How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. Research on consumers’ reaction to corporate response to negative publicity Sten Boerkamp – 306442 Economics and Business Economics Master Marketing Erasmus School of Economics Erasmus University Rotterdam First reader and thesis supervisor: Ms. I. Versluis September 2014 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Abstract Companies facing negative publicity have to decide whether or not they should respond, and if so, in what manner. In this thesis two different response strategies and a No response scenario are tested for a well-known and a less-known cola brand. It is found that consumers have significantly lower brand attitudes and likelihood to buy in all scenarios. For the well-known brand, Coca Cola in the experiment, it is important to respond to the publicity to minimise damage. The Adaptation of new processes strategy, a strong effort of a company involving a confirmation of the risks and a change of the production process, did not outperform the Denial of severity strategy. The latter strategy consist of a confirmation of the presence of disputed ingredients, but includes a strong denial on the alleged risks they form. In other words, the company states there is nothing to worry about. For lessknown cola brand First Choice, responding to the news did not have a dampening effect on the change in consumer brand attitudes. The proposed model introduces a direct effect of brand familiarity and moderating effects of source credibility and perceived article negativity. Brand familiarity does not have any predictive power on the change in brand attitudes. The credibility of the source does have a significant impact; consumers who perceive the source as more credible have a stronger negative change in brand attitudes. The perceived article negativity is a robust predictor of attitude changes. It is important to correct for this perceived article negativity to be able to find good results. Consumers expect a response of well-known brands on allegations. Although a response cannot prevent all damage to a brand, it has a considerably smaller negative effect on consumer brand attitudes as compared to a No response strategy. The more credible the source of the article is considered, the stronger the negative effect on consumer brand attitudes. 2 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Content Abstract ....................................................................................................................................... 2 1: Introduction ............................................................................................................................. 5 1.1: Problem statement + Research Questions .................................................................................. 6 1.2: Relevance of topic........................................................................................................................ 8 1.2.1: Theoretical ............................................................................................................................ 8 1.2.2: Practical................................................................................................................................. 8 2: Theory ..................................................................................................................................... 9 2.1: Literature review.......................................................................................................................... 9 2.1.1: Company crisis: product-harm.............................................................................................. 9 2.1.2: Company response effect ................................................................................................... 10 2.1.3: Buying intention .................................................................................................................. 13 2.1.4: Brand familiarity ................................................................................................................. 14 2.1.5: Publicity............................................................................................................................... 17 2.1.6: Source credibility ................................................................................................................ 18 2.1.7: Responsibility factor and attribution theory ...................................................................... 18 2.1.8: Publicity and the effect of marketing efforts ...................................................................... 19 2.1.9: Impact of publicity over time .............................................................................................. 20 2.2: Conceptual model + hypotheses................................................................................................ 21 3: Method.................................................................................................................................. 23 3.1: Research design ......................................................................................................................... 23 3.2: Experiment ................................................................................................................................. 25 3.2.1: Response bias...................................................................................................................... 28 3.2.2: Validity of scales.................................................................................................................. 28 3.2.3: Manipulation checks ........................................................................................................... 30 3.2.4: Outlier ................................................................................................................................. 32 4: Results ................................................................................................................................... 32 4.1: Differences between response strategies ................................................................................. 32 4.1.1: Effect on delta consumer brand attitude ........................................................................... 32 4.1.2: Ranking strategies ............................................................................................................... 34 4.2: Brand attitudes and likelihood to buy ....................................................................................... 36 4.3: Brand familiarity ........................................................................................................................ 37 4.3.1: Effect on delta consumer brand attitude ........................................................................... 37 3 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 4.3.2: Effect on delta likelihood to buy ......................................................................................... 38 4.4: Article negativity ........................................................................................................................ 40 4.5: Source credibility ....................................................................................................................... 43 4.6: Attitudes towards company response ....................................................................................... 44 5: General discussion ................................................................................................................. 45 5.1: Answering research questions ................................................................................................... 46 5.1.1: Problem Statement ............................................................................................................. 46 5.1.2: Research question 1 ............................................................................................................ 48 5.1.3: Research question 2 ............................................................................................................ 49 5.1.4: Research question 3 ............................................................................................................ 50 5.1.5: Research question 4 ............................................................................................................ 51 5.2: Academic & Managerial Implications ........................................................................................ 52 5.2.1: Academic ............................................................................................................................. 52 5.2.2: Managerial .......................................................................................................................... 53 5.3: Limitations & Further Research ................................................................................................. 54 5.3.1: Limitations .......................................................................................................................... 54 5.3.2: Further research ................................................................................................................. 55 6: References ............................................................................................................................. 56 7: Appendix ............................................................................................................................... 61 4 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 1: Introduction Consumers are increasingly aware of their health and safety and act upon it. But what happens when the trust of consumers is undermined by news articles? In 2009, Toyota issued a recall for 3.9 million vehicles in the U.S. due to problematic floor mats, leading to sudden and uncontrollable acceleration. The problems were widely described in blogs. Only after the news hit the majority of mainstream media, brand attitudes towards Toyota decreased significantly and Toyota was not able to recover for more than a year (Fan, Geddes, & Flory, 2011). In Belgium, reports of school children becoming ill after drinking Coca Cola led to mass hysteria (Nemery, Fischler, Boogaerts, Lison, & Willems, 2002). Allegedly the products contained dioxin, which was also found in chicken related food crises at the same time. The negative publicity forced Coca Cola to recall millions of beverages, although the products did not pose a health risk. The gold standard of company response to negative publicity is considered by many the case of the tampering with Johnson & Johnson’s Tylenol. Some of the bottles were deliberately contaminated with cyanide, a deadly poison, after they left the factory. Johnson & Johnson recalled all Tylenol products within a week and designed new tamper-proof bottles which were available within ten weeks. It also offered a $100,000 reward for the tip leading to the murderer, which was never claimed. The company regained their full market share (Murray & Shohen, 1992). Not all cases of negative publicity are as clear as the ones mentioned. In the previous examples there was an immediate health risk. In this research the focus lies on alleged health risks after repeated use of a product. For example, consuming too much sugar can pose serious health risks, but it can take years before it can harm an individual. Some products contain ingredients that are potentially harmful, but it may be subject of debate. One such ingredient is aspartame, found in numerous light variants of products. How consumers react to negative publicity on this kind of unclear risks is of interest in this study. Companies have a wide range of options when news gets out. In situations without an immediate health risk, it is not clear how consumers will react. For a company it is important to know how to respond to minimise damage to the brand. Bradford and Garrett (1995) identified five main response strategies: no response, denial, excuse, justification and concession. In the unclear risk situation a company may choose a different response strategy as compared to an immediate health risk situation. For instance, when a company does not respond to an immediate health risk, this can be seen as irresponsible. Concerning a non-immediate health risk it might be acceptable to choose a no response strategy, as not all information is available. 5 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Negative publicity can have different subjects, such as, product risks, incidents, unethical behaviour, recalls or other company actions (Kalaitzandonakes, Marks, & Vickner, 2004; Kasperson et al, 1988). Negative publicity has been defined as the noncompensated dissemination of potentially damaging information by presenting disparaging news about a product, service, business unit, or individual in print or broadcast media or by word of mouth (Sherell & Reidenbach, 1986). Blogs, social media, reviews and other forms of online communication can also be considered as mediums broadcasting negative publicity. This research is focussed on negative publicity in credible traditional media. Kasperson et al. (1988) found a negativity bias in media coverage, a preference of journalist and editors to publish negative articles over positive articles. Consumers’ perceived risk is related to the amount of positive and negative media coverage they receive. Since media have a preference to cover rare or dramatic risks, companies face increased attention of media when something goes wrong. The negativity bias also increases the difficulty for companies to have their response be heard. The statement “there is no such thing as negative publicity” is popular, although this is not correct for most cases (Berger, Sorensen, & Rasmussen, 2010). And as publicity gets spread more quickly through social media and other innovative technologies, it becomes harder for firms to manage publicity and react properly and in time. From a marketing perspective, brands are built carefully, which reflects in intangible assets (Simon & Sullivan, 1993). Publicity has an effect on the strength of a brand. Most often publicity management is done by the public relationship department, with other goals than the marketing department (Jeffries-Fox, 2001). Companies aware of this inconsistency can align the targets of public relationship with the targets of marketing. Increased cooperation can create a single message communicated to external parties, protecting brand value. 1.1: Problem statement + Research Questions More and more research on publicity explores different relationships between publicity, consumer brand attitudes, choice and sales, and a number of possible moderating variables. In this research the main questions is about the moderating effect of a company’s response. What strategy do companies have to follow for best results? Do more familiar brands need a different strategy? 6 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Problem Statement: This paper investigates the best response strategy as measured by consumer brand attitudes for two brands of cola. Four research questions are formulated to find an answer to the problem statement and to provide structure. First the general effect of negative publicity on consumer brand attitudes is examined. Research question 1: What is the effect of negative publicity on consumer brand attitudes? To find out what the best response strategy on negative publicity is, first the effect of negative publicity itself must be clear. Consumer brand attitudes are used to measure how consumers feel about the brand on different aspects. Research question 2: Can a company’s response strategy limit the effect of negative publicity on consumer brand attitudes? Companies can use different types of responses, in which they accept responsibility, propose solutions, or deny allegations. The reaction of consumers on the negative publicity and on the company response combined does lead to updated consumer brand attitudes. Of primary interest is the change in consumer brand attitudes between the initial attitudes and the attitudes after the news and company response. Research question 3: Does company familiarity moderate the effect of negative publicity on consumer brand attitudes? It is not expected that negative publicity affects consumer brand attitudes in the same manner under all conditions. The familiarity of a consumer with a brand is a proxy for their knowledge about the brand. It is interesting to see if consumers react differently on news regarding familiar brands as they have more prior information and possible stronger attitudes. Research question 4: Does negative publicity affect purchase intentions? The experiment measures consumer brand attitudes and likelihood to buy. Reibstein (1978) created a model for attitudes and behaviour: Attitude forms preference, which leads to behavioural intention, and results in behaviour. The seriousness of the negative publicity may influence the effect on purchase intentions. From a managerial point of view the link between attitude change and purchase intention and behaviour is especially important as it affects sales directly. 7 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 1.2: Relevance of topic 1.2.1: Theoretical The effect of publicity on brand attitudes and choice did not receive as much attention in the past as research on the effect of advertising, price, promotion and other marketing tools did. In the last decade the knowledge about the effect of publicity has increased, specifically related to moderating and mediating variables on the severity of the news, consumer attitudes, brand reputation, and response strategies. It is interesting to see if publicity can be managed or countered to the benefit of the company. Some research to company’s crisis management strategies have been conducted, but to my knowledge there are few experiments conducted to specifically measure the effects of different company responses. One example of a relevant research is the paper of Dean (2004) which measures the reaction of consumers on negative publicity and the effects of company reputation, style of response and responsibility. His experiment uses fictitious brands and is therefore likely to overstate consumer response, since consumers do not have pre-existing attitudes towards the brands which can form a buffer (Griffin, Babin, & Attaway, 1991). In real world data the effect of publicity is incredibly hard to measure, given the number of factors that influence sales. In a research conducted in Australia, on iodized salt, a time series analysis was possible since iodized salt was never promoted, advertised or discounted (Li, Chapman, Agho, & Eastman, 2008). Publicity on the positive effects of iodized salt versus regular salt resulted in a significant increase of sales. The current research adds to the existing literature the effect of publicity on well-known real world brands and the moderating effect of different company responses. Furthermore, it provides insight on specific factors of consumer behaviour on negative publicity and company response. The experimental setting using real world brands is preferable over case studies when isolation of factors is important. 1.2.2: Practical Companies can lose sales when the quality and safety of their products is disputed in the media. In a state of ambiguity, consumers change their attitudes towards a brand based on publicity. For a company it is important to counter negative publicity most effectively to protect market share and company reputation. This study focuses on the effect of publicity and the moderating effect of company communication. Advertising and other marketing efforts of the company and the relationship with publicity have been researched before (Stammerjohan, Wood, Chang, & Thorson, 2005; Jeffries-Fox, 2001) and are 8 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 outside the scope of this research. In the current experiment the effect of company response on the consumer brand attitudes after negative publicity is the main research goal. Contrasting to most studies on negative publicity, this experiment considers the effect of a news article that does not include direct health risks. The effect of limited health risks has not received a lot of scientific attention and it is therefore unclear if the effect of consumers is comparable to the reaction on more severe risks, such as the Tylenol case in the introduction (Murray & Shohen, 1992). Responding to less significant health risks is a more relevant case for companies, as the majority of product risks is not considered directly lethal. 