Chapter 2: Psychology As a Science

Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception

Chapter Outline

1.

2.

3.

4.

5.

Common features of sensation and perception

The chemical senses: Smell and taste

The tactile or cutaneous senses: Touch, pressure, pain, vibration

The auditory sense: Hearing

The visual sense: Sight

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Basic Definitions

Sensation—the act of using our sensory systems to detect environmental stimuli

Perception— recognizing and identifying sensory stimulus

Raw Sensory Data

Vision Light waves

Hearing Sound waves

Smell Airborne chemicals

Taste Food chemicals

Touch Pressure

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Common Features of Sensation and

Perception

What do we need to do to the raw sensory data so that our brain can understand it?

Sensory receptor cells—specialized cells that convert a specific form of environmental stimuli into neural impulses.

Sensory transduction—the process of converting a specific form of sensory data into a neural impulse that our brain can read

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Sensory Receptor Cells

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Thresholds: Testing the Limits

Absolute threshold is the smallest amount of a stimulus that one can detect

For example, what is the dimmest light you can see?

 Difference threshold (or JND)—the minimal difference needed to notice a difference between two stimuli

For example, when do you perceive a difference in change of volume in your ear?

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Sensory Adaptation

Constant stimulation decreases the number of sensory messages sent to the brain, which causes decreased sensation. Why? We can’t afford to waste attention on unchanging stimuli

Examples:

A crying baby will wake us, but not a thunderstorm that might be even louder

You notice how tight your pants are when you first put them on but over the course of the day they seem looser

(you don’t register the sensation of the tightness)

You do not notice how much perfume you put on; at first it seems good but then you do not smell it so you put on more and more…

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Processing Sensory Information

Bottom-up processing—the raw sensory data is sent to the brain and your brain uses all of that data to build a perception

You take 1,000s of data points of visual stimuli and put them together to create an image of your mother

Top-down processing—you use previously learned information to help recognize and interpret the data coming into your brain

You recognize some of those data points and immediately match them to your previous knowledge about your mother’s face

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Perceptual Set

Perceptual set is the readiness to interpret a certain stimulus in a certain way

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

How Do We Smell?

 o

Chemicals, called odorants, are carried through the air and reach the 5 million receptor cells located at the top of each nasal cavity

Olfactory receptor neurons o

The receptor cells turn that molecule into a neural impulse (transduction) and send that impulse to the olfactory bulb (smell centre in the brain)

Lock-and-key binding

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Human Olfactory System

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

How Do We Taste?

Chemical substances in the food we eat dissolve in saliva and fall into the crevices between the bumps

(papillae) of the tongue

The bumps hold our taste buds

Each taste bud contains 60 to 100 sensory receptor cells for taste

Taste buds are our receptor cells for taste

The taste buds translate the chemical message into a neural impulse and send that impulse to the thalamus and, eventually, the cerebral cortex

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

A Taste Bud

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Five Taste Receptors

1.

4.

5.

2.

3.

Sweet

Sour

Bitter

Salt

Umami—the taste of monosodium glutamate

(MSG)

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Eating Is More than Taste and Smell

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Smell and Taste Disorders

Anosmia—inability to smell

Ageusia—inability to taste

Both are usually caused by head trauma

Other conditions:

Reflex epilepsy—a seizure occurs only after exposure to a specific odour

Migraine headaches—specific odours can trigger migraines

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Tactile or Cutaneous Senses

 The tactile or somatosensory system is a combination of skin senses:

Pressure, touch, temperature, vibration, pain

The tactile senses rely on a variety of receptors located in different parts of the skin

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Sensory Receptors in the Skin

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Different Sensory Receptors

Free nerve endings

Located near the surface of the skin

Function: Detect touch, pressure, or pain

Meissner’s corpuscles

Located in fingertips, lips, and palms (hairless skin areas)

Function: Transduce information about sensitive touch

Merkel’s discs

Located near the surface of the skin

Function: Transduce information about light to moderate pressure against the skin

Ruffini’s end organs

Located deep in the skin

Function: Register heavy pressure and movement of the joints

Pacinian corpuscles

Located deep in the skin

Function: Respond to vibrations and heavy pressure.

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Steps to Perceiving Touch

1.

2.

3.

Sensory neurons register pressure

Information sent to the spinal cord, then the thalamus

(telephone operator)

Information is then sent to the somatosensory cortex that registers the sensation

1.

