Factors that Influence the Development of Prosocial Behavior Why are some people good and others not as much? Prosocial behavior is the behavior that people would describe as “good”; it is the caring about the rights and well being of others. It encompasses feeling empathy and concern for others as well as for society as a whole. The important thing to understand about prosocial behavior is that it often times does not benefit the person that is doing it in any way. As a developed trait, humans are not born with this behavior, and thus learn it from societal institutions, be it the family, the school, etc. The development of prosocial behavior is affected by factors in the school culture, jealousy, anxiety, socioeconomic status, gender, parental socialization, emotional expression, physical aggression, video games, and the expectation of prosocial behavior. In a study lead by J. Barr and Higgins-D’Alessandro, the researchers evaluated changes in prosocial behavior, empathy and school culture. The longitudinal study is focused around a sample of 30 adolescent students and their examinations (twice within two years). 25 students were in the tenth and 5 were in the eleventh grade during the first examination for the study. Out of those 30 students, 13 were from Just Community Schools and 17 were from traditional high schools. The sample was assessed using three different tools. The School Culture Scale is a 25-question survey that measures how the participants feel about their school culture, such as fairness and respect in the classroom. The second tool, the Self-Report Altruism Scale, was used in order to measure the frequency of prosocial behavior in given situations by the participants. The third and final tool used in this study was the Interpersonal Reactivity Index. This tool was used to assess empathy in the individual participants. According to the author, significant changes in socio-cognitive skills and affective responses take place during adolescence. Because these changes have been linked, both conceptually and empirically, with the development of prosocial behavior, more research must be done o understand environmental influences on prosocial behavior development, especially among adolescents. Research done focusing on prosocial behavior has shown that prosocial behavior is connected to perspective-taking abilities and emotional concern. This study attempted to find out if changes in school culture were associated with empathy and prosocial behavior. Using the three aforementioned tools, it was concluded that no differences were found between the two schools regarding prosocial behavior. However, the study does propose that it is possible that being in an environment that promotes more positive school culture stimulates students to minimize the avoidance of helping others. In another study done by C. Culotta and S. Goldstein, how prosocial behavior (among other things) was related to jealousy and social anxiety was examined. This study had a diverse sample of 60 middle school students, made up of 29 girls and 35 boys (4 boys did not complete the survey and so could not be included in the remainder of the study). The researchers used item from the Friendship Jealousy Questionnaire (Parker et al., 2005) to measure jealousy and the Aggressive and Prosocial Behavior Questionnaire (Boxer, et al., 2004) to measure prosocial behavior. The results of the study showed that pro-active prosocial behavior was positively correlated with all of the variables researched in the study, including jealousy and social anxiety. Another correlation that emerged was that relational aggression was positively associated with proactive social behavior (among others). When the participants described that they developed levels of social anxiety, they also inclined to report more proactive prosocial behavior. The authors explain that socially anxious middle school students experienced distress in social situations, and so proactive prosocial behavior might be used in order to gain peer acceptance. A study done by psychologists E. Flouri and N. Tzavidis focused on which of the two variables is a better prosocial behavior and adolescent psychopathology predictor, proximal or distal contextual risk. Proximal contextual risk is defined as the number of confrontational life events experienced within the last year while distal contextual risk is defined as the number of confrontational life events faced before the last year. The sample for this study was made up of 119 children within the age range of 11 to 18 that had a disadvantaged socio-economic background. The researchers used the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to gather their data. Along with measuring prosocial behavior, the SDQ measures hyperactivity, colleague problems, emotive symptoms, and demeanor problems. The study showed that both contextual risks could be linked as predictors for adolescent psychopathology. Although distal contextual risk predicted broad and externalizing psychopathology, the number of proximal adverse life events the children experienced was a more reliable predictor of externalizing psychopathology. Regarding prosocial behavior, the study found that neither proximal nor distal risk was