2: Theory 2.1: Literature review There are a number of common themes in research on publicity. Most studies focus on negative publicity, as it is associated with higher risk and larger stakes. Studies vary by using different independent variables; such as consumer commitment, trust or preference, or company characteristics such as brand image or responsibility, or response strategy. Case studies are popular for examining overall effect of certain strategies, but the results of these studies are complex to extrapolate due to noise (Korkofingas & Ang, 2011; Ahluwalia, Burnkrant, & Unnava, 2000). Experiments can focus on the effect of specific factors, but may guide respondents to certain answers or may be subject to social desirability bias. When using fictitious brands in an experiment, real world implications may be limited, although the effect of publicity is easier to measure. Literature suggests a negative effect of negative publicity on sales and on consumer brand attitudes (Griffin, Babin, & Attaway, 1991). News articles provide a relatively credible source of information, especially compared to advertising. Consumers use this information in their decision making processes. A positive effect on sales may occur when awareness is low as the negative publicity can significantly increase awareness offsetting the negative effect of the news itself (Berger, Sorensen, & Rasmussen, 2010). This positive effect of negative publicity scenario is very uncommon. 2.1.1: Company crisis: product-harm Negative publicity can take many different forms. Accounting scandals, employee discrimination, bribery cases or terrible customer service are all significant potential problems for a company 9 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 leading to reputational damage (Greyser, 2007). Product-harm crises can be considered as the most severe problems. A product-harm crisis is characterised by the (direct) negative effect of a product on the health or well-being of a consumer. Two main types of product-harm can be distinguished. First, products that risk or affect health or well-being directly, for instance when consuming one unit. These risks are hardly arguable by companies or other institutions and demand a swift response and a thorough solution to the problem. Second, products may contain harmful ingredients that may pose a threat when used regularly. In this non immediate health risk situation consumers are not in immediate danger and companies may challenge the claims or point at the amounts a customer needs to consume before it is considered unhealthy or risky. Alternatively the ingredient may be beneficial for one whilst it may increase the risk of harm for others. Sugar for instance is considered unhealthy when used excessively. And some health improving products such as Becel Pro-Activ can lower blood cholesterol for people with high cholesterol levels, but it may pose unintended health risks for others. Events in the first category are more severe and companies often decide to recall the affected products. Johnson and Johnson recalled their Tylenol products when some of them were tampered with deadly poison, and Toyota recalled millions of vehicles worldwide for sticking accelerator pedals. Laufer and Coombs (2006) state that before and during a product recall situation the situation is ambiguous. In line with the attribution theory, consumers try to attribute blame to either the company, the user, or another person or institution (Kelley, 1973). The initial reputation of the company and their responsibility in the event is likely to affect brand attitudes. 2.1.2: Company response effect Consumers desire a response from a company when they encounter negative publicity (Menon, Jewell, & Unnava, 1999). In their paper on response strategies for companies experiencing negative publicity, they find that no response or a weak response leads to about the same consumer attitudes, whilst a strong response generates more positive attitudes towards the company. An appropriate response to negative publicity is predicted to reduce the negative effect of publicity on consumer brand attitudes (Folger & Cropanzano, 2001). Companies may try to shift the blame on others, counter argue the statements, accept responsibility and act on it, or take other steps. Depending on the credibility of the company, the response might weaken the perceived threat to 10 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 consumers, and limit the effect of the negative publicity on brand attitudes. Different appropriate responses to publicity exist. In this experiment, three main strategies are defined. The Adaptation of new processes response (1) consists of an acknowledgement of the problem and a promise to change the production processes to eliminate the risk. In the Denial of severity response (2) a company confirms the initial claim, but argues that the risk is minimal or non-existent. The company may also choose a No response strategy (3). This can be due to litigation, or the company can try to keep the problem silent to minimise damage. Finally, the company may just be too slow or indecisive in their actions. The impact of the company response is measured by the delta consumers brand attitude. This is the difference between the final attitudes of consumers and the initial preferences. The consumer brand attitude at time 2 minus the consumer brand attitude at time 1 results in the delta consumer brand attitude. Time 2 is the moment after the respondents read the news article and the company’s response. It is expected that consumers react differently on the response strategies of companies. This leads to the first hypothesis: H1: The three company response strategies have different effects on the delta consumer brand attitude. To evaluate the different response strategies theoretically, Folger and Cropanzano (2001) developed the fairness theory. This theory is based on three pillars: Could, Should and Would. In order to understand a negative event consumers analyse the situation based on three questions: Could the company do something about the occurrence of the event, Should the company have acted otherwise to comply with ethical and moral standards, and what Would be the impact of alternative actions. The first two questions determine whether or not a company is morally accountable for an event. The Would question compares the event with the best alternative given the situation, and the difference between those situations is the magnitude of the negative publicity. A company that can reduce the perceived accountability for an event (Could and Should factors) is hypothesized to be more highly regarded by consumers (Dean, 2004). This is the case when a company is presented with product tampering that takes places after the product has left the factory, as the company is no longer realistically able to control the circumstances in all distribution and retail channels. An example is the Tylenol case described in the introduction of this thesis. The company is not to blame for the event as it has lived up to the ethical and moral standards. Logically, the outcome of the best alternative action would not differ too much from the actual outcome as the company was on par with these standards and cannot be expected to greatly exceed them. 11 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Bradford and Garrett (1995) created the corporate communication response model to evaluate five different response tactics in four different situations. The tactics used are: no response, denial, excuse, justification and concession. The authors hypothesized that: (a) a company should use a denial response when it can provide evidence that it did not commit an unethical action, (b) when a company can prove that it did not have control over the event it should use an excuse response, (c) when the company believes inappropriate standards are being used to evaluate its actions it should use a justification response, and (d) when the company agrees on the allegations it should use the concession approach. Furthermore they state that when a company uses the right strategy and the response is accepted, the event will not harm the company’s image. The results of their experiment did not provide support for this model, as it showed the concession response as the most preferable response in all situations except for the control situation. In the control situation the excuse, justification and concession strategy did not significantly differ from each other. This implies that the best strategy for companies facing negative publicity is to use a concession response, measured by impact on brand image. An Adaptation of new processes response is expected to lead to the highest brand attitudes when consumers are satisfied with the actions and believe the company acts in their benefit (Siomkos & Shrivastava, 1993). More specifically, it is expected that the drop in brand attitudes measured from before the news article and after the response is the smallest in this scenario. Bradford and Garrett (1995) find that consumers perceive the company as acting irresponsible when it denies allegations. The Denial of severity strategy in this experiment differs from this Denial scenario. The Denial of severity strategy confirms the allegation that certain ingredients are present in the product, where the Denial scenario of Bradford and Garrett (1995) denies all allegations. The effect will be dependent on how consumers evaluate the response of the company. It is expected that Denial of severity results in brand attitudes higher than the No response strategy when consumers agree with the arguments of the company. Not responding to the allegations can imply that the allegations are indeed true. Menon, Jewell, and Unnava (1999) find about the same negative impact of a no response scenario and a weak argument. The results for the Denial of severity response in the experiment cannot be directly compared to the weak argument results of Menon et al. (1999) since the Denial of severity claims are considerably stronger. Siomkos and Shrivastava (1993) found that low-reputation companies need to put in more effort when a product-harm crisis hits the news. These companies need to convince consumers that they are concerned about consumer well-being, about minimising risk and that they are socially 12 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 responsible. High-reputation companies are advised to voluntarily recall harmful products directly. Denial of the problem should be avoided for both high- and low-reputation companies, although this is even more important for low-reputation companies. A Denial of severity response does not convince consumers of a company’s innocence but is seen as defensive and selfish. The first strategy in this experiment a company can use in times of negative publicity is to choose not to react, or postpone a response. In both situations, consumers are in ambiguous situation and not sure if the risk is valid. This strategy is hypothesized to have the strongest negative effect on consumer brand attitudes. Companies using the Denial of severity strategy are expected to do better than companies who do not respond. The Adaptation of new processes strategy is expected to have the highest consumer brand attitudes. This leads to the following hypotheses: H1a: “No company response” will have the strongest negative effect on delta consumer brand attitude. H1b: “Denial of severity” will have a somewhat negative effect on delta consumer brand attitude. H1c: “Adaptation of new processes” will have a minimal effect on delta consumer brand attitude. 2.1.3: Buying intention Korkofingas and Ang (2011) support the link between brand attitudes and brand equity, and subsequently between brand equity and brand value. Brand value is likely to affect brand choices. Higher brand value equals greater perceived benefit for consumers, and can prevent switching behaviour. The reverse is true for low consumer brand attitudes. It is expected that buying likelihood correlates with brand attitudes. A full correlation is not expected, since consumers can have lower brand attitudes but still buy the same brand. This can be due to switching costs, lack of alternatives or consumers’ perceived utility threshold. Ajzen (1991) created the Theory for Planned Behaviour. It consists of the attitude towards the behaviour, the subjective norm, and perceived behavioural control. These predict intention, which in turn explains a substantial part of variance in behaviour. One example of the use of this theory is whether or not one wears a seatbelt. The attitude towards the behaviour is measured by questions like: I consider the wearing of a seatbelt as something bad/good. Subjective norms are measured by asking if people who are important to the individual would approve the use of seatbelts. Intention is 13 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 the reported intention, and actual usage can also be measured by self-reported use (Torquato, Franco, & Bianchi, 2012). Rosenstock, Strecher, and Becker (1994) evaluate the Health Belief Model, an alternative model to explain consumer behaviour, specifically for health related intentions and behaviour. The basis of the model is that one will change their behaviour if the following conditions apply: One perceives a personal health risk, one perceives the risk as severe, one perceives a behavioural change as effective, the negative consequences of the behavioural change do not outweigh the benefits, one receives bodily or environmental cues to action, and one believes he/she can change their behaviour positively. The behavioural change can be indirectly influenced by demographic, sociopsychological, and other variables that influence attitudes, perception and behaviour. Negative publicity is, in this model, primarily an environmental cue to action. It reminds consumers of the health risk and stimulates them to take action. The effect of negative publicity is not limited to a call to action. It may also influence the perceived individual health risk, provide information on the severity of the risk, change the perceived effectiveness of behavioural change, change the evaluation of positive and negative aspects of behaviour change, and it can alter the belief one has in the ability to change their behaviour. In this experiment, the distinction between the impact of all different aspects is not crucial. Therefore, it is aggregated in the self-reported delta likelihood to buy. The delta likelihood to buy is measured by the likelihood to buy at time period 2, after the news article and the company response, minus the likelihood to buy at time period 1, the initial preferences of consumers. This leads to the following hypothesis: H2: Decreasing delta consumer brand attitude will decrease consumers’ delta likelihood to buy. 2.1.4: Brand familiarity In an empirical research on publicity, advertising, and consumer brand attitudes Stammerjohan, Wood, Chang, and Thorson (2005) investigate the synergy of using multiple advertising mediums and publicity. For familiar brands, the respondents did not have a different attitude after viewing positive news stories. Their reasoning is that people already have opinions about brands and new information is not that influential compared to previous opinions. This so-called buffer effect may limit the impact of publicity conflicting with a consumer’s opinion (Griffin, Babin, & Attaway, 1991). Contradictory, consumer brand attitude towards unfamiliar brands was found to be influenced by news stories (Stammerjohan et al., 2005). In the present experiment, one well-known brand, Coca 14 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Cola, and one less-known private label brand, First Choice, is used to allow tests on the difference in expected effects. An exception to this theory is described by Berger, Sorensen, and Rasmussen (2010) in a paper on negative reviews. The authors find a positive effect of negative reviews under the condition of low awareness, reasoning that the increase in awareness has a positive influence on the probability the brand is part of the consideration set. This potentially offsets the negative effect of lower choice probability in the final choice stage of a consumer purchase decision. When there is a delay between publicity and the purchase opportunity, consumers do not remember the valence of a review very well, and therefore the increased awareness can help product sales. Negative publicity is likely to have a larger effect on unfamiliar brands as opposed to familiar brands. Consumer brand attitudes are less robust for unfamiliar brands compared to familiar brands, and therefore it is easier to change perceptions. Companies with higher reputation are better able to buffer negative effects during crisis (Van Riel & Fombrun, 2007). The negativity effect specifies that high-commitment users of brands view negative publicity as less diagnostic (Ahluwalia, Burnkrant, & Unnava, 2000), decreasing the effect of negative publicity for brands with loyal customers. Their results show that low-commitment consumers do express a far greater attitude change than highcommitment consumers. High-commitment consumers counter argue negative information more than they do positive information. Dean (2004) states that the expectations-evidence framework suggests company reputation will generate expectations about the company’s response. In the current research, the well-known high reputation company is expected to be less vulnerable to negative publicity as compared to the lessknown company. Reputation can help consumers make a choice between products. Sago and Hinnenkamp (2014) state that consumers’ brand attitude for their favourite brand is affected by negative publicity. Consumers’ willingness to buy their favourite brand decreases with decreasing brand perception. For males, the amount they are willing to pay for their favourite brand decreases and the time they are willing to shop for competing brands increases with decreasing brand perception. Strong brands are thus not invulnerable to negative publicity. For females, this relationship was not supported. Concluding Sago and Hinnenkamp (2014) state that companies should use marketing to counter the effect of negative publicity. However, in their experiment they did not test the impact of marketing. Other researchers found that this would be a wasted effort (Jeffries-Fox, 2001), as it can remind consumers of the negative publicity and because consumers may perceive advertising as not credible in this situation. 15 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Dawar and Lei (2009) created a framework to evaluate a brand crisis and the moderating effects of brand familiarity and crisis relevance on brand evaluations. They find support for the hypothesis that for consumers familiar with the brand, irrelevant brand crises do not affect brand evaluations, whereas relevant brand crises do have a negative effect on brand evaluations. A brand crisis did lead to a negative effect on evaluation of the brand for consumers unfamiliar with the brand, regardless of crisis relevance. The impact of brand familiarity is measured by the delta consumers brand attitude. This leads to the following hypothesis: H3a: Consumer brand familiarity will positively correlate with delta consumer brand attitude. Brand familiarity is also correlated with brand choice (Haley & Case, 1979). In automatic decision making processes, consumers favour the familiar brand as it is a less risky choice. This decision is not necessarily based on their personal attitude towards the brand. Therefore, the model includes a direct effect of brand familiarity on delta likelihood to buy. H3b: Consumer brand familiarity will positively correlate with delta likelihood to buy. Interestingly, Korkofingas and Ang (2011) state that although some evidence may support that reputable or strong brands are less vulnerable for negative publicity, some reputable or strong brands experience a stronger loss in sales than other less strong brands. A possible explanation for this phenomenon is that consumers have higher expectations of premium, reputable brands and negative publicity is a greater disappointment compared to brands of which consumers have low expectations. In the two studies mentioned by Korkofingas and Ang (2011) they find support for this prominent fall effect. The first study was on automobile recalls, where high-quality brands were more likely to see a decrease in market share (Rhee & Haunschild, 2006). The other study concerned the delivery service of a sandwich shop, were a service failure resulted in a severe drop of ratings only for the well-known sandwich shop (Roehm & Brady, 2007). This drop was only noticeable when respondents had some time between the service failure and the moment of rating the shop. The buffer effect and the prominent fall effect are contradictory theories on the moderating impact of brand familiarity. In this study, the results are expected to follow the buffer effect theory (Griffin, Babin, & Attaway, 1991). The support for the prominent fall theory is limited and therefore it is hard to generalise the situations under which this theory holds. Presumably there must be a large discrepancy between the expected service of the well-known brand and the less-known brand. In 16 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 this case, the main difference between the products is taste. It can be expected that both products meet safety and quality standards. 