THE EFFECTS OF MATCHED STIMULATION AND RESPONSE INTERRUPTION AND REDIRECTION ON VOCAL STEREOTYPY Jessica June Love B.A., California State University, Sacramento, 2007 THESIS Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in PSYCHOLOGY (Applied Behavior Analysis) at CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, SACRAMENTO FALL 2010 THE EFFECTS OF MATCHED STIMULATION AND RESPONSE INTERRUPTION AND REDIRECTION ON VOCAL STEREOTYPY A Thesis by Jessica June Love Approved by: _____________________________________________, Committee Chair Caio F. Miguel, Ph.D _____________________________________________, Second Reader Becky Penrod, Ph.D _____________________________________________, Third Reader Daniela Fazzio, Ph.D. _________________________ Date ii Student: Jessica June Love I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. __________________________, Graduate Coordinator JianJian Qin, Ph.D Department of Psychology iii _________________ Date Abstract of THE EFFECTS OF MATCHED STIMULATION AND RESPONSE INTERRUPTION AND REDIRECTION ON VOCAL STEREOTYPY by Jessica June Love Stereotypy has been classified as repetitive behavior that does not serve an apparent function. Two procedures that have been found to effectively reduce rates of vocal stereotypy are Response Interruption and Redirection (e.g., Ahearn, Clark, & MacDonald, 2007) and Matched Stimulation (e.g., Rapp, 2007). The purpose of the study was to evaluate the effects of Response Interruption & Redirection (RIRD) alone, Matched Stimulation (MS) alone, and MS implemented in conjunction with RIRD. Data were analyzed using a multitreatment design. Results for participant one suggested suppressive effects on vocal stereotypy were equivalent for all treatment conditions. For participant two, a slightly greater suppression of stereotypy was associated with MS + RIRD. Both participants exhibited greater frequency of appropriate vocalizations with conditions containing an RIRD component. Session length and number of implementations was also found to be lower during MS + RIRD for one participant. The data suggested that the addition of matched stimulation may facilitate the implementation of RIRD in applied settings; however replication with additional participants is needed. _____________________________________________, Committee Chair Caio F. Miguel, Ph.D ______________________________ Date iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The author would like to thank her advisor and mentor Dr. Caio Miguel for all of his guidance in the development and implementation of the study. She would also like to thank Dr. Becky Penrod for all of her helpful suggestions with the manuscript and design of the study. Also, thank you to Dr. Daniela Fazzio for all of her feedback with the manuscript and defense. Moreover, she would like to recognize Jonathan Fernand for his aid in the data collection of this study. Special thanks to Jillian LaBrie, Timothy Fechter, and the entire Verbal Behavior Lab for direct or indirect support as the study progressed from design to reality. Finally, she would like to thank those who provided support or encouragement during her graduate years at Sac State. v TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Acknowledgements……………………………………………………………………………………… v List of Figures…………………………………………………………………………………………. viii Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION…………………………………………………………………..………....….......1 Etiological Theories………………………………………………………………………….....2 Functions of stereotypy…………………………………………………….………....2 Additional Functions……………………………………………………....……..…...4 Social Significance…………………………………………………………………....………..4 Treatment .....................………………………………………………………….....……….....7 Antecedent Interventions……………………………………………...……….……..7 Consequence Interventions………..…………………………………….…………....9 2. METHOD……………………………………………………………………………...…..…...…...17 Participants, Settings, and Materials………………………….……………………...........….17 Stimulus Preference Assessment..............................................................................................18 Response Measurement............................................................................................................20 Functional Analysis..................................................................................................................21 Assessment Design....................................................................................................21 Assessment Conditions..............................................................................................22 Assessment of Matched Stimuli..............................................................................................24 Assessment Design...................................................................................................24 Assessment Conditions.............................................................................................24 Treatment................................................................................................................................25 Response Measurement………………………...…………………………….…....25 Experimental Design and Conditions……...........………………....……………....26 Social Validity………………………………………..............…………...…...…..32 vi Interobserver Agreement (IOA)…..……….…………...………….….………........33 Treatment Integrity……………………..………………….………...………..........34 3. RESULTS……………………………………………...………………………...…...……….........36 Stimulus Preference Assessment…………………...………………..………..……...…..…..36 Functional Analyses (FA)………………………………...……………...……....……...........37 Matched Stimuli Assessment (MSA)...………………………………...………...…………..39 Treatment…………………………...……………………...………………………...….......................43 Social Validity……………………………………………………………..……………..………….....51 4. DISCUSSION………………………...………………………………………..…..………….…....54 Implications…..…………………………………………..……………………....………......56 Limitations..……………………………………………………..,…………….......……........57 Conclusions………..…………………………………...……………………..……...…........59 Future Research……….……………………………………...………………….....…….......59 Appendix A. Modified RAISD Intake Form……..…………………………………..…..……….…...62 Appendix B. Preference Assessment Data Sheet……………………………………..…...…………..63 Appendix C. Functional Analysis Data Sheet……………………………………………....………....64 Appendix D. Experiment Data Sheet…………………………………………………........……….....65 Appendix E. VRIRD List 1………………………………………………………………........……....66 Appendix F VRIRD List 2………………………………………………………………..…..….…....67 Appendix G VRIRD List 3……………………………………………………………..…..………....68 Appendix H. Social Validity VRIRD Scale……………………………………………..…..………...69 Appendix I. Social Validity MS Scale…………………………………………………..…..………...70 Appendix J. Treatment Integrity RIRD DATA Sheet…………………………………….…..….........71 Appendix K. Treatment Integrity MS DATA Sheet………….…………………………….…..……..72 REFERENCES……………………………………………………………………………….….…...73 vii LIST OF FIGURES Page 1. Figure 1 Functional Analysis data – Ivan and Troy ………………………………………….........38 2. Figure 2 Matched Stimulation Assessment data –Ivan………………………………………..........40 3. Figure 3 Matched Stimulation Assessment data –Troy……………………………………….........42 4. Figure 4 Vocal Stereotypy and Appropriate Vocalizations data – Ivan..........……,,,…...................47 5. Figure 5 Vocal Stereotypy and Appropriate Vocalizations data – Troy.…...............……….....…..48 6. Figure 6 (top) Average RIRD Implementations – Both participants………………….…...……....50 7. Figure 6 (bottom) Average Session Length data – Both participants……………...….…………...50 8. Figure 7 Social Validity for RIRD and Matched Stimulation……………….……………………..53 viii 1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION The term stereotypy is used to define behavior that is characterized by repetitive movements that do not appear to serve an adaptive function (Baumeister & Forehand, 1973; Berkson & Davenport, 1962). Repetitive behaviors are present in typically developing infants (Kravitz, & Boehm, 1971; Thelen, 1981; Berkson, Tupa, & Sherman, 2001; Berkson, 2002) and are comprised of various topographies such as body rocking, head rolling, and complex hand movements (Maclean, Ellis, Galbreath, Halpern, Baumeister, 1991). Typically, engaging in repetitive behaviors does not become a problem unless performed excessively. Berkson (1967) suggests two broad categories under the term “stereotypy,” specifically repetitive movements such as handflapping, as well as non-repetitive movements such as limb and muscle tensing. Although the term stereotypy is commonly used in behavior analysis to refer to specific behavioral topographies, the term does not imply any particular function. Hagopian and Toole (2009) for instance, suggest that the word ‘‘stereotypy’’ should be reserved only for referring to behavior that has been demonstrated to be maintained by automatic reinforcement namely, response produced stimuli (Vaughan & Michael, 1982). Despite this suggestion, the term stereotypy continues to be loosely used to describe topographic qualities of behavior (e.g., repetitive, rhythmical). This poses a problem for those in the field of behavior analysis who treat stereotypy because there is no general consensus for a precise definition. Currently the term “stereotypy” continues to be used as a label for a structural response class (defined by its repetitive characteristics) as opposed to a functional class (defined by maintenance through specific consequences the behavior(s) produce). From a behavioral analytic perspective, this is an issue to be resolved; as many behaviors although topographically identical can serve different functions both within and across individuals. Tr Öster (1994) echoes this notion, relating that, “it is conceivable that certain forms of stereotypy adopt specific functions depending on their own particular sensory and/or social consequences, so that they are 2 controlled by specific discriminative stimuli.”(p. 82). Such notions are supported by Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman and Richman (1982/1994) as findings regarding functions of self-injury varied across individuals as well. Etiological Theories Regarding the etiology of stereotypical behavior, there are many accounts as to its initial acquisition and function(s). Two of the most widely accepted viewpoints are homeostasis and operant interpretations. The homeostasis interpretation assumes that stereotypy increases arousal in understimulating environments and decreases it in overstimulating environments (Willemsen-Swinkels, Buitelaar, Dekker, & Van Engeland, 1998). The operant account describes stereotypy as an early response in an infant’s repertoire that is first maintained through automatic reinforcement, but may also later serve to produce other social consequences such as caregiver attention or the presentation of tangible items (e.g., Thelen, 1987). Functions of Stereotypy Stereotypy often appears to be automatically maintained by consequences of a sensory nature. Behavior-analytic research suggests that stereotypical responding functions to provide sensory input to an individual, either because of too little or too much environmental input (Lovaas, Newsom, & Hickman, 1987). These consequences may be so reinforcing that opportunities to engage in them may even serve as reinforcers for other behaviors (Hung, 1978; Wolery, Kirk, & Gast, 1985, Charlop-Christy & Haymes, 1996, 1998; Charlop, Kurtz, & Casey, 1990). Lovaas, Newsome and Hickman (1987) described what they called “perceptual reinforcement” as explanation for the maintenance of stereotypical behaviors. They proposed that behaviors classified in a stereotypical response class were operant responses which were maintained by either interoceptive or exteroceptive perceptual consequences. These consequences were asserted to be produced by physically engaging in the behavior itself, or a product from an individual engaging with a particular arrangement of external stimuli in the environment (e.g., spinning a plate to produce visual stimulation). 3 Although not a conceptually original work, Lovaas et al. (1987) was a significant publication regarding stereotypic behavior. It provided a more complete theory of stereotypy’s development and characteristics through specific reference to behavioral principles. The notion of automatic reinforcement was first communicated by Skinner in previous works, for example, Verbal Behavior in 1957. Later, Vaughn and Michael (1982) formally extended and clarified Skinner’s work on automatic reinforcement. They defined automatic reinforcement as “reinforcement that is not mediated by the deliberate action of another person” (p. 219). Lovaas et al. suggested the term of perceptual reinforcement to help explain the contingencies which might lead to the acquisition and maintenance of stereotypic behavior. Their emphasis was placed on the idea that stereotypy is a learned behavior and may be initially reinforced by the social community; but later maintained without mediation by others. Although an interesting argument, currently there are no studies to support the notion that stereotypy is initially acquired through instruction by the social community. One rationale for this idea is supported by the highly elaborate and idiosyncratic properties of stereotypy. Because of the wide variation of stereotypic responses; it is difficult to discard environmental variables that contribute to shaping or maintenance of such complex behavior. Examples included repetition of vocal phrases and songs; thought initially to be responses that were taught via reinforcement by the social environment. However, when the individual(s) contact the sensory consequences of engaging in specific behaviors, sensory consequences may later compete and effectively function to reinforce and maintain the behavior(s). Overall, the main argument was that the authors did not feel there was enough evidence in favor of the notion that stereotypical behavior could be maintained by social consequences. Rapp and Vollmer (2005) later refined the perceptual reinforcement theory and related that stereotypical behavior may be explained through more parsimonious means. Specifically, their conceptualization of the maintenance of stereotypy was called, “The automatic reinforcement hypothesis” They described several key features of stereotypy that supports this hypothesis. They are: (1) stereotypy persists in the absence of social consequences (2) environmental enrichment is correlated 4 with reductions of stereotypy (3) reduction or elimination of the sensory product(s) of stereotypy produces a reduction in stereotypy itself (4) contingent access to stereotypy functions as reinforcement for other behaviors and (5) restricting access to or providing prior access to stereotypy results in subsequent increases or decreases, suggesting that stereotypy is affected by motivating operations (p. 530). Additional Functions Although many authors suggest that the primary function of stereotypy is automatic reinforcement, it is important to note that many have shown that stereotypic behavior can serve multiple functions including social positive/negative consequences (e.g., Durand & Carr, 1987; Mace, Browder & Lin, 1987; Mace & Belfiore, 1990; Kennedy, Meyer, Knowles, and Shukla, 2000; Tang, Patterson, & Kennedy 2003). To further explain, stereotypy may appear and persist in an individual’s repertoire simply due to the reinforcing sensory properties of engaging in the behavior itself; however, the individual may contact other contingencies in the environment. As a result the stereotypical behavior(s) become sensitive to other consequences (i.e., social positive or social negative reinforcement) and may be emitted later under different stimulus conditions. It is also noteworthy to mention that stereotypy may only occur in the presence of specific stimuli (Watters & Wood, 1983; McEntee & Saunders, 1997; Carr, Yarborough, & Langdon, 1997; Van Camp, Lerman, Kelley, Roane, Contrucci, & Vorndran, 2000; Friman, 2000; Rapp, 2004). Social Significance Stereotypic behavior is a socially significant behavior to address as it constitutes a significant portion of the repertoires of individuals with disabilities. In a recent study in 2003, Hall, Thorns, and Oliver found via continuous recording methods that duration of stereotypy averaged as high as 83% of leisure periods and 33% of vocational training periods for 4 students with severe mental retardation. Although stereotypy is not exclusive to individuals with Autism, an increased prevalence of both its rate and intensity of responding characterize the disorder. It is so prevalent, that stereotypy has been a defining feature of Autism since the original description of the disorder in 1943 (Lewis & 5 Bodfish, 1998). Current diagnostic criteria in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth edition, (DSM IV) still includes stereotyped or ritualistic behaviors and patterns of interest, along with social interaction and communication deficits as the three major domains of Autism Spectrum Disorder (Lewis & Bodfish, 1998). It has been asserted that individuals with Autism engage in a significantly higher rate of repetitive behaviors and a greater number of topographies of stereotypy in comparison to individuals with other developmental disorders such as mental retardation (Bodfish, Symons, Parker, & Lewis, 2000). Because of the widespread occurrence of stereotypy in populations with Autism, this warrants the use of participants with such a diagnosis in the proposed study. There are various reasons why stereotypy is a socially significant behavior to target for reduction. It can interfere with learning, compete with more functional responses in the individual’s repertoire, and stigmatize the individual. General detrimental effects of stereotypy have been demonstrated in literature such as Matson, Kiely, and Bamburg (1997). They found that individuals who engage in stereotypy exhibited larger deficits in listener and speaker behavior, performing daily living activities necessary for upkeep of a safe and clean home environment, developing interpersonal relationships, and engaging in functional, leisure activities alone or with others. To further expand the argument of general social significance, stereotypy may constitute a large percentage of an individual’s repertoire. For example, Repp and Barton (1980) found during a comparison observation of licensed versus unlicensed residential units for individuals with mental retardation; clients in licensed housing were engaging in self-stimulatory behavior in a range between 7% and 47% during the day; and an average of 27% for unlicensed cottages. Competition with engagement in appropriate behaviors in general was also demonstrated by Repp, Karsh, Deitz and Singh (1992). By observing the behavior of individuals with developmental disabilities consistently over several days, they found that stereotypic behaviors constituted 22% to 74% of individuals’ time. Previous literature has also demonstrated a negative relationship between engagement of stereotypy and acquisition of academic skills. Koegel and Covert (1972) in particular demonstrated that 6 stereotypy shared an inverse relationship with correct discrimination trials. Results indicated that during discrimination training for when and when not to engage in lever pressing, correct discrimination responses did not occur until a punishment procedure was implemented contingent upon engagement in stereotypy. Similar findings of detrimental relations of stereotypy