Mary McClure & Ally Satterlee PSY 285 Dr. Jarcho October 24, 2013 Understanding the Effects of Cocaine on Mothers and Infants It is no surprise that an expecting mother’s cocaine use will have adverse effects on the developing fetus. Such effects range from a smaller head size to delayed cognitive development of the child later in life. These delays occur partly because cocaine is able to cross the placental barrier and affect the fetus similarly to how it affects the mother. This paper explores the drug itself, and more specifically its effects on the reproductive aged female population. It also considers the consequences of prenatal exposure to cocaine and how stigma has developed around the notion of “crack babies.” Recent research shows that much of these claims are not well founded. While cocaine use is harmful to the developing fetus, other factors of the mother’s use of the drug, such as her parenting style and socioeconomic status, are equally, if not more significant. Cocaine Cocaine is derived from the leaves of Erythroxylon coca, which is native to South America and has been used by natives since ancient times (Julien, 2001). Modern cocaine was developed in 1855, when the active alkaloid from the leaf was isolated, and later in 1860, purified (Julien, 2001). It was initially used for surgeries; doctors would use the drug as an anesthesia until the early 1900’s (Julien, 2001). Early in its conception, Sigmund Freud, the father of psychology, used, studied, and prescribed cocaine (Julien, 2001). While he first touted its features and used it to treat his personal depression, he ultimately identified its devastating consequences and side-effects, some of which being “tolerance, dependence, a state of psychosis, and withdrawal depression.” He even later referred to the substance as the “third scourge of humanity” (Julien, 2001). Around the same time, no restrictions existed in the United States from selling or consuming cocaine, so many medicines and products, such as Coca-Cola, included small doses of the drug (Julien, 2001). This was banned in 1914 by the Harrison Narcotic act, as many reports had surfaced about deaths and intoxication (Julien, 2001). Use decreased as other cheaper drugs surfaced (Julien, 2001). Wide availability of “crack” cocaine resulted in the drug resurfacing in the late 1970s and early 1980s (Julien, 2001). Today, it comes in water soluble cocaine hydrochloride which can be “injected intravenously or absorbed through the nasal mucosa (snorted)”, or it can be inhaled vapor called freebase or crack cocaine (Julien, 2001). When cocaine is snorted, the blood vessels constrict and limit the absorption, so only 20-30 percent of the drug enters the bloodstream, but the slow absorption prolongs the euphoric effect (Julien, 2001). Smoked cocaine has a rapid and complete absorption with onset of effects within seconds, lasting for about 30 minutes (Julien, 2001). When entered intravenously, the entire dose enters the bloodstream and takes about 30- to 60-seconds to reach the brain (Julien, 2001). The biological half-life of cocaine in plasma is about 30 to 90 minutes (Julien, 2001). With 50 million Americans who have tried cocaine and 6 million who use it regularly, cocaine is an extremely addictive and reinforcing drug (Julien, 2001). Another significant demographic factor is that in 2010, nearly 4.6 million regular cocaine users were female, and 90% of drug-abusing women were at reproductive age (Srathearn and Mayes, 2010). This raises concern about fetal exposure risks and the effects on parenting. Cocaine and Pregnancy In the United States, over 750,000 babies are born each year that have been exposed to drugs (Srathearn and Mayes, 2010). Cocaine is especially pervasive to the unborn baby, as it is able to cross the placental barrier, giving the baby equal levels of the drug that the mother intakes (Julien, 2001). Furthermore, Boskovik et al. (2001) explained that when the drug enters the fetus’s circulation, the drugs intermix with amniotic fluid. The fetus can swallow up to one liter of fluid every day intensifying the drug exposure. One method of assessment of babies suspected to have been exposed to drugs is to test their hair for traces of the drug. Researchers in the Canary Islands also used this method, and explained that due to unreliable maternal self-reports, testing the hair is a useful procedure. This is because the drugs are permanently in the protein matrix of the baby’s hair and can be detected months after drug use. (Cocaine use, 2012). Boskovik et al. notes that in 1988, newborn hair was tested for cocaine, but also described one case in which a mother admitted to cocaine use but her newborn’s hair showed no trace of the drug (2001). However, this method is useful for detecting drugs in the infant’s system. One similar factor among fetal cocaine exposure incidents is poverty. Zuckerman and Frank (1992) note that poor children are prenatally exposed to cocaine more often than their nonpoor counterpants. Boskovik et al. (2001) list