2.1.5: Publicity The case study mentioned earlier on news coverage of iodized salt in Australia (Li, Chapman, Agho, & Eastman, 2008) shows a significant increase in sales after publicity on the results of a nutrition study on the benefits of iodized salt. This case study is unique since salt was never promoted, advertised or discounted. This provides an opportunity for a time series analysis in a real world scenario, with minimum noise. Such opportunities are rare and therefore the need for experimental research stays intact. The link between negative publicity and consumer brand attitudes has been researched extensively (Laufer & Coombs 2006; Vassilikopoulou, Siomkos, Chatzipanagiotou, & Pantouvakis, 2009; Sago & Hinnenkamp, 2014; Menon, Jewell, & Unnava, 1999; Griffin, Babin, & Attaway, 1991). As consumers become aware of product risks about a brand they will consider this information and act upon it. When the information is plausible and credible, a negative change in attitude is expected. The effect of negative publicity is stronger than the effect of positive publicity on consumer brand attitudes. Ahluwalia, Burnkrant, and Unnava (2000) state that people tend to find negative information more important than positive information in the forming of evaluations of unfamiliar persons or brands. A reason for this effect is argued to be that negative information has more informational value to consumers. Most brands can be considered to be able to live up to expectations and to be safe to use, thus positive publicity does not significantly alter the consumer attitudes towards those brands. Negative publicity does provide consumers with information they can use to change their behaviour. This effect does not hold for high-commitment consumers. These consumers were found not to have a negativity effect; they do not react stronger on negative news than on positive news. They can be biased and are likely to counter argue the negative publicity (Ahluwalia et al., 2000). These findings are in line with smaller effect of publicity on consumer brand attitudes for familiar brands as discussed earlier. Not all consumers perceive news in the same manner. A negative article for one may be neutral or positive for another. To correct for this difference, the following hypothesis is established: H4: Perceived article negativity will negatively affect delta consumer brand attitude. 17 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 2.1.6: Source credibility Source credibility is assumed to be one of the moderators of the effect of negative publicity. This is supported by the attitude and attribution theory (Griffin, Babin, & Attaway, 1991). The attitudes of a consumer can change when exposed to a message of a credible source. On the other hand, low credibility sources are likely to be discounted. Although source credibility did not have a significant effect on consumer attitudes in some research (Rosenbaum & Levin, 1969), the effect is supported by Griffin et al. (1991). In the latter experiment however, source credibility did not have a significant effect on purchase intentions. Publicity is seen as a credible source of information, especially compared to information from a company source (Bond & Kirschenbaum, 1998). Consumers use the most credible information to check other sources on their reliability (Stammerjohan, Wood, Chang, & Thorson, 2005). In the current experiment, the reliability or trustworthiness of the news article is likely to be higher than the reliability or trustworthiness of the company response. Furthermore, as consumers are expected to be less affected by new information when they are familiar with the brand, both the news item and the company response are likely to have a smaller impact on the attitudes for a well-known brand. Despite the general credibility of publicity, news articles are not always objective, trustworthy and complete. Journalists frame stories for different reasons, such as their knowledge, tight deadlines and available space (Kalaitzandonakes, Marks, & Vickner, 2004). Framing is not expected to alter the credibility of a message directly, but it can increase the perceived significance of events. In product harm situations, minor risks can be amplified. This “agenda setting” leads to biased news coverage and a disproportionate coverage of negative events (McCombs & Shaw, 1972). Companies therefore face an increased risk of being subject to negative publicity. The perceived credibility of a source is likely to moderate the effect of negative publicity on consumer brand attitudes. H5: Perceived source credibility will negatively affect delta consumer brand attitude. 2.1.7: Responsibility factor and attribution theory Whether or not the company is responsible for a product-harm crisis or other negative events is important for the effect on consumer behaviour. Responsibility can be seen as perceived accountability for an event. When a company is responsible, the questions ‘Could the company do something about the occurrence of the event’ and ‘Should the company have acted otherwise to 18 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 comply with ethical and moral standards’ will result in a negative outcome for the company. The remaining question ‘What would have happened in the best alternative scenario’ then measures the magnitude of the event (Folger & Cropanzano, 2001). The theory behind the responsibility factor is the attribution theory (Kelley, 1973). This theory suggests that consumers seek causal relationships for observed events. This is combined with the discounting principle, which states that consumers discount the perceived relationship if new plausible information is received. As soon as consumers read or see the negative publicity, they attribute blame to the company, the user or another person or organisation. When the company issues a response, the consumer evaluates this information and if it is plausible it will discount the negative publicity in favour of the company response (Dean, 2004). A positively perceived response of the company is expected to reduce the impact of negative publicity. This leads to the final hypothesis: H6: Attitude towards company response will negatively moderate the effect of negative publicity and company response on delta consumer brand attitude. 2.1.8: Publicity and the effect of marketing efforts The effects of advertising and other marketing efforts have been a popular topic in research for decades. Publicity was often not part of marketing research and the relationship between advertising and publicity was long unclear. Stammerjohan, Wood, Chang, and Thorson (2005) did analyse this relationship in an experiment on integrated marketing communications. They found that positive advertising lessened the effect of negative publicity. Positive publicity enhanced positive advertising effects. In contrast, Jeffries-Fox (2001) argues that heavy advertising in a time of negative publicity is wasted as it cannot compensate for the effect of negative publicity. He states that heavy advertising could draw consumer’s attention to the company and thereby remind consumers of the negative publicity, increasing the effect of negative publicity. This is supported by an analysis of the customers aware of the specific news story who felt it had some or great impact against the customers who were not aware of that news story. Advertising for the first group did not significantly increase attitudes, whereas it did have a significant positive effect for the other group. Eisend and Küster (2011) analysed the effectiveness of publicity versus advertising. In this metaanalysis literature the effect of positive publicity is compared to advertising. In general, positive publicity is preferable over marketing for less-known brands. The credibility of a less-known brand is 19 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 limited, translating in a smaller impact of advertising, as consumers seek for credible information. This also highlights the vulnerability of less-known brands. As brands gain credibility, the impact of their advertising increases. 2.1.9: Impact of publicity over time In an analysis of the Toyota recall crisis, Fan, Geddes, and Flory (2011) show that the brand reputation of Toyota is significantly lower when news about the safety issues appears in mainstream media. Toyota was not able to regain the pre-crisis level of reputation for more than a year after the crisis. The combined impact of hundreds of reports in a half-year period did substantial damage to the brand which Toyota could not recover from easily. The performance history of a company can influence consumer purchase intentions. A company which experienced multiple negative events is more likely perceived as the cause of these problems, compared to companies experiencing only a single problem. Subsequent negative publicity for a company may therefore lead to stronger negative effects (Griffin, Babin, & Attaway, 1991). Another way to look at the time variable in publicity is the impact of a single event or a single news item on consumer brand attitudes and the duration of the effect. Fan and Cook (2003) assume nonexistent persuasiveness of news before publication, maximum persuasiveness at the day of publication and an exponential decay over time. This is in line with the half-life theory commonly found in biologic and chemical science. The effect of a substance is decreased by 50% in every time period, resulting in a quick decrease in impact but a long period before the effect is practically worn out completely. The time period varies among different substances. Hill, Lo, Vavreck and Zaller (2013) find that the half-life of mass communication varies from only one or two days for political publicity to about 30 days for Iraq casualty reports. In cases of a one or two day half-life, long-term effects are virtually non-existent. But a continuous stream of publicity is expected to create long-term attitudinal change. For mass communication with longer half-life, this effect is likely also present and can explain the long duration of the effect on attitudes. The authors advise politicians to preserve advertising budgets for the last week to gain an advantage over competitors. Spending on advertising early on in the election race does not have a significant impact on the results. This implies a minimal long-term effect even for continuous campaign communication. In an experiment on the duration of product-harm crisis effects, Vassilikopoulou, Siomkos, Chatzipanagiotou and Pantouvakis (2009) find support for a short duration of effects of negative 20 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 publicity on perceived danger and purchase intentions. One year after the crisis in their experiment, the purchase intentions were 2.62 times greater than three months after the crisis. The severity of the crisis was also quickly forgotten: after three days, the severity influenced the purchase intentions, but after three months, no significant relationship was found. Socially responsible companies and companies issuing a voluntary recall experience higher brand attitudes, as consumers seem to remember the issue less well compared to companies who do not act appropriately. Likewise, in food risk events, a modest temporary impact of sales was found (Kalaitzandonakes, Marks & Vickner, 2004). As it is very complex to measure the impact of time in an experimental setting without significant noise, this is not part of this experiment. 2.2: Conceptual model + hypotheses The central theory in this model is the negative combined effect of negative publicity and company response on delta consumer brand attitudes (Figure 1). These delta consumer brand attitudes, the difference between consumer brand attitudes at time 1 and time 2, in turn affect the change in choice probability for the brand. Lower consumer brand attitudes are hypothesized to result in lower choice probability. Company response to negative publicity may diminish the effect of negative publicity on consumer brand attitudes. Depending on the severity of the publicity and the strength of the company response, the net effect may be negative, neutral or positive. Company response is not hypothesized to affect the link between consumer brand attitudes and choice probability, as the effect of the company response is assumed to be fully captured by the consumer brand attitudes. Company familiarity is expected to directly affect delta consumer brand attitudes. A higher company familiarity is hypothesized to increase delta consumer brand attitudes. When consumers have more knowledge about the brand, a single news article does not create a strong association with the brand easily. Less-known brands do not have a rich associative network, and consumers relate the brand more strongly to the news article. Finally, company familiarity is hypothesized to directly affect the choice probability. The reasoning is that consumers will still choose the brand over competitors if it is a very familiar brand, irrespective of negative publicity and lower consumer brand attitudes. 21 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Figure 1: Conceptual model. Negative publicity Perceived article negativity No company response H1a (--) Denial of severity response H1b (-) Adaptation new processes response H1c (+-) Perceived source credibility H5 (-) H4 (-) H2 (+) Delta consumer brand attitude H3a (+) Delta likelihood to buy H3b (+) H6 (-) Attitude towards company response H1: Consumer brand familiarity The three company response strategies have different effects on the delta consumer brand attitude. H1a: “No company response” will have the strongest negative effect on delta consumer brand attitude. H1b: “Denial of severity” will have a somewhat negative effect on delta consumer brand attitude. H1c: “Adaptation of new processes” will have a minimal effect on delta consumer brand attitude. H2: Decreasing delta consumer brand attitude will decrease consumers’ delta likelihood to buy. H3a: Higher consumer brand familiarity will positively correlate with delta consumer brand attitude, i.e. well-known companies experience a smaller impact on brand attitudes. H3b: Higher consumer brand familiarity will positively correlate with delta likelihood to buy. H4: Perceived article negativity will negatively affect delta consumer brand attitude. H5: Perceived source credibility will negatively affect delta consumer brand attitude. H6: Attitude towards company response will negatively moderate the effect of negative publicity and company response on delta consumer brand attitude. 22 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 3: Method 3.1: Research design To test the hypotheses, two brands are needed, one highly familiar brand and one unfamiliar brand. Therefore the well-known cola brand Coca Cola and the less-known private label cola brand First Choice are used. Coca Cola has a high brand value since people have a lot of knowledge about and positive attitudes towards the brand. The effect of one news article is likely to be buffered by the pre-existing knowledge. The results on the Coca Cola part of the experiment are therefore conservative as people are less likely to switch based on one article. In real world situations, consumers may obtain information about product risks from multiple sources and on different points in time. This increases the impact of negative publicity. For First Choice, the brand awareness is expected to be relatively low. As the brand is sold in a limited number of supermarket chains in the Netherlands, the majority of the Dutch population does not encounter products of the brand regularly. Furthermore, First Choice does not make significant use of advertising and promotional campaigns, and has not been subject of news articles for the last four years, when it was in a dispute with Coca Cola on the design of their bottles. The results of the First Choice scenarios are expected to be somewhat more volatile, especially for respondents who are totally unfamiliar with the brand. The strength of individual brands is a function of consumer perceived utility, attraction, and brand choice probability (Poulsen et al, 2005). Assuming private label, less-known or fictitious brands have less loyal customers, and they have less knowledge about the brand or the company producing it, switching behaviour and attitude change will be stronger and easier to measure. Compared to experiments on fictitious brands, the current experiment has a higher managerial usability as it more closely resembles a real life situation, especially in the Coca Cola scenarios. Previous experiments on publicity commonly use fictitious brands for internal validity. This limits the external validity and consequently managerial usability. Since the current experiment controls for respondents initial preference, the internal validity can maintain a high level. The external validity of this experiment is low, as any experiment, but higher than previous experiments with fictitious brands. The explanation is that a survey creates a hypothetical situation which is not comparable to the environment of real world situations. For instance, respondents may report intention to buy, but their report is often too positive (Chandon, Morwitz, & Reinartz, 2005). For consumers with high purchase intentions, there is support for the hypothesis that they are more likely to buy a product when they are asked to fill out a survey. There is also support for the reverse relation. This 23 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 effectively leads to more extreme results, creating a considerable limitation of usability of all forms of research making use of surveys. Another reason for low external validity is the fact that a convenience sample is used. The demographics of the experiment do not represent a random sample of the total population. This limits the generalizability of the results for the total population. A better spread in the age, gender and educational factors could improve this. The experiment is a 2x3 between subjects design. Two levels for Brand (well-known and less-known) and three levels for response strategy are used (Adaptation of new processes, Denial of Severity, and No company response). Respondents are randomly assigned to one of six scenarios (Table 1). The first scenario is a news article about small amounts of chemicals causing cancer in Coca Cola. After stating their preferences, respondents get to read the company’s response to this article, in which Coca Cola confirms the news and communicates the adaptation of new processes worldwide. In the second scenario, respondents are presented with the exact same news article, but the response of Coca Cola is different. This time, the company denies the allegations that the chemicals used can cause cancer when consuming normal amounts. In the third scenario, respondents are presented the news, accompanied by statement that Coca Cola did not want to respond to the allegations. Table 1 Survey scenarios and timeline. Coca Cola First Choice Time 1 Publicity Response strategy Time 2 group 1 Attitude group 2 Attitude Negative Adaptation Attitude Negative Denial of severity Attitude group 3 Attitude Negative No response Attitude group 4 Attitude Negative Adaptation Attitude group 5 Attitude Negative Denial of severity Attitude group 6 Attitude Negative No response Attitude The fourth, fifth and sixth scenario show the same news article as in the previous scenarios, but modified for a private label First Choice. Respondents are presented with either the Adaptation of new processes response (scenario four), a Denial of severity response (scenario five), and a control scenario: No company response (scenario six). By testing preferences before and after the experiment the change in attitudes and preference is measured, so the initial preference levels are not specifically important. The relative performance of response strategies and the effects of moderating variables are of interest here. In all scenarios the company is responsible for the event. Events for which the company is not responsible, such as misuse or abuse by consumers, require different strategies which are not within the scope of this experiment as it would considerably increase the number of scenarios and the 24 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 number of respondents required. Furthermore, it would introduce company responsibility and blame as factors in the model, increasing the complexity. The warning in the news article is a non-immediate health risk, as defined in Chapter 2.1.1 on corporate crisis and product harm. This can form an ambiguous situation for consumers, as they may or may not agree with the initial accusation in the news article and with the company’s response. An ambiguous situation is typical for product crises, as information on the events tends to come to the surface slowly and incompletely. The source of both the initial article on the warning for product harm and for the response of the company is stated as NRC Handelsblad, a credible newspaper in the Netherlands. It is found to be the most credible newspaper in The Netherlands (Newcom Research & Consultancy, 2011). Not mentioning a source would create an unclear situation for respondents, where they could evaluate the credibility more than the content of the article. It is also more realistic as most serious negative news is broadcasted by credible media. The news article is based on an actual news article on the website of Belgium newspaper Het Laatste Nieuws (“Ingrediënt in cola is kankerverwekkend”, 2011). It is edited to make it more relevant for Dutch respondents, by referring to Dutch research institutions and the Dutch market. The original article is left intact for most part to maintain the realism of the news. The company response article is based on an article in the Dutch newspaper De Volkskrant (“Coca-Cola en Pepsi passen recept aan om kankerwaarschuwing”, 2012). As with the news article, it is tailored to the Dutch market. The article maintains the message of the original article for the Adaptation of processes response. For the Denial of severity response, the edited article uses arguments of the original article to provide arguments against the allegations. In the No response scenario, the sentence “ [Coca Cola / First Choice] does not wish to respond the news” was added at the end of the news article. 