GENERAL INTRODUCTION 1.1 Introduction Obesity has emerged as a major challenge facing the world today and its prevalence has increased substantially to epidemic proportions in both the UK and across the globe (James, 2008; Hossain, Kawar, & Nehas, 2007). Obesity has become a putative public health concern (Zhu, Norman, & While, 2011), acknowledged in 2002 by the World Health Organisation (WHO), who recently coined the term ‘Globesity’ in reference global epidemic of obesity (www.who.int) and to represent the rising trends in obesity across the world as opposed to solely the Western World, where the condition has been most common in the past. There is however a marked discrepancy in prevalence rates of obesity across regions within different countries and across countries within continents (James, 2002). For example, Rennie and Judd (2005) report a higher prevalence of obesity in Wales and Scotland relative to England in the UK. With reference to tackling obesity, the former US Surgeon General (Richard Camona) has warned that “unless we do something about it, the magnitude of the dilemma will dwarf 9/11 or any other terrorist attempts” (Associated Press, 2006). The Surgeon General has also suggested that as a result of the obesity epidemic, the US nation is now battling on two fronts: one a ‘war on terror’ and the other a ‘war on obesity’, that he refers to as “the terror within” (Biltekoff, 2010). This is not an uncommon reference with a cluster of studies that refer to tackling obesity as a “war” (e.g., Evans, 2009; Friedman, 2003; O’Hara & Gregg, 2006). Similarly, the Department of Health (2002) reported that the “growth of overweight and obesity in the population of our country – particularly amongst children – is a major concern. It is a health time bomb with the potential to explode over the next three decades…. Unless this time bomb is defused the consequences for the population’s health, the costs to the NHS and losses to the economy will be disastrous” (p. 44). The extent of the obesity 1 GENERAL INTRODUCTION epidemic has been framed alongside climate change concerns, with both purportedly priority areas that require a rapid response (Foresight, 2007, p. 14). It should be noted here, that overweight and obesity are often discussed together where obesity represents the enemy and overweight the associate, reflecting the notion of a ‘war on obesity’ (Evans, 2009). The metaphor of a war on obesity also highlights the importance of appreciating how the obese are perceived by others, where the ability to assign fault for a condition is key: “In an all out war, expenditure is all out, imprudent - war being defined as an emergency in which no sacrifice is excessive. But the wars against diseases are not just calls for more zeal, and more money to be spent on research. The metaphor implements the way particularly dreaded diseases are envisaged as an alien "other," as enemies are in modern wars; and the move from demonization of the illness to the attribution of fault to the patient is an inevitable one” (Sontag, 1989, p. 99). It would also be worth noting here that the terms overweight and obesity have been and are continued to be used interchangeably. Although they have been defined differently by the World Health Organisation (1998) where overweight is defined as lower than obesity on the basis of Body Mass Index (BMI), there appears to be no evidence to suggest these cut off ranges are beneficial to the understanding of psychological and social consequences of these conditions (Schwartz & Brownell, 2004). In response to the epidemic and the significance placed on treating, managing and preventing the condition, scientific investigation has revealed that obesity represents an array of consequences for individuals and society. It comes as no surprise that an obsession with body weight has been reported (O’Hara & Gregg, 2006) given the idealisation of thinness and the stigmatisation of fatness. 2 GENERAL INTRODUCTION Paradoxically, this obsession with body weight is incoherent with the nature of the obesity problem. Despite a greater likelihood of obesity when body weight increases, this does not always reflect obesity as it is excess body fatness that should be the focus of attention. Obesity by definition refers to “an excess accumulation of body fat, is a heterogeneous disorder with a final common pathway in which energy intake chronically exceeds energy expenditure” (McArdle, Katch, & Katch, 2001, p. 823). Similarly, Janssen, Katzmarzyk, and Ross (2004) report waist circumference and not body mass index as an explanation for obesity related ill health, thus the emphasis placed on body weight is often misplaced on the basis of scientific evidence. In the United States, which currently has the highest rates of obesity across the world, poor diet and physical inactivity is the second most common cause of death and with healthcare costs soaring it has been proposed to overtake tobacco as the leading cause of death (Mokdad, Marks, Stroup, & Gerberding, 2004). In the UK, obesity has trebled over the last twenty years (Royal College of Paediatrics & Child Health, Faculty of Public Health, 2004), with costs for overweight and obesity estimated at approximately £3.2billion (Allender & Rayner, 2007). In 2006, approximately 24% of adults in the UK aged over 16 years were classified as obese, which is an overall increase of 15% since 1993. A