significant enough to be reliable predictors of prosocial behavior, along with peer problems or emotional symptoms. Hastings and Rubin contributed research focusing on the links among gender, inhibition, and the socialization done by parents, and how that affects prosocial behavior. They studied a total of 108 families in an attempt to find links between inhibition (nature of expressing anxiety/distress towards unfamiliar people or situations), parental socialization (how a child raises the child to act), and gender. Out of the initial sample of families, 46 male and 42 female children along with their parents participated in a 2-year follow up for the study. The sample was made up of primarily Caucasian families (97%), and most families were of “average” socioeconomic status. The study concluded that the only direct predictor of prosocial behavior was maternal protective parenting. The authors go on to explain that even though there were few significant interaction terms linking gender, inhibition, or parenting, a common factor that was present in all three was the maternal parenting measures taken by the parents. The researchers concluded that the authoritative and protective parenting of mothers towards their children’s development of prosocial behavior. Regarding inhibition, the study could not show this variable as a predictor of prosocial behavior in the toddlers. The study also concluded that, as predicted, the girls studied in the sample showed more prosocial behavior, This is possibly attributed to the idea that girls are more modifiable to maternal influence, or that parents attempt to socialize their girl children with the stereotypical qualities of females (caring, sympathetic, etc.). Research done by M. McGrath and B. Brown shows the differences in prosocial motives and behavior in children who come from a low-socioeconomic background. The study sample was composed of a total of 122 children from families considered to have low-socioeconomic statuses, with twenty percent of the families living below the poverty level. The sample included children from both the second-grade (31 girls and 30 boys) and fourth-grade (31 girls and 30 boys). The researchers, in private interviews, began a story (a friend encouraging another to give to the poor children that were new to a school) that each child had to complete. The purpose of the study was to find developmental differences in the reasons children from low-socioeconomic status families choose prosocial behavior. When the children were being interviewed and described prosocial behavior, statistical analysis showed fourth grades to comment on abstract prosocial behavior, such as to become a friend to the children in the story, more likely. The researchers also found that there were no notable correlations between the gender of the child and the types of prosocial behavior that they described during the interviews. Finally, McGrath and Brown concluded that, when it came to describing a problem as being reacted to using prosocial behavior or not, younger girls and older boys where more likely to choose prosocial behavior than their counterparts. In 1999, William Roberts did a study in order to find links between the socialization of children’s emotions and prosocial behavior. The five samples were made up of 150 families. Three samples were from Toronto, Ontario, and the other two samples were from Kamloops, British Columbia, and from Vancouver, BC, respectively. The author states that although from very different locations, the samples were not affected significantly by this factor (79% of all comparisons were “replicated across [all] samples”). Roberts indicates that the meta-analysis of the samples showed distinct links between emotional socialization applications and children's capable behaviors. Regarding boys, the author concluded that non-hostile behaviors with peers could be linked to peaceful and non-castigatory responses to conflict. The study also positively related the friendly demeanor shown by boys to the control of emotional expression by maternal influences. Unlike the conclusions made about the boys, the girls in the study showed no pattern in non-aggressive and friendly behavior. For girls, the associations made between emotional socialization and prosocial behaviors were not as frequent and, in general, to a lesser extent than those for boys. The results generated by this study follow the trend of those done before it, where “girls and boys may differ in the factors associated with prosocial behaviours, with boys' prosocial behaviours more clearly and strongly linked with emotional factors such as empathy and emotional expressiveness”, as cited by the author from (Roberts & Strayer, 1996). Another group of psychologists led by E. Romano did a study with the purpose of looking for correlations or links between children’s physical aggression and their prosocial behavior. This was fueled by the lack of research studies measuring all three developmental contexts at the same time (individual, family, and community). The study was made up of 2,745 children in the age range of two to eleven years old. The children in the sample resided in one of 96 Canadian neighborhoods. The researchers note that on average, physical