and correct responding were demonstrated in Morrison and Rosales (1997) for a counting task. Stereotypic responses can also directly compete and displace socially appropriate behaviors such as toy play. For example, the mouthing of leisure items can compete with appropriate interaction with toys (Tarbox, Tarbox, Ghezzi & Wallace, 2007; Epstein, Doke, Sajwaj, Sorrel & Rimmer, 1974). In addition, Koegel, Firestone and Dunlap (1974) demonstrated that unreinforced, spontaneous, appropriate play increased when self-stimulatory behavior was suppressed. Durand and Carr (1987) also related that stereotypical behavior may preclude integration into nonsegregated environments as well as occasion social stigmatization. Regarding vocal stereotypy specifically, it is often considered a concern to caregivers because it can be disruptive to others around the individual (Athens, Vollmer, Sloman, Pipkin, 2008). Loftin, Odon, and Lantz (2008) findings support this notion as they experimentally demonstrated an inverse relationship between social initiations and engagement in repetitive motor responses. Additional literature demonstrating the effects stereotypy on social interaction was conducted by Matson, Minshawi, Gonzalez, and Mayville, (2006). Their study contained 120 participants, which consisted of individuals with self-injurious behavior (SIB), stereotypy, or neither SIB nor stereotypy (control group). They found that individuals who exhibited stereotypy (N = 30) had significantly fewer general positive social skills than the control group (N = 30). They also found that the comorbidity of both SIB and stereotypy amplified this negative effect on social skills. Finally, a more recent demonstration of stereotypy’s effects on social interaction was described by Lee, Odom and Loftin (2007). They evaluated the effectiveness of a peer-training intervention strategy to increase social skills in three children with Autism. During baseline the children with Autism emitted high rates of stereotypy and low rates of social interaction. After implementation of a peer 7 prompting strategy, results not only showed an increase in social interactions for the children, but also a concomitant decrease in simultaneous motor/vocal stereotypical responding. Treatment Because functions of behaviors vary across individuals, Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) is an ideal science to foster the development of treatment for stereotypy. ABA uses systematic collection of data and takes a functional approach toward behavior. As a result of such methods, many types of treatment for stereotypy have been developed and have been demonstrated to be effective. Some treatments also address other repetitive behaviors such as tics and SIB. Studies regarding other behaviors similar to stereotypy will be included as some researchers (e.g., Sprague, Holland, & Thomas, 1997) assert that both stereotypy and other types of repetitive behavior such SIB can manifest as rhythmic and/or repetitious patterns. Because of the essential topographical, functional, and developmental similarities they will be included in the review. Treatments can be divided into three broad categories, antecedent and consequence interventions, and a combination of both (packages). Antecedent interventions are those types of treatments in which the practitioner or experimenter seeks to control behavior through manipulation of environmental events before the individual has the opportunity to engage in the target behavior. In contrast, consequence interventions involve the delivery of a consequence contingent upon individual(s) engaging in the target behavior. Antecedent Interventions The main types of antecedent interventions are: environmental enrichment, physical exercise, non-contingent reinforcement, matched stimulation. Antecedent manipulations are stimulus conditions that are arranged before a behavior has an opportunity to occur. Their purpose is to arrange stimulus conditions that decrease the likelihood of target behavior occurrence. Environmental enrichment. Environmental enrichment involves introduction of a variety of activities and objects in the environment. Results in previous literature are mixed, some demonstrating a decrease in stereotypy as more opportunities to engage in activities are available (e.g., Berkson, Mason 8 & Saxon 1963; Horner, 1980; Vollmer, Marcus & Le Blanc, 1994; Ringdahl, Vollmer, Marcus & Roane; 1997) and others (e.g., Duker & Rasing, 1989; McEntee & Saunders,1997) have found results counterintuitive to this notion. Physical exercise. Other treatments involve the modification of the individual’s internal environment, specifically through physical exercise. Several studies examined the effects of jogging on stereotypy (Kern, Koegal, Dyer, Dunlap, & Fenton, 1982; Kern, Koegal, & Dunlap, 1984). Other literature investigated the effects of antecedent exercise by evaluating other activities such as roller skating (Powers, Thibadeau, & Rose, 1992) or playing with leisure items such as rubber playground balls (Watters & Watters, 1980). Non-contingent reinforcement. Another antecedent manipulation that has been used for the successful reduction of stereotypic behavior is noncontingent reinforcement (NCR). The term noncontingent reinforcement (NCR) refers to the delivery of an aberrant behavior’s known reinforcer on a response-independent basis (Carr, Coriaty, Wilder, Gaunt, Dozier, Britton, Avina & Reed, 2000). Treatment effects of NCR after one year of implementation have been demonstrated across a variety of repetitive behaviors such as SIB ( e.g.,Lindberg, Iwata, Roscoe, Worsdell, & Hanley, 1997) as well as stereotypy (e.g., Britton, Carr, Landaburu, & Romick, 2002). Noncontingent reinforcement has also been considered to increase the efficacy of other procedures such as extinction for the reduction of other aberrant behaviors such as aggression (O’Reilly, et al., 2006; 2007). Matched stimulation. A behavioral intervention for reduction of stereotypy that can be even theoretically framed as non-contingent reinforcement is matched stimulation. Matched stimulation is based on the theoretical premise that some reinforcers may be considered “highly substitutable” and can be readily traded for each other due to their functional similarity (Green & Freed, 1993). The mechanism for the reduction of stereotypy is theorized to be that if an item’s properties can effectively substitute for the products of stereotypy, it serves as an abolishing operation for those consequences. An abolishing operation (AO) is a motivating operation that decreases the reinforcing effectiveness of a stimulus, object, or event (Cooper, Heron & Heward, 2007, p. 689). 9 Matched stimulation has been in practice since the late 1970’s (Rincover, Cook, Peoples, & Packard, 1979) and is also still part of more current interventions (Higbee, Change & Endicott, 2005; Goh, Iwata, Shore, Deleon, Lerman, Ulrich & Smith, 1995; Piazza, Fisher, Hanley, LeBlanc, Worsdell, Lindauer, & Keeney, 1998; Fisher, Lindauer, Alterson, & Thompson 1998; Piazza, Adelinis, Hanley, Goh, & Delia, 2000; Rapp, 2006, 2007). Again, although considerable literature has demonstrated the efficacy of treatment effects for matched stimulation some studies relate no such effects (e.g., Ahearn, Clark, Debar, & Florentino, 2005). Consequence Interventions Consequence interventions are those which involve the delivery of some type of stimulus or consequence contingent upon a behavior. The aim of consequence interventions is to create a learning history for the individual so that if similar stimulus conditions are encountered in the future, the behavior is less likely to occur. Many consequence interventions that will be discussed involve some form of punishment. Punishment is defined as the process of arranging a consequence for response that makes responding less likely. The stimulus specifically arranged as the consequence is labeled a punisher (Catania, 2007). Lerman and Vorndran (2002) argue that punishment may be critical to the treatment of some problem behaviors when the precise source of reinforcement cannot be identified or controlled. Stereotypy is a prime candidate for punishment interventions; as it is often difficult or impossible to control delivery of the reinforcer. This may be because the reinforcer is a sensory consequence, or completely unknown to the clinician/experimenter. Although punishment has been widely studied and effectively utilized, there is overall agreement that interventions should be designed to be as minimally intrusive or restrictive as possible (Vorndran & Lerman, 2006). Early punishment. Early consequence interventions involved the delivery of intrusive stimuli. These stimuli were assumed to function as punishers and included contingent shock, verbal reprimands, and even physically shaking the participant (e.g., Baumeister & Forehand, 1972). More humane interventions such as overcorrection were found to be effective (e.g., Epstein, Doke, Sajwaj, Sorrell, & 10 Rimmer, 1974; Wells, Forehand, Hickey, & Green, 1977; Harris & Wolchik, 1979) even when directly compared to more intrusive methods such as contingent shock and physical slapping (Fox & Azrin, 1973). Response cost. Response cost is the contingent removal of a preferred stimulus (Keeney, Fisher, Adelinis, & Wilder, 2000). It has been demonstrated to be effective either alone or in treatment packages to reduce a variety of problem behaviors such as: aggression through the contingent removal of preferred music (Keeney et al., 2000); SIB through the removal of attention contingent upon participant engagement in SIB; academic off-task behavior in children with ADHD through the removal of points (Jurbergs, Palcic, & Kelley, 2007); and destructive behavior for a child with Autism by contingent removal of tokens (Foxx & Meindl, 2007). Response cost has been utilized in more recent studies and has been found to be effective (e.g., Keeney, Fisher, Adelinis, & Wilder, 2000; Falcomata, Roane, Hovanetz & Kettering, 2004; Jurbergs, Palcic, & Kelley, 2007; Foxx & Meindl, 2007; Athens, Vollmer, Sloman, & Pipkin, 2008). Differential punishment. Response cost has also been utilized to differentially punish vocal stereotypy; thereby bringing the aberrant behavior under stimulus control. Stimulus control is exerted through the repeated presentation of the punisher in the presence of an external stimulus, to the extent that such pairings eventually inhibit the punished behavior in the presence of that stimulus (for further explanation, see Rilling, 1977). Punishing stereotypy in one environment and allowing it to occur in another may seem counterintuitive. Some may argue that stereotypy should never be allowed to occur; however, one may also argue that stereotypy is inappropriate only in specific contexts. It has even been demonstrated to provide some benefits, as studies have shown that contingent access to stereotypy can be used as a reinforcing event (e.g., Charlop, Kurtz, & Casey, 1990; Hanley, Iwata, Thompson, & Lindberg, 2000). If an individual has a limited leisure skill repertoire and limited array of stimuli available to effectively function as reinforcers for other behaviors, access to stereotypy may be utilized as a reinforcer during educational programming. Examples of interventions that successfully brought stereotypy under control 11 of specific stimuli are: Brusa and Richman (2008) and Rapp, Patel, Ghezzi, O’Flaherty, and Titterington (2009). Sensory extinction. Sensory extinction is a procedure in which attempts are made to mask or remove certain sensory consequences. The procedure was originally developed by Rincover (1979) in attempts to reduce “self stimulatory” behavior in three children: two with Autism and one with profound mental retardation. Rincover utilized procedures to mask or remove the auditory, visual, and proprioceptive consequences that were hypothesized to maintain stereotypic responding for each participant. For example, auditory feedback from one participant’s plate spinning was removed by carpeting the table on which the plate was set. Rincover was successful in reducing all participants’ stereotypical behavior through variations of sensory extinction, and other researchers found the procedure to be useful for object dropping (Dalrymple, 1989) and vocal stereotypy (Aiken & Salzberg, 1984). Response blocking. In the past, response blocking was thought to be a possible variation of sensory extinction (Reid, Parsons, Philips, & Green, 1993). However, it is not typical of most extinction procedures. Response blocking is a procedure in which the target behavior is permitted to occur but the individual is prevented (usually by physical means) from contacting subsequent reinforcement (Lerman & Iwata, 1996). In some circumstances, only the precursor to a behavior is allowed to occur before blocking is implemented. Some argue that an alternative interpretation of the effects of response blocking is that the physical contact associated with the procedure could function as a punishing stimulus (Lerman & Iwata, 1996). Response blocking has been demonstrated to be effective for the reduction of an array of repetitive behaviors that are at least partially maintained by automatic reinforcement. Such behaviors include but are not limited to: eye gouging (Lalli, Livezey & Kates, 1996) and handmouthing (Reid, Parsons, Philips, & Green, 1993; Lerman & Iwata, 1996; Carr, Dozier, Patel, Adams & Martin, 2002). Response blocking can also evoke detrimental side effects such as decreased object interaction (Lerman, Kelley, Vorndran, & VanCamp, 2003) and aggression (Hagopian & Adelinis, 2001; Hagopian & Toole, 2009). 12 Differential reinforcement of other behavior (DRO). DRO is a laboratory-based schedule in which reinforcement is delivered if a particular response has not been emitted for a specified interval of time (Barton, Brulle, & Repp, 1987). It has been demonstrated as effective for reducing handflapping, headweaving, (Barton, Bulle, & Repp, 1987; Aurand, Sisson, Aach, & Hassalt, 1989) and vocal stereotypy (Taylor, Hoch, & Weissman, 2005; Rozenblat, Brown, Reeve & Reeve, 2009). Response interruption and redirection (RIRD). The consequence intervention most relevant to this study is response interruption and redirection. RIRD has been shown to be effective in suppressing both motor (Fisher, Lindauer, Alterson & Thompson, 1998; Paisey, Whitney, & Wainczak, 1993; Thompson, Fisher, Piazza, & Kuhn, 1998) and vocal stereotypy (Ahearn, Clark, & MacDonald, 2007; Miguel, Clark, Tereshko, & Ahearn, 2009). Given that response interruption in conjunction with other treatments (noncontingent reinforcement, differential reinforcement of incompatible behavior) has been shown to be effective for reducing motor stereotypy (Fisher, Lindauer, Alterson & Thompson, 1998; Paisey, Whitney, & Wainczak, 1993; Thompson, Fisher, Piazza, & Kuhn, 1998) it will be discussed more in depth regarding the reduction of vocal stereotypy; as this is the topic of interest for this study. Only three studies on RIRD for the treatment of vocal stereotypy (Ahearn, Clark, & and MacDonald, 2007; Miguel, Clark, Tereshko, & Ahearn, 2009; Liu-Gitz & Banda, 2010) have been conducted. Ahearn et al. conducted a study with 4 participants: 2 boys and 2 girls diagnosed with Autism. They were selected for vocal stereotypy that interfered with participation in educational activities or occurred at unacceptable levels outside class. Their ages ranged from 3 to 11-years old. Functional analyses indicated that two participant’s vocal stereotypy was maintained in the absence of social consequences with a clear differentiation of highest levels in the alone condition; the other two participants displayed a lack of differentiation in responding, but highest levels occurred during the play and alone conditions, suggesting an automatic function. The effects of response interruption and redirection (RIRD) were tested in an ABAB withdrawal design. Sessions were five minutes long; however, the session clock was stopped for the entire duration that RIRD was implemented. RIRD consisted of the immediate delivery of three 13 demands either in the form of vocal imitation or social questions, contingent upon participant engagement in vocal stereotypy. Demands continued to be delivered until the participant complied with three consecutive demands in the absence of stereotypy. Social praise was delivered at the end of the third consecutive demand, and appropriate vocalizations were honored (if possible) during all sessions. Results indicated that stereotypy reduced for all participants as a function of implementing RIRD; however, appropriate vocalizations increased for only three out of four participants. In addition, maintenance probes in the natural environment displayed significant treatment gains for three participants: participant one’s pretreatment levels reduced from a pretreatment range of 33% and 44%, to a post-treatment range of 1% and 4% in the natural environment. Participant three displayed a pretreatment range of 25% and 77% and the post-treatment range subsequently reduced to 3% and 13%. The fourth participant engaged in stereotypy during pretreatment between 54% and 78% of the time, and during post-treatment engaged in stereotypy for 16% and 24% of observations. Probe data was not available for participant two. In a follow-up study, Miguel, Clark, Tereshko, and Ahearn (2009), evaluated the effects of RIRD as the sole intervention component compared with RIRD in conjunction with a Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors (SSRIs) medication. The specific SSRIs medication under examination was sertraline (Zoloft®) and an ABABC reversal design was used. The participant was a 4-year-old boy diagnosed with Autism. A functional analysis suggested maintenance of stereotypy via automatic reinforcement. During baseline conditions there were no programmed consequences for stereotypy, however the medication of 10 mg of Sertraline was present. Treatment for vocal stereotypy was then instated through the subsequent treatment phase through the implementation of RIRD (while the medication was still in place.) The next treatment condition faded out sertraline over the course of five days and RIRD was still used as a consequence for stereotypy. Follow up sessions were then conducted with implementation of RIRD in the absence of sertraline; to evaluate the effects of the efficacy of RIRD with the reduced likelihood that traces of sertraline were present in the participant. Results indicated that stereotypy during the presence of only sertraline was averaging at 49%, and an immediate decrease to 14 averages around 11% was observed when RIRD was introduced in the following condition. In addition, appropriate vocalizations averaged 9.3 occurrences per session and increased to 22.4 across all sessions of RIRD. A reversal following this condition displayed recovery of baseline levels of vocal stereotypy. During the fading of RIRD effects were replicated as there was again an immediate decrease in vocal stereotypy and increase in appropriate vocalizations. Follow up sessions confirmed the unlikelihood that sertraline was influencing responding; as vocal stereotypy remained low and appropriate vocalizations maintained rates at previous treatment levels. More recently, Liu-Gitz & Banda (2010) replicated Ahearn et al,. by evaluating the effectiveness of the RIRD on vocal stereotypy for a ten-year old boy with Autism in an educational setting. Findings indicated that RIRD effectively reduced vocal stereotypy. Its suppressive effects were replicated within the participant through use of an ABAB reversal design. This study further expanded research on RIRD by using social questions related to the participant’s preferred topics (e.g., “Who do you like in Toy Story?”). The procedure was also implemented by the participant’s teacher, thereby providing more evidence for feasibility of the implementation in the natural classroom environment. Purpose The aforementioned studies on the treatment of stereotypy which are most relevant to this study are those which examined efficacy of matched stimulation (e.g., Rapp, 2006, 2007) and RIRD for either vocal (e.g., Ahearn et al., 2007; Miguel et al., 2009; Liu-Gitz & Banda, 2010) or motor stereotypy (e.g., Fisher, Lindauer, Alterson & Thompson, 1998; Paisey, Whitney, & Wainczak, 1993; Thompson, Fisher, Piazza, & Kuhn, 1998). It seems that there are no treatment package analyses of both matched stimulation (MS) and RIRD components for the reduction of vocal stereotypy. To date, there are only three published studies on RIRD for the treatment of vocal stereotypy (Ahearn et al., 2007; Liu-Gitz, 2010; Miguel et al., 2009) and none which specifically evaluate both RIRD and MS treatments in conjunction. Matched stimulation and response interruption and redirection to toy manipulation have been evaluated together as a treatment package for motor stereotypy (Cicero, 2007) but none to date have evaluated the package’s efficacy for vocal stereotypy. 