3.2: Experiment For the experiment a convenience sample of high school and university students was recruited. To diversify the group of respondents it was distributed to a wider public in personal networks. The survey was conducted online, in the period from July 14th to July 28th 2014. The questionnaire, presented in Appendix 2, is in Dutch. In total 301 respondents opened the survey, of which 89 did not complete it. A total of 212 cases were useful for analyses. The response rate is 70.4%. Of the 212 respondents, 183 respondents fully completed the survey, 6 did not enter demographic information, and the remaining 23 respondents 25 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 finished the main part of the survey but did not answer questions about article negativity, age, gender, and a few other non-critical subjects. Four respondents entered an incorrect age (1 and 99) and these values have been labelled missing. In the results in Section 4.1.1 an extra test is described to check for different effects between the group who completed the question on article negativity (N=189) and the full group (N=212). About one in three respondents is female, 33% of the 183 respondents who fully completed the survey. The spread in age is wider than in comparable research, M = 30.79, SD = 14.16. It is somewhat younger than the Dutch population, due to a somewhat overrepresentation of high school and university students. The highest current or finished education (Table 2) of the sample is not representative of the general population of the Netherlands. The percentage of respondents who finished or are enrolled in University (WO) or University of applied science (HBO) is a lot higher than the national average of 29% (CBS Statistisch Jaarboek, 2014). Educational level, gender and age are not expected to have significant direct effects or interactions in this experiment. Therefore the use of this sample is justified. Table 2 Current or finished education. Frequency Valid Percent VMBO 7 3.8 HAVO 14 7.7 VWO 13 7.1 MBO 17 9.3 HBO 59 32.2 WO 66 36.1 Other 7 3.8 Total 183 100.0 The survey design allows a between subjects analysis. The changes in attitude and likelihood to buy are measured per individual. Every individual can only be part of one group. Since the experiment did not include a control group for non-negative publicity, the absolute change in attitudes due to negative publicity was not statistically measurable. Although interesting, this was not the goal of the experiment. Rather, the differences in delta attitudes between the scenarios were the main research goal. From a managerial point of view, it is most important to know what the most appropriate response to negative publicity is, rather than the absolute impact. Furthermore, absolute impact has been the part of numerous studies (Fan, Geddes, & Flory, 2011; Kalaitzandonakes, Marks, & Vickner, 2004; Sago & Hinnenkamp,2014; Stammerjohan, Wood, Chang, & Thorson, 2005). 26 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 The external validity of the experiment is affected by three main factors: people, places, and times (Lynch, 1982). Regarding the people factor; the sample is non-random, limiting the generalizability of the results. The unrepresentative share of highly educated people could have some impact on the results as highly educated people are known to pay more attention to both news and personal health. No significant support for different delta consumer brand attitudes between the educational levels is found, as seen in Table 3. The ANOVA tests for a different effect on delta brand attitude for each of the six groups, two brands times three response strategies per brand. Article negativity is added to since it accounts for a substantial part of the variance explained. By adding education, the hypothesis that the impact on delta brand attitude is different for people with different educational levels is tested. That would implicate that higher educated people react either more negative or less negative to negative publicity compared to people with less high education. The insignificant result indicates no support for this hypothesis. Education does not play a role in the change of consumer brand attitudes after negative publicity. This justifies the use of the somewhat higher educated than average sample. Table 3 ANOVA test on educational effects on delta brand attitude. (N=183) Source Corrected Model SS df MS F p 60.808a 12 5.067 4.195 .000 Intercept 8.098 1 8.098 6.704 .010 ARTC.NEG 32.105 1 32.105 26.577 .000 EDUCATION 7.424 6 1.237 1.024 .411 SCENARIO.123 9.594 2 4.797 3.971 .021 .194 1 .194 .161 .689 3.130 .046 BRAND SCENARIO.123 * BRAND 7.561 2 3.781 Error 205.360 170 1.208 Total 425.333 183 Corrected Total 266.169 182 a. R2 = .228 (Adj. R2 = .174) Regarding the places criterion, cultural factors are not expected to have a substantial impact on the relationship between negative publicity and brand attitudes and buying likelihood. By using a global brand and a national brand it is supposed that the place factor does not play a role when generalizing the results for Western countries. Time: The study was done in the summer, for beverage brands. It is possible that attitudes towards beverage brands are different in the summer, when consumers spend more time outside. For the delta brand attitudes, no significant difference of the factor time is expected. Next to that, no 27 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 significant real world publicity on beverage brands was observed in the period of the study and the weeks before. 3.2.1: Response bias Socially desirable response is a risk to the validity of every survey (Steenkamp, De Jong, & Baumgartner, 2009). It is a form of response bias in which a respondent selects answers to make him or herself look good. Surveys with controversial subjects, such as the use of alcohol or illegal drugs, are more vulnerable to this response bias. The survey’s anonymous design somewhat limits the bias, but it is not practically possible to eliminate socially desirable response bias. In this survey, a respondent could try to be a good experimental respondent by reporting lower brand attitude after the negative news. In the initial setup of the experiment, respondents had to report their attitude at the start of the survey, after reading the news article, and after the company response. To lessen this form of response bias, the questions about brand attitudes after reading the news were removed. However, response bias may still exist in the final design of the experiment. To reduce bias any further, respondents were told that the survey was about consumer behaviour. Due to the randomised scenarios, respondents did not know about the fact that multiple scenarios existed. A check on IP-addresses minimised the possibility of a single person filling out the survey multiple times. 3.2.2: Validity of scales Spears and Singh (2004) analysed brand attitude and purchase intention scales used in marketing literature. They propose a set of questions based on research on all scales used. The questions in the survey are based on the English counterparts of their suggestions. Attitude towards the brand is measured on a seven point scale, on three topics: unappealing/appealing, bad/good, and unfavourable/favourable. The brand attitude is calculated by taking the sum of the answers on brand attitude and dividing it by three. Purchase intentions are measured on a five point scale, ranging from definitely would not buy to definitely would buy. The question is related to a relevant time frame (within the next 10 days) to provide respondents with a clear question. The interpretation of the question without a timeframe could significantly differ amongst respondents and therefore make it harder to interpret results. 28 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 All questions and rating scales are based on literature in the most credible marketing journals (Menon, Jewell, & Unnava, 1999; Tolboom, Bronner, & Smit, 2012). This increases replicability and makes it easier to compare results over studies. In Appendix 2 the full survey can be found. The questions used for analysis are described in detail here. 1. On brand familiarity – FAM.t1 Scale of 1 to 5 How familiar are you with [Coca Cola / First Choice]? Totally Unknown – Somewhat Unknown – Neutral – Somewhat Known – Totally Known 2. On consumer brand attitudes – ATT.time1 / ATT.time2 Scale of 1 to 7, calculated attitude is the average of the answers for the three questions. What is you impression of [Coca Cola / First Choice]? Unappealing Bad Negative ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ Appealing Good Positive 3. On likelihood to buy – BUY_INTENT.t1 / BUY_INTENT.t2 Scale of 1 to 5. The 10 days’ time factor is used to make the question more relevant. How likely is it that you buy [Coca Cola / First Choice] in the next 10 days? Absolutely Not – Probably Not – Neutral – Probably – Absolutely 4. On article negativity – ARTC.NEG: Scale of 1 to 5 What do you think of the news about [Coca Cola / First Choice]? Very positive – Positive – Neutral – Negative – Very Negative 5. On source credibility – SOURCE.CRED: Scale of 1 to 5 Rate the following statement I find the news article about [Coca Cola / First Choice] believable 29 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Strongly Agree – Agree – Neither Agree Nor Disagree – Disagree – Strongly Disagree 6. On attitude towards response – ATT.RESPONSE: Scale of 1 to 5, calculated attitude is the average of the answers for the three questions. Rate the following statements I find the response of [Coca Cola / First Choice] positive I find the response of [Coca Cola / First Choice] believable I find the response of [Coca Cola / First Choice] strong Strongly Agree – Agree – Neither Agree Nor Disagree – Disagree – Strongly Disagree 7. On education – EDUCATION Recoded values, most common education levels in The Netherlands are used. What is your highest finished or current educational level? (1) VMBO (2) HAVO (3) VWO (4) MBO (5) HBO (6) WO (7) Other, please specify … 3.2.3: Manipulation checks To check the whether or not the manipulations are perceived as intended, the survey includes questions on brand familiarity, perceived article negativity and on response positivity. Brand familiarity was added to check for a significant difference in familiarity between Coca Cola and First Choice. Results show that indeed Coca Cola is considered more familiar, t(162.2) = 13.26, p < .001. The news article is perceived as negative or somewhat negative, MeanCocaColaNews = 3.62 and MeanFirstChoiceNews = 4.01. Article negativity is measured on a scale of five, where one is very positive and five is very negative. The difference between the perceived negativity is significant, t(187) = 2.86, p = .005. The articles are perceived as negative as intended. 30 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Figure 2 Histogram of perceived article negativity. In Figure 2 the spread of perceived article negativity is shown. It is clear that for First Choice, the pattern is different as compared to Coca Cola. The difference in perceived negativity between brands can be explained by the theory discussed in Chapter 2.1.5, which states that consumers perceive information as less negative if it concerns their favourite or familiar brands (Ahluwalia, Burnkrant, & Unnava, 2000). Figure 3 Histogram of perceived company response positivity. Concerning the positivity of the company response, Figure 3 displays respondents’ answers. There is no support for a difference of response positivity between brands, t(121.6) = 1.23, p = .219. In other words, respondents do not experience the response more positive for one of the brands. Significant difference between attitude towards response between response strategies, t(120.6) = 6.72, p < .001. The Adaptation of new processes response is perceived as more positive. The manipulation is perceived as it was intended. 31 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 3.2.4: Outlier One outlier was found in the Coca Cola Denial of severity group. It has the theoretical maximum delta consumer brand attitude of -6, and the delta likelihood to buy decreased to the minimum of 1 from the maximum of 5. All calculations are done including this outlier. Where appropriate, the analysis is also run without the outlier and the impact of the outlier is stated. Analyses excluding this outlier are accompanied by a footnote. Test including perceived article negativity or attitude towards company response are not affected by this outlier, as this respondent did not answer the related questions. 4: Results 4.1: Differences between response strategies 4.1.1: Effect on delta consumer brand attitude The first hypothesis compares the three different response strategies companies follow in the experiment. H1: The three company response strategies have different effects on the delta consumer brand attitude. Using an ANOVA analysis, no significant support for differences between the means of the consumer brand attitude under the three scenarios can be found (Table 4). The low R-square value (R2 = .014) indicates the result is not useful for prediction. Table 4 ANOVA test of between subjects effects on delta brand attitude. (N=211) 1 Source SS Corrected Model Intercept SCENARIO.123 df MS 2.144 1.473 .232 159.650 1 159.650 109.645 .000 1.473 .232 4.289 2 2.144 302.861 208 1.456 Total 466.444 211 307.150 210 a. R2 = .014 (Adj. R2 p 2 Error Corrected Total F 4.289a = .004) 1 Excluding the outlier mentioned in Chapter 3.2.4. Including the outlier does not make a difference in the results, N = 212, df= 2, F = 1.00, p = .371, R2 = .009. 32 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Further research revealed the reason for the lack of significant results. As it turns out, the delta consumer brand attitude is influenced by the perceived negativity of the news article. As some respondents do not perceive the article as negative as others, they do not change their attitude as the others do. By controlling for this effect, there is a significant difference between the means of the three groups for the differences in delta consumer brand attitude, as can be seen in Table 5. Table 5 ANOVA test of between subjects effects on delta brand attitude, including article negativity. (N=189) Source SS Corrected Model 47.850a 3 15.950 12.670 .000 Intercept 9.393 1 9.393 7.461 .007 ARTC.NEG 40.511 1 40.511 32.180 .000 3.267 .040 SCENARIO.123 df MS 8.227 2 4.113 Error 232.890 185 1.259 Total 442.778 189 Corrected Total 280.741 188 F p a. R2 = .170 (Adj. R2 = .157) In a pairwise comparison significant support for a difference between the Denial of severity response (I) and the No response strategy (J) is found, Mean difference (I-J)= .501, p = .012. This indicates higher delta consumer brand attitude for the Denial of severity response strategy. Whereas companies who do not respond experience a drop of 1.178 in attitude, companies who deny the severity of the event only have .677 lower brand attitudes as compared to the initial consumer brand attitudes (Figure 4). Concluding, H1 is supported. There is a significant difference in delta consumer brand attitudes for different response strategies, when corrected for perceived article negativity. 33 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Figure 4 Estimated effect of response strategy on consumer brand attitudes. It is important to note that the respondent group size differs between aforementioned tests. Some respondents did not answer the question about perceived article negativity, but are included in the ANOVA of Table 4. An repeated test, without the respondents who did not answer the specified question, revealed similar results to the results in Table 3, F(1,188) = 2.50, p = .085. Without controlling for perceived article negativity, the response strategy does not have a significant effect on delta consumer brand attitude. This confirms the conclusion on H1. 4.1.2: Ranking strategies H1a, b and c state the ranking of the response strategies. H1a: “No company response” will have the strongest negative effect on delta consumer brand attitude. H1b: “Denial of severity” will have a somewhat negative effect on delta consumer brand attitude. H1c: “Adaptation of new processes” will have a minimal effect on delta consumer brand attitude. An ANOVA on delta consumer brand attitude, with perceived article negativity as covariate, was performed to test these hypotheses (N = 189, Article negativity mean = 3.82). No company response has the most negative estimated mean, at -1.178, followed by adaptation response at -.945, and 34 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 finally the denial of severity response at -.677. Although the difference between the No response and the Denial of severity group was found to be significant in the previous analysis, the Adaptation response does not differ significantly from both other groups (p = .187 and p = .257). Concluding, there is not enough support for the ranking of the response strategies. Further analysis of the ANOVA on delta consumer brand attitude, with perceived article negativity as covariate, is done for each brand separately. When comparing the different response strategies per brand, only in Coca Cola scenarios a significant difference between response strategies can be found. In a pairwise comparison of the Response strategies (Table 6) the No response strategy for Coca Cola results in lower delta consumer brand attitudes as compared to the Adaptation and Denial strategies. Table 6 Pairwise comparisons of Coca Cola response strategies on delta attitude (N=91) 95% Confidence Interval for Differenceb (I) SCENARIO.123 (J) SCENARIO.123 Mean Difference (I-J) S.E. Coca Cola Adaptation Coca Cola Denial -.265 Coca Cola Denial Coca Cola No Response pvalueb Lower Bound Upper Bound .260 .311 -.782 .252 .716* .265 .008 .190 1.242 Coca Cola Adaptation .265 .260 .311 -.252 .782 Coca Cola No Response .981* .260 .000 .464 1.499 Coca Cola Adaptation -.716* .265 .008 -1.242 -.190 Coca Cola Denial -.981* .260 .000 -1.499 -.464 Coca Cola No Response Based on estimated marginal means *. The mean difference is significant at the .05 level. b. Adjustment for multiple comparisons: Least Significant Difference (equivalent to no adjustments). The same analysis is also done for First Choice. No significant differences between response strategies are reported (Table 7). The ranking of strategies as hypothesised in H1a, b, and c is not supported for this brand. 35 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Table 7 Pairwise comparisons of First Choice response strategies on delta attitude (N=98) 95% Confidence Interval for Differencea (I) SCENARIO.123 (J) SCENARIO.123 Mean Difference (I-J) S.E. pvaluea Lower Bound Upper Bound First Choice Adaptation First Choice Denial -.302 .305 .324 -.907 .303 First Choice No Response -.209 .310 .502 -.824 .407 First Choice Adaptation .302 .305 .324 -.303 .907 First Choice No Response .093 .286 .745 -.475 .662 .209 .310 .502 -.407 .824 -.093 .286 .745 -.662 .475 First Choice Denial First Choice No Response First Choice Adaptation First Choice Denial Based on estimated marginal means a. Adjustment for multiple comparisons: Least Significant Difference (equivalent to no adjustments). 