Tactile information is processed contralaterally

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Two Pathways of Pain

Fast pathway pain (myelinated pathway)—sharp, localized pain is felt quicker because it travels along myelinated neurons to the brain

Slow burn (unmyelinated pathway)—nagging, burning pain is slower to be felt because it travels along unmyelinated pathways

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Why Do We Feel or Not Feel Pain?

Gate control theory—patterns of neural activity can actually close a “gate” that prevents messages from reaching parts of the brain where they are perceived as pain

Pain threshold

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Chronic Pain

Chronic pain is the most common abnormality associated with the somatosensory system

Endorphins and enkephalins are naturally occurring pain-killing chemicals in the brain

They can be found in opiates such as morphine or heroine

They are produced naturally through intense physical activity, sex, or intense stress (endogenous opiates)

Cingulotomy—destruction of the cingulate cortex

An extreme form of neurosurgery to relieve chronic pain

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Pain Abnormalities

Familial dysautonomia—rare genetic condition associated with an inability to detect pain or temperature

Phantom limb sensations—tactile hallucinations of touch, pressure, vibration, and pain in the body part that no longer exists

“Mirror box” therapy

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Properties of Sound

 Sound waves—vibrations of the air in the frequency of hearing

 Frequency—the number of cycles per second in a wave

Determines pitch of sound

Measured in units called Hertz (Hz), which represent cycles per second

 Amplitude—the magnitude (height of a wave)

Determines loudness

Measured in units called decibels (dB)

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

How the Ear Hears

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Hearing

1.

2.

3.

Sound waves enter the outer ear

Hits the eardrum (tympanic membrane)

Thin membrane that moves with sound waves

Passes into the middle ear

Contains the 3 smallest bones

(or ossicles) in the human body: maleus (hammer), incus (anvil), and stapes

(stirrup)

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Hearing

4.

5.

6.

Stapes hits the oval window and creates vibrations that move fluid in the cochlea

Oval window—membrane separating the ossicles and the inner ear

The vibrations move the basilar membrane (in the cochlea), which is covered with sensory receptors called hair cells

As hair cells move, neural impulses are created and sent to the brain

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Different Theories of Hearing

Place theory

Vibration of the basilar membrane (BM) at different places results in different pitches/frequencies

Near the oval window (where BM is thinner)—higher frequencies; lower frequencies occur farther from the oval window

Frequency theory

Different sound frequencies are converted into different rates of action potentials or firing

High-frequency sounds produce a more rapid firing than do low-frequency sounds

Different firing rates contribute to sound perception of low frequency tones

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Sound Localization

General loudness—louder sounds seem closer

Loudness in each ear—the ear closer to the sound hears a louder noise than the ear farther from the sound

Timing—sound waves will reach the ear closer to the source of the sound before they reach the ear farther away

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Hearing and the Brain

 Cochlea  brainstem  thalamus  auditory cortex

 auditory association areas in the cortex

Tonotopic map—information transmitted from different parts of the cochlea is projected to specific parts of the auditory cortex

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Auditory Abnormalities

Deafness

Can be genetic or caused by infection, physical trauma, or exposure to toxins, including overdose of common medications such as Aspirin

Tinnitus—ringing in the ear

Usually due to abnormalities in the ear

One of every 200 people experiences tinnitus

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Stimulus for Vision

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Steps Involved in Vision

1.

Light waves enter the cornea

Cornea is protective outer layer

2.

Passes through the pupil

The pupil is a small opening in the eye

3.

Passes through the lens

The lens focuses the light waves

4.

Projected onto the retina

Retina contains all of the receptor cells (rods and cones)

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Steps Involved in Vision

5.

The rods and cones transduce the light waves into a neural impulse.

Rods

Used for periphery and night vision

Not as acute as cones (i.e., fuzzy vision)

Many more rods than cones (over

100 million)

Cones

Used for central and colour vision

Very acute (i.e., very clear)

The fovea in the centre of the eye is all cones

Not as many cones (4–5 million)

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Steps Involved in Vision

6.

The neural impulses are sent to the optic nerve

• The optic nerve contains the axons of 1 million ganglion cells that extend through the wall of the retina and go to the brain.

7.

The optic nerve carries messages from each eye to the visual cortex (occipital lobe)

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Why Do We See in Colour?