15 Hypothetically, access to matched stimulation produces a reduction in vocal stereotypy due its abolishing effect for the auditory stimulation produced by engagement in vocal stereotypy. Rapp’s (2006) findings support the notion of the presence of MOs for products of stereotypy in that participant post session levels of stereotypy were higher after implementation of response blocking; as opposed to low levels after access to matched stimulation. O’Reilly, Sigafoos, Edrisinha, Lancioni, Cannella, Choi, and Baretto (2006) evaluated the effects of antecedent manipulations alone and in conjunction with extinction (EXT). Results of their study also indicated that problem behavior occurred at higher levels during extinction sessions when participants did not have prior access to the reinforcers. Later studies by O’Reilly and colleague (2007, 2008) replicated these findings with additional participants and under more naturalistic conditions. Taken together, these results advocate the supplementation of NCR to either a punishment or extinction procedure. In light of these findings, this study will seek to extend the literature that has used combinations of response interruption and an NCR procedure on motor stereotypy (e.g., Lerman, Kelley, Vorndran, and Van Camp, 2003; Thompson et al., 1998; Fisher, Lindauer, Alterson, and Thompson, 1998; Carr, Dozier, Patel, Adams, & Martin, 2002) to the behavior of vocal stereotypy. The matched stimulation in conjunction with RIRD condition will be evaluated to determine if matched stimulation can increase the effectiveness of RIRD by (theoretically) abolishing or attenuating the MO for the auditory products of vocal stereotypy. The treatment phase comprised solely of RIRD will experimentally demonstrate whether vocal stereotypy will remain suppressed; given that the MO will again be present. There are several purposes of this study. The first is to extend treatment packages comprised of response interruption and NCR for reduction of motor stereotypy (e.g., Lerman, Kelley, Vorndran, and Van Camp, 2003; Thompson et al., 1998; Fisher, Lindauer, Alterson, and Thompson, 1998; Carr, Dozier, Patel, Adams, & Martin, 2002; Cicero, 2007) to the behavior of vocal stereotypy. Next, it will also extend sparse literature which has evaluated RIRD for the treatment of vocal stereotypy (Ahearn et al., 2007; Miguel et al., 2009; Liu-Gitz & Banda, 2010). It will also evaluate effects of antecedent manipulations on the efficacy of punishment procedures (RIRD specifically) as opposed to extinction 16 procedures (O’Reilly et al., 2006; 2007; 2008). Finally, it will evaluate relative efficacy of MS and RIRD treatment components both alone and in combination for the suppression of vocal stereotypy. This will be accomplished through a component analysis within a reversal design. 17 Chapter 2 METHOD Participants, Setting and Materials Participants were two children with a diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) (DSM IVTR, 2005, American Psychiatric Association, 2005). Participants were recruited through direct caregiver contact. Both participants were able to communicate vocally and could sit for a minimum of 5 minutes at a desk or chair in the absence of aggression (for example, hitting, kicking, punching, etc.) or self-injury (e.g., scratching, hitting, biting him/herself.) Finally, both participants were found to engage in vocal stereotypy that was at least partially maintained by automatic reinforcement as determined by a functional analysis (Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman and Richman, 1982/1994). Participants were referred to the study by parents because the repetitive language was intrusive in either school settings, public settings, or both. Ivan was an 8 year old male whose primary means of communication was vocal behavior. He was able to request for items using the mand frame, “I want _____,” but was not observed to do so unless the item was very highly preferred. He could tact colors, numbers, count, and his fine motor skills were that of typical peers. His parents reported that language deficits and vocal stereotypy were the areas containing his greatest deficits and that his academic skills were comparable to typical peers. His sessions were conducted in the parent’s home located in Folsom, California. Sessions were conducted in the participant’s bedroom. The room was 9 by 11 feet and was equipped with a bed, a small table and two chairs, various leisure items, and dual-mirrored closet doors. Ivan was not on any kind of medications throughout the duration of the study. Sessions were conducted 2-3 days per week, and total session duration varied depending on the condition. Troy was a 9 year old male who was able to request for items using full sentences (e.g., “I want Elmo please.”). He could also tact colors, count, and identify numbers. His parents reported that his general academic abilities were comparable to his same-age peers. He did not take any medication for 18 the duration of the study, and sessions were conducted in the experimental room at the university. The room was 7 by 10 feet and equipped with a table and two chairs, a bulletin board, and equipped with a video camera and toys. Sessions were conducted 3 days per week, and total session duration varied depending on the condition. Stimulus Preference Assessment The preference assessment was used to identify potentially reinforcing items in a systematic manner; as reinforcement effects are often idiosyncratic across individuals (Vollmer & Iwata, 1991). Before any assessments began, three sessions of a multiple stimulus without replacement (MSWO) preference assessment were conducted. The procedure was based on those by DeLeon and Iwata (1996). Some items used for the preference assessments were identified via a modified Reinforcer Assessment for Individuals with Severe Disabilities (RAISD) form (Fisher, Piazza, Bowman, & Amari, 1996; see Appendix A). Other items used were supplied by the experimenter and anecdotally observed as preferred. The brief MSWO assessments functioned to identify preferred items pertinent to the individual's current motivating operation(s), prevent satiation of items by identifying several preferred items to rotate between or among sessions, and reaffirm in a systematic manner that items used during previous session(s) remain preferred. The selection of the brief MSWO preference assessment was chosen based on results of Carr, Nicolson, and Higbee (2000). It also served to identify the relative rankings of preference among items. Finally, the brevity of assessments were used to prevent satiation effects during sessions that required leisure items (Egel, 1981; Roane, Vollmer, Ringdahl and Marcus, 1998). Ten items were present for an initial 3 sessions of the brief MSWO. After the initial 3 sessions, the seven items identified as highly and moderately preferred were made available during the daily, single sessions of a brief MSWO. A single brief MSWO was conducted each day before sessions commenced. 19 Leisure items were arranged in a semi-circle on the floor, and the participant was free to manipulate the item(s) of his/her choice. The participant began on the outside of the semi-circle. Prior to a session, the experimenter led the participant around the circle to ensure contact each stimulus. During initial sampling, the experimenter placed the participant’s hand on the items and modeled appropriate item manipulation. After each item was sampled, the participant was moved approximately 0.6 m (about 2 feet) from the assessment area. The therapist then withdrew from the assessment area, and the assessment began. The experimenter then delivered the instruction, “Go play” and the participant selected an item. The experimenter was located beside the stimulus array and presented social attention. Upon selection, participants had 30-s to engage with the item while the experimenter randomly rearranged the remaining items to a new location. Items were rearranged so that they were across the circle from their previous location. Rearrangement of items helped to control for location bias. After the 30-s elapsed, the item was removed from the array. This process continued until only one item remained or no response occurred for 30-s. During the assessment, observers scored item selection by recording the order in which items were chosen. A choice was defined as any participant contact with an item using his or her hand, and manipulating the object for 5 seconds or longer. Only one brief MSWO assessment was conducted each day before sessions, regardless of the number of sessions. Items classified as highly preferred were those that were ranked overall first to third of the pool, while moderately preferred items were items that were overall ranked fourth through seventh. Scoring for the preference assessment was live. Items used for the functional analysis (FA) sessions were highly and moderately preferred toys that did not produce sound. The use of toys that did not produce sounds served to control for potential suppressive effects that these sounds may have exerted on stereotypy levels (e.g., Fisher, Lindauer, Alterson, & Thompson, 1998; Higbee, Chang, & Endicot, 2005; and Rapp 2006, 2007). Highly preferred items that were used during FA sessions were not anecdotally observed to suppress stereotypy. 20 The four items identified as moderately preferred from the daily MSWO were used during the treatment and baseline sessions. Items were rotated to prevent satiation (Egel, 1981). For Ivan, a Mr. Potato Head, mosaic magnet and face-making magnet toys, books and puzzles, animal figurines, coloring books, cars, whiteboard, Dora blocks, and Diego blocks were made available during the preference assessment. The same toys were made available for Troy during the intial MSWO. Before treatment began, new toys were introduced for Troy due to satiation effects of the first set. The second set consisted of a wire ball toy, silly putty, magna doodle, magnet and faces toy, a pin-head toy, cars, “Don’t break the Ice” game, Mr. Potato Head, coloring books, and whiteboard. Response Measurement For Ivan, vocal stereotypy was defined as any instance of noncontextual phrases or repetitions of noncontextual words, phrases or sounds. It included repetitions of phonemes (e.g. “Na na na”) or phrases (e.g. “J is for Jaguar.”), delayed echolalia (e.g., “We go to the Dr. First, then blockbuster”), lip popping and repetitious sounds with a closed mouth (e.g. “bbbbbb” sound with mouth closed but lips vibrate). Sound effects in the absence of appropriate toys (e.g., “Shoo!” or “Vroom”), rhythmic breathing patterns (“huh huh huh” or “heh heh heh”), and noncontextual repetitive “Blowing” (as though blowing out a candle in the absence of appropriate stimuli) were also included. Appropriate vocalizations included requests for items, attention, or breaks. Tacting items (e.g. “Tiger” in the presence of a tiger figurine) and repeating a delivered instruction were also appropriate responses. For Troy, vocal stereotypy was defined as any instance of noncontextual or nonfunctional speech, it included breathing in rhythmic or patterned episodes (“huh huh huh” or “heh heh heh”), “sipping sounds” (breathing inward through pursed lips), and single episodes of the phoneme “MMM”. Repetitions of words, phonemes (k-k-k, tuh tuh tuh), or noncontextual high pitched squeals or squeaks, and reciting of movie or video game scripts were also included. Conversely, appropriate vocalizations included requests for items, attention, or breaks, tacting items, (e.g. “Blue” in the presence of a blue toy), or singing or humming with a tune while playing with toys. 21 For both participants, repetitive mands (e.g., “I want break, I want break”), or tacts (e.g., “Tiger, Tiger”) were not scored as stereotypy unless the phrase was noncontextual (e.g., “Crocodile, Crocodile” when no crocodile was present, or when the participant wasn’t looking at the crocodile while repeating the tact). Data on vocal stereotypy were collected using continuous duration recording as modeled by Ahearn, et al. (2007) (see Appendix B). This consisted of dividing the total number of seconds in which vocal stereotypy occurred by the total number of seconds in the session (300 s) multiplied by 100%. Interobserver agreement was calculated by dividing the number of intervals with agreements by the total number of intervals with agreements plus disagreements and multiplying by 100%. Functional Analysis To experimentally determine the function of the participant’s vocal stereotypy, a functional analysis (FA) was conducted (Iwata et al., 1982/1994). Three conditions were included in the functional analysis in the fixed order: no consequence, attention, and demand. The rationale for the fixed order was to increase the likelihood that the appropriate corresponding motivating operations for each consequence were present for each condition. The purpose of the FA was to determine if the function of vocal stereotypy was not mediated by social consequences; namely, maintained by automatic reinforcement. For the purpose of this study, only participants who had automatically maintained stereotypy were retained for the experiment. Results from the functional analysis were used to systematically infer the function(s) of each participant’s engagement in vocal stereotypy. Previous studies have shown than stereotypy can be maintained by socially mediated consequences (Kennedy, Meyer, Knowles, & Shukla, 2000). Functions of stereotypy were evaluated through visual inspection and comparison of each condition’s data via a scatter-plot graph. Assessment Design Each session was five minutes in duration, as brief sessions in functional analyses have been experimentally demonstrated to yield results equivalent to longer session lengths such as 15 minutes (Wallace & Iwata, 1999). The abbreviated functional analysis was conducted according to procedures by Roscoe, Carreau, MacDonald, and Pence (2008). The same experimenter conducted the all conditions of 22 the functional analysis for both participants. A 2:1 ratio of no consequence conditions per attention and demand was conducted. This ratio was found by Roscoe and colleagues to be effective to more quickly assess the function of a behavior if it is maintained by automatic reinforcement. This method was considered appropriate as both participants’ stereotypy appeared to be insensitive to social consequences during intake observations. Assessment Conditions For both Ivan and Troy, a no consequence condition was implemented based on the procedures of Athens and colleagues (2008). A no consequence condition was used for Ivan instead of a not interaction because parent report indicated that he frequently engaged in stereotypy when playing alone with toys. A no consequence condition was used for Troy because he was frequently observed to elope from the experimental room in the absence of a tangible items or activities. He was also observed to wiggle loose teeth in the absence of an activity. This behavior directly competed with stereotypy, so this modification was deemed appropriate. No Consequence. The purpose of this condition was to evaluate stereotypy in the absence of social consequences. In other words, to test for maintenance though automatic positive or automatic negative reinforcement. The participant had access to two moderately preferred toys that did not produce sound. The experimenter sat in a chair across from the participant and neither talked nor looked at the participant. The person filming was across from the participant and also did not interact with him. Any objects that may have produced sound if hit (e.g., a metal bookshelf) were removed if possible. No edible items were made available. Moderately preferred toys were used so that they would not compete with stereotypy (Ahearn et al., 2005). In other words, no social consequences were delivered if stereotypy occurred. Appropriate vocalizations were not reinforced. If the behavior persists under in the absence of social consequences, evidence is provided that the behavior is possibly maintained by some aspect of the sensory consequences that the behavior(s) produce (e.g., Rincover, 1978; Rincover, Cook, Peoples, & Packard, 1979; Lovaas, Newsom, & 23 Hickman, 1987); or possibly attenuate private stimuli such as aversive physiological stimulation (Van Camp, et al., 2000). Attention. The purpose of this condition test was to test sensitivity of stereotypy to social positive reinforcement. In previous literature, bizarre and repetitive vocalizations have been found by either functional analysis or experimental design to be maintained by or at least sensitive to social consequences such as the delivery of attention (Liberman, Teigen, Patterson, & Baker, 1973; Wilder, Masuda, O’Conner, & Baham, 2001; Arntzen, Tønnessen, & Brouwer, 2006). In this condition, the participant and experimenter sat in a room where two moderately preferred toys (that did not produce sound) were available on a table. Only two toys were made available because previous research has shown that stimulus variation with low to moderately preferred stimuli can compete with highly preferred stimuli (Najdowski, Wallace, Penrod, & Cleveland, 2005). This was to prevent the possibility that a variety of stimuli may compete with attention as a reinforcer. No edible items were made available. The experimenter directed the participant to "play with the toys" while she "does some work." The experimenter sat in a chair across the room and behaved to look as though she was reading a book or magazine. Contingent upon the participant engaging in vocal stereotypy, the experimenter responded with a brief verbal interaction (e.g., “Please be quiet,” or “That’s too loud”) paired with brief physical contact of a non-punitive nature (e.g., hand on shoulder). The experimenter responded to the participant on an FR 1 schedule for each occurrence of vocal stereotypy. All other behaviors