similar pattern has also been observed for children aged 2-15 years with 16% classified as obese, an 11% increase since 1995 (Health Survey for England, 2006). Thus, there is an increased concern for the rapid rate at which children and adolescents are becoming obese (British Medical Report, 2003; Seidell, 2000; World Health Organization, 2000). It was proposed that by 2010, 38% of children in the European Union will become overweight (Wang & Lobstein, 2006), although statistics on the prevalence in 2010 are yet to be released, thus this prediction cannot be verified. Obesity is a chronic disorder that is particularly prevalent among poor and minority children (Troiano & 3 GENERAL INTRODUCTION Flegal, 1998). For example, a higher prevalence of obese boys and girls in the UK live in the lowest household income group (Health Survey for England, 2006). Despite this finding, obesity appears to affect individuals of all backgrounds; however, the answer to why prevalence is higher in certain sections of the population may lie in the environment. Environmental Factors There has been much interest in genetic influences on obesity and while this is of high importance, the role of environmental factors and others within a given environment can not be underestimated with regards to not only the development but also the management of the condition (Brownell, 2002). Barnett and Casper (2001) refer to the social environment as encompassing the “immediate physical surroundings, social relationships and cultural milieus within which defined groups of people function and interact” (p. 465). In a foresight review of obesity in 2007, MPs heard that the UK population is becoming a victim of its own economic success. Human life has become more comfortable as a result of the modern transport systems, sedentary employment and the convenience of food, which appear to be major contributors to the predicament. Humans evolved in a world where food was scarce and physical work was tough, however, in the current world food and labour saving devices are in abundance (Johnson, 2007). It is purported that the current environment is ‘obesogenic’ resulting from the generation of an elevated equilibrium level of ‘fatness’ across the entire population comparative to average amounts of activity and therefore to combat the ‘obesity crisis’, one must intervene with the environment as opposed to directly with the individual (Marks, Murray, Evans, Willig, Woodall, & Sykes, 2006). Hill and Peters (1998) support this view proposing that to protect against obesity it is essential for the individual to control the size of food portions, consume a 4 GENERAL INTRODUCTION low fat diet and to be physically active; however, performing these behaviours has become increasingly difficult due to advancements that make human life easier. Similarly, Wadden, Brownell, and Foster (2002) refer to the current world as a “toxic environment” which encourages the consumption of super-sized food portions high in fat and sugar, whilst actively discouraging physical activity. One factor that has played a role in the consumption of super-sized food portions is the fast food restaurant. For example, Macdonald, Cummins and Macintyre (2007) reported a greater density of fast food restaurants in more deprived areas of Scotland and England (where levels of obesity are elevated) whilst Davis and Carpenter (2009) purported that poor quality eating environments increase adolescents’ likelihood to become overweight. It is also reported that in the United States, almost 5,000 schools have fast food outlets in their cafeterias (Brownell, 2002). Furthermore, Jamie Oliver (celebrity chef) examined the content of school dinners reporting that only 37 pence is spent on ingredients that lack nutritional goodness (Marks et al., 2006). Thus, UK school dinners are viewed as ‘toxic’ and as a potential contributor to the increasing levels of fatness in UK children. Another major component of the purported “toxic environment” is the media and its role in advertisement of the so-called big four being pre-sugared breakfast cereals, soft drinks, confectionary and savoury snacks (Marks et al., 2006). The recent ban on advertisement of junk food around television programmes aimed at children aged 4-9 years is an attempt to manage the growing prevalence of overweight and obese children, however, individuals are reportedly unconvinced as to whether a major effect will be observed, with beliefs that this ban does not extend far enough (Potter, 2007). The ‘toxic environment’ is also characterised by the discouragement of physical activity, of which there has been a major decline since the 5 GENERAL INTRODUCTION mid-1900s, where sedentary behaviour has become increasingly appealing as a result of computers, televisions, and video games, especially for children (Brownell, 2002). Holistic Appreciation In addition to the potential health consequences of suffering from the condition, the biological causes and prevalence of obesity are well researched (e.g., Biddle & Mutrie, 2003; Racette, Deusinger, & Duesinger, 2003); however, less is known about the social and psychological costs of obesity (Schwartz & Brownell, 2004; Teachman & Brownell, 2001). The importance of gaining a holistic understanding of obesity cannot be underestimated and as Farrell (2006) states “all biological crises are also cultural crises” (p. 517), and therefore the link