4.2: Brand attitudes and likelihood to buy The next step of evaluating the impact of response strategies to negative publicity is to check whether the change in consumer brand attitudes is reflected in the likelihood to buy. H2: Decreasing delta consumer brand attitude will decrease consumers’ delta likelihood to buy. The regression analysis of Table 8 shows significant support for the prediction of the delta likelihood to buy by the delta consumer brand attitude. An increase of one in consumer brand attitudes, measured on a seven point scale, is predicted to be lead to an increase of .262 in the likelihood to buy, on a five point scale. Table 8 Regression of delta consumer brand attitudes on delta likelihood to buy. (N=211) 2 Variable b S.E. (Constant) .138 .058 DELTA.ATT .262 .039 β .421 t p-value 2.367 .019 6.700 .000 R2 = .177; F = 44.90 (p-value < .001). The R-square is adequate considering the experimental nature of the study. H2 is supported; there is significant support for correlation of delta consumer brand attitude and delta likelihood to buy. As consumers’ attitude towards the brand decreases, so does their intention to buy the brands’ products. As expected in Chapter 2.1.3, the likelihood to buy does not decrease as strong as the brand attitudes. Consumers may have previous knowledge about brands and still prefer the brand 2 Excluding the outlier mentioned in Chapter 3.2.4. Including outlier: N = 212. b = .301, t = 7.81, p < .001; R2 = .225; F = 61.00 36 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 over others. For unfamiliar brands, likelihood to buy is supposedly already at low levels and strong reactions are therefore exceptional. Another explanation is that the lower brand attitudes do not exceed a threshold, and therefore the affected brand is still the preferred one. 4.3: Brand familiarity 4.3.1: Effect on delta consumer brand attitude Continuing that line of thought, consumers are expected to respond differently to less familiar brands. In the survey, respondents were asked to evaluate the level of familiarity they had with Coca Cola and First Choice. Well-known brands are expected to have formed a buffer in the minds of consumers, as the associative network for the brand is enriched with previous news, advertising, product experiences and more. H3a: Higher consumer brand familiarity will positively correlate with delta consumer brand attitude, i.e. well-known companies experience a smaller impact on brand attitudes. Table 9 Regression of brand familiarity on delta brand attitude. (N=189) Variable b S.E. (Constant) .792 .414 FAM.t1 .021 .053 ARTC.NEG -.470 .087 R2 β t p-value 1.915 .057 .027 .400 .689 -.372 -5.428 .000 = .142; F = 15.38 (p-value < .001). From Table 9, there is no significant support for H3a. Brand familiarity does not predict the change in consumer brand attitude. Clearly, respondents’ reaction on negative publicity is as negative for wellknown as for less-known brands. Understandably, higher brand familiarity did correspond with higher brand attitudes, r(209) = .56, p < .01. But at the different levels of brand attitudes, negative publicity did not result in significantly different reaction. As in Chapter 4.1, perceived article negativity is included in the model. The difference between the mean brand attitude at time one (4.42) and at time two (3.55) is significant, t(210) = 10.44, p < .0013. On average, respondents have a .89 lower brand attitude on a seven point scale. This rules out the explanation that there is no interaction with brand familiarity due to lack of significant changes in brand attitude itself. 3 Excluding the outlier mentioned in Chapter 3.2.4. Including outlier: t(211) = 10.35, p < .0013. 37 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 4.3.2: Effect on delta likelihood to buy As brand familiarity did not have a direct effect on delta brand attitudes, the impact on delta likelihood to buy is most likely also insignificant. H3b: Higher consumer brand familiarity will positively correlate with delta likelihood to buy. Table 10 Regression of brand familiarity on delta likelihood to buy. (N=211) 4 Variable b S.E. (Constant) .004 .134 FAM.t1 -.025 .034 β -.052 t p-value 0.026 .979 -.757 .450 R2 = .003; F = .57 (p-value = .450). The results in Table 10 confirm this prediction, as the results are highly insignificant. H3b is thus not supported. The reaction of consumers to more familiar brands does not differ from the reaction to an unfamiliar brand. Article negativity is not included in this analysis, as it is not hypothesised to directly affect delta likelihood to buy. Based on the review of literature, these results are unexpected. No support for the buffer effect can be found, in which more familiar brands are protected for immediate damage as consumers make choices based on more information than a single news article and the accompanying company response (Griffin, Babin, & Attaway, 1991). On the other hand, the competing prominent fall theory cannot be supported as well. A possible explanation on the lack of impact of brand familiarity might be related to the questionnaire. Respondents are asked to evaluate their brand familiarity on a five point scale, ranging from fully unknown (1) to fully known (5). Table 11 Brand familiarity counts. (N=211) 5 FAM.t1 1 2 3 4 5 Coca Cola 2 2 2 8 90 First Choice 33 23 9 31 11 Table 11 shows the distribution of answers on brand familiarity. The results for Coca Cola are not unexpected, 94% indicates familiarity or high familiarity with Coca Cola. The familiarity with First Choice is more evenly spread. These results do not indicate misunderstanding of the question. The explanatory power of the test may be lower due to the almost unanimous fully familiar score for 4 Excluding the outlier mentioned in Chapter 3.2.4. Including outlier: N = 212. b = -.035, t = -.996, p = .320; R2 = .005; F =.99 5 Excluding the outlier mentioned in Chapter 3.2.4. The outlier adds 1 to Coca Cola – Familiarity = 5, resulting in 91 for that group. 38 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Coca Cola. However, a regression for First Choice of brand familiarity on delta likelihood to buy did not have significant results (Table 12). Table 12 Regression of brand familiarity on delta likelihood to buy for First Choice (N=107) Variable b (Constant) -.054 .148 .017 .049 FAM.t1 R2 S.E. β .033 t p-value -.366 .715 .343 .732 = .001; F = .12 (p-value = .732). Regarding H3b, the impact of familiarity on delta likelihood to buy, limited explanatory power is most likely the result of the very small delta likelihood to buy itself. Only 16% of the respondents (N=212) reported a lower likelihood to buy, where 7% reported an increased likelihood to buy. The remaining 78% did report the no change in likelihood to buy. The difference between the likelihood to buy at time one and at time two is not significant, t(210) = 1.74, p = .0846. Figure 5 Frequencies of delta likelihood to buy. In Figure 5 the delta likelihood to buy is presented. Obviously, the negative publicity did not have a strong negative effect on the likelihood to buy. This indicates that consumers perceive the news as not severe or credible enough to make different buying decisions. 6 Excluding the outlier mentioned in Chapter 3.2.4. Including outlier: t(211) = 1.98, p = .049. The outlier creates a marginally significant result, but the measured change in likelihood to buy is only small: from 2.25 (time 1) to 2.14 (time 2). 39 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 4.4: Article negativity How consumer perceive news articles and how negative publicity impacts attitudes and choices is personal. Where some may find an article very negative, others might find it irrelevant or not negative at all. It is hypothesized that respondents who perceive an article as more negative will have stronger reactions as measured by delta brand attitude. In Chapter 4.1.1 it was found that article negativity needs to be controlled for to find different effects between response strategies. H4: Perceived article negativity will negatively affect delta consumer brand attitude. To test this hypothesis, an ANOVA of article negativity and response strategy on delta brand attitude was performed. Response strategy was included to divide the respondents in groups. In Table 5 results of this ANOVA are displayed. The perceived negativity of the article has a significant effect on the delta brand attitude, F(1,188) = 32.18, p < .001. Regression analysis confirms the result, β = -.48, t(188) = -5.54, p < .001, indicating that a one point higher perceived article negativity (on a five point scale) leads to almost half a point more negative delta brand attitude (on a seven point scale). H4 is supported. The model was extended to test for significant differences between the three response strategies for each brand specifically. The factor BRAND, which holds either Coca Cola or First Choice, was added. An interaction effect, SCENARIO.123*BRAND, representing the six different scenarios in the experiment design was added. Table 13 ANOVA test of Between subjects effects on delta brand attitude. (N=189) Source SS df MS F p 8.123 6.567 .000 1 165.358 133.685 .000 1 36.623 29.608 .000 0.44 1 .438 .354 .552 SCENARIO.123 8.98 2 4.492 3.631 .028 BRAND 0.11 1 .106 .085 .770 SCENARIO.123 * BRAND 8.55 2 4.274 3.456 .034 Error 223.88 181 1.237 Total 442.78 189 Corrected Total 280.74 188 Corrected Model 56.86a 7 Intercept 165.36 36.62 FAM.t1.cent ARTC.NEG.CENT a. R2 = .203 (Adj. R2 = .172) Table 13 confirms earlier results on article negativity and response strategy. The added BRAND factor does not significantly improve the model. There is support for an interaction between brand and response strategy, indicating that the mean delta brand attitude differs significantly between 40 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 brands under a specific scenario. The No company response strategy for Coca Cola leads to a considerable lower estimated mean delta brand attitude, as seen in Figure 6. Figure 6 Estimated delta consumer brand attitude. Table 14 Estimated Marginal Means of interaction Scenario.123*BRAND on delta brand attitude. (N=189) 95% Confidence Interval BRAND Coca Cola Mean -.821a S.E. .205 Lower Bound -1.225 Upper Bound -.418 -.549a .200 -.944 -.154 No company response -1.526a .207 -1.934 -1.118 Adaptation response -1.083a .215 -1.507 -.659 Denial of severity response -.795a .185 -1.159 -.430 No company response -.872a .192 -1.251 -.492 Adaptation response Denial of severity response First Choice a. Covariates appearing in the model are evaluated at the following values: ARTC.NEG.CENT = .0000, FAM.t1.cent = -.0122. The results in Table 14 suggest that for Coca Cola it is important to respond to negative publicity, just like in Figure 6. In the No company response scenario, the delta consumer brand attitude is significantly lower than in either the Adaptation or Denial of severity response scenario. This is 41 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 supported by Independent sample T-tests, t(59.68) = 2.17, p = .034; t(59.04) = 2.36, p = .0227 respectively. A Denial of severity response has the smallest impact on consumer brand attitudes in the Coca Cola scenarios, but this result is not significant. An independent sample T-test of the Adaptation response versus the Denial of severity response confirms this result, t(67) = -.23, p = .8188. For First Choice the three response strategies do not lead to significantly different results in delta brand attitude. The absolute brand attitudes for First Choice are lower compared to Coca Cola, which limits the possibilities for a strong negative delta brand attitude (Figure 7a). Consequently, this limits the possibility of statistical differences between response strategies. Figure 7b shows the attitudes at time 2. It clearly shows that brand attitudes for First Choice are near the absolute minimum. Larger samples or more precise measurement of brand attitudes is recommended to find differences or to confirm this result. Alternatively, the responses presented to respondents could be re-written in more extreme styles, to make it easier to detect differences. However, this would limit the external validity as it does not resemble real life events. Figure 7a Consumer brand attitude time 1 Figure 7b Consumer brand attitude time 2. Coca Cola and First Choice have been compared head to head for each response strategy. No significant differences were found, therefore concluding that the response strategies have no statistically different effect on delta brand attitudes for different brands. T-tests on the means of 7 Excluding the outlier mentioned in Chapter 3.2.4. Result including the outlier: t(67.62) = 1.60, p = .115. The outlier would invalidate the results for the Denial of severity scenario as compared to the No response scenario. 8 Excluding the outlier mentioned in Chapter 3.2.4. Result including the outlier: t(68) = .37, p = .701. 42 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 delta brand attitude for both brands has the following results: Adaptation of new processes, t(54.88) = 1.65, p = .105; Denial of severity, t(71) = 1.50, p = .1379; No response, t(68) = -.83, p = .409. 4.5: Source credibility Not all publicity is seen equally. The perceived credibility of a medium is predicted to moderate the effect of negative publicity on brand attitudes. As consumers perceive the article as more credible, it is expected that they react stronger. H5: Perceived source credibility will negatively affect delta consumer brand attitude. Table 15 Regression of source credibility on delta likelihood to buy. (N=189) Variable b S.E. (Constant) -.721 .135 ARTC.NEG.CENT -.435 .086 No.response -.460 Adaptation.response SOURCE.CRED.CENT β t p-value -5.349 .000 -.344 -5.082 .000 .193 -.178 -2.378 .018 -.167 .202 -.063 -.830 .407 -.246 .087 -.192 -2.831 .005 R2 = .205; F = 11,87 (p-value < .001). Table 15 provides significant support for H5, higher perceived source credibility affects the delta brand attitude negatively. The R2 of the model is satisfactory. Respondents rated the source credibility as reasonable credible (M = 3.28, SD = .96). The standard deviation shows sufficient variation. An ANOVA confirmed the significant main effect for source credibility, F(1, 188) = 8.079, p = .005. Introducing the BRAND factor to the model (Table 16) slightly increases the R2, to 23.7% from 20.7% (Adj. R2 to 20.3% from 18.5%) The interaction effect of BRAND with response strategy is significant, as it was in previous tests. 9 Excluding the outlier mentioned in Chapter 3.2.4. Result including the outlier: t(72) = .82, p = .418. 43 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Table 16 ANOVA test of between subjects effects on delta brand attitude. (N=189) Source SS F p Corrected Model 66.421a 8 8.303 6.973 .000 Intercept 164.912 1 164.912 138.504 .000 28.530 1 28.530 23.961 .000 9.562 1 9.562 8.031 .005 .569 1 .569 .478 .490 7.560 2 3.780 3.175 .044 .225 1 .225 .189 .664 3.410 .035 ARTC.NEG.CENT SOURCE.CRED.CENT FAM.t1.cent SCENARIO.123 BRAND SCENARIO.123 * BRAND df MS 8.121 2 4.060 Error 214.320 180 1.191 Total 442.778 189 Corrected Total a. R2 = .237 (Adj. R2 = .203) 280.741 188 4.6: Attitudes towards company response In two of the three scenarios, the news article is directly followed by a company response. As with negative publicity, it is expected that consumers vary in their reaction to the response based how they perceive the response of the company. H6: Attitude towards company response will negatively moderate the effect of negative publicity and company response on delta consumer brand attitude. Table 17 contains the results of the ANOVA for H6. The interaction effect was removed since the design of the test differs from the earlier tests. The test design is a 2x2 between subjects ANOVA with two brands and two response strategies. The No response strategy was not included since the attitude towards the response was non-existent for that group. 44 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Table 17 ANOVA on response attitude effects on delta brand attitude. (N=126) Source SS df MS F p 64.933a 25 2.597 2.250 .002 Intercept 4.333 1 4.333 3.753 .056 ARTC.NEG 15.440 1 15.440 13.373 .000 ATT.RESPONSE 15.175 12 1.265 1.095 .372 SCENARIO.123 2.231 1 2.231 1.932 .168 BRAND 1.845 1 1.845 1.598 .209 17.628 10 1.763 1.527 .141 Error 115.453 100 1.155 Total 259.222 126 Corrected Total a. R2 = .360 (Adj. R2 = .200) 180.385 125 Corrected Model ATT.RESPONSE * SCENARIO.123 The results of the ANOVA indicate no significant effect of the attitude towards the company response on the delta brand attitude. Companies do not have to choose the response strategy with the highest expected attitude towards the response, as it does not predict delta consumer brand attitudes. In other words, a disliked company response does not lead to lower attitudes towards the brand. The attitude towards the response does not moderate the effect of the response strategies on the delta consumer brand attitude. The interaction effect is insignificant, H6 is not supported. Put otherwise, the effect of the response strategy does not change for different levels of attitude towards the response. In Chapter 2.1.6, it is hypothesised that the perceived source credibility of the article is higher than the perceived credibility of the company response. The mean source credibility is slightly higher at 3.28 versus 3.13 for response credibility, but there is no support for a significant difference, t(125) = 1.14, p = .257. The mean of just over three out of five indicates that respondents perceived both the credibility of the source and the response as neutral/somewhat believable. A higher credibility might have increased the effect of the news article and the company response. 5: General discussion The experiment resulted in the confirmation of some theories, but also in some interesting new findings. Different response styles do lead to different outcomes, but not exactly as expected. Unsurprisingly, the No company response strategy leads to the strongest drop in consumer brand attitudes. Rather remarkably, the Denial of severity response performed significantly better than the 45 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 No company response strategy. Prior research indicated that a denial response would lead to about the same effect as a no response strategy, depending on circumstances (Bradford & Garrett, 1995). This difference can be explained by the exact denial message, in this case a denial of severity of the event, where it could also be a denial of the event itself. The most interesting finding was the effect of perceived article negativity. It turned out to be one of the most important factors in the model. By controlling for perceived article negativity, differences between response strategies became clear. For further research it is advised to control for individual perceived negativity instead of just using a measurement on the general negativity of an article. News articles regarding the less-known brand First Choice are perceived significantly more negative compared to the articles regarding the well-known brand Coca Cola. Since article negativity is an excellent predictor for attitude change, it suggests that less-known brands suffer more under negative publicity. This is confirmed by earlier research (Dawar & Lei, 2009). One could argue that the buffer effect causes this difference in perceived article negativity. The buffer effect states that consumers have more robust attitudes towards more familiar or higher reputation brands, and attitude change is therefore less likely (Griffin, Babin, & Attaway, 1991). There is no support for the buffer effect in this experiment. The impact of negative publicity for less-known brands is not significantly different from the effect for well-known brands. If the buffer effect would have been present, the well-known brand would have had a smaller impact on brand attitudes. Although respondents rate the article for First Choice as more negative, this does not translate in stronger negative delta brand attitudes. The predictive power of article negativity is highly individual. 5.1: Answering research questions 5.1.1: Problem Statement This paper investigates the best response strategy as measured by consumer brand attitudes for two brands of cola. Three main response strategies have been defined: Adaptation of new processes, Denial of severity, and No company response. The Denial of severity response outperforms the No company response strategy significantly. Based on the results in Chapter 4, the best strategy is to respond to negative publicity. Both the Adaptation of new processes and the Denial of severity strategy outperform the No company response strategy. 