were ignored. Appropriate vocalizations were not reinforced during this condition, as was considered implementation of treatment. Demand. The demand condition is designed to approximate stimulus conditions in the natural environment in which vocal stereotypy may be maintained though social negative reinforcement by producing the postponement, reduction, or removal of a demand/task. Previous literature (Durand & Carr, 1987; Durand & Crimmins, 1987; and Mace & Belfiore, 1990) has found a range of aberrant 24 and/or repetitive vocalizations that serve this function or are at least sensitive to the aforementioned social consequences. In the demand condition, the experimenter presented tasks similar to those presented during the participant’s school or day treatment program. Tasks were selected based on caregiver report and consisted of 4 tasks relevant to the participant’s programming at school or in afterschool programs. Tasks were not mastered (e.g. less than 80% correct) and non-vocal to allow physical prompting if needed. Tasks were randomly rotated. Demands for Ivan included: hanging a shirt on a coat hanger, tying a shoe, folding a shirt, and buttoning a shirt. Demands for Troy included: solving 3-digit multiplication problems, tying a shoe, catching a ball, and putting pennies into a box with one hand. A least-to-most prompting hierarchy was used for all task presentations. Contingent upon the participant engaging in stereotypy, the experimenter removed the task for 15 seconds. This procedure was implemented on a reinforcement schedule of FR 1. The experimenter did not reinforce appropriate vocalizations during this condition. No edible items or toys were made available. Assessment of Matched Stimuli Assessment Design The purpose of this pre-experimental procedure was to demonstrate that the items used in the treatment conditions would suppress vocal stereotypy and were therefore appropriately classified as “matched stimuli.” The assessment was conducted in multielement fashion and three conditions were rapidly alternated: baseline, toys with sound, and toys with no sound. Baseline was alternated with one condition at a time to avoid carryover effects. During phase one, baseline was rapidly alternated with toys with sound condition, during phase two, baseline was rapidly alternated with toys with no sound condition. Phase one was then reinstated to replicate effects of matched toys. Assessment Conditions Baseline (no interaction). The purpose of the baseline condition was to evaluate vocal stereotypy levels in the presence of the experimenter only. The experimenter sat in the room with the participant, but did not interact with him/her in any way. No leisure items or materials were present. Any 25 objects in the room were removed which potentially produced sound if hit (e.g., a metal bookshelf). Although sessions began seated in a chair, the participant was free to move about the room and was not redirected back to it. Toys with sound. The purpose of the toys with sound condition was to evaluate vocal stereotypy levels in the presence of toys which produced sound. All items made available produced some form of auditory stimulation; as it was assumed that the auditory stimulation matched or effectively substituted for the sensory consequences typically produced by engaging in vocal stereotypy (Rapp 2007). Four preferred items (as indicated by the preference assessment) were used. These toys were anecdotally observed to be associated with low levels of stereotypy. Two sets comprised of two toys each were evaluated in the matched stimulation assessment. The participant was seated at a table or on the floor and presented with the matched stimuli. If the participant did not manipulate an item (to produce the sound) for a minimum of two seconds, the experimenter manipulated an item to produce auditory stimulation. This functioned to ensure consistent delivery of matched stimulation throughout and across sessions. After demonstrating that in the presence of both sets of toys stereotypy was at least partially suppressed; the two sets were used and rotated during treatment conditions which required a matched stimulation component. Sets were rotated during treatment conditions to avoid satiation effects. Toys without sound. The purpose of the toys without sound condition was to evaluate vocal stereotypy levels in the presence of the same toys as those utilized in the “toys with sound” condition; however the toys’ batteries were removed. This sought to isolate the effects of the toys’ sounds on vocal stereotypy; leaving only tactile and visual stimulation. Treatment Response Measurement Data were collected on five dependent variables for each participant: vocal stereotypy, appropriate vocalizations, frequency of implementation of RIRD, and session length, and social validity scores. Vocal stereotypy was measured using continuous duration recording (Ahearn et al., 2007). The 26 total number of seconds of participant engagement in vocal stereotypy in each session was divided by the total number of seconds in the session (300 s) and multiplying the quotient by 100% to calculate the proportion of the session in which stereotypic behavior occurred. A duration recording method was chosen to measure stereotypy because recent literature on response interruption and redirection and matched stimulation (e.g., Ahearn et al., 2007; Rapp, 2006, 2007) employed such methods. In addition, previous literature for other interventions for stereotypical behavior such as McEntee and Saunders (1997) and Fisher, Lindauer, Alterson, and Thompson (1998) utilized the same measurement method. Further support for duration recording is voiced by Gardenier, MacDonald, and Green (2004). They assert that duration recording is the most preferred method for accuracy of recording of multidimensional behaviors such as stereotypy; although more convenient methods such as momentary time sampling (MTS) have been shown to be accurate for applied settings. Appropriate vocalizations and implementations of RIRD were measured in cumulative frequency per session for each participant. One RIRD implementation was defined with the onset as the experimenter saying the participant’s name, prompt delivery and participant compliance with 3 vocal imitation/social questions, and the end consisted of the experimenter completing delivery of verbal praise (e.g., “Nice job talking”). Cumulative frequency recording method was chosen as an appropriate measure for both appropriate vocalizations and implementation of RIRD during each session because they were typically observed in discrete episodes for these participants. Experimental Design and Conditions A multitreatment design was used to compare the effects of Response Interruption and Redirection (RIRD) alone versus exposure to matched stimulation implemented in conjunction with RIRD (Holcomb & Wolery, 1994). The letter “A” represented baseline, “B” was RIRD and matched stimulation implemented together, “C” was matched stimulation alone, and “D” represented RIRD alone. The conditions “C’ and “D” were counterbalanced across participants. Finally, the letter “E” represented the most effective treatment. 27 For all participants, RIRD and matched stimulation were simultaneously introduced after the initial baseline. After evaluation of RIRD and matched stimulation together, baseline conditions were reinstated. The matched stimulation component was systematically removed and counterbalanced across participants in subsequent conditions. This experimentally evaluated rates of stereotypy and appropriate vocalizations during conditions in which the establishing operation (EO) for stereotypy was present (RIRD only) and in which it was absent (matched stimulation in conjunction with RIRD). It also functioned to control for sequence effects. The experiment ended with the reinstatement of the treatment associated with the greatest suppression of vocal stereotypy. Reversal components were added to Ivan’s design between RIRD alone conditions, MS alone conditions, and before the final condition. It functioned to demonstrate experimental control and an absolute treatment effect because a relative treatment effect between MS alone, RIRD alone, and MS + RIRD were not discernable. The final condition for Ivan was RIRD alone because it was associated with the highest levels of appropriate vocalizations. These additions made the final experimental design for Ivan to be an ABABCDADACAD design. For Troy, reversal components were instated before the introduction of each component alone to demonstrate experimental control, as well as to help reveal relative treatment effects. Troy’s experiment ended with MS + RIRD because it was associated with fewer implementations of RIRD and a similar suppression to RIRD alone. MS + RIRD was also deemed more practical if the parents were to be trained on the procedures because it was associated with fewer implementations than RIRD alone. These additions made the final experimental design for Troy to be an ABABADACAB design. RIRD probes. Prior to treatment, probes were conducted to identify appropriate vocal demands for use during conditions containing an RIRD component. Vocal probes were conducted to assess fluency of vocal imitation of mastered words, or responding to social questions (depending on the skill level of the child) to which participants could consistently respond. The skills were selected based on prior mastery as indicated by teacher or caregiver report. Fluency was defined as responding correctly 28 and independently (without prompts) for both vocal imitation and/or answering social questions, across at least two previous teachers and settings. Specifically, a list was created of 15 vocal imitations or social questions. These items were identified as those that the child could emit both fluently and independently. Three lists containing ten items were created and randomly rotated across all sessions (see Appendix C, D, and E). All words were asked in a systematic order from the lists created during the probe session. The purpose of the lists was to increase the likelihood of demand consistency across all experimenters. The order of items for each list was randomly selected by assigning numbers to each item and then drawing numbers from a hat. During session the lists were placed out of participant view on the floor underneath the table. The three lists were rotated so that a different list was used every day (see Appendices D and E). Prior to the start of the study, vocal skills were probed across two different experimenters and two different settings for 89% accuracy and fluency. Baseline. The purpose of the baseline condition was to provide a contrast condition to evaluate responding under stimulus conditions in which the independent variables were not present. Essentially, the baseline functioned as a predictor of the target behavior if left untreated (Wacker, et al., 1990). Baseline conditions were conducted before any treatment conditions were instated. Baseline conditions were also reinstated during reversal phases. During this condition the participant was seated at a table, presented with two toys, and had the opportunity to interact with the experimenter. The experimenter smiled and visually attended to the participant while he played, but did not say anything unless an appropriate vocalization was emitted. The two toys were identified as moderately preferred from the preference assessment and did not produce auditory stimulation. Moderately preferred toys were used, as previous literature has shown that highly preferred items, which produce unmatched stimulation, can occasion reductions in vocal stereotypy (e.g., Ahearn et al., 2005). No programmed consequences were implemented contingent upon vocal stereotypy. Response cost was not implemented during baseline as it was considered an inherent part of the RIRD procedure. 29 If the participant left his chair, a least-to-most prompting hierarchy was used to redirect him back to the chair. The least-to-most prompting hierarchy was implemented as modeled by Wilder, Atwell, and Wine (2006). The participant had the opportunity to manipulate only one toy at a time. He could produce the other toy by requesting the item. Both prompted and unprompted requests were reinforced. In addition, if the participant did not manipulate an item for 5 or more seconds, the experimenter would move the toy closer to the participant, or rearrange pieces of it to occasion visual attending to the toy and manipulation of the item. Appropriate vocalizations were followed by praise and, in case of a mand, the delivering of the requested item if possible. For instance, if the participant said, ‘‘I want clapping,’’ the experimenter responded by saying, ‘‘Nice job asking for clapping,’’ and briefly clapped for the participant. If the participant requested an item (e.g., soda) that was not available, the experimenter responded by saying, ‘‘Nice job asking for soda, maybe we can have some later.’’ Baseline was implemented for at least three sessions or until vocal stereotypy displayed a steady state and no trend in the data path. If a steady state was not achieved within 10 sessions of any particular condition, the next condition was instated. Matched stimulation in conjunction with RIRD. The main purpose of this condition was to evaluate the effect(s) of the presence of matched stimuli as well as contingent delivery of the RIRD procedure on levels of vocal stereotypy. Responding in this condition was compared to response levels in baseline as well as the RIRD alone and matched stimulation alone. This treatment condition was adapted from both Rapp (2007) for matched stimulation procedures and Ahearn et al. (2007) for the RIRD procedure. In the matched stimulation plus RIRD condition, the experimenter was present in the room with the participant. The participant had continuous access to two toys that produce auditory stimulation. These were either set 1 or set 2 from the matched stimuli assessment. The two sets of matched stimuli were rotated when more than one session was conducted each day. The participant had the opportunity to manipulate only one toy at a time. He could produce the other toy by requesting the item. The experimenter smiled and visually attended to the 30 participant while he played, but did not say anything unless an appropriate vocalization or stereotypy was emitted. A session clock/stopwatch started when session began. The participant was seated at a table and had the opportunity to engage with two toys and interact with the experimenter. Contingent upon engagement in vocal stereotypy, participant responding was interrupted and re-directed by the experimenter as described by Ahearn et al. (2007). The experimenter implemented interruption by saying the participant’s name, and delivering instructions to occasion 3 consecutive appropriate vocalizations from the participant. If the participant was engaging with a toy, it was removed until RIRD was completed. This procedure was classified as response cost (Falcomata, Roane, Hovanetz, & Kettering, 2004). Response cost was implemented according to the following protocol: contingent upon participant engagement in vocal stereotypy, the experimenter removed the participant’s current leisure item from his/her physical grasp until RIRD was completed. The item was returned to the participant contingent upon the absence of vocal stereotypy and the emission of a prompted or unprompted appropriate request according to his skill level. Response cost was incorporated into the RIRD procedure to increase the likelihood of participant attending to the experimenter and complying with the vocal demands. In addition, response cost also set the occasion for appropriate vocalizations, by creating a motivating operation for the toy. In other words, response cost had the potential to alter the reinforcing effects of a particular item, and evoke appropriate manding from the participant. Prompts were delivered if the participant did not ask for the item back within 3 seconds after RIRD was completed. If the participant reached toward an item and did not request, a 2 second delay was inserted before the prompt (“What do you want?”) was delivered. This was inserted to avoid possible adventitious reinforcement of a reaching and requesting chain. RIRD was delivered in the following manner: Contingent upon engagement in vocal stereotypy, the experimenter said the participant’s name in a neutral tone of voice to evoke attending. At this time a secondary observer stopped the session clock. The experimenter simultaneously initiated eye contact with the participant and issued the instructions to occasion a vocal imitation response. The 31 instructions were a series of 3 consecutive vocal imitations of words (e.g., “Troy say ‘hopscotch,’, say ‘binder,’ say, ‘sofa’). If the participant engaged in stereotypy between appropriate responses, instructions continued to be delivered until the participant emitted 3 consecutive appropriate vocalizations. If the participant requested appropriately for the item during RIRD implementation, the experimenter acknowledged the response and replied with, “in a minute we can have ____,” and then continued delivering vocal imitation instructions until three consecutive vocalizations were emitted. After the participant complied with the 3 vocal imitation instructions, the experimenter delivered verbal praise (e.g., “Nice job talking”). Compliance was defined as the participant engaging in three consecutive appropriate vocalizations in the absence of stereotypy. When the secondary observer heard the completion of verbal praise, the session clock was resumed. In addition, the leisure item was returned to the participant contingent upon appropriate requesting. If the participant was engaging in vocal stereotypy at the time the experimenter returned the item, the item was removed again and RIRD was implemented. All behaviors other than vocal stereotypy and appropriate vocalizations were ignored (e.g. throwing of items, hitting the table). Sessions ended when 5 minutes elapsed as timed by the session clock. RIRD alone. This condition was implemented in the same manner as the matched stimulation plus RIRD condition, although the two moderately preferred leisure items that were made available to the participant did not produce any sound. The items were identified as moderately preferred from the day’s corresponding preference assessment. Moderately preferred toys were used because highly preferred items that produce unmatched stimulation have been demonstrated to suppress vocal stereotypy (e.g., Ahearn et al., 2005). Matched stimulation alone. The purpose of the matched stimulation alone condition was to evaluate levels of stereotypy in the presence of toys that produce matched stimulation (sound). This condition was exactly the same as the MS in conjunction with RIRD condition; however, neither RIRD nor response cost was implemented. Essentially, no programmed consequences were delivered 32 contingent upon stereotypy. The experimenter smiled and visually attended to the participant while he played, but did not say anything unless an appropriate vocalization was emitted. To facilitate participant discrimination between matched stimulation and RIRD and RIRD alone conditions, the experimenter wore a blue shirt when matched stimulation was available (paired with the Matched stimulation + RIRD condition) and a black shirt when matched stimulation was not available (paired with the RIRD alone condition). The experimenter also paired the instruction with the blue shirt, “We’re going to play with toys that make noise.” With the black shirt, the experimenter paired the instruction, “We’re going to play with quiet toys.” For baseline conditions, the experimenter wore any color shirt other than blue or black and no instructions were given. These stimuli were used because utilization of discriminative stimuli has been demonstrated to aid participant discrimination between similar conditions (Doughty, Anderson, Doughty, Williams & Saunders, 2007). Social Validity The purpose of the social validity portion of the study was to better understand the likelihood of caregiver comfort with watching, receiving training, and potentially implementing RIRD or matched stimulation themselves. It was measured at the conclusion of the study through two questionnaires to assess caregiver opinion on practicality and feasibility of each procedure. Caregiver opinion was expressed using a 5-point Likert-scale. A lowest rating of 1 represented “strongly disagree” and the highest rating of 5 represented “strongly agree.” Twenty-one items were included in total. Examples of RIRD items included responding to statements such as “I liked the vocal RIRD procedure”, “If trained, I could easily implement this procedure in my home”, etc. (see Appendix E). An example of an item for the matched stimulation procedure included, responding to such statements as “Matched stimulation reduced my child’s stereotypy during the session,” and “I liked the matched stimulation procedure” (see Appendix F). The numerical scores were calculated to interpret the overall social validity of the study. In addition, an open ended section concluded the questionnaire to provide an opportunity for caregivers to express additional comments. 