal choices, some requires gathering people together in formalized settings to seek, discuss, or provide information about risks having societal as well as personal implications. How to conduct public outreach7 effectively was a frequent topic of interest during the last decade, as was what it meant to be ‘‘effective’’ or ‘‘successful’’ in public outreach efforts.8 Questions surrounding audience representation was a common line of inquiry (Allen, 1998). Representation is considered important to health risk communication on several fronts. For one, successful risk characterization can depend on having a representative sample of stakeholder input (Stern & Fineberg, 1996). For another, having the target audience present ensures that people receive the risk information they need to take appropriate precautions. Finally, having decisions or recommendations based 6 One exception was a study showing that knowledge of the risk may be a greater predictor of risk perception than trust or credibility (Sjoberg, 2001). 7 This article uses the term ‘‘outreach’’ rather than participation or involvement to refer to the numerous formalized techniques used to communicate health risks to members of the public in organized social settings. It recognizes that public outreach has many formats and functions, ranging from simply providing health risk information to targeted audiences to fully engaging audience members in collective deliberation about health risks. Whereas the former may typify many health risk communication efforts, the latter endeavors to make audience members or stakeholders more active contributors to the characterization, assessment, and management of risk. 8 Among challenges associated with determining what counts as ‘‘effective’’ or ‘‘successful’’ public outreach is a lack of common criteria or benchmarks for evaluation (Chess & Purcell, 1999; Rowe & Frewer, 2000). For example, some studies focus on procedural elements, such as who participated in the process, while others focus on outcomes, such as whether the process resulted in better decisions. Participants and organizers of public outreach processes also sometimes disagree on criteria for evaluating success (Santos & Chess, 2003). 84 K. A. McComas on representative opinions also can influence perceptions of fairness and legitimacy in health risk policy making (Renn et al., 1996). Among studies on representation, research on public meetings found that meeting participants are demographically similar to nonparticipants; however, often they are more concerned about the potential risks and are more skeptical of the official sources of information (McComas, 2001, 2003b). Outside of public meetings or hearings, research has examined ways to solicit a more representative sample of viewpoints, such as via the use of public opinion surveys (McComas & Scherer, 1999; Pidgeon et al., 2005). Some findings suggest that surveys can provide rapid and representative insight into people’s attitudes, beliefs, and knowledge during crises (Blendon et al., 2003; Pollard, 2003); however, questions about response rate bias linger. For example, recent research conducted in the United Kingdom about the use of biotechnology in agriculture found significant differences in viewpoints between a representative sample and a self-selected sample of respondents (Pidgeon et al., 2005). In addition to issues of representation, research has offered evidence that perceived fairness of discussion procedures in government-sponsored outreach efforts can influence the audience’s satisfaction with the process (McComas, 2003a; Webler & Tuler, 2000) and acceptance of the decisions (Arvai, 2003). Promoting dialogue and consideration of alternatives among participants and experts is increasingly emphasized as a way of achieving more informed judgments about health risks (Arvai et al., 2001; Renn, 1998a, 1999, 2003). Sharing control over the decisionmaking procedures also is important to perceptions of fairness (Heath et al., 2002), although this control may not necessarily translate into support for risk management policies (Heath & Abel, 1996). Increased attention to the fairness of deliberative