46 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Table 13 in Chapter 4.4 contains the result of an ANOVA including the interaction of response strategy and brand (Scenario.123*BRAND). This significant interaction indicates that the response strategies do not have the same effect for both brands. For Coca Cola, the No company response strategy results in the highest negative impact on consumer brand attitudes. For Coca Cola, it is important to react timely on news, since the Denial of severity and the Adaptation response resulted in a smaller impact. There is no clear preference for one of the two scenarios by impact on brand attitudes, but considering the substantial costs of an Adaptation of new processes response, the Denial of severity strategy may be the most effective. It is important to note that denial of an event as discussed in some literature (Siomkos & Shrivastava, 1993; Menon, Jewel, & Rao Unnava, 1999; Bradford & Garrett, 1995) is different than denial of severity. The denial of an event as discussed in prior research differs in the fact that it denies the whole event, such as an alleged bribery. In others (Laufer & Coombs, 2006) it denies the risk or threat, but does not deny the circumstances or event, just like this experiment. The Denial of severity is expected to work when consumers accept the explanation and find that the risk does not have an impact on them. For First Choice, the three strategies did not have significantly different results. This is somewhat surprising, given the reported differences between scenarios for Coca Cola. The respondents in First Choice scenarios did not attach high value to the response of the company to the allegations. This could be because the respondents are not impacted themselves, as they are in general more unfamiliar with the brand and thus do not consume it regularly. The mean delta consumer brand attitude for First Choice did also not differ significantly from the Coca Cola equivalent. There is no support for either the buffer effect (Griffin, Babin, & Attaway, 1991), in which more familiar brands experience a smaller impact on brand attitudes, or for the prominent fall effect (Korkofingas & Ang, 2011), where higher quality or higher reputation brands are theorized to see a higher impact on brand attitudes. When comparing the effects between brands, no difference in Adaptation of new processes and Denial of severity was found. Consumers react as strongly on negative publicity in combination with a company response for the well-known brand as for the less-known brand. In the No response strategy, consumers report a stronger drop in attitudes for Coca Cola. As the well-known and higher reputation brand, consumers demand a response. The allegations are not in line with the expectations of customers for high reputation brands, and without a response consumers are left in 47 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 an ambiguous situation and are not sure of the impact and relevance of the event (Dean, 2004; Korkofingas & Ang, 2011). A summary of all hypotheses and results is presented in Table 18. Table 18 Overview of hypotheses H1: The three company response strategies have different effects on the delta consumer brand attitude. Supported, when controlled for perceived article negativity. H1a: “No company response” will have the strongest negative effect on delta consumer brand attitude. Supported for well-known brand H1b: “Denial of severity” will have a somewhat negative effect on delta consumer brand attitude. Not Supported, no significant difference with Adaptation strategy H1c: “Adaptation of new processes” will have a minimal effect on delta consumer brand attitude. Not Supported, no significant difference with Denial strategy H2: Decreasing delta consumer brand attitude will decrease consumers’ delta likelihood to buy. Supported H3a: Higher consumer brand familiarity will positively correlate with delta consumer brand attitude, i.e. well-known companies experience a smaller impact on brand attitudes. Not supported H3b: Higher consumer brand familiarity will positively correlate with delta likelihood to buy. Not supported H4: Perceived article negativity will negatively affect delta consumer brand attitude. Supported H5: Perceived source credibility will negatively affect delta consumer brand attitude. Supported H6: Attitude towards company response will negatively moderate the effect of negative publicity and company response on delta consumer brand attitude. Not supported 5.1.2: Research question 1 What is the effect of negative publicity on consumer brand attitudes? Extensive literature (Kalaitzandonakes, Marks, & Vickner, 2004; Kasperson et al, 1988; Tolboom, Bronner, & Smit, 2012) on the subject shows that negative publicity lowers consumer brand attitudes. This research confirms this negative impact of publicity. In the experiment, the magnitude of the negative impact is measured, but cannot be confirmed as the experiment did not contain a no-news scenario. 48 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 The impact of negative publicity is personal, as it is highly related to perceived article negativity. Further research could focus on why consumers find an article negative. The seriousness of the event, the relevance of the news, knowledge about the subject and the risk, and level of consumption are most probably the relevant factors. Significant support for source credibility was also found. The stronger effect of credible media is also found in a study of Fan, Geddes, and Flory (2011), where numerous blogs did not have a significant effect on brand attitudes, but traditional media did. It appears that consumers find the risk more valid when it appears in credible media. 5.1.3: Research question 2 Can a company’s response strategy limit the effect of negative publicity on consumer brand attitudes? Well-known brands benefit from a response to negative publicity. For less-known brands, the chosen strategy did not result in different levels of impact on consumer brand attitude. Negative publicity leads to lower attitudes in all tested scenarios, and a response was not able to prevent damage. Bradford and Garrett (1995) did find positive effects of negative publicity if countered well, arguing that the company did not have control over the situation. Given the scenario described in this experiment, a positive nett impact was not expected. Siomkos and Shrivastava (1993) argue that high-reputation firms benefit from a strong response in the form of a product recall when products are determined to be unsafe. Low-reputation firms should use an even stronger response by recalling the products and by being transparent about risks and procedures. These recommendations suppose a serious product harm crisis, which is not the case in the current experiment. It is not surprising that consequently the best response strategy in this case is different. Well-known brands such as Coca Cola can minimise damage to the brand by responding with either an announcement of the adaptation of new processes, or by denying the severity of the risk. Both response strategies effectively state that consumers do not need to worry about consuming the product. In the No response scenario, consumers do not know if the risks are valid and as a result lower their attitudes towards the brand. 49 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 For less-known brands such as First Choice in the experiment the response strategy did not matter. As consumers are not impacted by the news, they do not attach much value to the response of the company. The prominent fall effect introduced by Korkofingas and Ang (2011) finds support, albeit only in the No response scenario. Laufer and Coombs (2006) note that a super effort, comparable to this research’ adaptation response, can harm a company when consumer perceive it as an overreaction. This might be the reason for the lack of difference in delta brand attitude between the Adaptation of new processes and the Denial of severity response. In retrospect it is not possible to determine whether or not the respondents indeed perceived the Adaptation approach as an overreaction. Consequently this could not be tested. The attitude towards the company response was not found to predict delta consumer brand attitudes. As found earlier, it is more important that a response is given. The actual content and attitude towards the response did not make a difference. 5.1.4: Research question 3 Does company familiarity moderate the effect of negative publicity on consumer brand attitudes? Company familiarity can have different impacts according to prior research (Stammerjohan, Wood, Chang, & Thorson, 2005; Van Riel & Fombrun, 2007). The most common theory for the moderating effect of familiarity on the impact of publicity is the buffer effect (Griffin, Babin, & Attaway, 1991). In this experiment, it has no predictive value on delta consumer brand attitudes and on delta likelihood to buy. It is possible that no effect was found due to the already low initial attitudes for the lessknown brand First Choice. Another possible explanation is that it is not familiarity per se that differentiates well-known brands from less-known brands, but factors such as reputation, quality, social responsibility, and personal relevance of the brand. This could be tested by using multiple brands. Interestingly, the BRAND factor did have predictive power for both delta consumer brand attitudes and delta likelihood to buy. It is therefore not the familiarity that affects the expectations and reactions of consumers on negative publicity, but some other factor that differs between Coca Cola and First Choice. Brand reputation could be this factor. For instance, consumers may be fully aware 50 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 of both Coca Cola and First Choice, but have higher expectations of Coca Cola. This difference in expectations is not captured in familiarity but can be captured by brand reputation. The strong negative reaction of consumers in the Coca Cola No response scenario is not related to the familiarity with the brand but rather with some other attribute of the Coca Cola brand, which is lacking for First Choice. Further research could identify this specific factor. 5.1.5: Research question 4 Does negative publicity affect purchase intentions? Supported by the decision making theory (Bonaiuto et al., 2012) it is found that changes in brand attitudes cause changes in likelihood to buy. In the experiment, 78% of the respondents did not change their likelihood to buy. This statistic makes it harder to find results. No change in likelihood to buy for less-known brand was found. In the initial likelihood to buy, 84% reported the lowest possible likelihood to buy. Consequently, the possibilities for a lower average likelihood to buy are very limited. For Coca Cola, again it is best to respond to the news (Figure 8). The Denial of severity strategy minimizes the change in likelihood to buy, where the No company response strategy leads to an estimated drop of .49 on a scale of five. The Adaptation response did not significantly differ from both other scenarios. 51 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Figure 8 Estimated delta likelihood to buy. It was assumed that the high brand value of Coca Cola, a function of expected future cash flows, would limit switching behaviour (Simon & Sullivan, 1993). However, the experiment does not find support for this theory. 5.2: Academic & Managerial Implications 5.2.1: Academic This study is useful for academic purposes for a number of reasons. First it replicates parts of previous research in a new setting, thereby confirming the results. It does also find contradictory results, which increase the knowledge about negative publicity. For instance, the response strategies recommended in the paper of Siomkos and Shrivastava (1993) did not prove useful for the companies in this research. Next to that, this thesis adds to current literature by setting up an experiment on real world brands and a realistic news article. The article is serious, but the mentioned risk is not directly life threatening. A substantial proportion of research on product harm crises, especially case studies, is about events with casualties (Fan, Geddes, & Flory, 2011; Murray & Shohen, 1992; Nemery, Fischler, Boogaerts, Lison, & Willems, 2002). These extreme events do not represent regular situations, and tactics used for them may not be applicable for less serious events. 52 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 The results of this research indicate that it is important to measure article negativity individually. It is possible to measure the general perceived negativity as manipulation check, but it would disregard the differences in perception between respondents. Finally, this research creates a model for the evaluation of the impact of negative publicity. This model could be improved by including more variables, such as level of news seriousness, company responsibility, and time since event. The amount of factors influencing the impact of negative publicity is enormous and analysis of all of them in one model would be highly impractical. Repeated experiments on negative publicity can identify the most important characteristics. When the most important factors are known, it will be easier to incorporate the impact of publicity in wider marketing models. 5.2.2: Managerial Companies should be alert on possible negative publicity. In a world where information is available quicker than ever before, the risk of damage to a brand increases. It is found that when well-known companies do not respond to allegations, that the most damage is done. The ambiguous situation after a news article gets out disturbs consumers (Laufer & Coombs, 2006), and their attitude towards the company drops. Contrary to prior research (Bradford & Garrett, 1995; Menon, Jewell, & Unnava, 1999), it can be the best strategy to deny allegations, when a good argument can be made. For companies it is important to know exactly under what circumstances this response can benefit them. The importance of a corporate response on negative publicity, at least for well-known brands, creates a need for a swift and clear response. Communication and marketing managers should work together in preparing guidelines and evaluating possible scenarios to be able to have an immediate response on allegations (Jeffries-Fox, 2001). In more serious crises, marketing can remind customers of the negative publicity and should therefore be avoided at first (Stammerjohan, Wood, Chang, & Thorson, 2005). When the crisis has settled, marketing tools can restore faith in the brand and repair damage to the brands’ value. A late response is expected to lead to the same results as a No response scenario, as it creates the same ambiguous situation. A clear Denial of the severity could be used at first, and when the situation demands a stronger reaction it is still possible to use an Adaptation response. It should be avoided to alarm consumers by responding overly enthusiastic. A strong effort does not result in less damage for the brand, and can be significantly more costly. 53 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 The experiment is interesting for companies, as they cannot do a real world test on the impact of publicity. For instance, the influence of perceived article negativity could lead to a targeted approach of customers. Those known to have a strong reaction could be targeted individually or as a group and receive more information and extra service. Other consumers may find that unnecessary and are better off with a simple message. 5.3: Limitations & Further Research 5.3.1: Limitations This research is subject of a range of limitations. The scope of the experiment is restricted to one level of negative publicity and three response scenarios, for two brands. The number of factors in the model was constrained by concerns of practicality. The experiment is subject to all limitations generally applicable to surveys. For instance, consumers who are surveyed report more extreme intentions (Chandon, Morwitz, & Reinartz, 2005). The scenarios used in prior research on negative publicity differ from study to study, and this experiment is no exception. Due to the impossibility of using a standard news article, it is hard to make comparisons between studies. A Dutch, fairly high educated sample of respondents was used. The impact of news may be different in other cultures and for less high educated people. Only 33% of respondents is female. Women do tend to react stronger on negative news (Laufer & Coombs, 2006). Brand attitudes do not differ significantly before and after negative news in a research of Sago and Hinnenkamp (2014). In this experiment, females did react more negative, Mean DifferenceDelta brand attitude = -.42, p = .020. The age of the respondents has a wider spread than commonly found in academic research, and is somewhat representative of the Dutch population. The research design does not include real choices but is limited to reported intentions. Self-reported attitudes and purchase intentions may be biased to overstate intention and can falsely give managers and researchers the impression of strong impact. Chandon, Morwitz, and Reinartz (2005) analysed the validity of self-reported intentions. They conclude that intentions measured in surveys overstate the effect on real world behaviour. In this experiment actual behaviour is not measured and therefore some limitations to the results apply. It is expected that the purchase intentions are correlated with brand attitudes, based on the model of Reibstein (1978). 54 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 An observational study may overcome this problem, although it is nearly impossible to control news articles and relevant factors without notifying test subjects. Time series analyses have a large potential in the era of big data and number crunching. However, due to the number of factors of influence for buying decisions, it is very hard to distil the effect of publicity. The experiment does not include an analysis of impact of publicity over time. It is focussed on the short term effect of publicity, which is not expected to last for long when the message is not repeated. The timing of the company response could also influence results, but it is not possible to incorporate a gap of a few weeks between the news and the company response, without respondents being affected by environmental variables outside researchers’ control. The focus on negative publicity does limit analyses to the direct effect of news articles. The power of word of mouth is not considered as a factor. Word of mouth may have a positive effect for the Adaptation strategy, a somewhat positive effect for the Denial of severity strategy and an increasing negative effect for No response scenarios. 5.3.2: Further research Further research can focus on minimising the impact of the aforementioned limitations. Of primary interest would be the interaction of negative publicity with various marketing efforts. Does advertising dampen the effect of negative publicity? Or can it make matters worse by focussing attention on the brand? A research on this subject would benefit from an experimental design to isolate marketing and publicity effects. It requires great effort to set up a research in a real world setting, but it would be very interesting. One can start with regular monitoring of brand attitudes, purchase intentions and actual sales data. When negative publicity about the brand gets out, respondents can be asked whether or not they have seen the news. The regular monitoring creates a baseline, and the effect of the news can be measured with time series analyses on the per-event and post-event data. The main drawback of this setup is that a researcher cannot know when negative publicity is expected and obviously companies try to prevent negative publicity at all. Other factors that are recommended to be incorporated in new studies are: impact of social responsibility, effect of publicity over time, different levels of seriousness of the news, and responsibility of the company. The extensiveness of factors and levels makes for a complex research design. It also needs very large groups of respondents, as the number of potential scenarios grows rapidly. Some of these factors have been studied separately, but possible interaction effects cannot be determined. 55 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Especially interesting would be an analysis of the impact of word of mouth and online sharing on the effect of company response strategies. More and more brands use social media to manage brand reputation, answer consumer questions and generate attention and awareness. Consumers may get used to this direct communication channel and expect companies to give information directly in case of negative news. Evaluation of impact of the time till first response and the availability of information could be one of the main subjects of a study. This would give managers guidance on how fast they would need to react and how much information they should share to minimize the impact of negative publicity. A different point of view on effects of negative publicity is to see if negative publicity for one brand affects consumer brand attitudes for other brands. Siomkos, Triantafillidou, Vassilikopoulou, and Tsiamis (2010) did research on the threats and opportunities for competitors of a company in product-harm crisis. Opportunities for further research lie in evaluating different response strategies for competing brands, both in publicity and in marketing communication. The research design can have the same basic elements of the current experiment. Instead of company response, a competitor response or advertisement can be shown. For competitors of companies in a negative spotlight, this could create opportunities to gain trust or market share. It can also defend competitors against potential allegations when consumers believe that the problem of the company is a potential problem for more companies in the industry. General recommendations on further research are to use a broader and more diverse sample with different cultural backgrounds in follow-up studies. Improvements in the research design can be sought in crafting higher credibility articles and more diverse responses. Furthermore, the scale on which the likelihood to buy is measured should be evaluated. Currently, it reveals no significant changes. A more detailed scale may be able to produce better results. 