33 Interobserver Agreement (IOA) A secondary observer who was previously trained in behavioral observation also scored data on at least 33% of all conditions by watching the video footage. IOA was calculated for all parts of the experiment: the functional analysis, matched stimuli assessment, baseline, and treatment sessions. An exact agreement calculation method was used for vocal stereotypy (using the total seconds of vocal experiment. Stereotypy was recorded by each observer and agreement was calculated as adapted from Ahearn et al. (2007). It was calculated by dividing the smaller total of seconds recorded by one observer by the larger total of seconds by the second observer; and then multiplied by 100%. IOA was also calculated for appropriate vocalizations and implementations using an exact agreement method. It was calculated for each participant by comparing the cumulative frequencies recorded by observers. The smaller total from one observer was divided by the larger total from the second observer, and multiplied by 100%. During the functional analysis IOA assessed for Ivan on average 44% of all sessions. Agreement for the occurrence of vocal stereotypy was an average of 97.0% (range 92-100%) across all conditions. For Troy, IOA was assessed an average of 42% of all sessions of the functional analysis. Agreement for the occurrence of vocal stereotypy was an average of 97.1% (range 91-100%). For the matched stimuli assessment, IOA assessed for Ivan an average of 33% for set one and 33% of sessions for set two. For set one, agreement for the agreement for the occurrence of vocal stereotypy was an average of 98.5% (range 95 – 100%). For set two, agreement for the occurrence of vocal stereotypy was an average of 99.0% (range 97-100%). For Troy’s matched stimuli assessment, IOA was assessed an average of 39% of sessions for set one and 38% of sessions for set 2. For set one, agreement for the occurrence of vocal stereotypy was an average of 97.9% (range 94-100%). For set two, agreement for the occurrence of vocal stereotypy was an average of 97.8% (range 93-100%). For the experiment, mean IOA for vocal stereotypy and appropriate vocalizations was assessed. IOA for Ivan was conducted on 38% of sessions. Agreement for the occurrence of vocal stereotypy was 98.9% (range 92-100%). Agreement for the occurrence of appropriate vocalizations was 100% across all 34 conditions. IOA for Troy was conducted on 38% of sessions. Agreement for the occurrence of vocal stereotypy was 96.9% (range 80-100%) across all conditions. Agreement for the occurrence of appropriate vocalizations was 100% across all conditions. Across both participants, overall IOA for vocal stereotypy was an average of 97.9% and overall IOA for appropriate vocalizations was 100%. Treatment Integrity Treatment integrity can provide stronger support for the independent variable as the source of behavior change Gresham, Gansle, and Noell, (1993). For this experiment, treatment integrity of RIRD was assessed for an overall average of 41% of sessions. Treatment integrity for matched stimulation was assessed an average of 36% of sessions. For the treatment integrity assessment of RIRD, each implementation was comprised of two parts (see Appendix G). Part one was scored as correct (+) if the experimenter delivered RIRD immediately (within two seconds) contingent upon vocal stereotypy. Likewise, part two was scored as correct if experimenters ensured that participants responded with three consecutive compliances in the absence of stereotypy. The entire implementation trial was scored as incorrect (-) if either part of RIRD was implemented incorrectly. The total number of correct trials for RIRD delivery was divided by the total number of opportunities to implement RIRD in the session. The quotient was multiplied by 100 to yield a percentage of overall integrity for RIRD. Finally, treatment integrity was also gathered for the conditions that required the delivery of matched stimulation (see Appendix H). Delivery was evaluated using a partial interval recording method. Five second intervals were used. An interval was scored as containing matched stimulation if at any time during the 5-second interval the toys’ sounds were present. The total number of intervals for each session (60) was divided by the number of intervals scored as containing matched stimulation. The quotient was multiplied by 100 to yield an overall percentage of integrity for matched stimulation. Session percentages were then added and averaged to yield a percentage of overall integrity of matched stimulation for each participant. 35 Across both participants, treatment integrity for implementation of RIRD was 96.0% (range 92100%). In addition, matched stimulation was present for an average of 99.3% (range 93-100%) of session intervals across both participants. 36 Chapter 3 RESULTS Stimulus Preference Assessment The results of Ivan’s initial stimulus preference assessment of quiet toys yielded an animal puzzle, coloring book, and magnet toys as highly preferred. Animal figurines, Dora blocks, a whiteboard, and Play-doh™ were moderately preferred. It was found anecdotally that Play-doh ™ occasioned mouthing and competed with stereotypy, so the next preferred item (Mr. Potato head) was used. The results of the preference assessment using sound toys showed that a toy phone, Incredibles ™ book, and fire truck were highly preferred. A leapfrog™ letter board, Elmo ™ piano, toy helicopter, and ladybug-shaped song toy were moderately preferred. Anecdotal observations noted that the fire truck and ladybug occasioning moderate levels of stereotypy; therefore these toys were not used during the matched stimulation assessment. The four most highly preferred toys included in the matched stimulation assessment were the toy phone, Incredibles ™ book, leapfrog ™ letter board, and Elmo ™ piano. For Troy, the results of his stimulus preference assessment of quiet toys showed that Dora ™ blocks, Diego ™ blocks, and Play-doh™ were highly preferred. A Mr. Potato head, puzzles, cars, and figurines were found to be moderately preferred. It was anecdotally found that cars competed with stereotypy because of the rolling sound the wheels made, so the next preferred item (magnet toys) replaced cars. A second preference assessment was conducted before the experiment began due to satiation effects. The results of this assessment showed Don’t Break the Ice™, a magna doodle, and silly putty to be highly preferred. Magnet toys, a pin-head toy, coloring books, and a folding wire-ball were found to be moderately preferred. Results of the preference assessment for sound toys showed a Disney™ Hunchback of Notre Dame electronic book, ladybug song toy, and Elmo™ piano were highly preferred. A musical helicopter, quacking duck keychain light, and Elmo™ poke toy were found to be moderately preferred. The Hunchback of Notre Dame book, ladybug song toy, and quacking duck light were associated with 37 moderate to high levels of stereotypy, so they were used for the matched stimulation assessment. The musical helicopter, Elmo™ piano, Elmo™ poke, and pirate ship were chosen as they were the most highly preferred items associated with the lowest levels of stereotypy. Functional Analyses (FA) Figure 1 (top panel) depicts the results for Ivan’s functional analysis. His FA showed some differentiation between conditions. Rates of stereotypy were slightly lower during demand (M = 24%) and attention conditions (M = 22%) compared to the no consequence condition (M = 35%). Because there was no clear differentiation, an extended no consequence condition was conducted. Stereotypy persisted with repeated exposure to the extended no consequence condition (M = 60%), suggesting an automatic function for his stereotypy. Figure 1 (bottom panel) depicts the results for Troy’s functional analysis. His data showed clear differentiation between conditions during the second half of the functional analysis. Rates of stereotypy were lower during demand (M = 14%) and attention conditions (M = 14%) compared to the no consequence condition (M = 35%); thereby suggesting an automatic function. 38 Figure 1. The top panel depicts the percent occurrence of vocal stereotypy during Ivan’s functional analysis sessions. The bottom panel depicts percent occurrence of vocal stereotypy during Troy’s functional analysis sessions. 39 Matched Stimuli Assessment (MSA) For Ivan’s MSA, Set 1 was comprised of an electronic telephone toy that produced songs, animal sounds, and labels of letters and numbers; and a Disney Incredibles ™ electronic book that produced sound effects. Results for Ivan’s MSA for Set 1 are depicted in Figure 2 (top panel). During phase one, there was differentiation between the no interaction (M = 33.3%) and toys with sound condition (M = 5.3%); demonstrating that the toys suppressed stereotypy. During phase two, rates of stereotypy were substantially higher during the no interaction condition (M = 62.3%) and the toys with no sound condition (M = 32%). This demonstrated that tactile effects of the toys were not suppressing stereotypy during phase 1. After reinstating phase 1 conditions, stereotypy again reduced to low levels during the toys with sound condition (M = 8%) compared to the no interaction condition (M = 42.3%); thereby demonstrating that the toys in Set 1 functioned as matched stimuli. During Ivan’s MSA for Set 2, he had access to a Leapfrog™ letter board which produced letter labels, sounds of letters, and sound effects; and an electronic Elmo™ piano. Results for Set 2 are depicted in Figure 2 (bottom panel). During phase one, there was differentiation between the no interaction (M = 41%) and toys with sound condition (M = 2.7%); demonstrating that the toys suppressed stereotypy. For phase 2, rates of stereotypy occurred at a high rates in both the no interaction (M = 60.3%) and toys with no sound condition (M = 46.3%). When phase 1 conditions were reinstated, the effects were replicated with low levels during the toys with sound condition (M = 7.7%) compared to the no interaction condition (M = 56%). Results indicated that the toys in Set 2 also functioned as matched stimuli. 40 Figure 2. Percentage of vocal stereotypy occurrence for Ivan during matched stimulation assessment of Set 1 (top panel) and Set 2 (bottom panel) 41 For Troy’s MSA, Set 1 was comprised of an electronic Elmo™ piano and a battery operated pirate ship that produced songs, counting, and cannon sounds. Results for Troy’s MSA for Set 1 are depicted in Figure 3 (top panel). During phase one, there was differentiation between the no interaction (M = 23.3%) and toys with sound condition (M = 4%); demonstrating that the toys suppressed stereotypy. During phase two, rates were similar between the no interaction condition (M = 14%) and the toys with no sound condition (M = 13.5%). This demonstrated that the toy’s tactile stimulation alone was not sufficient to suppress stereotypy. Phase 1 conditions were then replicated with similar suppressive effects during the toys with sound condition (M = 1%) compared to the no interaction condition (M = 20.7%). This assessment demonstrated that the toys in Set 1 functioned as matched stimuli. Set 2 was comprised of a battery operated helicopter that produced songs, counting, color labels, etc.; and an Elmo ™ poke toy that produced various phrases and sound effects. Results for Troy’s MSA for Set 2 are found in Figure 3 (bottom panel). During phase one, there was clear differentiation between the no interaction (M = 1.3%) and toys with sound condition (M = 20%); demonstrating that the toys suppressed stereotypy. During phase two, the no interaction condition (M = 9.7%) and the toys with no sound condition (M = 12.3%) displayed somewhat equivalent levels. The increase in stereotypy during the no sound condition demonstrated that the toy’s tactile stimulation was not suppressing stereotypy during phase 1. Phase 1 conditions were replicated with similar differentiation compared to the first implementation. The toys with sound condition suppressed stereotypy (M = 2%) although stereotypy was on average lower during the no interaction condition (M = 9.7%). This assessment provided evidence that the toys in Set 2 functioned as matched stimuli. 42 Figure 3. Percentage of vocal stereotypy occurrence for Troy during matched stimulation assessment for Set 1 (top panel) and Set 2 (bottom panel). 43 Treatment Figure 4 depicts the percentage of vocal stereotypy for Ivan during baseline and treatment conditions. Vocal stereotypy occurred at moderate to high levels during initial baseline levels at an average of 37%. Responding was variable but stable. Upon introduction of MS + RIRD, stereotypy levels immediately decreased; averaging 2.1% across the phase. Return to baseline resulted in similar levels of the first baseline with an average of 33.7%. MS + RIRD was again instated, and suppressive effects of the package were replicated with a slightly higher average of 5.8% rate of stereotypy. RIRD was then removed from the package to examine effects of MS alone on stereotypy, resulting in an average rate of 5.5% for stereotypy. RIRD alone was subsequently instated as the sole treatment component, with an average of 5%; showing little change in stereotypy levels. Baseline was then instated and stereotypy exhibited some carryover effects from the previous RIRD condition. After two sessions, stereotypy levels recovered and increased to a high of 57% and averaged 19.3%. The MS alone condition was then implemented a second time, yielding a higher average (11.8%) compared to the first implementation of this component. Baseline was then reinstated a fifth time and stereotypy levels immediately increased to a single session of 61%. Finally, RIRD alone was implemented as the final treatment because it was associated with the higher rates of appropriate vocalizations, and low rates of stereotypy. This phase replicated suppressive effects of the first implementation of RIRD. It was associated with an average stereotypy rate of 4.3%. Figure 4 also displays Ivan’s rates of appropriate vocalizations. During the initial baseline, appropriate vocalizations occurred at very low frequency. The average was 0.7 per session and a high of 2 per session. With the introduction of MS + RIRD, appropriate vocalizations modestly increased to an average of 2.6 per session, with a high of 7 appropriate vocalizations corresponding to the first session of this phase. Although the increase was small, experimental control was demonstrated during the second implementation of baseline conditions. In this phase appropriate vocalizations immediately fell to zero and averaged zero across all three consecutive sessions. When MS + RIRD was instated a second time, appropriate vocalizations immediately increased and averaged a higher rate of 7 per session. 