between biology and culture should not be overlooked in attempts to understand obesity. Farrell elaborates further suggesting that “biological and medical problems are also cultural sites, where social power and ideological meanings are played out, contested and transformed” (p. 517). Thus, a greater appreciation for other aspects common to the condition alongside biological knowledge is warranted. Specifically, understanding of the role others play and the experiences the obese population have as a result of the condition is required. It is anticipated that by gaining a greater insight into psychosocial aspects of the condition, help to suggest coping mechanisms and inform the development of efficient and effective interventions, which appear to lacking regardless of the ever rising prevalence of obesity. Lewis, Cash, Jacobi, and Bubb-Lewis (1997) reported that being perceived as “fat” is the ultimate failure and this can be perceived as a public demonstration of internal weakness. In modern society, there are several influential factors that contribute to social awareness of body appearance and shape, which includes the booming diet industry that has been estimated to draw $30 billion annually (Gaesser, 2002), the mass media through thin-ideals (Fouts & 6 GENERAL INTRODUCTION Burggraf, 2000), and the social value placed on having a lean physique (Hesse-Biber, 1996; Wolf, 1991). There is persistent societal pressure to comply with a trim appearance and with high failure rates, this has created a billion dollar industry that has lowering calorie intake as the objective (Heil & Henschen, 1996). As a result of the diet industry and the pressures to comply with idealised physiques, attitudes are likely to be developed about those who do achieve the desired physique (favourable) and those who deviate from the ideal (unfavourable). 1.1.1 Attitudes Attitudes are conceived as evaluative summaries of people, behaviours, or objects (Fazio, 1989) and often reflect a variety of information sources (Betsch, Kaufmann, Lindow, Plessner, & Hoffmann, 2006). Attitudes and beliefs influence the way individuals behave towards and think about other people, and as a result refer to “a state of readiness, based on past experiences, which guides, biases or otherwise influences our behaviour” (Cardwell, 2003, p. 20). It is believed that attitudes consist of three components: 1. Affective component: referring to emotions or feelings towards an object. 2. Behavioural component: referring to actions and behaviour towards an object. 3. Cognitive component: referring to knowledge and perceptions of an object or direct experiences and information gained from relevant sources. (Cardwell, 2003) The formation of attitudes is of great interest given their influence on behaviour (e.g., Gross, 1992). When there is uniformity between the three attitude components, it would be rational to propose that one would act or behave in a manner consistent with one’s thoughts and feelings; however, this is not necessarily the case and inconsistencies have been reported. For 7 GENERAL INTRODUCTION example, early research by La Piere (1934) examining ‘anti-Oriental’ attitudes demonstrated that although 91% of establishments responded that they would not allow members of the Chinese race to stay in their accommodation, six months earlier only 1 out of 251 establishments had prevented a Chinese couple from staying there when visited previously. Thus, behaviour observed may be somewhat inconsistent with one’s attitudes. An explanation offered for attitude formation and change is the Elaboration Likelihood Model (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). A key component of the conceptual model is persuasion that operates via two routes: central and peripheral (Figure 1.1). Attitude formation through central route processing involves repeated exposure and direct attention to relevant stimuli resulting in stronger, more robust attitudes, whereas peripheral route processing is less complex, and attitudes may be shaped by superficial aspects of a given message (e.g., source credibility) resulting in weaker, more malleable attitudes (O’Brien, Puhl, Latner, Mir, & Hunter, 2010). Consequently, less cognitive effort is required to change attitudes formed through the peripheral route (Hague & White, 2005). 8 GENERAL INTRODUCTION Figure 1.1: Elaboration Likelihood Model Persuasive Communication Peripheral Attitude Shift Attitude is relatively temporary, susceptible, and unpredictive of behaviour Motivated to Process? Personal relevance; need for cognition; personal responsibility etc. Peripheral Cue Present Positive or negative affect; attractive expert sources; number of arguments etc. Ability to Process? Distraction; repetition; Prior knowledge; message comprehensibility etc. Nature of Cognitive Processing: (Initial attitude, argument quality, etc) Favourable thoughts predominate Unfavourable thoughts predominate Neither or neutral predominate Retain or Regain Initial Attitude Cognitive Structure Change: Are new cognitions adopted and stored in memory? Are different responses made salient than previously? Central Positive Attitude Change Central Negative Attitude Change Attitude is relatively enduring, resistant, and predictive of behaviour (Petty, Kasmer, Haugtvedt, & Cacioppo, 1987) 9 GENERAL INTRODUCTION The extent to which individuals attend to relevant information is an integral