nutrition, alcohol and cigarette use, and poor prenatal care as factors that can also explain adverse outcomes for these children. The disparities between what is considered a healthy lifestyle and the lives of these children, which are categorized by inadequate nutrition, healthcare, education and the culture of poverty show that cocaine use is not the only factor at play. (Zuckerman & Frank, 1992). When solely cocaine use is considered and these other significant factors are ignored, mother-blaming is a probably outcome (Zuckerman & Frank, 1992). Furthermore, mothers with drug addictions often face emotional challenges while parenting. The developmental delays that some children whose mothers abused cocaine demonstrate may be partly due to a lack of nurture, rather than chemical harm done by the actual drug. Mothers who abused cocaine expressed an undecided attitude towards raising children and did not know much in regard to basic childcare (Strathearn & Mayes, 2010). They also displayed disengagement in relation to responding to their child’s needs, as well as being less likely to interact with the child. This might allude to an issue of parenting tactics rather than a chemical imbalance. One study found that expectant mothers who abuse cocaine display lower levels of plasma oxytocin (Strathearn & Mayes 2010). This is relevant because for other species of mammals, such as rats and sheep, a higher level of plasma oxytocin is necessary to activate maternal behaviors. This could signify that mothers are chemically unable to create a maternal bond with the child. Another study researched the levels of cortisol in infants that were exposed to cocaine while in the womb (Eiden, Veira, & Granger 2009). The study found evidence that supported the theory that this group of infants would have higher levels of cortisol, the stress hormone. This may be responsible for some of the negative affect and susceptibility to stress that they often experience. Women who abuse cocaine during pregnancy are very likely to suffer from psychological distress, such as paranoia, depression, anxiety, and many other mental health related concerns (Minnes et al., 2008). Furthermore, cocaine activates the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis stress response; this is intensified by female sex hormones which are increased during pregnancy (Williams et al., 2012). Not only is the health of developing fetus impacted by cocaine use, but also the mother’s ability to parent, which will affect the child throughout his or her lifetime. Cocaine use can also physically affect the mother during pregnancy, which is evidenced by research conducted by Hulse and Milne (1997). Their research shows that abruptio placentae, or the separation of the placenta from the uterus before the baby is delivered, is common with cocaine use. By studying available data, they found a strong correlation between maternal cocaine use and abruptio placentae, but noted that the analysis did not take cigarette smoking into consideration (Hulse & Milne, 1997). They identified that other confounding variables could include factors present in poverty, such as poor nutrition and lack of prenatal care, along with the use of multiple drugs (Hulse & Milne, 1997). The Notion of “Crack Babies” There are a fair amount of misconceptions surrounding the public’s understanding of the long term effects of cocaine on a baby. Boskovik et al. (2001) found that fetal cocaine exposure often has negative effects including low birth weight, intrauterine growth retardation, congenital anomalies, and cerebrovascular accidents. This became particularly of interest during the late 1980’s and early 1990’s when the media was flooded with reports about the negative long term negative effects of prenatal cocaine exposure. The infants that resulted were referred to as “crack babies” and many people predicted that their development would be severely delayed. This is evidenced through Zuckerman and Frank (1992), who referenced a quote from a 1990 Rolling Stone Magazine article, which described children who had been prenatally exposed to cocaine as “brain damaged in ways yet unknown” and “oblivious to any affection.” They also reference a New York Times article that reported that children who had been prenatally exposed to drugs were unable to make friends, control impulses, concentrate, feel and return love, and make friends due to brain damage (Zuckerman & Frank, 1992). Articles from popular publications such as these likely influenced the public perception. Discrediting the Perception of “Crack Babies” The notion of “crack babies” was revisited in a New York Times article, which explained that limited research and uncritical evaluations led to the public belief that cocaine would seriously impair the infants’ lives (Winerip, 2013). It stated that in essence, alcohol use during pregnancy is a much more serious problem. The current research supports the notion that cocaine is not a primary cause for an epidemic response. Current research supports this claim, such as