44 Subsequently, the RIRD alone component was removed and MS alone was introduced. Appropriate vocalizations occurred at a low average of 0.8 per session, with highest rates of 2 per session. RIRD alone was then instated and the matched stimulation component removed. Rates of appropriate vocalizations increased to an average of 4.6 per session, and a high of 9. Next, another reversal to baseline was inserted, and appropriate vocalizations fell to zero; thereby demonstrating experimental control. The effects of RIRD alone were replicated during the subsequent reinstatement of the condition, with appropriate vocalizations averaging 6 per session. Reversal to baseline conditions yielded three consecutive sessions of zero appropriate vocalizations. The second introduction of MS alone demonstrated little change in appropriate vocalizations, with an average of 0.3 per session and the highest rate per session was a single occurrence. During the final reversal to baseline Ivan’s appropriate vocalizations increase slightly to a rate of 2 for the single session. The final treatment component of RIRD was implemented and chosen because it was the treatment component associated with moderate rates of appropriate vocalizations and low rates of stereotypy. It was concluded as the most appropriate treatment because RIRD alone and MS + RIRD produced similar results, and Ivan would not have regular access to sound toys during school programming. During the final phase appropriate vocalizations occurred at an average of 3.9 per session, however a session high of 6 was observed. Figure 5 depicts the percentage of vocal stereotypy for Troy during baseline and treatment conditions. Vocal stereotypy occurred at moderate levels during initial baseline levels at an average of 16%. Responding was stable with the exception of a rate of 25% during session four. Upon introduction of MS + RIRD, percentage of stereotypy immediately fell to 3% during the first session, and averaged 1% over all sessions of this condition. Baseline conditions were then reinstated, and stereotypy levels recovered to an even greater average of 25%. Anecdotally, it was noted during these sessions that Troy was looking at the experimenters and the camera at a lower rate. There may have been reactivity to the experimenter and camera during the first baseline conditions. With the second implementation of MS + RIRD, stereotypy immediately decreased to a percentage of zero during the first session, and over three sessions averaged 0.7%. Another reversal to baseline was introduced, again with stereotypy recovering 45 to previous baseline levels at a rate of 29%. RIRD alone was instated, and stereotypy was found to occur at a slightly higher average of 6.7%. Data were observed to be stable before a single session of baseline was introduced, and stereotypy recovered to a higher rate of 35%. MS alone was subsequently instated, and stereotypy was occurred at lower levels than RIRD alone, averaging 2.4% across sessions. A final return to baseline was inserted, and yielded an even higher average of 45% across sessions. This was a fourth demonstration of experimental control. Finally, MS + RIRD was introduced as the last treatment because it was observed to be associated with the lowest frequency of implementations, as well as a moderate rate of appropriate vocalizations. Because of the lower rate of implementations, it was deemed more feasible to train parents or others to implement the procedure. This condition’s effects were replicated a third time, with rates of stereotypy occurring with low variability and an average of 1.5%. Figure 5 also depicts the frequency of appropriate vocalizations for Troy during baseline and treatment sessions. During initial baseline, appropriate vocalizations occurred at low levels; averaging only 1.3 per session. With introduction of MS + RIRD, appropriate vocalizations increased to an average of 3.8, with one session associated with a high of 8 per session. When baseline conditions were reinstated, appropriate vocalizations reversed to previous baseline levels, with an average of 1 per session. MS + RIRD was introduced a second time, and previous treatment effects were replicated with an average of 3.3 per session. A single session of baseline conditions was inserted, and associated with a rate of 1 appropriate vocalization. The MS component was then removed to examine sole effects of RIRD. This condition was associated with a higher average of appropriate vocalizations per session (16.6 occurrences). Although this is undoubtedly a greater increase, it is important to note that appropriate vocalizations are yoked to the number of times RIRD is implemented. That is, at least one appropriate vocalization is associated with the implementation of response cost in order for the participant to re-gain access to the item after the RIRD is complete. Therefore, if RIRD is implemented more times during one session versus another, the session containing the larger amount of implementations will also yield a larger sum of appropriate vocalizations. On average during the RIRD 46 alone condition, RIRD was implemented 15 times per session. Another single-session return to baseline conditions showed a decrease in appropriate vocalizations, with only one occurring during the session. Matched stimulation was introduced as the sole treatment, and was associated with a rate of 4.4 per session. Baseline conditions were reintroduced a final time, and exhibited a slightly higher average of 3 per session over previous baseline conditions. MS + RIRD was implemented as the final treatment, and occurrence of appropriate vocalizations were greater than previous instatements of the condition. Average rate of appropriate vocalizations was 6.8 per session. 47 Figure 4. Depicts the percentage of vocal stereotypy per session and frequency of appropriate vocalizations for Ivan across all treatment conditions. 48 Figure 5. Depicts the percentage of vocal stereotypy per session and frequency of appropriate vocalizations for Troy across all treatment conditions. 49 Figure 6 (top panel) displays the average number of RIRD implementations per session for each participant. Overall, the number of implementations of RIRD for Ivan was relatively equal between the two conditions. The MS + RIRD condition averaged 4.5 implementations and RIRD alone condition was slightly higher with an average of 5.8. There was a large discrepancy between the two treatments for Troy. The MS + RIRD condition was associated with a considerably lower frequency of implementations, averaging 2.3 per session. The RIRD alone condition averaged 15 implementations per session. Figure 6 (bottom panel) displays the average session length for the MS + RIRD and RIRD alone conditions for each participant. For Ivan, session length for the two conditions was similar. The average session length for MS + RIRD conditions was 342.9 seconds; and the average length for RIRD alone was 349.8. Troy’s average length per session showed MS + RIRD as lower than RIRD alone. Average length for MS + RIRD was 313.5 seconds; RIRD alone was 389.8. Overall, both participants’ session length did not display any decreasing trends throughout the experiment. Ivan’s session length data displayed some variability, but relatively stable. For Troy the session length remained constant for MS + RIRD, despite increased exposure to the RIRD contingency as the study progressed. 50 Figure 6. The top panel depicts the average frequency of RIRD implementations for each participant during RIRD alone and MS + RIRD conditions. The bottom panel depicts the average session length in seconds for each participant during RIRD alone and MS + RIRD conditions. 51 Social Validity Social validity measures were gathered from both participants’ caregivers at the conclusion of the study. One measure evaluated parent opinion for the RIRD procedure and the other for the Matched Stimulation alone procedure. Measures were dispersed after the parent viewed a brief video clip of an RIRD treatment session, or a matched stimulation treatment session. Scores for each question ranged from 1 as strongly disagree to 5 as strongly agree, and 3 as neutral. For the item, “I liked the vocal RIRD procedure”, parents overall rated this item at an average of 4.5. For the item, “If trained, I could easily implement the vocal RIRD procedure in my home”, parents rated this item as a 4.5. For the item, “I am satisfied with my child’s response to the vocal RIRD procedure”, parents rated this at an average of 4. The item, “Vocal RIRD reduced my child’s stereotypy during the session” was rated at an average of 4. The item, “I thought the vocal RIRD procedure was intrusive”, parents rated at an average of 1.5. For the item, “Vocal RIRD is a socially appropriate intervention method for my child”, parents rated at an average of 4. For the item, “I think vocal RIRD improved my child’s rate of appropriate vocalizations during the session”, parents rated at an average of 4.5. The item, “My child will benefit in the long run from the procedures implemented in this study,” parents rated at an average of 4.5. The item, “Vocal RIRD produced negative emotional reactions in my child”, was rated at an average of 1. Lastly, the item, “I feel vocal RIRD produced little change in my child’s stereotypy rates during session”, was rated at an average of 3 (one parent rated 2 and one parent rated 4). Parent opinion was also assessed for the matched stimulation procedure. For the item, “I liked the matched stimulation procedure”, parent rated an average of 4.5. For the item, “If trained, I could easily implement the matched stimulation procedure in my home, was rated an average of 4.5. For the item, “I am satisfied with my child’s response to the matched stimulation procedure”, parents rated an average of 4. For the item, “Matched stimulation reduced my child’s stereotypy during the session”, parent rated an average of 4.5. For the item, “I thought the matched stimulation procedure was intrusive” parents rated an average of 1. For the item, “Matched Stimulation is a socially appropriate intervention method for my child” parents rated an average of 4.5. For the item, “I think matched stimulation 52 improved my child’s rate appropriate vocalizations during the session”, parents rated an average of 4. For the item, “My child will benefit in the long run from the procedures implemented in this study”, parents rated an average of 4.5. For the item, “Matched Stimulation produced negative emotional reactions in my child”, parents rated an average of 1. Finally, for the item, “I feel matched stimulation produced little change in my child’s stereotypy rates during session”, parents rated an average of 3 (one parent rated 2 and one parent rated 4). Parents were then asked to circle the name of the preferred procedure, or none at all if they preferred all procedures equally. Across both parents, the most preferred procedure was the treatment package of MS + RIRD. An additional comment made by one mother was that she enjoyed learning about the two techniques available and wanted to someday try the procedures in a home or school setting. Figure 7 displays the average ratings of each item in graphic form. 53 Figure 7. Depicts the average rating across both participant caregivers for RIRD (top panel) and Matched Stimulation (bottom panel). 54 Chapter 4 DISCUSSION For both participants, RIRD was found to both decrease stereotypy and increase appropriate vocalizations. This is consistent with previous studies (Ahearn et al., 2007; Liu-Gitz & Banda, 2010; Miguel et al., 2009). Vocal stereotypy was also observed to decrease for both participants during the matched stimulation assessment and the matched stimulation alone condition. This is also consistent with findings from Rapp (2007). Suppression in the presence of matched stimulation may have been a result of the toys’ stimulation functioning as an abolishing operation for the products of vocal stereotypy. Findings support results obtained by Rapp (2006) that stereotypy is sensitive to the manipulation of motivating operations. For Ivan, all treatment conditions showed similar effects for suppression of stereotypy, session length and number of RIRD implementations. For Troy, matched stimulation + RIRD was more efficient for producing low levels of stereotypy with less session time and considerably fewer implementations of RIRD. Both participants exhibited greater frequency of appropriate vocalizations with conditions containing an RIRD component. Overall, both participants’ results showed different treatments as the most effective. The results for which treatment condition was most effective may be explained in a number of ways. For Ivan, comparable suppressive effects on stereotypy for the RIRD alone and MS + RIRD conditions may be attributed to the RIRD component. Specifically, RIRD may have been the source of stereotypy suppression. This is supported by Ivan’s variability of stereotypy levels during the first MS alone condition, as well as the slight increase in rate of stereotypy during the second MS alone condition. RIRD is also likely to be the source of the increase in appropriate vocalizations. This is supported by observations during the MS alone condition. During MS alone Ivan engaged in fewer appropriate vocalizations than during conditions containing an RIRD component. This may be explained by the response cost component embedded within the RIRD procedure. Response-cost can function as a 55 transitive conditioned establishing operation (Michael 1993/2003) establishing the value of the removed toy as a reinforcer and increasing the probability of appropriate requests. Although RIRD is likely the source of Ivan’s behavior change, it is difficult to discern whether response cost or vocal imitation demands were responsible for the decrease in stereotypy. Previous research on response cost (e.g. Falcomata et al.; Rapp, 2005) has shown that contingent removal of preferred stimuli can function as punishers for automatically maintained behavior. However, research on vocal imitation in RIRD (Ahearn et al., 2007; Liu-Gitz & Banda, 2010; Miguel et al., 2009) also showed suppressive effects on stereotypy. All three studies also used a response cost procedure. Ivan’s increased amount of appropriate vocalizations associated with conditions containing RIRD was observed to be correlated to the number of times the response cost procedure was implemented. During MS alone conditions, response cost was not implemented. The omission of response cost during this condition may have provided fewer opportunities for requests to be emitted. Since MS alone did not contain response cost, a decrease in appropriate vocalizations was observed. Future studies may want to keep the response cost component constant across all conditions to facilitate comparison. Another suggestion is to categorize types of appropriate vocalizations, especially those that are related to the response cost procedure. For example, it would be helpful to record the number of mands for the specific toys that were removed while RIRD was implemented. Such information could provide better insight when large changes in appropriate vocalizations are observed. In contrast, Troy’s most effective treatment condition was MS + RIRD. In addition to decreasing stereotypy and increasing appropriate vocalizations, MS + RIRD produced an average shorter session and fewer trial implementations of RIRD. These effects are consistent with findings by O’Reilly et al. (2006, 2007, 2008) and Rapp (2005, 2007). O’Reilly and colleagues (2006, 2007, 2008) found that the addition of noncontingent reinforcement increased the effectiveness of procedures designed to suppress aberrant behavior (e.g. aggression, self-injury). For Troy, because there were consistent concomitant decreases in stereotypy during sessions with matched stimulation, matched stimulation may be considered noncontingent reinforcement for vocal stereotypy. Matched stimulation also seemed to 56 increase the efficiency of the RIRD procedure. Rapp (2005, 2006) also found that stereotypy was sensitive to motivating operations, and that matched stimulation suppressed stereotypy. Decreased rates of stereotypy during conditions containing matched stimulation provide evidence that matched stimulation functioned as an abolishing operation and was responsible or partially responsible for the decrease. Similar to Ivan, Troy exhibited greater frequency of appropriate vocalizations during conditions containing RIRD. His increased rate of appropriate vocalizations during the RIRD only condition was also observed to be correlated to the number of times response cost was implemented. A final note about treatment is that the effects of matched stimulation were long lasting over the course of the study (approximately 3-4 months). However, future studies should evaluate the toys’ effectiveness over a longer period of time. The use of a multiple-treatments design may answer this question. Social validity scores indicated that both participants’ parents felt the procedures were useful, and the preferred treatment was MS + RIRD. Both parents gave favorable ratings for both RIRD and matched stimulation as to their utility for reduction of stereotypy and increase in appropriate vocalizations. The only question that did not indicate favor of the procedures was, “I feel matched stimulation produced little change in child’s stereotypy rates during session.” For this question, one mother indicated that she agreed with this statement. However, she agreed with all other questions indicating favorable evaluation of both RIRD and matched stimulation. The overall results of the social validity scoring supports the piloting of parent training in RIRD, matched stimulation, or MS + RIRD. Parents may enjoy the flexibility of having a variety of procedures to use, depending on the situation and their availability to provide treatment for their child. Implications Because both participants differed in the most effective treatment, this indicates that treatment effects of RIRD, MS, and MS + RIRD may vary across individuals. For Ivan, since all treatment conditions showed similar effects for suppression of stereotypy, this is valuable information for practitioners. His results suggest that there are several alternatives available to treat stereotypy. If a 57 parent or therapist is unavailable to implement RIRD, stereotypy may be prevented by providing access to matched stimulation. In situations in which an adult is available, RIRD may then be implemented with potentially equally effective results. In addition, data showing that MS + RIRD is equally effective to RIRD alone is also useful information. In environments in which matched stimulation may not be available or feasible to implement (e.g., stores, movie theaters, etc.); practitioners can be confident that RIRD alone can be a viable alternative to MS + RIRD in the event that sound toys are unavailable. The most interesting finding for Troy was that conditions containing matched stimulation and RIRD contained fewer implementations than RIRD alone . One possible limitation of RIRD is the effort that may be necessary to consistently implement it. In the applied setting, these results can lead to decreased therapist time and human resources needed to treat vocal stereotypy. In addition, fewer implementations may result in faster training for parents, teachers, aids, etc.; as well as increase the likelihood that RIRD is implemented more consistently in non-controlled environments. Overall, the addition of matched stimulation may address a possible limitation of RIRD and make it more feasible to use. Limitations Although important findings were drawn from this study, some of its limitations will be noted. Ivan’s data was undifferentiated across conditions of MS +RIRD, RIRD alone, and matched stimulation alone. Although treatment effects were replicated, it cannot be determined that data were the result of equivalent effects or carryover effects. One possibility is that there may have been some carryover effects from the MS + RIRD condition to the MS alone. The presence of the same toys and the same blue shirt of the experimenter may have possibly functioned as discriminative stimulus for contingent delivery of RIRD (punishment). It is possible that since the second implementation of MS alone resulted in higher rates of stereotypy, that the first effects are in part due to carryover effects from the previous MS + RIRD condition. Future studies may pair an additional instruction with the MS +RIRD condition to convey to the participant that RIRD will be delivered during the session. Another way to facilitate 58 discrimination could be to use different sound toys during the MS + RIRD condition compared to MS alone. Another limitation of the study was the brevity of the 5-minute session length. Because sessions were short, this may have contributed to Ivan’s results of equivalent effects across treatments. It is possible that matched stimulation effects may have decreased in efficacy if longer sessions of exposure were used. Although RIRD was found to be effective for stereotypy suppression, long-term effects cannot be determined from this study because sessions were brief. Anecdotally, Troy was observed to engage in stereotypy while waiting in the lobby or while riding the elevator before session. Ivan was also observed to engage in stereotypy throughout the home before sessions began. Both participants were also observed to engage in increased rates of stereotypy when on breaks between treatment sessions. It may be that the brevity of 5-minute sessions may have prevented the treatment effects from generalizing to different times of the day, or different environments. Other studies (e.g., Ahearn et al., 2007; Liu-Gitz & Banda, 2010; Miguel et al., 2009) all used 5-minute session lengths, so this limitation is not unique to this study. A final limitation of the study was that a small amount of toys remained constant and participants had access to only a total of two toys during a session. This was true of all conditions containing both matched and unmatched toys. Overall, frequency of appropriate vocalizations for each participant was somewhat low and observed to be correlated to the RIRD procedure. There is a possibility that appropriate vocalizations were