aggression in children decreases with age, while prosocial behavior increases with age. The individual context analyzed in this study includes a child’s understanding of society’s cues regarding prosocial behavior, and the children’s ability to control emotions as they age. The context of family is referred to as communication between family members, family income, and parenting behaviors. Lastly, the community factors that can affect the child’s physical aggressiveness and prosocial behavior include violence within the community, living conditions, and social cohesion within the community. The researchers were able to find statistical associations between the three developmental contexts of the individual, the family and the community. They also concluded that the results displayed a significant negative correlation between the physical aggression of a child and that child’s prosocial behavior. The results followed the trend that as a child increases; his/her physical aggression decreases while prosocial behavior increases. With regards to gender, the researchers found that, on average, boys had the tendency to show more physical aggression and less prosocial behavior than the girls of the same age group. When speaking of the developmental contexts, the study showed that 66 percent of the distinction in physical aggression scores occurred between individuals, over 25 percent was seen in the family context, and nearly 4 percent was in the community. In regards to prosocial behavior, the results were extremely similar, with the only difference being in the community (with 9 percent). E. Rutten and a group of researchers did a study that focused on how teenage antisocial and prosocial behaviors are affected by being athletes in youth sports clubs and organizations. The study was made up of 260 male and female participants from Amsterdam and Utrecht that were between twelve and eighteen years old. The teenage participants were either soccer athletes or competitive swimmers. These two sports were chosen to compare different sport settings; soccer being a team contact sport, and competitive swimming an individual sport. The exposure to the socio-moral atmosphere found in sports is hypothesized to decrease antisocial behavior and increase prosocial behavior in the teenage athletes. The data was analyzed using multilevel regression. The researchers found that socio-moral atmosphere and coach to athlete relationships were significant in the antisocial and prosocial behaviors of the athletes. Having a positive socio-moral atmosphere, along with a good coach to athlete relationship can be linked to low amounts of antisocial behavior (negatively associated with) and high amounts of prosocial behavior (positively associated with). It was also noted that more positive socio-moral atmosphere reports came from the competitive swimmers, and better/more coach to athlete relationships were reported by women athletes. Finally, the female athletes in this study reported higher amounts of prosocial behavior than their male counterparts. Using an inventory, J. Procházka and M. Vaculík focused on finding a correlation between the prosocial behavior of 80 (39 female and 41 male) high school students and the expectation of this behavior from others. Putting emphasis on the social exchange theory and norm of reciprocity, the researchers hypothesized that the participants would behave prosocially because the rewards for doing so would be beneficial to them. The rewards could include being treated as they are treating another, social acceptance, and many more. The norm of reciprocity is key in this study, because of what it implies of both the person behaving prosocially and whom they are helping. By helping others, the expectation of prosocial behavior can manifest as the expectation in ones self to help others or the expectation that the person being helped will return the help in the future. The methods used to measure the levels of prosocial behavior included inventory and observation. These two were implemented in three standardized situations, in all of which prosocial behavior was possible. The study was able to find a moderate correlation between prosocial behavior and the expectation of it when participants reflected on past experiences. In the part of the study that dealt with hypothetical situations, no links between prosocial behavior and its expectations were found. The researchers point out that participants could judge their expectations of prosocial behavior towards themselves as egocentric, and that the expectation of said behavior would best be measured by the expectation of prosocial behavior towards others. The study also rejects the idea that the norm of reciprocity plays a significant role in prosocial behavior. Finally, The effects of the social exchange theory could not be properly measured in this study. Psychologists O. Wiegman and E. van Schie focused on the effects of playing video games on both aggressive and prosocial behavior in children. The sample was made up of 278 children from The Netherlands. The group of participants was selected from five different elementary schools in Enschede, and they were in the seventh and eighth grades. The study also focused on the