procedures also is considered essential to efforts seeking to maintain or rebuild trust in risk management institutions (Lofstedt, 2005). Future Directions A decade ago, scholars predicted a new era of risk communication research, one moving beyond a narrow vision of risk communication as persuasion and toward a broader understanding of the psychological, cultural, and social influences on risk communication (Leiss, 1996). In particular, researchers pointed to the role that trust plays in risk communication, while noting a pervasive—and some might suggest deserved—distrust in many risk management institutions (Leiss, 1995). A review of the literature suggests that many researchers answered the call, offering strong theoretical contributions as well as some practical applications. Moreover, there are several promising directions for future research. In particular, research on the affect heuristic and risk-as-feelings hypothesis undoubtedly will see continued attention. Among avenues for study include further integration of affect into the social amplification of risk framework and the RISP model, both of which currently use some affective measures. The influence of affect on assessments of social trust seems another important line of inquiry. For example, given that narratives influence affective judgments (Loewenstein et al., 2001), how do stories that communicate shared values influence affective judgments and the promotion of social trust? The interplay of affect, risk narratives, and visuals also merits further study, as do the possible ethical implications involved with the potential misuse of such message strategies. Risk Communication: 1996–2005 85 The last decade saw increasing risk communication over the Internet. Although some research has addressed the Internet’s impact on risk communication (e.g., Hobbs et al., 2004), this is clearly an area for future growth. The influence of perceived efficacy is another important line of inquiry, including its impact on risk perceptions and intentions to employ risk reduction behaviors. A lack of control is frequently noted as a factor that increases risk perceptions. Accordingly, it might be useful to integrate some of the efficacy research emerging from communication studies with the social amplification of risk concept to specify the influence of efficacy on risk amplification. Research should also consider efficacy related to public outreach efforts. That is, to what extent does believing that one can influence health risk decisions (one measure of efficacy) impact social trust in risk management and satisfaction with outcomes? Methods for public participation deserve continued attention, including how perceptions of procedural fairness influence people’s willingness to participate in outreach sessions. These are just a few of the possible research directions.9 The review also underscores some challenges in the field, notably a literature that is very dispersed among several disciplines. Among the strengths of risk communication research is its multidisciplinary nature, allowing it to draw from a well-rounded pool of knowledge and expertise. Yet this strength poses challenges in that knowledge is not centralized. Perhaps due to this diversity, risk communication research presently is characterized by many, sometimes overlapping, variable analytic studies but few integrative theoretical frameworks. Some exceptions include the social amplification of risk, as well as the RISP model. Future research should strive for more theoretical integration, as researchers capitalize on the advances made in neighboring sciences. Risk events continue to try public confidence in risk management institutions, necessitating concerted risk communication efforts to sustain and rebuild public trust. If the increased visibility of the field and productivity of researchers offer an indication, the next decade will see continued and more effective efforts aimed at meeting these challenges. References af Wahlberg, A. & Sjoberg, L. (2000). Risk perception and the media. Journal of Risk Research, 3(1), 31–50. Ajzen, I. (1988). Attitudes, personality, and behavior. Milton Keynes, UK: Open University Press. Allen, P. (1998). Public participation in resolving environmental disputes and the problem of representativeness. Risk: Health, Safety & Environment, 9, 297–308. Arvai, J. L. (2003). Using risk communication to disclose the outcome of a participatory decision-making process: Effects on the perceived acceptability of risk-policy decision. Risk Analysis, 23(2), 281–289. Arvai, J. L., Gregory, R., & McDaniels, T. L. (2001). Testing a structured decision approach: Value-focused thinking for deliberative risk communication. Risk Analysis, 21(6), 1065–1076. Berger, C. R. (1998). Processing quantitative data about risk and threat in news reports. Journal of Communication, 48(3), 87–106. Bier, V. M. (2001). On the state of the art: Risk communication to the public. Reliability Engineering & System Safety, 71(2), 139–150. 9 Others have suggested additional research agendas that merit attention (Renn, 1998b). 86 K. A. McComas Blendon, R. J., Benson, J. M., Desroches, C. M., & Weldon, K. J. (2003). Using opinion surveys to track the public’s response to a bioterrorist attack. Journal of Health Communication, 8, 83–92. Boholm, A. (1998). Comparative studies of risk perceptions: A review of twenty years of research. Journal of Risk Research, 1, 135–163. Bostrom, A. & Lofstedt, R. E. (2003). Communicating risk: Wireless and hardwired. Risk Analysis, 23(2), 241–248. Chess, C. & Purcell, K. (1999). Public participation and the environment: Do we know what works? Environmental Science & Technology, 33, 2685–2692. Connelly, N. A. & Knuth, B. A. (1998). Evaluating risk communication: Examining target audience perceptions about four presentation formats for fish consumption health advisory information. Risk Analysis, 18(5), 649–659. Cox, P., Niewohner, J., Pidgeon, N., Gerrard, S., Fischhoff, B., & Riley, D. (2003). The use of mental models in chemical risk protection: Developing a generic workplace methodology. Risk Analysis, 23(2), 311–324. Cvetkovich, G. & Lofstedt, R. E. (Eds.) (1999). Social trust and the management of risk. London: Earthscan Publications. Cvetkovich, G., Siegrist, M., Murray, R., & Tragesser, S. (2002). New information and social trust: Asymmetry and perseverance of attributions about hazard managers. Risk Analysis, 22(2), 359–367. Eagly, A. H. & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. San Diego: Harcourt Brace. Earle, T. C. & Cvetkovich, G. (1995). Social trust: Toward a cosmopolitan society. Westport, CT: Praeger. Earle, T. C. & Cvetkovich, G. (1999). Social trust and culture in risk management. In G. Cvetkovich & R. E. Lofstedt (Eds.), Social trust and the management of risk (pp. 9–21). London: Earthscan. Fischhoff, B. (1995). Risk perception and communication unplugged—20 years of process. Risk Analysis, 15(2), 137–145. Fischhoff, B. (1999). Why (cancer) risk communication can be hard. Journal of the National Cancer Institute Monographs, 25, 7–13. Flynn, J., Peters, E., Mertz, C. K., & Slovic, P. (1998). Risk, media, and stigma at Rocky Flats. Risk Analysis, 18(6), 715–727. Flynn, J., Slovic, P., & Kunreuther, H. (Eds.) (2001). Risk, media and stigma: Understanding public challenges to modern science and technology. London: Earthscan. Freudenburg, W. R., Coleman, C., Gonzales, J., & Helgeland, C. (1996). Media coverage of hazard events: Analyzing the assumptions. Risk Analysis, 16(1), 31–42. Frewer, L. J., Howard, C., Hedderley, D., & Shepherd, R. (1996). What determines trust in information about food-related risks? Underlying psychological contructs. Risk Analysis, 16(4), 473–500. Frewer, L. J., Miles, S., & Marsh, R. (2002). The media and genetically modified foods: Evidence in support of social amplification of risk. Risk Analysis, 22(4), 701–711. Frewer, L. J., Scholderer, J., & Bredahl, L. (2003). Communicating about the risks and benefits of genetically modified foods: The mediating role of trust. Risk Analysis, 23(6), 1117–1133. Frost, K., Frank, E., & Maibach, E. (1997). Relative risk in the news media: A quantification of misrepresentation. American Journal of Public Health, 87(5), 842–845. Gaskell, G., Allum, N., Wagner, W., Kronberger, N., Torgersen, H., Hampel, J., et al. (2004). GM foods and the misperception of risk perception. Risk Analysis, 24(1), 185–194. Gibson, R. & Zillmann, D. (2000). Reading between the photographs: The influence of incidental pictorial information on issue perception. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 77(2), 355–366. Gordon, J. (2003). Risk communication and foodborne illness: Message sponsorship and attempts to stimulate perceptions of risk. Risk Analysis, 23(6), 1287–1296. Risk Communication: 1996–2005 87 Greene, K. & Brinn, L. S. (2003). Messages influencing college women’s tanning bed use: Statistical versus narrative evidence format and a self-assessment to increase perceived susceptibility. Journal of Health Communication, 8(5), 443–461. Griffin, R. J., Dunwoody, S., & Neuwirth, K. (1999). Proposed model of the relationship of risk information seeking and processing to the development of preventive behaviors. Environmental Research, 80(2), S230. Griffin, R. J., Neuwirth, K., Dunwoody, S., & Giese, J. (2004). Information sufficiency and risk communication. Media Psychology, 6(1), 23–61. Griffin, R. J., Neuwirth, K., Giese, J., & Dunwoody, S. (2002). Linking the heuristicsystematic model and depth of processing. Communication Research, 29(6), 705–732. Gross, K. & Aday, S. (2003). The scary world in your living room and neighborhood: Using local broadcast news, neighborhood crime rates, and personal experience to test agenda setting and cultivation. Journal of Communication, 53(3), 411–426. Gurabardhi, Z., Gutteling, J. M., & Kuttschreuter, M. (2004). The development of risk communication. Science Communication, 25(4), 323–349. Gurmankin, A. D., Baron, J., & Armstrong, K. (2004). Intended message versus message received in hypothetical physician risk communications: Exploring the gap. Risk Analysis, 24(5), 1337–1347. Heath, R. L. & Abel, D. D. (1996). Types of knowledge as predictors of company support: The role of information in risk communication. Journal of Public Relations Research, 8(1), 35. Heath, R. L., Bradshaw, J., & Lee, J. (2002). Community relationship building: Local leadership in the risk communication infrastructure. Journal of Public Relations Research, 14(4), 317. Heath, R. L., Seshadri, S., & Lee, J. (1998). Risk communication: A two-community analysis of proximity, dread, trust, involvement, uncertainty, openness=accessibility, and knowledge on support=opposition toward chemical companies. Journal of Public Relations Research, 10(4), 35. Hill, A. (2001). Media risks: The social amplification of risk and the media. Journal of Risk Research, 4, 209–226. Hobbs, J., Kittler, A., Fox, S., Middleton, B., & Bates, D. W. (2004). Communicating health information to an alarmed public facing a threat such as a bioterrorist attack. Journal of Health Communication, 9(1), 67–75. Jardine, C. G. & Hrudey, S. E. (1997). Mixed messages in risk communication. Risk Analysis, 17(4), 489–498. Johnson, B. B. (2003). Are some risk comparisons more effective under conflict? A replication and extension of Roth et al. Risk Analysis, 23(4), 767–780. Johnson, B. B. (2004a). Risk comparisons, conflict, and risk acceptability claims. Risk Analysis, 24(1), 131–145. Johnson, B. B. (2004b). Varying risk comparison elements: Effects on public reactions. Risk Analysis, 24(1), 103–114. Johnson, B. B. & Chess, C. (2003). How reassuring are risk comparisons to pollution standards and emission limits? Risk Analysis, 23(5), 999–1007. Jungermann, H., Pfister, H. R., & Fischer, K. (1996). Credibility, information preferences, and information interests. Risk Analysis, 16(2), 251–261. Kahlor, L., Dunwoody, S., Griffin, R. J., Neuwirth, K., & Giese, J. (2003). Studying heuristicsystematic processing of risk communication. Risk Analysis, 23(2), 355–368. Kasperson, J. X. & Kasperson, R. E. (2005). The social contours of risk: Publics, risk communication and the social amplification of risk (Vol. 1). London: Earthscan. Kasperson, J. X., Kasperson, R. E., Pidgeon, N. F., & Slovic, P. (2003). The social amplification of risk: Assessing fifteen years of theory and research. In N. F. Pidgeon, R. E. Kasperson, & P. Slovic (Eds.), The social amplification of risk (pp. 13–46). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 88 K. A. McComas Knuth, B. A., Connelly, N. A., Sheeshka, J., & Patterson, J. (2003). Weighing health benefit and health risk information when consuming sport-caught fish. Risk Analysis, 23(6), 1185–1197. Leiserowitz, A. (2004). Before and after the Day After Tomorrow: A U.S. study of climate change risk perceptions. Environment, 46(9), 22–37. Leiss, W. (1995). Down and dirty: The use and abuse of public trust in risk communication. Risk Analysis, 15(6), 685–692. Leiss, W. (1996). Three phases in the evolution of risk communication practice. Annals AAPSS, 545, 85–94. Leiss, W. (2003). Searching for the public policy relevance of the risk amplification framework. In N. F. Pidgeon, R. E. Kasperson, & P. Slovic (Eds.), The social amplification of risk (pp. 355–373). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lipkus, I. M., Crawford, Y., Fenn, K., Biradavolu, M., Binder, R. A., Marcus, A., et al. (1999). Testing different formats for communicating colorectal cancer risk. Journal of Health Communication, 4(4), 311–324. Lipkus, I. M., Green, L. G., & Marcus, A. (2003). Manipulating perceptions of colorectal cancer threat: Implications for screening intentions and behaviors. Journal of Health Communication, 8(3), 213–228. Lipkus, I. M. & Hollands, J. G. (1999). The visual communication of risk. Journal of the National Cancer Institute Monographs, 25, 149–163. Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 267–286. Lofstedt, R. E. (2003). Science communication and the Swedish acrylamide ‘‘alarm.’’ Journal of Health Communication, 8(5), 407–432. Lofstedt, R. E. (2005). Risk management in post-trust societies. London: Palgrave Macmillan. MacGregor, D. G. (2003). Public response to Y2K: Social amplification and risk adaptation: or, ‘‘how I learned to stop worrying and love Y2K’’. In N. F. Pidgeon, R. E. Kasperson, & P. Slovic (Eds.), The social amplification of risk (pp. 243–261). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. MacGregor, D. G., Slovic, P., & Malmfors, T. (1999). ‘‘How exposed is exposed enough?’’— Lay inferences about chemical exposure. Risk Analysis, 19(4), 649–659. Maeda, Y. & Miyahara, M. (2003). Determinants of trust in industry, government, and citizen’s groups in Japan. Risk Analysis, 23(2), 303–310. Marino, C. & Gerlach, K. K. (1999). An analysis of breast cancer coverage in selected women’s magazines, 1987–1995. American Journal of Health Promotion, 13(3), 163–170. McCarthy, T. (2001). Why can’t we be friends? Time, 158, 34–40. McComas, K. A. (2001). Public meetings about local waste management problems: Comparing participants to non participants. Environmental Management, 27(1), 135–147. McComas, K. A. (2003a). Citizen satisfaction with public meetings used for risk communication. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 31(2), 164–184. McComas, K. A. (2003b). Public meetings and risk amplification: A longitudinal study. Risk Analysis, 23(6), 1257–1270. McComas, K. A. & Scherer, C. W. (1999). Providing balanced risk information in surveys used as citizen participation mechanisms. Society & Natural Resources, 12(2), 107–119. McComas, K. A. & Trumbo, C. W. (2001). Source credibility in environmental health-risk controversies: Application of Meyer’s credibility index. Risk Analysis, 21(3), 467–480. McInerney, C., Bird, N., & Nucci, M. (2004). The flow of scientific knowledge from lab to the lay public—the case of genetically modified food. Science Communication, 26(1), 44–74. Mebane, F., Temin, S., & Parvanta, C. F. (2003). Communicating anthrax in 2001: A comparison of CDC information and print media accounts. Journal of Health Communication, 8, 50–82. Minor, E. J. (2001, September 5). Shark expert says we’re crying wolf. Palm Beach Post, p. 1. Moran, K. (2002, April 21). Shark attacks aren’t on the rise. The New York Post, p. 67. Risk Communication: 1996–2005 89 Morgan, M. G., Fischhoff, B., Bostrom, A., & Atman, C. J. (2002). Risk communication: A mental models approach. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. Morton, T. A. & Duck, J. M. (2001). Communication and health beliefs—mass and interpersonal influences on perceptions of risk to self and others. Communication Research, 28(5), 602–626. Neuwirth, K., Dunwoody, S., & Griffin, R. J. (2000). Protection motivation and risk communication. Risk Analysis, 20(5), 721–734. Niewohner, J., Cox, P., Gerrard, S., & Pidgeon, N. (2004). Evaluating the efficacy of a mental models approach for improving occupational chemical risk protection. Risk Analysis, 24(2), 349–361. Peters, E. M., Burraston, B., & Mertz, C. K. (2004). An emotion-based model of risk perception and stigma susceptibility: Cognitive appraisals of emotion, affective reactivity, worldviews, and risk perceptions in the generation of technological stigma. Risk Analysis, 24(5), 1349–1367. Peters, R. G., Covello, V. T., & McCallum, D. B. (1997). The determinants of trust and credibility in environmental risk communication: An empirical study. Risk Analysis, 17(1), 43–54. Petts, J. & Niemeyer, S. (2004). Health risk communication and amplification: Learning from the MMR vaccination controversy. Health Risk & Society, 6(1), 7–23. Pidgeon, N. F., Kasperson, R. E., & Slovic, P. (Eds.) (2003). The social amplification of risk. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. Pidgeon, N. F., Poortinga, W., Rowe, G., Jones, T. H., Walls, J., & O’Riordan, T. (2005). Using surveys in public participation processes for risk decision making: The case of the 2003 British GM nation? Public debate. Risk Analysis, 25(2), 467–479. Plough, A. & Krimsky, S. (1987). The emergence of risk communication studies: Social and political context. Science, Technology, and Human Values, 12(3&4), 4–10. Pollard, W. E. (2003). Public perceptions of information sources concerning bioterrorism before and after anthrax attacks: An analysis of national survey data. Journal of Health Communication, 8, 93–103. Poortinga, W. & Pidgeon, N. F. (2005). Trust in risk regulation: Cause or consequence of the acceptability of GM food? Risk Analysis, 25(1), 199–209. Prue, C. E., Lackey, C., Swenarski, L., & Gantt, J. M. (2003). Communication monitoring: Shaping CDC’s emergency risk communication efforts. Journal of Health Communication, 8, 35–49. Renn, O. (1998a). The role of risk communication and public dialogue for improving risk management. Risk Decision and Policy, 3, 5–30. Renn, O. (1998b). Three decades of risk research: Accomplishments and new challenges. Journal of Risk Research, 1, 49–71. Renn, O. (1999). A model for an analytic-deliberative process in risk management. Environmental Science & Technology, 33(18), 3049–3055. Renn, O. (2003). Acrylamide: Lessons for risk management and communication. Journal of Health Communication, 8(5), 435–441. Renn, O., Webler, T., & Kastenholz, H. (1996). Procedural and substantive fairness in landfill siting: A swiss case study. Risk: Health, Safety & Environment, 7, 145–168. Rimal, R. N. (2001). Perceived risk and self-efficacy as motivators: Understanding individuals’ long-term use of health information. Journal of Communication, 51(4), 633–654. Rimal, R. N. & Real, K. (2003). Perceived risk and efficacy beliefs as motivators of change: Use of the risk perception attitude (RPA) framework to understand health behaviors. Human Communication Research, 29(3), 370–399. Romer, D. & Jamieson, P. (2001). The role of perceived risk in starting and stopping smoking. In P. Slovic (Ed.), Smoking: Risk, perception and policy (pp. 64–80). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. 90 K. A. McComas Rowe, G., Frewer, L., & Sjoberg, L. (2000). Newspaper reporting of hazards in the UK and Sweden. Public Understanding of Science, 9(1), 59–78. Rowe, G. & Frewer, L. J. (2000). Public participation methods: A framework for evaluation. Science Technology & Human Values, 25(1), 3–29. Sachsman, D. B., Simon, J., & Valenti, J. M. (2004). Risk and the environment reporters: A four-region analysis. Public Understanding of Science, 13(4), 399–416. Salmon, C. T., Park, H. S., & Wrigley, B. J. (2003). Optimistic bias and perceptions of bioterrorism in Michigan corporate spokespersons, Fall 2001. Journal of Health Communication, 8, 