low because the participant satiated on the two toys; thereby reducing motivating operations to request for the toys. In addition, the small amount of toys may also have removed other potentially evoking stimuli from the environment. Additional toys may have occasioned more appropriate tacts or comments from the participants. Another explanation for the low frequency of appropriate vocalizations could have been the nature of interaction protocol for the conditions. The experimenter did not talk to the participant unless the participant appropriately vocalized first. This is an unnatural setting, as adults typically do not play by watching and waiting for children to speak. In contrast, adults embed comments and questions 59 throughout play. Therefore, the lower frequency of independent, appropriate vocalizations may have been the result of the absence of evoking stimuli from the adult. Increased adult comments may have occasioned additional appropriate comments, requests, etc. from the participant; although it may have also functioned to reinforce stereotypy. Conclusions In summary, the addition of matched stimulation was found to increase suppression of vocal stereotypy for one participant, and for another participant all treatment combinations were found to yield somewhat equivalent results. Appropriate vocalizations were found to be the highest when RIRD was implemented. Parent report indicated a preference for the treatment package of MS + RIRD. Essentially, it was found that stereotypy can be reduced in a number of ways by different behavioral mechanisms. It is likely that matched stimulation alone was effective to reduce stereotypy for both participants because it functioned as an abolishing operation for stereotypy. For Troy, the addition of a matched stimulation component to RIRD led to a greater decrease in stereotypy than each component alone; indicating that treatment packages may have additive effects. These findings provide more information for practitioners for the types of treatments that are available for vocal stereotypy. They can use this information to help decide which treatment may be the most effective, efficient, and appropriate for their unique environment conditions. Matched stimulation alone may be useful when adult interruption is not possible or feasible. MS + RIRD may be an effective procedure to aid the training of parents. This is supported by the fewer implementations associated with the package. Furthermore, fewer implementations have the potential to facilitate greater consistency of implementation in the natural environment. Overall, treatment effects were slightly different across the two participants, showing that programs for stereotypy reduction should be tailored to the individual and his/her environment. Future Research While this study examined the effects of MS, RIRD, and MS + RIRD on appropriate vocalizations, the type of appropriate vocalization and the context were not recorded. Most of the 60 participant appropriate vocalizations were observed to be mands evoked from the response cost procedure of RIRD. There were also some tacts of objects. Future research should examine different types of appropriate vocalizations associated with RIRD. In addition, it should examine the environmental conditions during RIRD sessions to occasion a variety of appropriate vocalizations rather than mands alone. It may also be helpful to examine more naturalistic means of evoking vocal imitation during RIRD instead of using arbitrary words. Examples might include taking the toy the child currently is playing with (e.g. a tiger toy) and prompting the appropriate tact (e.g., “say tiger”). Anecdotally, Ivan was observed to sometimes emit the RIRD probe words in the absence of stereotypy. The aforementioned modifications to the RIRD vocal imitation component may increase the likelihood that appropriate vocalizations will come under the control of the appropriate stimuli in the environment; rather than under control of other contingencies. Two possibilities were that Ivan was engaging in appropriate vocalizations either to prevent RIRD delivery, or because in the past sessions these specific topographies produced reinforcement. Future research using different vocal imitation procedures may help decrease the likelihood that arbitrary vocalizations evoked by the RIRD procedures will not be inadvertently reinforced. Future research should also examine the long-term effects of both matched stimulation and RIRD. Long-term effects of matched stimulation once the toy(s) are removed are unknown. Future research should measure stereotypy for longer than 15 minutes (Rapp, 2006) after access to matched stimulation. Furthermore, duration of matched stimulation effects after prolonged exposure (e.g. 2 hours of session time) is also unknown. It may be helpful to examine levels of stereotypy both before and after the session to both determine duration of effects, in addition to examining any behavioral contrast effects of RIRD. This latter suggestion is based on the anecdotal reports of participants engaging more stereotypy both between and after sessions. Finally, since parent opinion of this study indicated a preference for MS + RIRD, the efficacy of MS + RIRD, RIRD, and MS alone should be evaluated in the context of parent implementation. 61 Evaluating the ease of parent implementation and preference for each procedure may yield valuable information on their feasibility and practicality for the home environment. Such information may produce more socially valid procedures that facilitate generalization of treatment effects from the lab to the home. 62 APPENDIX A Modified RAISD Intake Form The purpose of this survey is to obtain information about the toys that you believe would be useful as rewards for your child. If you have signed the consent form, please answer the following questions regarding your child’s preferences: 1. Some children really enjoy toys like puzzles, books, blocks, figurines, musical toys, playdoh, etc. What are the specific toys your child really likes? _____________________ _____________________ _____________________ _____________________ _____________________ _____________________ _____________________ _____________________ _____________________ _____________________ 2. Please go back to the list in question number one, and place a number in each box to rank these toys from most favorite (1) to least favorite (10). 3. Are there any toys that you would prefer your child not to play with during our study? ___________________________________________________________________ 63 APPENDIX B Preference Assessment Data Sheet 64 APPENDIX C Functional Analysis Data Sheet 65 APPENDIX D Experiment Data Sheet 66 APPENDIX E VRIRD List 1 1. Scissors 2. Ladder 3. Zipper 4. Blanket 5. Marker 6. Sofa 7. Magnet 8. Closet 9. Rhino 10. Pencil 67 APPENDIX F VRIRD List 2 1. Blanket 2. Binder 3. Sofa 4. Magnet 5. Closet 6. Rhino 7. Pencil 8. Ladder 9. Marker 10. Carpet 68 APPENDIX G VRIRD List 3 1. Zipper 2. Marker 3. Carpet 4. Ladder 5. Scissors 6. Backpack 7. Binder 8. Rhino 9. Hopscotch 10. Magnet 69 APPENDIX H Social Validity VRIRD Scale After watching the video sessions, please answer all questions honestly and to the best of your ability. This will help to interpret the overall social validity of the study for future reference. 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neutral, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly agree 1. I liked the vocal RIRD procedure. 1 2 3 4 5 2. If trained, I could easily implement the vocal RIRD procedure in my home. 1 2 3 4 5 3. I am satisfied with my child’s response to the vocal RIRD procedure. 1 2 3 4 5 4. Vocal RIRD reduced my child’s stereotypy during the session. 1 2 3 4 5 5. I thought the vocal RIRD procedure was intrusive. 1 2 3 4 5 6. Vocal RIRD is a socially appropriate intervention method for my child. 1 2 3 4 5 7. I think vocal RIRD improved my child’s rate of appropriate vocalizations during the session. 1 2 3 4 5 8. My child will benefit in the long run from the procedures implemented in this study. 1 2 3 4 5 9. Vocal RIRD produced negative emotional reactions in my child. 1 2 3 4 5 10. I feel vocal RIRD produced little change in my child’s stereotypy rates during session. 1 2 3 4 5 Please provide any additional comments/suggestions about the procedure: 70 APPENDIX I Social Validity MS Scale After watching the video sessions, please answer all questions honestly and to the best of your ability. This will help to interpret the overall social validity of the study for future reference. 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neutral, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly agree 1. I liked the matched stimulation procedure. 1 2 3 4 5 2. If trained, I could easily implement the matched stimulation procedure in my home. 1 2 3 4 5 3. I am satisfied with my child’s response to the matched stimulation procedure 1 2 3 4 5 4. Matched stimulation reduced my child’s stereotypy during the session. 1 2 3 4 5 5. I thought the matched stimulation procedure was intrusive. 1 2 3 4 5 6. Matched Stimulation is a socially appropriate intervention method for my child. 1 2 3 4 5 7. I think matched stimulation improved my child’s rate of appropriate vocalizations during the session. 1 2 3 4 5 8. My child will benefit in the long run from the procedures implemented in this study. 1 2 3 4 5 9. Matched Stimulation produced negative emotional reactions in my child. 1 2 3 4 5 10. I feel matched stimulation produced little change in my child’s stereotypy rates during session. 1 2 3 4 5 11. Please circle one as your preferred procedure, or circle no preference if all are equally preferred.. Vocal RIRD Matched Stimulation Matched stimulation plus RIRD No Preference Please provide any additional comments/suggestions about the procedure(s): 71 APPENDIX J Treatment Integrity RIRD DATA Sheet 72 APPENDIX K Treatment Integrity MS DATA Sheet 73 REFERENCES Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., DeBar, R. & Florentino, C. (2005). On the role of preference in response competition. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 247-250. Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., MacDonald, R. P. F., & Chung, B. I. (2007). Assessing and treating vocal stereotypy in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 263-275. Aiken, J. M. & Salzberg, C. L. (1984) The effects of a sensory extinction procedure on stereotypic sounds of two autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14(3), 291-299. American Psychiatric Association (2005). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. (4th ed.- text revision). Arlington, VA: Author. Arntzen, E., Tønnessen, I. R., & Brouwer, G. (2006). Reducing aberrant verbal behavior by building a repertoire of rational verbal behavior. Behavioral Interventions, 21, 177-193. Athens, E. S., Vollmer, T. R., Sloman, K. N., St. Peter Pipkin, C. (2008). An analysis of vocal stereotypy and therapist fading. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 291-297. Aurand, J. C., Sisson, L. A., Aach, S. R., Van Hassalt, V. B. (1989). Journal of the Multihandicapped Person, 2(1), 51-61. Barton, L. E., Brulle, A. R., & Repp, A. C. (1986). Maintenance of therapeutic change by momentary DRO. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19, 277-282. Baumeister, A. A. & Forehand, R. (1972). Effects of contingent shock and verbal command on body rocking of retardates. The Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28(4), 586-590. Baumeister, A. & Forehand, R. (1973). Stereotyped acts. International review of research in mental retardation: VI Oxford England: Academic Press. Berkson, G., & Davenport, R. K. (1962). Stereotyped movements of mental defectives: I. initial survey. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 66(6), 849-852. 74 Berkson, G., Mason, W. A., Saxon, S. V. (1962). Situation and stimulus effects on stereotyped behaviors of chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56(4), 786-792. Berkson, G. (1967). Abnormal stereotyped motor acts. In J. Zubin, & H. Hunt (Eds.), Comparative Psychology (pp. 76–94). New York: Grune and Stratton. Berkson, G., Tupa, M., & Sherman, L. (2001). Early development of stereotyped and self-injurious behaviors: I. Incidence. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 104, 539–547. Berkson, G. (2002). Early development of stereotyped and self-injurious behaviors: II. Age trends. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 107(6), 468-477. Bodfish, J. W., McCuller, W. R., Madison, J. M., Register, M., Mailman, R. B., & Lewis, M. H. (1997). Placebo, double-blind evaluation of long-term Naltrexone treatment effects for adults with mental retardation and self-injury. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 9(2), 135-152. Britton, L. N., Carr, J. E., Landaburu, H. J., & Romick, K. S. (2002). Behavioral Interventions, 17, 93– 103. Brusa, E. & Richman, D. (2008). Developing stimulus control for occurrences of stereotypy exhibited by a child with Autism. International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 4(3), 264269. Carr, J. E., Coriaty, S., Wilder, D. A., Gaunt, B. T., Dozier, C. L., Britton, L. N., Avina, C., & Reed, C. L. (2000). A review of “noncontingent” reinforcement as treatment for the aberrant behavior of individuals with developmental disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 21, 377391. Carr, J. E., Nicolson, A. C., & Higbee, T. S. (2000). Evaluation of a brief multiple-stimulus preference assessment in a naturalistic context. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33(3), 353-357. Carr, J. E., Dozier, C. L., Patel, M. R., Adams, A., & Martin, N. (2002). Treatment of automatically reinforced object mouthing with noncontingent reinforcement and 75 response blocking: Experimental analysis and social validation. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 23, 37-44. Carr, E. G., Yarborough, S. C., & Langdon, N. A. (1997). Effects of idiosyncratic stimulus variables on functional analysis outcomes. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 673–686. Carter, S. L., Devlin, S., Doggett, A., Harber, M. M., & Barr, C. (2004). Determining the influence of tangible items on screaming and handmouthing following and inconclusive functional analysis. Behavioral Interventions, 19, 51-58. Charlop, M. H., Kurtz, P. F., & Casey, F. G. (1990). Using aberrant behaviors as reinforcers for autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 163–181. Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Haymes, L. K. (1996). Using obsessions as reinforcers with and without mild reductive procedures to decrease inappropriate behaviors in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(5), 527-546. Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Haymes, L. K. (1998). Using objects of obsession as token reinforcers for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(3), 189-198. Cicero, F. R. (2007). The effects of noncontingent reinforcement and response interruption on stereotypic behavior maintained by automatic reinforcement [Dissertation]. Dissertation obtained from Proquest: Interdisciplinary Dissertations and Theses, UMI No. 3278419. Cooper, J. O., Heorn, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied Behavior Analysis (2nd ed.). Columbus, Ohio: Pearson Merrill Prentice Hall. Dalrymple, A. J. (1989). Sensory extinction of stereotyped object dropping: Identification of a reinforcer for skill training. Behavioral Residential Treatment, 4(2), 99-111. Doughty, S. S., Anderson, C. M., Doughty, A. H., Williams, D. C. & Saunders, K. J. (2007). Discriminative control of punished stereotyped behavior in humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87, 325-336. DeLeon, I. G., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Evaluation of a multiple-stimulus presentation format for assessing reinforcer preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29(4), 519-533. 76 Duker, P. C. & Rasing, E. (1989). Effects of redesigning the physical environment on self-stimulation and on-task behavior in three autistic-type developmentally disabled individuals. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(3), 449-460. Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1987). Social influences on "self-stimulatory" behavior: Analysis and treatment applications. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20(2), 119-223. Durand, V. M. & Crimmins, D. B. (1987). Assessment and treatment of psychotic speech in an autistic child. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(1), 17-28. Egel, A. L. (1981). Reinforcer variation: Implications for motivating and developmentally disabled children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14(3), 345-350. Epstein, L. H., Doke, L. A., Sajwaj, T. E., Sorrell, S., & Rimmer, B. (1974). Generality and sideeffects of overcorrection. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 385–390. Falcomata, T. S., Roane, H. S., Hovanetz, A. N., & Kettering, T. L. (2004). An evaluation of response cost in the treatment of inappropriate vocalizations maintained by automatic reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 83-87. Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., & Amari, A. (1996). Integrating caregiver report with a systematic choice assessment to enhance reinforcer identification. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 10(1), 15-25. Fisher, W. W., Lindauer, S. E., Alterson, C. J., & Thompson, R. H. (1998). Assessment and treatment of destructive behavior maintained by stereotypic object manipulation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 513-527. Foxx, R. M. & Azrin, N. H. (1973). The elimination of autistic self-stimulatory behavior by overcorrection. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 6(1), 1–14. Foxx, R. M. & Meindl, J. (2007). The long-term successful treatment of the aggressive/destructive behaviors of a preadolescent with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 22, 83–97. Friman, P. C. (2000). ‘Transitional objects’ as establishing operations for thumb sucking: A case study. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 507–509 77 Gardenier, N. C., MacDonald, R., & Green, G. (2004).Comparison of direct observational methods for measuring stereotypic behavior in children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 25, 99–118. Goh, H., Iwata, B. A., Shore, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., Lerman, D. C., Ulrich, S. M., & Smith, R. G. (1995). An analysis of the reinforcing properties of hand mouthing. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 269-283. Green, L. & Freed, D. E. (1993). The substitutability of reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 141-158. Gresham, F. M., Gansle, K. A., & Noell, G. H. (1993). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 257263. Hagopian, L. P., & Adelinis, J. D. (2001). Response blocking with and without redirection for the treatment of pica. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 527–530. Hagopian, L. P. & Toole, L. M. (2009). Effects of response blocking and competing stimuli on stereotypic behavior. Behavioral Interventions, 24(2), 117–125. Hall, S., Thorns, T., & Oliver, C. (2003). Structural and environmental characteristics of stereotyped behaviors. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 108, 391–402. Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., Thompson, R. H., & Lindberg, J. S. (2000). A component analysis of "stereotypy as reinforcement" for alternative behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 285-297. Harris, S. L., & Wolchik, S. A. (1979). Suppression of self-stimulation: Three alternative strategies. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 12, 185-198. Harris, S. L., Handleman, J. S., Gordon, R., Kristoff, B. & Fuentes, F. (1991). Changes in cognitive and language functioning of preschool children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21(3), 281-290. Higbee, T.S., Chang, S., & Endicott, K. (2005). Noncontingent access to preferred sensory stimuli as a treatment for automatically reinforced stereotypy. Behavioral Interventions, 20, 177–184. 78 Holcomb, A. & Wolery, M. (1994). Comparative single subject research: Description of designs and discussion of problems. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 14(1), 119-127. Horner, R. D. (1980). The effects of an environmental “enrichment” program on the behavior of institutionalized profoundly retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, 473491. Howard, J. S., Sparkman, C. R., Cohen, H. G., Green, G.., & Stanislaw, H. (2005). A comparison of intensive behavior analytic and eclectic treatments for young children with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities 26, 359–383. Hung, D. W. (1978). Using self-stimulation as reinforcement for autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 369. Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & Richman, G. S. (1982/1994). Toward a functional analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 197-209. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarcone, J. R., Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R. G., Rodgers, T. A., Lerman, D. C., Shore, B. A., Mazaleski, J. L., Goh, H. L., Cowdery, G. E., Kalsher, M. J., McCosh, K. C., Willis, K. D. (1994). The functions of self-injurious behavior: An exeperimental epidemiological analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 215-240. Johnston, J. M. & Pennypacker, H. S. (2009). Steady states and transitions. (3rs Ed.), Strategies and tactics of behavioral research (191-222).New York: Routledge Taylor and Francis Group. Jurbergs, N., Palcic, J., & Kelley, M. (2007). School-home notes with and without response cost: Increasing attention and academic performance in low-income children with attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder. School Psychology Quarterly, 22(3), 358-379. Kahng, S. W. & Iwata, B. A. (1998). Play versus alone conditions as controls during functional analyses of self-injurious escape behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 669-672. Keeney, K. M., Fisher, W. W., Adelinis, J. D., & Wilder, D. A. (2000). The effects of response cost in the treatment of aberrant behavior maintained by negative reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 255–258. 79 Kennedy, C. H. (1994). Automatic reinforcement: Oxymoron or hypothetical construct? Journal of Behavioral Education, 4, 387–396. Kennedy, C. H., Meyer, K. A., Knowles, T., & Shukla, S. (2000). Analyzing the multiple functions of stereotypical behavior for students with autism: Implications for assessment and treatment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 559-571. Kern, L., Koegel, R. L., Dyer, K., Blew, P. A., & Fenton, L. R. (1982). The effects of physical exercise on self-stimulation and appropriate responding in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 12(4), 399-419 Kern, L., Koegel, R. L. & Dunlap, G. (1984). The influence of vigorous versus mild exercise on autistic stereotyped behaviors. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14(1), 57-67. Kish, G. B. (1955). A demonstration of learning when the onset of illumination is used as a reinforcing stimulus. American Psychologist, 48(4), 261-264. Koegel, R. L., & Covert, A. (1972). The relationship of self-stimulation to learning in autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 381-387. Koegal, R. L., Firestone, P. B., Kramme, K. W., & Dunlap, G. (1974). Increasing spontaneous play by suppressing self-stimulation in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7(4), 521528. Kravitz, H., & Boehm, J. J. (1971). Rhythmic habit patterns in infancy: Their sequence age of onset and frequency. Child Development, 42, 399–413. Lalli, J. S., Livezey, K., & Kates, K. (1996). Functional analysis and treatment of eye poking with response blocking. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 2, 129-132 Lee, S., Odom, S. L., & Loftin, R. (2007). Social engagement with peers and stereotypic behavior in autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 9(2), 67-79. Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). A methodology for distinguishing between extinction and punishment effects associated with response blocking. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 231–234. 80 Lerman, D. C., Kelley, M. E., Vorndran, C. M., & Van Camp, C. M. (2003). Collateral effects of response blocking during the treatment of stereotypic behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 119–123. Lerman, D. C., & Vordran, C. M. (2002). On the status of knowledge for using punishment: Implications for treating behavior disorders. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 431 464. Lewis, M., & Bodfish, J. (1998). Repetitive behavior disorders in autism. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 4(2), 80-89. Leuba, C. (1955). Toward some integration of learning theories: The concept of optimal stimulation. Psychological Reports, 1, 27-33. Liberman, R. P., Teigen, J., Patterson, R., & Baker, V. (1973). Reducing delusional speech in chronic, paranoid schizophrenics. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 6, 57–64. Lindberg, J. S., Iwata, B. A., Roscoe, E. M., Worsdell, A. S., & Hanley, G. P. (2003). Treatment efficacy of noncontingent reinforcement during brief and extended application. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 1-19. Liu-Gitz, L. & Banda, D. R. (2010). A replication of the rird strategy to decrease vocal stereotypy in a child with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 25, 77-87. Loftin, R., Odom, S., & Lantz, J. (2008). Social interaction and repetitive motor behaviors. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(6), 1124-1135. Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal intellectual and educational functioning in autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 3-9. Lovaas, O. I., Newsom, C., & Hickman, C. (1987). Self-stimulatory behavior and perceptual reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 45–68. Mace, F. C., & Belfiore, P. (1990). Behavioral momentum in the treatment of escape motivated stereotypy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 507-514. 81 Mace, F. C., Browder, D. M., & Lin, Y. L. (1987). Analysis of demand conditions associated with stereotypy. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 18, 25-31. Mace, F.C., & Knight, D. (1986). Functional analysis and treatment of severe pica. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19(4), 411-416. Maclean, W. F., Ellis, D. N., Galbreath, H. N., Halpern, L. F., & Baumeister, A. A. (1991). Rhythmic motor behavior of perambulatory, motor impaired and nondisabled children: A comparative analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 19(3), 319-330. Matson, J. L., Kiely, S. L., & Bamburg, J. W. (1997). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 18(6), 471-476. Matson, J. L., Minshawi, N. F., Gonzalez, M. L., & Mayville, S. B. (2006). The relationship of comorbid problem behaviors to social skills in persons with profound mental retardation. Behavior Modification, 30, 496–506. McEachin, J. J., Smith, T., & Lovaas, I. O. (1993). Long-term outcome for children with autism who received early intensive behavioral treatment. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97(4), 359-372. McEntee, J. E. & Saunders, R. R. (1997). A response restriction analysis of stereotypy in adolescents with mental retardation: Implications for applied behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 485–506. Michael, J. (1982). Distinguishing between discriminative and motivational functions of stimuli. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37(1), 149–155. Michael, J. L. (1993/2003). Concepts and Principles of Behavior Analysis. Revised Edition. Kalamazoo, MI: Association for Behavior Analysis International. Miguel, C. M., Clark, K., Tereshko, L., & Ahearn, W. H. (2009). The effects of response interruption and redirection and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (sertraline) on vocal stereotypy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 883–888. 82 Morrison, K., & Rosales-Ruis, J. (1997). The effect of object preferences on task performance and stereotypy in a child with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 18(2), 127–137. Najdowski, A. C., Wallace, M. D., Penrod, B., & Cleveland, J. (2005). Using stimulus variation to increase reinforcer efficacy of low preference stimuli. Behavioral Interventions, 20, 313-328. O’Reilly, M. F., Sigafoos, Lancioni, G., Edrisinha, C., & Andrews, A. (2005). An examination of the effects of a classroom activity schedule on levels of self-injury and engagement for a child with severe autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(3), 305-311. O’Reilly, M. F., Sigafoos, J., Edrisinha, C., Lancioni, G., Cannela, H., Choi, H. Y., & Barretto, A. (2006). A preliminary examination of the evocative effects of the establishing operation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 239-242. O’Reilly, M., Edrisinha, C., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G., Cannella, H., Machalicek, W., & Langthorne, P. (2007). Manipulating the evocative and abative effects of an establishing operation: influences on challenging behavior during classroom instruction. Behavioral Interventions, 22, 137-145. O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G., Rispoli, M., Lang, R., Chan, J., Machalicek, W., & Langthorne, P. (2008). Manipulating the behavior-altering effect of the motivating operation: Examination of the influence on challenging behavior during leisure activities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 29, 333-340. Paisey, T. J. H., Whitney, R. B., & Wainczak, S. M. (1993). Case study: Noninvasive behavioral treatments of self-injurious hand stereotypy in a child with rett syndrome. Behavioral Residential Treatment, 8(2), 133-145. Piazza, C. C., Adelinis, J. D., Hanley, G. P., Goh, H., & Delia, M. D. (2000). An evaluation of the effects of matched stimuli on behaviors maintained by automatic reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 13-27. Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., LeBlanc, L. A., Worsdell, A. S., Lindauer, S. T., &Keeney, K. M. (1998). Treatment of pica through multiple analyses of its reinforcing functions. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 165–189. 83 Powers, S., Thibadeau, S., & Rose, K. (1992). Antecedent exercise and its effects on self-stimulation. Behavioral Residential Treatment, 7(1), 15-22. Ramey, S. L., & Ramey, C. T. (1999). Early experience and early intervention for children at risk for developmental delay and mental retardation. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 5, 1–10. Rapp, J. T., Dozier, C. L., Carr, J. E., Patel, M. R., & Enloe, K. A. (2000). Functional analysis of hair manipulation: A replication and extension. Behavioral Interventions, 15: 121-133. Rapp, J. T., Vollmer, T. V., Dozier, C. L., St. Peter, C., & Cotnoir, N. (2004). Analysis of response allocation in individuals with multiple forms of stereotyped behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 481–501. Rapp, J. T. & Vollmer, T. R. (2005). Stereotypy I: A review of behavioral assessment and treatment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26(6), 527-547. Rapp, J. T. (2006) Toward an empirical method for identifying matched stimulation for automatically reinforced behavior: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 137-140. Rapp, J. T. (2007). Further evaluation of methods to identify matched stimulation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 73-88. Reid, M. S., Ho, L. B., Tolliver, B. K., Wolkowitz, O. M., Berger, S. B. (1998). Partial reversal of stressinduced behavioral sensitization to amphetamine following metyrapone treatment. Brain Research, 783, 133–142. Repp, A. C., & Barton, L. E. (1980). Naturalistic observations of institutionalized retarded persons: A comparison of licensure decisions and behavioral observations. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13(2), 333-341. Repp, A. C., Karsh, K. G., Deitz, D. E. D., & Singh, N. N. (1992). A study of the homeostatic level of stereotypy and other motor movements of persons with mental handicaps. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 36, 61–75. 84 Rilling, M. (1977). Stimulus control and inhibitory processes. In W. K. Honig, & J. E. R. Statton (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Rincover, A. (1978). Sensory extinction: A procedure for eliminating self-stimulatory behavior in developmentally disabled children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 6(3), 299- 310. Rincover, A., Cook, R., Peoples, A., & Packard, D. (1979). Sensory extinction and sensory reinforcement principles for programming multiple adaptive behavior change. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 12, 221-233. Ringdahl, J. E., Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & Roane, H. S. (1997). An analogue evaluation of environmental enrichment: The role of stimulus preference. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 203–216. Roane, H. S., Vollmer, T. R., Ringdahl, J. E., & Marcus, B. A. (1998). Evaluation of a brief stimulus preference assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 605–620. Roscoe, E. M., Carreau, A., MacDonald, J., & Pence, S. T. (2008). Further evaluation of leisure items in the attention condition of functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 351364. Rozenblat, E., Brown, J. L., Brown, A. K., Reeve, S. A., & Reeve, K. F. (2009). Effects of adjusting DRO schedules on the reduction of stereotypic vocalizations in children with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 24(1), 1–15. Sheinkopf, S. J., & Siegel, B. (1998). Home-based behavioral treatment of children with autism. Joumal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(1), 15-23. Skinner, B. F. (1945/1992). ‘Superstition’ in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121(3), 273-274. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal Behavior. Acton, Massachusetts: Copley Publishing Group. Smith, T. (1999). Outcome of early intervention for children with autism. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 6(1), 33–49. 85 Smith, T., Eikeseth, S., Klevstrand, M., & Lovaas, O. I. (1997). Intensive behavioral treatment for preschoolers with severe mental retardation and pervasive developmental disorder. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 102(3), 238-249. Sprague, J., Holland, K., & Thomas, K. (1997).The effects of noncontingent sensory reinforcement, contingent sensory reinforcement, and response interruption on stereotypical and self-injurious behavior. Research in Developmental Disabilities,18(1), 61-77. Tang, J. C., Patterson, T. G., & Kennedy, C. H. (2003). Identifying specific sensory modalities maintaining the stereotypy of students with profound disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26, 433–451. Taylor, B.A., Hoch, H., & Weissman, M. (2005). The analysis and treatment of vocal stereotypy in a child with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 20(4), 239-253. Thelen, E. (1981). Rhythmical behavior in infancy: An ethological perspective. Developmental Psychology, 17(3), 237-257. Thompson, R. H., Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., & Kuhn, D. E. (1998). The evaluation and treatment of aggression maintained by attention and automatic reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 103-116. Tröster, H. (1994). Prevalence and functions of stereotyped behaviors in nonhandicapped children in residential care. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22(1), 79-97. Vaughan, M. E., & Michael, J. (1982). Automatic reinforcement: An important but ignored concept. Behaviorism, 10(2), 217-227. Van Camp, C. M., Lerman, D. C., Kelley, M. E., Roane, H. S., Contrucci, S. A., & Vorndan, C. M. (2000). Further analysis of idiosyncratic antecedent influences during the assessment and treatment of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 207–221. Vollmer, T. R., & Iwata, B. A. (1991). Establishing operations and reinforcement effects. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 279–291. 86 Vollmer, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Duncan, B. A., & Lerman, D. C. (1993). Extensions of multielement functional analyses using reversal-type designs. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 5, 311-325. Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & LeBlanc, L. (1994). Treatment of self-injury and handmouthing following inconclusive functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 331-44. Vordran, C. M., & Lerman, D. C. (2006). Establishing and maintaining treatment effects with less intrusive consequences via a pairing procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 3548. Wacker, D., McMahon, C., Steege, M., Berg, W., Sasso, G., & Melloy, K. (1990). Applications of a sequential alternating treatments design. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 333-339. Wallace, M. D. & Iwata, B. A. (1999). Effects of session duration on functional analysis outcomes. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 175-183. Watters, R. G., & Watters, W. E. (1980). Decreasing self-stimulatory behavior with physical exercise in a group of autistic boys. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10(4), 379-387. Watters, R. G., & Wood, D. E. (1983). Play and self-stimulatory behaviors of autistic and other severely dysfunctional children with different classes of toys. Journal of Special Education, 17, 27–35. Wells, K. C., Forehand, R., Hickey, K., & Green, K. D. (1977). Effects of a procedure derived from the overcorrection principle on manipulated and nonmanipulated behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 679-687. Wilder, D. A., Atwell, J. & Wine, B. (2006). The effects of varying levels of treatment integrity on child compliance during treatment with a three-step prompting procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 369-373. Wilder, D. A., Masuda, A., O’Conner, C., & Baham, M. (2001). Brief functional analysis and treatment of bizarre vocalizations in an adult with schizophrenia. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 65-68. Willemsen-Swinkels, S. H. N., Buitelaar, J. K., Dekker, M., & van Engeland, H. (1998). 87 Subtyping stereotypic behavior in children: The association between stereotypic behavior, mood, and heart rate. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 547-557. Wolery, M., Kirk, K., & Gast, D.L. (1985). Stereotypic behavior as a reinforcer: Effects and side effects. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15(2), 149-161.