aspect of the model and dictates through which of the routes information is processed. Elaboration and diligent consideration of all available information are likely to be high when the individual is motivated and has the ability to attend to the content of the message, resulting in central route processing, whereas low elaboration is likely to result in peripheral route processing (Petty et al., 1987). The complexity of central route processing is evident from Figure 1.1, where an individual needs to be motivated to process the information as this will determine whether one attends to a message, have the ability to process the information, for example, by repeated exposure to the message, have either a favourable or unfavourable preference as indecisiveness will lead to peripheral processing, and to embed those preferences in memory. Attitudes also serve a number of motivational functions and therefore the rationale for forming or changing an attitude may be determined by the desired purpose: 1. Ego-defensive: These attitudes protect individuals from experiencing negative feelings about themselves whilst projecting negative feelings onto others. 2. Value-expressive functions: Attitudes are the way in which individuals express those things that are important to them. 3. Instrumental functions: Attitudes may be adopted and expressed because they enable individuals to gain social acceptance or avoid social disapproval. 4. Knowledge functions: These attitudes help individuals to organise their social worlds along evaluative dimensions (e.g., ‘things I do like’) and allow them to make predictions about events. (Cardwell, 2003) 10 GENERAL INTRODUCTION Whilst attitudes are formed on the basis of evaluative summaries, prejudice has less of an evaluative dimension, thus in its absence prejudice has no grounding and resultant behaviours displayed towards an individual or group are unwarranted (Allport, 1966). 1.1.2 Prejudice Prejudice means “to ‘prejudge’ somebody on the basis of their membership of a particular category or group” (Cardwell, 2003, p. 190). However, not all prejudgments are necessarily prejudice, but only those which when exposed to contradictory knowledge remain constant, resisting reversal. Prejudice refers to more than simply having preconceived ideas about another person or group, but includes forming evaluations of them. Although prejudice may be positive or negative, within the psychological literature prejudice is normally accepted as a negative (i.e. unflattering) attitude towards certain persons or groups. Through stereotyping a person or group, one can maintain their prejudice, where stereotyping refers to “the belief that all members of a particular group share the same characteristics” (Cardwell, 2003, p. 190). For example, individuals may be prejudiced towards all overweight people, believing that they are all lazy and consume vast quantities of food. Socio-cultural theories (e.g., Social Identity Theory: Tajfel & Turner, 1986; Social Role Theory: Eagly, 1987) offer explanations for the origins of prejudice, perceiving prejudice as part of a ‘shared wisdom’ of a culture. Prejudice may develop as a consequence of competition between groups, or as a result of an association with one’s own group as the ‘ingroup’, where others are seen as the ‘outgroup’. Prejudice does not necessarily include behaviours, and therefore is often confused with discrimination, which is behavioural, whereas prejudice is an attitude. Thus, individuals may have a prejudice towards obese individuals, but this does not necessarily mean that this affects their behaviour towards obese 11 GENERAL INTRODUCTION them (Cardwell, 2003). Similar to the suggested origins of prejudice, stereotyping is also proposed to have a social agreement or consensual aspect, and therefore it is of great importance to comprehend others’ attitudes and beliefs and how they change given the significance of others’ influence on the perceptions formed by an individual. According to the Group Norm Theory (Sherif & Sherif, 1953) prejudice and stereotyping are developed and maintained through social consensus, where prejudice develops through a group’s formation, identification and continuous interaction (Brownell Puhl, Schwartz, & Rudd, 2005). With group formation and identity come pressures to adhere to the norms, attitudes and beliefs of a group, whilst rejecting those who are not part of the group. Given the adaptable nature of attitudes, those who wish to remain in a group must also shift their attitudes and behaviours to remain accepted within the group. Crandall, Eshleman, and O’Brien (2002) demonstrated the influence groups have on the attitudes and prejudices members form, reporting that participants used social norms to guide their expressions of prejudice, reactions to hostile jokes and to evaluate discrimination scenarios. An illuminating example of how perceptions are influenced by others were the findings of Blanchard, Crandall, Bringham, and Vaughn (1994) who demonstrated that when a student heard another student from their college condemn racist comments this increased anti-racist opinion, whereas when the student condoned the racist comments, anti-racist opinion decreased compared with a control condition where no comment was provided. These findings provide empirical support for both the influence others have regarding the formation of attitudes and prejudices and for their adaptable nature. There is substantial evidence to suggest that society holds negative perceptions of overweight and obese individuals (Neumark-Szainer, Story, & Faibisch, 1998; Puhl & Brownell, 2001). 