130–143. Santos, S. L. & Chess, C. (2003). Evaluating citizen advisory boards: The importance of theory and participant-based criteria and practical implications. Risk Analysis, 23(2), 269–279. Savadori, L., Savio, S., Nicotra, E., Rumiati, R., Finucane, M., & Slovic, P. (2004). Expert and public perception of risk from biotechnology. Risk Analysis, 24(5), 1289–1299. Scherer, C. W. & Cho, H. C. (2003). A social network contagion theory of risk perception. Risk Analysis, 23(2), 261–267. Scherer, C. W., McComas, K. A., Juanillo, N., & Pelstring, L. (1999). Promoting informed decision-making: The role of message structure. Risk: Health, Safety & Environment, 10, 209–220. Shrum, L. J. (2001). Mainstreaming, resonance, and impersonal impact—testing moderators of the cultivation effect for estimates of crime risk. Human Communication Research, 27(2), 187–215. Siegrist, M., Cvetkovich, G., & Roth, C. (2000). Salient value similarity, social trust, and risk=benefit perception. Risk Analysis, 20(3), 353–362. Siegrist, M., Cvetkovich, G. T., & Gutscher, H. (2001). Shared values, social trust, and the perception of geographic cancer clusters. Risk Analysis, 21(6), 1047–1053. Siegrist, M., Earle, T. C., & Gutscher, H. (2003). Test of a trust and confidence model in the applied context of electromagnetic field (EMF) risks. Risk Analysis, 23(4), 705–716. Siegrist, M., Gutscher, H., & Earle, T. C. (2005). Perception of risk: The influence of general trust, and general confidence. Journal of Risk Research, 8(2), 145–156. Sinaceur, M., Heath, C., & Cole, S. (2005). Emotional and deliberative reactions to a public crisis—Mad Cow disease in France. Psychological Science, 16(3), 247–254. Sjoberg, L. (2001). Limits of knowledge and the limited importance of trust. Risk Analysis, 21(1), 189–198. Slovic, P. (1987). Perception of risk. Science, 236(17 April), 280–285. Slovic, P. (1993). Perceived risk, trust, and democracy. Risk Analysis, 13(6), 675–682. Slovic, P. (1999). Trust, emotion, sex, politics, and science: Surveying the risk-assessment battlefield (Reprinted from Environment, ethics, and behavior, pp. 277–313, 1997), Risk Analysis, 19(4), 689–701. Slovic, P. (Ed.) (2000). Perception of risk. London: Earthscan Publications. Slovic, P. (Ed.) (2001). Smoking: Risk, perception and policy. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2004). Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Analysis, 24(2), 311–322. Stern, P. C. & Fineberg, H. V. (Eds.) (1996). Understanding risk: Informing decisions in a democratic society. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Trumbo, C. W. (1996). Examining psychometrics and polarization in a single-risk case study. Risk Analysis, 16(3), 429–438. Trumbo, C. W. & McComas, K. A. (2003). The function of credibility in information processing for risk perception. Risk Analysis, 23(2), 343–353. Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1974, September 27). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131. Risk Communication: 1996–2005 91 Tyler, T. & Cook, F. (1984). The mass media and judgments of risk: Distinguishing impact on personal and societal level judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 693–708. Verbeke, W., Viaene, J., & Guiot, O. (1999). Health communication and consumer behavior on meat in Belgium: From BSE until dioxin. Journal of Health Communication, 4(4), 345–357. Webler, T. & Tuler, S. (2000). Fairness and competence in citizen participation: Theoretical reflections from a case study. Administration & Society, 32, 566–595. Weinstein, N. D. (1989). Optimistic biases about personal risks. Science, 246, 1232–1233. Witte, K. (1994). Fear control and danger control: A test of the extended parallel process model (EPPM). Communication Monographs, 61(2), 113–134. Witte, K., Cameron, K., McKeon, J. K., & Berkowitz, J. M. (1996). Predicting risk behaviors: Development and validation of a diagnostic scale. Journal of Health Communication, 1(4), 317–342. Zaksek, M. & Arvai, J. L. (2004). Toward improved communication about wildland fire: Mental models research to identify information needs for natural resource management. Risk Analysis, 24(6), 1503–1514.