possible correlations between aggressive video games and aggressive and prosocial behavior. The amount of time spent playing videogames was measured by the children themselves; something that the researchers explained thoroughly to each participant until they all understood how to do so. In diaries, they all wrote one of five responses daily, ranging from “Not” (did not play video games at all) to “more than two hours”. In order to measure the aggressiveness of the participants, the study used the peer-nomination technique. This technique allows the participants to indicate who the children who exhibited aggressive behavior (such as fighting, sticking out their tongues, etc.) were. The same technique was employed in order to measure the prosocial behavior of other participants by participants. Almost three fourths of the participants played video games at least once a week while 17 of the participants played video games daily. The study found that children who played a heavy amount of videogames (at least half of an our per day) showed much less prosocial behavior than those who played a moderate amount of video games (less than half of an hour per day) and than those who were nonplayers. The researchers concluded that the amount of time spent playing video games was negatively correlated to prosocial behavior as a whole (groups). When the analysis was done with the groupings of boys and girls, the above-mentioned results were not prevalent. In the study, boys played more aggressive videogames than girls on average. The study found a positive relationship between the number of aggressive video games a child played and their aggressive behavior. When the children were grouped by gender, the results were not the same for this part of the study either. The study was able to confirm that boys who preferred aggressive video games showed more aggressive behavior than those who did not prefer aggressive videogames. The school culture children are in, jealousy, anxiety, socio-economic status, gender, parental socialization of the child, emotional expression, physical aggression, video games, and its expectation all affect the development of prosocial behavior to some extent. The most important thing to recognize from all ten studies is that prosocial behavior is not something innate in a human being; it is something that is learned through many different things throughout life. The more that researchers, and the public, are able to understand the development of this behavior, along with how to assist it’s growth and in turn inhibit anti-social behavior, the more that members of society can benefit from each other. References Barr, J. J., Higgins-D’Alessandro, A. (2009). How adolescent empathy and prosocial behavior change in the context of school culture: a two-year longitudinal study. Adolescence, 44(176), 751-72. Retrieved from ProQuest Psychology Journals database. Culotta, C. M., Goldstein, S. E. (2008). Adolescents' aggressive and prosocial behavior: Associations with jealousy and social anxiety. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 169(1), 21-33. Retrieved from ProQuest Psychology Journals database. Flouri, E., Tzavidis, N. (2008). Psychopathology and prosocial behavior in adolescents from socio-economically disadvantaged families: The role of proximal and distal adverse life events. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 17(8), 498-506. Retrieved from ProQuest Psychology Journals database. Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., DeRose, L. (2005). Links among gender, inhibition, and parental socialization in the development of prosocial behavior. MerrillPalmer Quarterly, 51(4), 467-93. Retrieved from ProQuest Psychology Journals database. McGrath, M. P., Brown, B. C. (2008). Developmental differences in prosocial motives and behavior in children from low-socioeconomic status families. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 169(1), 5-20. Retrieved from ProQuest Psychology Journals database. Roberts, W. (1999). The socialization of emotional expression: relations with prosocial behaviour and competence in five samples. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 31(2), 72-85. Retrieved from ProQuest Psychology Journals database. Romano, E., Tremblay, R. E., Boulerice, B., Swisher, R. (2005). Multilevel correlates of childhood physical aggression and prosocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33(5), 565-78. Retrieved from ProQuest Psychology Journals database. Rutten, E. A., Geert, J. J. M., Biesta, G. J., Schuengel, C., Dirsk, E., et al (2007). The contribution of organized youth sport to antisocial and prosocial behavior in adolescent athletes. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36(3), 255-64. Retrieved from Proquest Psychology Journals database. Procházka, J., Vaculík, M. (2011). The relationship between prosocial behavior and the expectation of prosocial behavior. Studia Psychologia, 53(4), 363-72. Retrieved from ProQuest Psychology Journals database. Wiegman, O., van Schie, E. (1998). Video game playing and its relations with aggressive and prosocial behavior. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 367-78. Retrieved from ProQuest Psychology Journals database.