6: References Ahluwalia, R., Burnkrant, R. E., & Unnava, H. R. (2000). Consumer response to negative publicity: the moderating role of commitment. Journal of marketing research, 37(2), 203-214. Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational behavior and human decision processes, 50(2), 179-211. Berger, J., Sorensen, A. T., & Rasmussen, S. J. (2010). Positive effects of negative publicity: when negative reviews increase sales. Marketing Science, 29(5), 815-827. 56 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Bradford, J. L., & Garrett, D. E. (1995). The effectiveness of corporate communicative responses to accusations of unethical behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 14(11), 875-892. Bonaiuto, M., Caddeo, P., Carrus, G., De Dominicis, S., Maroni, B., & Bonnes, M. (2012). Food reputation impacts on consumer's food choice. Corporate Communications: An International Journal, 17(4), 462-482. Bond, J., & Kirshenbaum, R. (1998). Under the radar: Talking to today's cynical consumer. New York: Wiley. CBS Statistisch Jaarboek. (2014). Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek. Chandon, P., Morwitz, V. G., & Reinartz, W. J. (2005). Do intentions really predict behavior? Selfgenerated validity effects in survey research. Journal of Marketing, 69(2), 1-14. Coca-Cola en Pepsi passen recept aan om kankerwaarschuwing (2012, March 9). De Volkskrant. Retrieved from http://www.volkskrant.nl. Dawar, N., & Lei, J. (2009). Brand crises: the roles of brand familiarity and crisis relevance in determining the impact on brand evaluations. Journal of Business Research, 62(4), 509-516. Dean, D. H. (2004). Consumer reaction to negative publicity effects of corporate reputation, response, and responsibility for a crisis event. Journal of Business Communication, 41(2), 192-211. Eisend, M., & Küster, F. (2011). The effectiveness of publicity versus advertising: A meta-analytic investigation of its moderators. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 39(6), 906921. Fan, D. P., & Cook, R. D. (2003). A differential equation model for predicting public opinions and behaviors from persuasive information: application to the Index of Consumer Sentiment. Journal of Mathematical Sociology, 27(1), 29-51. Fan, D., Geddes, D., & Flory, F. (2013). The Toyota Recall Crisis: Media Impact on Toyota's Corporate Brand Reputation. Corporate Reputation Review, 16(2), 99-117. Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (2001). Fairness theory: Justice as accountability. Advances in organizational justice, 1, 1-55. Fournier, S. (1998). Consumers and their brands: developing relationship theory in consumer research. Journal of consumer research, 24(4), 343-353. Greyser, S. A. (2009). Corporate brand reputation and brand crisis management. Management Decision, 47(4), 590-602. Griffin, M., Babin, B. J., & Attaway, J. S. (1991). An empirical investigation of the impact of negative public publicity on consumer attitudes and intentions. Advances in Consumer Research, 18(1), 334-341. 57 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Haley, R. I., & Case, P.B. (1979). Testing Thirteen Attitude Scales for Agreement and Brand Discrimination. Journal or Marketing, 43, 20-32. Hill, S. J., Lo, J., Vavreck, L., & Zaller, J. (2013). How quickly we forget: The duration of persuasion effects from mass communication. Political Communication, 30(4), 521-547. "Ingrediënt in cola is kankerverwekkend” (2011, February 2). Het Laatste Nieuws. Retrieved from http://www.hln.be. Jeffries-Fox, B. (2001). Toward an understanding of how news coverage and advertising impact consumer perceptions, attitudes and behavior. Institute for Public Relations. Kalaitzandonakes, N., Marks, L. A., & Vickner, S. S. (2004). Media coverage of biotech foods and influence on consumer choice. American Journal of Agricultural Economics, 86(5), 12381246. Kasperson, R. E., Renn, O., Slovic, P., Brown, H. S., Emel, J., Goble, R., Kasperson, J. X. & Ratick, S. (1988). The social amplification of risk: A conceptual framework. Risk analysis, 8(2), 177-187. Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American psychologist, 28(2), 107. Korkofingas, C., & Ang, L. (2011). Product recall, brand equity, and future choice. Journal of Marketing Management, 27(9-10), 959-975. Laufer, D., & Coombs, W. T. (2006). How should a company respond to a product harm crisis? The role of corporate reputation and consumer-based cues. Business Horizons, 49(5), 379-385. Li, M., Chapman, S., Agho, K., & Eastman, C. J. (2008). Can even minimal news coverage influence consumer health-related behaviour? A case study of iodized salt sales, Australia. Health education research, 23(3), 543-548. Lynch Jr, J. G. (1982). On the external validity of experiments in consumer research. Journal of consumer Research, 225-239. McCombs, M. E., & Shaw, D. L. (1972). The agenda-setting function of mass media. Public opinion quarterly, 36(2), 176-187. Menon, G., Jewel, R. D., & Rao Unnava, H. (1999). When a company does not respond to negative publicity: cognitive elaboration vs. negative affect perspective. Advances in Consumer Research, 26, 325-329. Murray, E., & Shohen, S. (1992). Lessons from the Tylenol tragedy on surviving a corporate crisis. Medical Marketing and Media, 27(2), 14-19 Nemery, B., Fischler, B., Boogaerts, M., Lison, D., & Willems, J. (2002). The Coca Cola Incident in Belgium, June 1999. Food and Chemical Toxicology, 40(11), 1657–1667. Newcom Research & Consultancy. (2011). Newcom Vertrouwensindex 2011. Retrieved from http://www.scribd.com/doc/70391283/Newcom-Vertrouwensindex-2011 58 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Oliver, R. L. (1999). Whence consumer loyalty? The Journal of Marketing, 33-44. Poulsen, S.P., Juhl, H.J., Esbjerg, L., Brunsø, K,. Bech-Larsen, T., & Grunert, K.G. (2005) Measuring Brand Strength and Its Sources Using Choice Modeling. Unpublished manuscript, Aalborg University, Aalborg, Denmark. Reibstein, D. J. (1978). The prediction of individual probabilities of brand choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 163-168. Roehm, M. L., & Brady, M. K. (2007). Consumer Responses to Performance Failures by High‐Equity Brands. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(4), 537-545. Rosenbaum, M. E., & Levin, I. P. (1969). Impression formation as a function of source credibility and the polarity of information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 12(1), 34. Rosenstock, I. M., Strecher, V. J., & Becker, M. H. (1994). The health belief model and HIV risk behavior change. In Preventing AIDS (pp. 5-24). Springer US. Rhee, M., & Haunschild, P. R. (2006). The liability of good reputation: A study of product recalls in the US automobile industry. Organization Science, 17(1), 101-117. Sago, B., & Hinnenkamp, C. (2014). The Impact of Significant Negative News on Consumer Behavior towards Favorite Brands. Global Journal of Business Research (GJBR), 8(1). Sherell, D. L., & Reidenbach, E. R. (1986). A consumer responses framework for negative publicity: Suggestions for responses strategies. Akron Business and Economic Review, 17(2), 37-44. Simon, C. J., & Sullivan, M. W. (1993). The measurement and determinants of brand equity: a financial approach. Marketing science, 12(1), 28-52. Siomkos, G., & Shrivastava, P. (1993). Responding to product liability crises. Long Range Planning, 26(5), 72-79. Siomkos, G., Triantafillidou, A., Vassilikopoulou, A., & Tsiamis, I. (2010). Opportunities and threats for competitors in product-harm crises. Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 28(6), 770-791. Spears, N., & Singh, S. N. (2004). Measuring attitude toward the brand and purchase intentions. Journal of Current Issues & Research in Advertising, 26(2), 53-66. Stammerjohan, C., Wood, C. M., Chang, Y., & Thorson, E. (2005). An empirical investigation of the interaction between publicity, advertising, and previous brand attitudes and knowledge. Journal of Advertising, 34(4), 55-67. Steenkamp, J. B. E., De Jong, M. G., & Baumgartner, H. (2010). Socially desirable response tendencies in survey research. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(2), 199-214. Tolboom, M., Bronner, F., & Smit, E. (2012). The Potential Danger of Negative Free Publicity for the Consumer-Brand Relationship. Advances in Advertising Research (Vol. III) (pp. 391-402). Gabler Verlag. 59 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Torquato, R., Franco, C. M., & Bianchi, A. (2012). Seat Belt Use Intention among Brazilian Undergraduate Students. Revista Colombiana de Psicología,21(2), 253-263. Van Riel, C. B., & Fombrun, C. J. (2007). Essentials of corporate communication: Implementing practices for effective reputation management. Routledge. Vassilikopoulou, A., Siomkos, G., Chatzipanagiotou, K., & Pantouvakis, A. (2009). Product-harm crisis management: Time heals all wounds? Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 16(3), 174180. 60 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 7: Appendix Appendix 1. List of factors (commands), meanings and levels. FACTOR Scenario.# Scenario.name AGE FEMALE EDUCATION EDU_TEXT SCENARIO.123.number SCENARIO.123 DELTA.ATT ATT.time1 ATT.time2 CC.FC.scenario BRAND Scenario_Response Scenario_Adaption BUY_INTENT.t1 BUY_INTENT.t2 DELTA.BUY BUY_INTENT.t1.cent NPS.t1 NPS.t2 DELTA.NPS FAM.t1 FAM.t1.cent ARTC.NEG ARTC.NEG.CENT SOURCE.CRED SOURCE.CRED.CENT ATT.RESPONSE ATT.RESPONSE.CENT RESP.CRED RESP.POS Levels Meaning 1,2,3,4,5,6 Company response strategy Coca Cola Adaption, Coca Cola Denial, Coca Cola No Response, First Choice Adaption, First Choice Denial, First Choice No Response 0,1 1,2,3,4,5,6,7 open question 1,2,3 Adaptation, Denial, No Response Min=-6 / Max=6 Min=1 / Max=7 Min=1 / Max=7 1,2 Coca Cola, First Choice 0,1 0,1 Min=1 / Max=5 Min=1 / Max=5 Min=-4 / Max=4 Min=0 / Max=10 Min=0 / Max=10 Min=-10 / Max=10 Min=1 / Max=5 Min=1 / Max=5 Min=1 / Max=5 Min=1 / Max=? Min=1 / Max=5 Age in years 0 = Male, 1 = Female VMBO, HAVO, VWO, MBO, HBO, WO, other Other education Adaptation, Denial, No Response =ATT.time1-ATT.time2 Initial brand attitude Post-test brand attitude Select brand 1 for a company response 1 for adaptation response Initial likelihood to buy Post-test likelihood to buy =BUY_INTENT.t1-BUY_INTENT.t2 Centered values for likelihood to buy Net promotor score time 1 Net promotor score time 2 =NPS.t1-NPS.t2 Initial brand familiarity Centered values for familiarity Article negativity Centered values for article negativity Source credibility Centered values for source credibility Attitude towards company response Centered values for comp. response Response credibility Response positivity 61 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Appendix 2. Survey Q1.1 Beste respondent, Met deze enquête zou ik graag een aantal opvattingen over het gebruik van producten te weten komen. Deze enquête is volledig anoniem: uitkomsten ervan zullen niet terug te voeren zijn op individuele respondenten. Het invullen van deze enquête zal enkele minuten van je tijd in beslag nemen. Ik wil je bij voorbaat hartelijk danken voor je medewerking. Eventuele vragen en opmerkingen kunnen aan het einde van deze enquête gemeld worden. Q2.1 Coca-Cola is een merk cola. Het is een suikerhoudende koolzuurhoudende frisdrank die wordt verhandeld door The Coca-Cola Company en is bijna overal ter wereld verkrijgbaar. In hoeverre ben je bekend met Coca-Cola? Totaal onbekend Enigszins onbekend Neutraal Enigszins bekend Totaal bekend Q2.2 Wat is jouw indruk van Coca-Cola? ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ Onaantrekkelijk Slecht Negatief Aantrekkelijk Goed Positief Q2.3 Hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je Coca-Cola koopt in de komende 10 dagen? Absoluut niet Waarschijnlijk niet Neutraal Waarschijnlijk wel Absoluut wel Q2.4 Op een schaal van 0-10, hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je Coca-Cola bij vrienden of familie aanbeveelt? Helemaal niet waarschijnlijk ○0 ○1 ○2 ○3 Heel erg waarschijnlijk ○4 ○5 ○6 ○7 ○8 ○9 ○ 10 Q3.1 62 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 First Choice Cola is een Nederlands colamerk dat sinds 1996 op de markt is. Het is onder meer verkrijgbaar bij supermarkten Coop, Emté, Dekamarkt en Nettorama. In hoeverre ben je bekend met First Choice Cola? Totaal onbekend Enigszins onbekend Neutraal Enigszins bekend Totaal bekend Q3.2 Wat is jouw indruk van First Choice Cola? ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ Onaantrekkelijk Slecht Negatief Aantrekkelijk Goed Positief Q3.3 Hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je First Choice Cola koopt in de komende 10 dagen? Absoluut niet Waarschijnlijk niet Neutraal Waarschijnlijk wel Absoluut wel Q3.4 Op een schaal van 0-10, hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je First Choice Cola bij vrienden of familie aanbeveelt? Helemaal niet waarschijnlijk ○0 ○1 ○2 ○3 Heel erg waarschijnlijk ○4 ○5 ○6 ○7 ○8 ○9 ○ 10 63 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Scenario 1 Q4.1 Lees het onderstaande nieuwsartikel en ga door op de volgende pagina. "Ingrediënt in Coca-Cola is kankerverwekkend" Bron: NRC Handelsblad © Reuters. Een ingrediënt dat gebruikt wordt in Coca-Cola vormt een kankerrisico en moet verboden worden, aldus een belangrijke lobbygroep. "In tegenstelling tot de karamel die je thuis zou maken door suiker te smelten, ontstaat de kunstmatige kleurstof in cola's en andere producten door suikers te laten reageren met ammoniak en sulfieten onder hoge druk en hoge temperaturen. Hierdoor ontstaan de twee stoffen 2-MI en 4MI, uit onderzoek van de overheid blijkt dat dit leukemie, long-, lever- of schildklierkanker veroorzaakt bij laboratoriumratten of -muizen." De bezorgdheid omtrent deze kunstmatige bruine kleurstof groeit, volgens onderzoekers kan dit honderden kankergevallen veroorzaken. "De karamelkleurstof gebruikt in Coca-Cola moet verbannen worden", aldus de gezondheidslobbygroep CSPI. Het 'National Toxicology Program' zegt dat er duidelijk bewijs is dat zowel 2-MI en 4-MI kanker veroorzaakt bij dieren, daarom is de kans groot dat dit bij mensen ook het geval is." Onderzoekers aan de universiteit van Wageningen ontdekten een beduidend niveau van 4-MI in alle varianten Coca-Cola. Directeur van de lobbygroep CSPI vraagt de overheid om actie te ondernemen: "Kankerverwekkende kleurstoffen horen niet thuis in voeding, zeker niet wanneer het enkel van esthetisch belang is. De naam karamelkleurstof beschrijft niet duidelijk dat het om een additief gaat. Het is een geconcentreerde bruine mengeling van chemische stoffen die niet in de natuur voorkomen." De CSPI voegt er aan toe dat het verhoogde risico geassocieerd met de consumptie van deze chemische stoffen extreem klein is. De tien theelepels suiker in een blikje cola vormt een groter gezondheidsprobleem. Maar het niveau van 4-MI in de geteste cola's veroorzaakt toch jaarlijks honderden kankergevallen bij de bevolking. Voor zover bekend hebben andere merken cola geen last van dit gezondheidsrisico vanwege een ander productieproces. 64 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Q4.2 Lees het onderstaande nieuwsartikel en ga door op de volgende pagina. "Coca-Cola past recept aan om kankerwaarschuwing" Bron: NRC Handelsblad © Reuters. De grootste speler in de frisdrankmarkt, Coca-Cola, gaat zijn recept voor 's werelds meest populaire drankje aanpassen nu de overheid een ingrediënt op de lijst van kankerverwekkende stoffen heeft geplaatst. Coca-Cola zegt dat het de smaak van cola niet zal beïnvloeden. De colagigant ziet zich genoodzaakt tot de aanpassing van hun zo gekoesterde recepten over te gaan om te voorkomen dat ze een waarschuwingslabel op de verpakkingen van alle drankjes moeten plaatsen. Zo'n label dat voor kanker waarschuwt zou funest kunnen zijn voor de marketing van de producten. Eeuwenoud recept Het recept van Coca-Cola is ruim 100 jaar oud en was van oorsprong een medicijn in de vorm van een siroop waaraan in eerste instantie water en later koolzuurhoudend water werd toegevoegd. Coca-Cola gebruikt de stof '4-methylimidazole' (afgekort 4-MEI), om een caramelkleur aan de cola te geven. Maar die chemicalie is nu toegevoegd aan de lijst van kankerverwekkende stoffen, wat inhoudt dat fabrikanten de aanwezigheid van die stof moeten melden, gepaard met een waarschuwing dat het kanker kan veroorzaken. Om hogere kosten te voorkomen, gaat het merk het recept wereldwijd aanpassen. Schadelijk Een studie heeft aangetoond dat de stof 4-MEI een verhoogd risico tot kanker met zich meebrengt in ratten en muizen, maar er is volgens de Dranken Vereniging geen bewijs dat het ook schadelijk is voor mensen. Volgens de organisatie zou je elke dag een paar duizend blikjes cola moeten drinken gedurende 70 jaar om dezelfde dosis binnen te krijgen die de proefdieren in het laboratorium was toegediend. 65 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Q4.3 Wat is jouw indruk van Coca-Cola? ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ Onaantrekkelijk Slecht Negatief Aantrekkelijk Goed Positief Q4.4 Hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je Coca-Cola koopt in de komende 10 dagen? Absoluut niet Waarschijnlijk niet Neutraal Waarschijnlijk wel Absoluut wel Q4.5 Op een schaal van 0-10, hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je Coca-Cola bij vrienden of familie aanbeveelt? Helemaal niet waarschijnlijk ○0 ○1 ○2 ○3 Heel erg waarschijnlijk ○4 ○5 ○6 ○7 ○8 ○9 ○ 10 Q4.6 Wat vind je van het nieuws over Coca-Cola? Erg positief Positief Neutraal Negatief Erg Negatief Q4.7 Beoordeel onderstaande stellingen Helemaa l mee oneens Mee oneens Niet eens / niet oneens Mee eens Helemaal mee eens Ik vind het nieuwsitem over Coca-Cola geloofwaardig Ik vind Coca-Cola schuldig aan het probleem Ik vind de reactie van Coca-Cola positief Ik vind de reactie van Coca-Cola geloofwaardig Ik vind de reactie van Coca-Cola sterk Het nieuws over Coca-Cola heeft invloed op mijn gedrag Q4.8 Hoe vaak lees of kijk je het nieuws? (online & offline) Meerdere keren Ongeveer één Enkele keren Enkele keren per dag keer per dag per week Eéns per week per maand Nauwelijks 66 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Q4.9 Beoordeel onderstaande stellingen Helemaa l mee oneens Mee oneens Niet eens / niet oneens Mee eens Helemaal mee eens Ik vind nieuwsartikelen geloofwaardig Ik laat me beïnvloeden door het nieuws Ik ben kritisch op communicatie van bedrijven Scenario 2 Q5.1 Lees het onderstaande nieuwsartikel en ga door op de volgende pagina. "Ingrediënt in Coca-Cola is kankerverwekkend" Bron: NRC Handelsblad © Reuters. Een ingrediënt dat gebruikt wordt in Coca-Cola vormt een kankerrisico en moet verboden worden, aldus een belangrijke lobbygroep. "In tegenstelling tot de karamel die je thuis zou maken door suiker te smelten, ontstaat de kunstmatige kleurstof in cola's en andere producten door suikers te laten reageren met ammoniak en sulfieten onder hoge druk en hoge temperaturen. Hierdoor ontstaan de twee stoffen 2-MI en 4MI, uit onderzoek van de overheid blijkt dat dit leukemie, long-, lever- of schildklierkanker veroorzaakt bij laboratoriumratten of -muizen." De bezorgdheid omtrent deze kunstmatige bruine kleurstof groeit, volgens onderzoekers kan dit honderden kankergevallen veroorzaken. "De karamelkleurstof gebruikt in Coca-Cola moet verbannen worden", aldus de gezondheidslobbygroep CSPI. Het 'National Toxicology Program' zegt dat er duidelijk bewijs is dat zowel 2-MI en 4-MI kanker veroorzaakt bij dieren, daarom is de kans groot dat dit bij mensen ook het geval is." Onderzoekers aan de universiteit van Wageningen ontdekten een beduidend niveau van 4-MI in alle varianten Coca-Cola. Directeur van de lobbygroep CSPI vraagt de overheid om actie te ondernemen: "Kankerverwekkende kleurstoffen horen niet thuis in voeding, zeker niet wanneer 67 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 het enkel van esthetisch belang is. De naam karamelkleurstof beschrijft niet duidelijk dat het om een additief gaat. Het is een geconcentreerde bruine mengeling van chemische stoffen die niet in de natuur voorkomen." De CSPI voegt er aan toe dat het verhoogde risico geassocieerd met de consumptie van deze chemische stoffen extreem klein is. De tien theelepels suiker in een blikje cola vormt een groter gezondheidsprobleem. Maar het niveau van 4-MI in de geteste cola's veroorzaakt toch jaarlijks honderden kankergevallen bij de bevolking. Voor zover bekend hebben andere merken cola geen last van dit gezondheidsrisico vanwege een ander productieproces. Q5.2 Lees het onderstaande nieuwsartikel en ga door op de volgende pagina. "Coca-Cola negeert kankerwaarschuwing" Bron: NRC Handelsblad © Reuters. Frisdrankproducent Coca-Cola negeert de oproep om het gebruik van 4-MEI in haar producten te stoppen. Volgens het bedrijf is de stof onschadelijk in de hoeveelheden die consumenten normaal binnen krijgen. Een lobbygroep had om een verbod gevraagd, na resultaten van een onderzoek dat een verhoogd risico op kanker bij ratten en muizen had aangetoond. Er is volgens de Dranken Vereniging geen bewijs dat het ook schadelijk is voor mensen. Volgens de organisatie zou je elke dag een paar duizend blikjes cola moeten drinken gedurende 70 jaar om dezelfde dosis binnen te krijgen die de proefdieren in het laboratorium was toegediend. De colafabrikant ziet zich dan ook niet genoodzaakt tot de aanpassing van hun zo gekoesterde recept. Een aanpassing aan het recept zou kunnen betekenen dat de smaak verandert. Eeuwenoud recept Het recept van Coca-Cola is ruim 100 jaar oud en was van oorsprong een medicijn in de vorm van een siroop waaraan in eerste instantie water en later koolzuurhoudend water werd toegevoegd. 