12 GENERAL INTRODUCTION Experiences of body size stigmatisation can have serious consequences for the social interactions of overweight individuals (Cossrow, Jeffery, & McGuire, 2001), and the negative attitudes and behaviours displayed towards overweight individuals may impact self perception (Crandall & Biernat, 1990; Musher-Eizenman, Holub, Miller, Goldstein, & Edwards-Leeper, 2004). Whilst links between anti-fat attitude and negative affect have been reported (e.g., Muscher-Eizenman et al., 2004), other research (e.g., Latner and Stunkard, 2003) has highlighted concern for the increase anti-fat attitudes over time, alongside suggestion that these attitudes are likely to be shaped by parents and significant others (Davison & Birch, 2004). Worryingly, this may indicate that these societal attitudes may be more widespread than previously estimated (O’Brian, Hunter, Halberstadt, & Anderson, 2007). Anti-fat attitudes have been suggested to generate a social environment that can be very detrimental to obese individuals’ psychological and social functioning (Morrison & O’Connor, 1999). It is suggested that overweight people have a greater probability of being victimized whilst at school and a lower likelihood of being accepted by elite colleagues, than their “average weight counterparts” (Rothblum, Brand, Miller, & Oetjen, 1990). Consequently, Goldfield and Chrisler (1995) remark that “fat people” have a lower chance of being selected as friends by their peers, which was later supported by Latner, Stunkard, and Wilson (2005) who reported that obese adults were perceived as less favourable as friends than non-obese or disabled adults. A similar result has also been shown with childhood populations (e.g., Latner & Stunkard, 2003). Based on the findings of anti-fat attitude enquiry, Crandall, D’Anello, Sakalli, Lazarus, Wieczorkowska Nejtardt, and Feather (2001) proposed an attribution-value model of 13 GENERAL INTRODUCTION prejudice where it is suggested that “a significant amount of the affective component of attitudes and prejudice toward groups is based on two interrelated factors: attributions of controllability and cultural value” (p. 31). According to Lerner (1980) those who operate on the premise that one gets what they deserve in life or have acted in a way that deserves a certain response, should be treated or receive an appropriate outcome by others in accordance with the value of the outcome. On this basis, those who have positive characteristics such as sports stars should be treated as heroes and those with negative characteristics such as those who are overweight should be punished, avoided and stigmatised, and would therefore deserve the prejudice experienced (Feather, 1996). Prejudice towards a certain group develops as a result of a belief that members of that group share negative characteristics such as a physical handicap or laziness (Crandall et al., 2001). Previous research (e.g., Harris, 1990) has reported the typical perception of those perceived to be fat is of a lack of control, laziness and ugliness. Children and young people are the fastest growing segment of the overweight and obese population (Troiano & Flegal, 2000), and awareness of the potentially harmful perceptions of others is of importance and the consequential effects that this may have for that individual. For example, peer rejection is an important hurdle for overweight children to overcome when in an educational environment, with research reporting that some obese children experience rejection by peers at school (e.g., Puhl & Brownell, 2001). It has been reported that anti-fat prejudice has become so rife within American society, particularly in women, that it is relatively comparable to racial discrimination (Puhl, Andreyeva, & Brownell, 2008), demonstrating the importance of anti-fat attitude enquiry, and the need for investigation in the UK given the prevalence of obesity. Prejudice displayed towards the obese appears to hold a vastly different meaning to prejudice displayed towards other characteristics such as race or 14 GENERAL INTRODUCTION gender. Anti-fat prejudice appears to be openly accepted as opposed to being frowned on as is prejudice such as racism, which presents a problematic situation where social desirability of not expressing anti-fat attitudes or behaviours is rare. Brown and Rothblum (1989) refer to fat oppression as “hatred and discrimination against fat people, primarily fat women, solely because of their body size. It is the stigmatization of being fat, the terror of fat, the rationale for thousands of diets and an equal number of compulsive exercise programmes” (p. 1). Brown and Rothblum (1989) argue that fat oppression is common among females evolving from a white North American culture, where fat oppression is internalised and is a “catalyst for self-hatred” with the radical consequences including starvation and surgery, and the greater likelihood of suffering from eating disorders such as anorexia and bulimia. They also suggest that these cultures breed perceptions of the starved body as equating to value and class, through a message that one can “never be too thin or too rich” (Brown & Rothblum, 1989). Brown and Rothblum suggest that fat oppression is “like physical and sexual violence against women, sexism in action” (p. 1). 