Trichomatic theory—There are three different sensors for colour and each type responds to a different range of wavelengths of light

 We see more than three colours, which is he variety of colours arise from combining the three colours

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Opponent-Process Theory

The activation of one cone (at retinal level) inhibits another cone

This theory explains colour vision at the level of the ganglion cells

Ganglion cells are arranged in opposing cells: redgreen, yellow-blue, black-white

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Afterimages

• Opponent-process theory may explain colour afterimages: continual viewing of red weakens the ability to inhibit green; remove red and you see green

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Afterimages (blank page to see effect)

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Colour Blindness

• Most people who are colour blind cannot distinguish between red and green; they would see only a random pattern of dots in this figure.

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

“What” and “Where” Pathways

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

“What” Pathway

Visual agnosia—damage to the “what” pathway; cannot recognize objects

Prosopagnosia—a form of visual agnosia in which people cannot recognize faces

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

“Where” Pathway

Hemi-neglect—damage to the “where” pathway; people ignore one side of their visual field

Examples of effects: apply makeup to only one side of face, shave only one side of face, eat food on only one side of plate

People with damage to the right side of their “where” pathways neglect the left side of their visual field

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Top-Down Processing

The process by which we organize small pieces of sensory experience into meaningful wholes

Gestalt principles

Visual information is organized into coherent images

Gestalt means whole or totality

The whole is more than the sum of its parts

Proximity—stimuli near to one another tend to be grouped together

Similarity—stimuli resembling one another tend to be grouped together

Continuity—stimuli falling along the same plane tend to be grouped together

Good form—stimuli forming a shape tend to be grouped together while those that do not remain ungrouped

Closure—we tend to fill in small gaps in objects so that they are still perceived as whole objects

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Impossible Figure

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Grouping Rules

Proximity—we group nearby figures together

Similarity—figures similar to each other are grouped together

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Closure

Closure—tendency to perceive incomplete figures as whole and complete

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Depth Perception

Binocular cues are cues from both eyes

Convergence—the tendency of the eyes to move toward each other as we focus on objects up close

Binocular disparitydifferent images of objects are cast on the retinas of each eye

When images are very different, we perceive object is close by

When images are similar, we perceive object is farther away

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Monocular Cues

Monocular cues are cues from one eye

Interposition—when one object blocks part of another from our view, we see the blocked object as farther away.

Elevation—we see objects that are higher in our visual plane as farther away than those that are lower.

Texture gradient—we can see more details of textured surfaces, such as the wood grain on a restaurant table, that are closer to us.

Linear perspective—parallel lines seem to converge in the distance.

Shading—we are accustomed to light, such as sunlight, coming from above us. We use differences in the shading of light from the top to the bottom of our field of view to judge size and distance of objects.

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Monocular Cues

Clarity or arial perspective—we tend to see closer objects with more clarity than objects that are further away. Realizing that this is sometimes referred to as a fog or smog effect will clarify what we mean by it.

Familiar size—once we have learned the sizes of objects, such as people or restaurant plates, we assume that they stay the same size, so objects that look smaller than usual must be farther away than usual

Relative size—when we look at two objects we know are about the same size, if one seems smaller than the other, we see it as farther away than the other.

Motion Parallax—if we look out the side window of a moving car or train, objects that are closer to us appear to move past us more quickly than do objects that are further away (and very distant objects (like mountains) sometimes do not appear to move at all).

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Monocular Cues and Illusions

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Pavement Patty

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Perceptual Constancy

Size constancywe perceive objects as the same size regardless of the distance from which it is viewed

Shape constancy—we see an object as the same shape no matter from what angle it is viewed

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Shape and Size Constancy: The Ames Room

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Magnetic Hill, Moncton

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Visual Abnormalities

Blindness—About 278,000 people in Canada suffer from visual impairments, of which about 108,000 are legally blind

Amblyopia—a loss of visual abilities in a weaker eye

Caused by abnormal development of the brain’s visual cortex due to a failure to receive visual stimulation from both eyes by the age of six

Strabismus—lack of coordinated movement of both eyes; can lead to amblyopia; affects about 2 percent of the population

© John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd.

Copyright

Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd. All rights reserved. Reproduction or translation of this work beyond that permitted by Access Copyright (The Canadian Copyright

Licensing Agency) is unlawful. Requests for further information should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John

Wiley & Sons Canada, Ltd. The purchaser may make back-up copies for his or her own use only and not for distribution or resale. The author and the publisher assume no responsibility for errors, omissions, or damages caused by the use of these programs or from the use of the information contained herein.