a study by Bandstra et al. (2004), which reviewed the cases of nearly 500 infants. The infants included in this study were all of African American origins, of a low socioeconomic status, and were from the inner city. This longitudinal study followed the participants through age 7. The researchers assessed the abilities of each of these children and found that children whose mothers did not abuse cocaine had higher speech abilities than their counterparts. Although interesting, the researchers found that the differences between the two groups’ development were very subtle. This is notable because it alleviates some of the assumptions that people hold about “crack babies” and their development. Another article that references research from Boston University about “crack babies” attempts to discredit much of the stigma directed towards this population. (Huston, McLoyd & Cull 2001). The article compared the findings of various studies that explore elements such as the cognitive functioning, language skills, and physical growth of infants whose mothers were exposed to crack cocaine during the time of their pregnancy. It notes that the findings of such studies’ have results that do not often agree. For example, of nine studies conducted to measure cognitive abilities of infants with and without prenatal exposure to cocaine, only four of the studies found a significant effect produced by cocaine usage. In another case, researchers examined ties between neonatal cocaine exposure and physical growth of infants. Two of the three studies that the article assessed found that cocaine exposed infants had a head that was smaller in size and a lower weight, while one other study showed little to no difference. Once again public perception and scientific findings fail to reach a consensus. Cocaine is a powerful, addicting, and widespread drug in American society. Because its relevance to women of reproductive age and the drug’s high permeability of the placenta, its effects on prenatal development must be considered. While prenatal cocaine exposure is developmentally harmful to the child, it does not warrant the stigma that lingers from the negative media attention that the concept of “crack babies” previously received. Recent research discredits many of the exaggerated claims made in the past. It is important to not only consider prenatal cocaine exposure in these cases, but also the factors of poverty, inadequate nourishment, other drug use, and mother-child relationships. It would be worthwhile to continue the longitudinal studies to examine if fetal cocaine exposure makes an adult more likely to become addicted to cocaine or other drugs References Bandstra, E. S., Vogel, A. L., Morrow, C. E., Anthony, J. C., & Lihua Xue, J. C. (2004). Severity of Prenatal Cocaine Exposure and Child Language Functioning Through Age Seven Years: A Longitudinal Latent Growth Curve Analysis. Substance Use & Misuse, 39(1), 25-59. doi:10.1081/JA-120027765 Boskovic, R., Klein, J., Woodland, C., Karaskov, T., & Koren, G. (2001). The role of the placenta in variability of fetal exposure to cocaine and cannabinoids: A twin study. Canadian Journal Of Physiology & Pharmacology, 79(11), 942-945. Cocaine use during pregnancy assessed by hair analysis in a Canary Islands cohort. (2012). BMC Pregnancy & Childbirth, 12(1), 2-9. doi:10.1186/1471-2393-12-2 Eiden, R. D., Veira, Y., & Granger, D. A. (2009). Prenatal Cocaine Exposure and Infant Cortisol Reactivity. Child Development, 80(2), 528-543. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01277.x Hulse, G. K., & Milne, E. E. (1997). Assessing the relationship between maternal cocaine use and abruptio placentae. Addiction, 92(11), 1547-1551. Julien, Robert M. A Primer of Drug Action. New York: Holt, 2001. Print. Minnes, S., Singer, L. T., Kirchner, H. H., Satayathum, S., Short, E. J., Min, M., & ... Mack, J. P. (2008). The association of prenatal cocaine use and childhood trauma with psychological symptoms over 6 years. Archives Of Women's Mental Health, 11(3), 181-192. doi:10.1007/s00737-008-0011-z Researchers debunk stigma of 'crack babies'. (Cover story). (2001). DATA: The Brown University Digest of Addiction Theory & Application, 20(6), 1. Strathearn, L., & Mayes, L. C. (2010). Cocaine addiction in mothers. Annals Of The New York Academy Of Sciences, 1187(1), 172-183. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05142.x Williams, S. K., Barber, J. S., Jamieson-Drake, A. W., Enns, J. A., Townsend, L. B., Walker, C. H., & Johns, J. M. (2012). Chronic Cocaine Exposure During Pregnancy Increases Postpartum Neuroendocrine Stress Responses. Journal Of Neuroendocrinology, 24(4), 701-711. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2826.2012.02291.x Winerip, M. (2013, May 20). Revisiting the 'crack babies' epidemic that was not. New York Times, Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2013/05/20/booming/revisiting-thecrack-babies-epidemic-that-was-not.html?_r=0 Zuckerman, B., & Frank, D. A. (1992). 'Crack Kids': Not Broken. Pediatrics, 89(2), 337.