1.2 Purpose of the Thesis The primary purpose of this thesis is to examine the social perceptions of obesity, the portrayal of obesity in the UK media, the effect of interpersonal awareness on self perceptions and whether anti-fat attitudes can be modified. There is a growing appreciation for the role others play in the management and treatment of obesity, thus there is a clear need to examine perceptions of obesity in the UK given (1) its rising prevalence (James, 2008), (2) that the majority of previous research that has examined anti-fat attitudes has presented data from non-UK based samples (Costa, Del Pino, & Friedman, 2011; Hill, 2007; Vartanian, 15 GENERAL INTRODUCTION 2010) and (3) those which have used UK based samples have been small scale and specific to certain populations (e.g., Robertson & Vohora, 2008; Tailor & Ogden, 2009). There is also a need to examine the portrayal of obesity in the media. To date, research examining obesity portrayal is relatively scarce, with the majority focussing on the under representation of overweight and obese characters on television and their negative portrayal, with relatively few examining newspaper portrayal. There is a high readership of national newspapers in the UK, and therefore examining their portrayal of the condition appears warranted given that it is suggested newspaper portrayal may shape readers attitudes (Dietrich, Heider, Matschinger, & Anger Meyer, 2006). Furthermore, those which have examined obesity portrayals have used quantitative analysis; however, given the lack of newspaper portrayal investigations, qualitative analysis would appear more appropriate when exploring this relatively un-researched phenomena as well as the nature and tone of the article. Additionally, with the increasing attention paid to body shape and appearance research is required that examines the effect of media portrayal of obesity on self perception and body image given the previously identified deleterious effects of exposure to idealized bodies (e.g., Tiggemann, 2003). Finally, with a growing appreciation for the pervasive nature of anti-fat attitudes (e.g., Puhl & Heuer, 2009) and that they predict anti-fat behaviours (O’Brien, Latner, Halberstadt, Hunter, Andersen, & Caputi, 2008), there is a clear need to explore potential interventions to change anti-fat attitudes. Research examining anti-fat attitude interventions is still in its infancy and therefore with the adverse effects these attitudes present, this highlights the need for further attempts to reduce anti-fat attitudes. 16 GENERAL INTRODUCTION 1.3 Overview of Thesis The structure of this programme of research consists of seven chapters with two experimental studies, one descriptive and one observationally qualitative concentrating on the social perceptions of obesity; Chapter 2 provides a review of the literature examining perceptions of obesity and their associated negative effects, influential sources within society that play a role in attitude formation and a consideration of the theoretical underpinnings of the research. Chapter 3 introduces the first study of this research programme which examined the implicit and explicit perceptions of obesity in UK adults aged 18-65 years. Chapter 4 presents the second study which examined the portrayal of obesity in six UK national newspapers over the period of a year, comparing broadsheet and tabloid portrayals. Chapter 5 presents study three that examined the effects of media portrayal of obesity on self perception and body image, using four different sources in an attempt to increase interpersonal awareness through exposure to others’ physical appearance and behaviour. Chapter 6 then presents study four which examined the effects of exposure to images of overweight and obese populations as a counter-conditioning intervention to reduce anti-fat attitudes and finally Chapter 7 provides a discussion of the overall research programme, including practical implications as a result of the findings, strengths and limitations, and future research suggestions. As part of chapter 7, a conclusion is then offered that addresses the overall contributions made by this research programme. 1.4 Social Psychological Perspective The basic underlying assumption of the social psychological perspective is that the person and the environment interact to influence behaviour (Maio, Verplanken, Manstead et al., 2007). The person and the environment are proposed to be essential factors in the understanding of “upstream” and “downstream” approaches to lifestyle change (McKinlay, 17 GENERAL INTRODUCTION 1993; Verplanken & Wood, 2006). “Upstream” approaches concentrate on altering the environment where the problematic behaviour occurs as well as endorsing more desirable alternatives. Conversely, “downstream” approaches concentrate on altering or preventing the problematic behaviour occurring in individuals considered at risk (Verplanken & Wood, 2006). The research undertaken as part of this thesis has both “upstream” and “downstream” applications, where altering media content and therefore the environment with which the individual interacts to form, maintain and intensify anti-fat attitudes is proposed (Chapter 4), in addition to altering the anti-fat attitudes in individuals observed in Chapter 6. 18