68 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Coca-Cola gebruikt de stof '4-methylimidazole' (afgekort 4-MEI), om een caramelkleur aan de cola te geven. Maar een lobbygroep wil die chemicalie nu toevoegen aan de lijst van kankerverwekkende stoffen, wat inhoudt dat fabrikanten de aanwezigheid van die stof moeten melden, gepaard met een waarschuwing dat het kanker kan veroorzaken. Q5.3 Wat is jouw indruk van Coca-Cola? ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ Onaantrekkelijk Slecht Negatief Aantrekkelijk Goed Positief Q5.4 Hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je Coca-Cola koopt in de komende 10 dagen? Absoluut niet Waarschijnlijk niet Neutraal Waarschijnlijk wel Absoluut wel Q5.5 Op een schaal van 0-10, hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je Coca-Cola bij vrienden of familie aanbeveelt? Helemaal niet waarschijnlijk ○0 ○1 ○2 ○3 Heel erg waarschijnlijk ○4 ○5 ○6 ○7 ○8 ○9 ○ 10 Q5.6 Wat vind je van het nieuws over Coca-Cola? Erg positief Positief Neutraal Negatief Erg Negatief Q5.7 Beoordeel onderstaande stellingen Helemaa l mee oneens Mee oneens Niet eens / niet oneens Mee eens Helemaal mee eens Ik vind het nieuwsitem over Coca-Cola geloofwaardig Ik vind Coca-Cola schuldig aan het probleem Ik vind de reactie van Coca-Cola positief Ik vind de reactie van Coca-Cola geloofwaardig Ik vind de reactie van Coca-Cola sterk Het nieuws over Coca-Cola heeft invloed op mijn gedrag 69 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Q5.8 Hoe vaak lees of kijk je het nieuws? (online & offline) Meerdere keren Ongeveer één Enkele keren Enkele keren per dag keer per dag per week Eéns per week per maand Nauwelijks Q5.9 Beoordeel onderstaande stellingen Helemaa l mee oneens Mee oneens Niet eens / niet oneens Mee eens Helemaal mee eens Ik vind nieuwsartikelen geloofwaardig Ik laat me beïnvloeden door het nieuws Ik ben kritisch op communicatie van bedrijven Scenario 3 Q8.1 Lees het onderstaande nieuwsartikel en ga door op de volgende pagina. "Ingrediënt in Coca-Cola is kankerverwekkend" Bron: NRC Handelsblad © Reuters. Een ingrediënt dat gebruikt wordt in Coca-Cola vormt een kankerrisico en moet verboden worden, aldus een belangrijke lobbygroep. "In tegenstelling tot de karamel die je thuis zou maken door suiker te smelten, ontstaat de kunstmatige kleurstof in cola's en andere producten door suikers te laten reageren met ammoniak en sulfieten onder hoge druk en hoge temperaturen. Hierdoor ontstaan de twee stoffen 2-MI en 4MI, uit onderzoek van de overheid blijkt dat dit leukemie, long-, lever- of schildklierkanker veroorzaakt bij laboratoriumratten of -muizen." De bezorgdheid omtrent deze kunstmatige bruine kleurstof groeit, volgens onderzoekers kan dit honderden kankergevallen veroorzaken. "De karamelkleurstof gebruikt in Coca-Cola moet verbannen worden", aldus de gezondheidslobbygroep CSPI. Het 'National Toxicology Program' zegt dat er duidelijk bewijs is dat 70 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 zowel 2-MI en 4-MI kanker veroorzaakt bij dieren, daarom is de kans groot dat dit bij mensen ook het geval is." Onderzoekers aan de universiteit van Wageningen ontdekten een beduidend niveau van 4-MI in alle varianten Coca-Cola. Directeur van de lobbygroep CSPI vraagt de overheid om actie te ondernemen: "Kankerverwekkende kleurstoffen horen niet thuis in voeding, zeker niet wanneer het enkel van esthetisch belang is. De naam karamelkleurstof beschrijft niet duidelijk dat het om een additief gaat. Het is een geconcentreerde bruine mengeling van chemische stoffen die niet in de natuur voorkomen." De CSPI voegt er aan toe dat het verhoogde risico geassocieerd met de consumptie van deze chemische stoffen extreem klein is. De tien theelepels suiker in een blikje cola vormt een groter gezondheidsprobleem. Maar het niveau van 4-MI in de geteste cola's veroorzaakt toch jaarlijks honderden kankergevallen bij de bevolking. Voor zover bekend hebben andere merken cola geen last van dit gezondheidsrisico vanwege een ander productieproces. Coca-Cola wilde geen mededelingen doen over de beschuldigingen. Q8.2 Wat is jouw indruk van Coca-Cola? ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ Onaantrekkelijk Slecht Negatief Aantrekkelijk Goed Positief Q8.3 Hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je Coca-Cola koopt in de komende 10 dagen? Absoluut niet Waarschijnlijk niet Neutraal Waarschijnlijk wel Absoluut wel Q8.4 Op een schaal van 0-10, hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je Coca-Cola bij vrienden of familie aanbeveelt? Helemaal niet waarschijnlijk ○0 ○1 ○2 ○3 Heel erg waarschijnlijk ○4 ○5 ○6 ○7 ○8 ○9 ○ 10 Q8.5 Wat vind je van het nieuws over Coca-Cola? Erg positief Positief Neutraal Negatief Erg Negatief 71 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Q8.6 Beoordeel onderstaande stellingen Helemaal mee oneens Mee oneens Niet eens / niet oneens Mee eens Helemaal mee eens Ik vind het nieuwsitem geloofwaardig Ik vind Coca-Cola schuldig aan het probleem Het nieuws over Coca-Cola heeft invloed op mijn gedrag Q8.7 Hoe vaak lees of kijk je het nieuws? (online & offline) Meerdere keren Ongeveer één Enkele keren Enkele keren per dag keer per dag per week Eéns per week per maand Nauwelijks Q8.8 Beoordeel onderstaande stellingen Helemaa l mee oneens Mee oneens Niet eens / niet oneens Mee eens Helemaal mee eens Ik vind nieuwsartikelen geloofwaardig Ik laat me beïnvloeden door het nieuws Ik ben kritisch op communicatie van bedrijven Scenario 4 Q6.1 Lees het onderstaande nieuwsartikel en ga door op de volgende pagina. "Ingrediënt in First Choice Cola is kankerverwekkend" Bron: NRC Handelsblad © Reuters. Een ingrediënt dat gebruikt wordt in First Choice Cola vormt een kankerrisico en moet verboden worden, aldus een belangrijke lobbygroep. 72 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 "In tegenstelling tot de karamel die je thuis zou maken door suiker te smelten, ontstaat de kunstmatige kleurstof in cola's en andere producten door suikers te laten reageren met ammoniak en sulfieten onder hoge druk en hoge temperaturen. Hierdoor ontstaan de twee stoffen 2-MI en 4MI, uit onderzoek van de overheid blijkt dat dit leukemie, long-, lever- of schildklierkanker veroorzaakt bij laboratoriumratten of -muizen." De bezorgdheid omtrent deze kunstmatige bruine kleurstof groeit, volgens onderzoekers kan dit honderden kankergevallen veroorzaken. "De karamelkleurstof gebruikt in First Choice Cola moet verbannen worden", aldus de gezondheidslobbygroep CSPI. Het 'National Toxicology Program' zegt dat er duidelijk bewijs is dat zowel 2-MI en 4-MI kanker veroorzaakt bij dieren, daarom is de kans groot dat dit bij mensen ook het geval is." Onderzoekers aan de universiteit van Wageningen ontdekten een beduidend niveau van 4-MI in alle varianten First Choice Cola. Directeur van de lobbygroep CSPI vraagt de overheid om actie te ondernemen: "Kankerverwekkende kleurstoffen horen niet thuis in voeding, zeker niet wanneer het enkel van esthetisch belang is. De naam karamelkleurstof beschrijft niet duidelijk dat het om een additief gaat. Het is een geconcentreerde bruine mengeling van chemische stoffen die niet in de natuur voorkomen." De CSPI voegt er aan toe dat het verhoogde risico geassocieerd met de consumptie van deze chemische stoffen extreem klein is. De tien theelepels suiker in een blikje cola vormt een groter gezondheidsprobleem. Maar het niveau van 4-MI in de geteste cola's veroorzaakt toch jaarlijks honderden kankergevallen bij de bevolking. Voor zover bekend hebben andere merken cola geen last van dit gezondheidsrisico vanwege een ander productieproces. Q6.2 Lees het onderstaande nieuwsartikel en ga door op de volgende pagina. "First Choice Cola past recept aan om kankerwaarschuwing" Bron: NRC Handelsblad © Reuters. 73 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Een grote speler in de frisdrankmarkt, First Choice Cola, gaat zijn recept voor 's werelds meest populaire drankje aanpassen nu de overheid een ingrediënt op de lijst van kankerverwekkende stoffen heeft geplaatst. First Choice Cola zegt dat het de smaak van cola niet zal beïnvloeden. De colagigant ziet zich genoodzaakt tot de aanpassing van hun zo gekoesterde recepten over te gaan om te voorkomen dat ze een waarschuwingslabel op de verpakkingen van alle drankjes moeten plaatsen. Zo'n label dat voor kanker waarschuwt zou funest kunnen zijn voor de marketing van de producten. Eeuwenoud recept Het recept van First Choice Cola is ruim 100 jaar oud en was van oorsprong een medicijn in de vorm van een siroop waaraan in eerste instantie water en later koolzuurhoudend water werd toegevoegd. First Choice Cola gebruikt de stof '4-methylimidazole' (afgekort 4-MEI), om een caramelkleur aan de cola te geven. Maar die chemicalie is nu toegevoegd aan de lijst van kankerverwekkende stoffen, wat inhoudt dat fabrikanten de aanwezigheid van die stof moeten melden, gepaard met een waarschuwing dat het kanker kan veroorzaken. Om hogere kosten te voorkomen, gaat het merk het recept aanpassen. Schadelijk Een studie heeft aangetoond dat de stof 4-MEI een verhoogd risico tot kanker met zich meebrengt in ratten en muizen, maar er is volgens de Dranken Vereniging geen bewijs dat het ook schadelijk is voor mensen. Volgens de organisatie zou je elke dag een paar duizend blikjes cola moeten drinken gedurende 70 jaar om dezelfde dosis binnen te krijgen die de proefdieren in het laboratorium was toegediend. Q6.3 Wat is jouw indruk van First Choice Cola? ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ Onaantrekkelijk Slecht Negatief Aantrekkelijk Goed Positief Q6.4 Hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je First Choice Cola koopt in de komende 10 dagen? Absoluut niet Waarschijnlijk niet Neutraal Waarschijnlijk wel Absoluut wel Q6.5 Op een schaal van 0-10, hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je First Choice Cola bij vrienden of familie aanbeveelt? Helemaal niet waarschijnlijk ○0 ○1 ○2 ○3 Heel erg waarschijnlijk ○4 ○5 ○6 ○7 ○8 ○9 ○ 10 74 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Q6.6 Wat vind je van het nieuws over First Choice Cola? Erg positief Positief Neutraal Negatief Erg Negatief Q6.7 Beoordeel onderstaande stellingen Helemaal mee oneens Mee oneens Niet eens / niet oneens Mee eens Helemaal mee eens Ik vind het nieuwsitem over First Choice Cola geloofwaardig Ik vind First Choice Cola schuldig aan het probleem Ik vind de reactie van First Choice Cola positief Ik vind de reactie van First Choice Cola geloofwaardig Ik vind de reactie van First Choice Cola sterk Het nieuws over First Choice Cola heeft invloed op mijn gedrag Q6.8 Hoe vaak lees of kijk je het nieuws? (online & offline) Meerdere keren Ongeveer één Enkele keren Enkele keren per dag keer per dag per week Eéns per week per maand Nauwelijks Q6.9 Beoordeel onderstaande stellingen Helemaa l mee oneens Mee oneens Niet eens / niet oneens Mee eens Helemaal mee eens Ik vind nieuwsartikelen geloofwaardig Ik laat me beïnvloeden door het nieuws Ik ben kritisch op communicatie van bedrijven Scenario 5 Q7.1 Lees het onderstaande nieuwsartikel en ga door op de volgende pagina. "Ingrediënt in First Choice Cola is kankerverwekkend" Bron: NRC Handelsblad 75 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 © Reuters. Een ingrediënt dat gebruikt wordt in First Choice Cola vormt een kankerrisico en moet verboden worden, aldus een belangrijke lobbygroep. "In tegenstelling tot de karamel die je thuis zou maken door suiker te smelten, ontstaat de kunstmatige kleurstof in cola's en andere producten door suikers te laten reageren met ammoniak en sulfieten onder hoge druk en hoge temperaturen. Hierdoor ontstaan de twee stoffen 2-MI en 4MI, uit onderzoek van de overheid blijkt dat dit leukemie, long-, lever- of schildklierkanker veroorzaakt bij laboratoriumratten of -muizen." De bezorgdheid omtrent deze kunstmatige bruine kleurstof groeit, volgens onderzoekers kan dit honderden kankergevallen veroorzaken. "De karamelkleurstof gebruikt in First Choice Cola moet verbannen worden", aldus de gezondheidslobbygroep CSPI. Het 'National Toxicology Program' zegt dat er duidelijk bewijs is dat zowel 2-MI en 4-MI kanker veroorzaakt bij dieren, daarom is de kans groot dat dit bij mensen ook het geval is." Onderzoekers aan de universiteit van Wageningen ontdekten een beduidend niveau van 4-MI in alle varianten First Choice Cola. Directeur van de lobbygroep CSPI vraagt de overheid om actie te ondernemen: "Kankerverwekkende kleurstoffen horen niet thuis in voeding, zeker niet wanneer het enkel van esthetisch belang is. De naam karamelkleurstof beschrijft niet duidelijk dat het om een additief gaat. Het is een geconcentreerde bruine mengeling van chemische stoffen die niet in de natuur voorkomen." De CSPI voegt er aan toe dat het verhoogde risico geassocieerd met de consumptie van deze chemische stoffen extreem klein is. De tien theelepels suiker in een blikje cola vormt een groter gezondheidsprobleem. Maar het niveau van 4-MI in de geteste cola's veroorzaakt toch jaarlijks honderden kankergevallen bij de bevolking. Voor zover bekend hebben andere merken cola geen last van dit gezondheidsrisico vanwege een ander productieproces. 76 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Q7.2 Lees het onderstaande nieuwsartikel en ga door op de volgende pagina. "First Choice Cola negeert kankerwaarschuwing" Bron: NRC Handelsblad © Reuters. Frisdrankproducent First Choice Cola negeert de oproep om het gebruik van 4-MEI in haar producten te stoppen. Volgens het bedrijf is de stof onschadelijk in de hoeveelheden die consumenten normaal binnen krijgen. Een lobbygroep had om een verbod gevraagd, na resultaten van een onderzoek dat een verhoogd risico op kanker bij ratten en muizen had aangetoond. Er is volgens de Dranken Vereniging geen bewijs dat het ook schadelijk is voor mensen. Volgens de organisatie zou je elke dag een paar duizend blikjes cola moeten drinken gedurende 70 jaar om dezelfde dosis binnen te krijgen die de proefdieren in het laboratorium was toegediend. De colafabrikant ziet zich dan ook niet genoodzaakt tot de aanpassing van hun zo gekoesterde recept. Een aanpassing aan het recept zou kunnen betekenen dat de smaak verandert. Eeuwenoud recept Het recept van First Choice Cola is ruim 100 jaar oud en was van oorsprong een medicijn in de vorm van een siroop waaraan in eerste instantie water en later koolzuurhoudend water werd toegevoegd. First Choice Cola gebruikt de stof '4-methylimidazole' (afgekort 4-MEI), om een caramelkleur aan de cola te geven. Maar een lobbygroep wil die chemicalie nu toevoegen aan de lijst van kankerverwekkende stoffen, wat inhoudt dat fabrikanten de aanwezigheid van die stof moeten melden, gepaard met een waarschuwing dat het kanker kan veroorzaken. Q7.3 Wat is jouw indruk van First Choice Cola? Onaantrekkelijk Slecht Negatief ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ Aantrekkelijk Goed Positief 77 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Q7.4 Hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je First Choice Cola koopt in de komende 10 dagen? Absoluut niet Waarschijnlijk niet Neutraal Waarschijnlijk wel Absoluut wel Q7.5 Op een schaal van 0-10, hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je First Choice Cola bij vrienden of familie aanbeveelt? Helemaal niet waarschijnlijk ○0 ○1 ○2 ○3 Heel erg waarschijnlijk ○4 ○5 ○6 ○7 ○8 ○9 ○ 10 Q7.6 Wat vind je van het nieuws over First Choice Cola? Erg positief Positief Neutraal Negatief Erg Negatief Q7.7 Beoordeel onderstaande stellingen Helemaal mee oneens Mee oneens Niet eens / niet oneens Mee eens Helemaal mee eens Ik vind het nieuwsitem over First Choice Cola geloofwaardig Ik vind First Choice Cola schuldig aan het probleem Ik vind de reactie van First Choice Cola positief Ik vind de reactie van First Choice Cola geloofwaardig Ik vind de reactie van First Choice Cola sterk Het nieuws over First Choice Cola heeft invloed op mijn gedrag Q7.8 Hoe vaak lees of kijk je het nieuws? (online & offline) Meerdere keren Ongeveer één Enkele keren Enkele keren per dag keer per dag per week Eéns per week per maand Nauwelijks 78 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Q7.9 Beoordeel onderstaande stellingen Helemaa l mee oneens Mee oneens Niet eens / niet oneens Mee eens Helemaal mee eens Ik vind nieuwsartikelen geloofwaardig Ik laat me beïnvloeden door het nieuws Ik ben kritisch op communicatie van bedrijven Scenario 6 Q9.1 Lees het onderstaande nieuwsartikel en ga door op de volgende pagina. "Ingrediënt in First Choice Cola is kankerverwekkend" Bron: NRC Handelsblad © Reuters. Een ingrediënt dat gebruikt wordt in First Choice Cola vormt een kankerrisico en moet verboden worden, aldus een belangrijke lobbygroep. "In tegenstelling tot de karamel die je thuis zou maken door suiker te smelten, ontstaat de kunstmatige kleurstof in cola's en andere producten door suikers te laten reageren met ammoniak en sulfieten onder hoge druk en hoge temperaturen. Hierdoor ontstaan de twee stoffen 2-MI en 4MI, uit onderzoek van de overheid blijkt dat dit leukemie, long-, lever- of schildklierkanker veroorzaakt bij laboratoriumratten of -muizen." De bezorgdheid omtrent deze kunstmatige bruine kleurstof groeit, volgens onderzoekers kan dit honderden kankergevallen veroorzaken. "De karamelkleurstof gebruikt in First Choice Cola moet verbannen worden", aldus de gezondheidslobbygroep CSPI. Het 'National Toxicology Program' zegt dat er duidelijk bewijs is dat zowel 2-MI en 4-MI kanker veroorzaakt bij dieren, daarom is de kans groot dat dit bij mensen ook het geval is." Onderzoekers aan de universiteit van Wageningen ontdekten een beduidend niveau van 4-MI in alle varianten First 79 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Choice Cola. Directeur van de lobbygroep CSPI vraagt de overheid om actie te ondernemen: "Kankerverwekkende kleurstoffen horen niet thuis in voeding, zeker niet wanneer het enkel van esthetisch belang is. De naam karamelkleurstof beschrijft niet duidelijk dat het om een additief gaat. Het is een geconcentreerde bruine mengeling van chemische stoffen die niet in de natuur voorkomen." De CSPI voegt er aan toe dat het verhoogde risico geassocieerd met de consumptie van deze chemische stoffen extreem klein is. De tien theelepels suiker in een blikje cola vormt een groter gezondheidsprobleem. Maar het niveau van 4-MI in de geteste cola's veroorzaakt toch jaarlijks honderden kankergevallen bij de bevolking. Voor zover bekend hebben andere merken cola geen last van dit gezondheidsrisico vanwege een ander productieproces. First Choice Cola wilde geen mededelingen doen over de beschuldigingen. Q9.2 Wat is jouw indruk van First Choice Cola? ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ Onaantrekkelijk Slecht Negatief Aantrekkelijk Goed Positief Q9.3 Hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je First Choice Cola koopt in de komende 10 dagen? Absoluut niet Waarschijnlijk niet Neutraal Waarschijnlijk wel Absoluut wel Q9.4 Op een schaal van 0-10, hoe waarschijnlijk is het dat je First Choice Cola bij vrienden of familie aanbeveelt? Helemaal niet waarschijnlijk ○0 ○1 ○2 ○3 Heel erg waarschijnlijk ○4 ○5 ○6 ○7 ○8 ○9 ○ 10 Q9.5 Wat vind je van het nieuws over First Choice Cola? Erg positief Positief Neutraal Negatief Erg Negatief 80 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Q9.6 Beoordeel onderstaande stellingen Helemaal mee oneens Mee oneens Niet eens / niet oneens Mee eens Helemaal mee eens Ik vind het nieuwsitem over First Choice Cola geloofwaardig Ik vind First Choice Cola schuldig aan het probleem Het nieuws over First Choice Cola heeft invloed op mijn gedrag Q9.7 Hoe vaak lees of kijk je het nieuws? (online & offline) Meerdere keren Ongeveer één Enkele keren Enkele keren per dag keer per dag per week Eéns per week per maand Nauwelijks Q9.8 Beoordeel onderstaande stellingen Helemaa l mee oneens Mee oneens Niet eens / niet oneens Mee eens Helemaal mee eens Ik vind nieuwsartikelen geloofwaardig Ik laat me beïnvloeden door het nieuws Ik ben kritisch op communicatie van bedrijven Algemene vragen Q10.1 Wat is je leeftijd? Q10.2 Wat is je geslacht? Man Vrouw Q10.3 Welke opleiding volg je, of wat is je hoogst genoten opleiding? VMBO HAVO VWO MBO HBO WO Anders, namelijk ____________________ 81 How to handle negative publicity: An evaluation of response strategies. – Sten Boerkamp – 2014 Q10.4 Dank voor je deelname. Ter info: De nieuwsberichten in dit onderzoek zijn gemanipuleerd voor onderzoeksdoeleinden. Dit onderzoek is niet uitgevoerd in opdracht van een fabrikant maar is volledig onafhankelijk. Q10.5 Mocht je geïnteresseerd zijn in de uitkomsten van het onderzoek, vermeld dan hieronder vrijblijvend je e-mailadres. Q10.6 Eventuele opmerkingen 82