Introduction to Experimental Psychology module III 2009

advertisement
Chapter 11: Quasi-Experimentation

Quasi-Experimentation—experimental manipulations
in which the experimenter does not manipulate
variables as in a typical laboratory experiment
(Reichart & Mark, 1998)

In a quasi-experiment, some or all of the variables are
selected, which means that they are not under the direct
control of the experimenter


There are natural “treatments” such as disasters that can be observed
but not manipulated
Also, subjects cannot be randomly assigned to IV levels when you
have subject variables because a person’s status on these variables is
fixed

E.g., age, sex, race, gender, weight, height, IQ
Chapter 11 continued:

Threats to Internal Validity in quasi-experiments:


For observation-treatment-observation designs, there is no true reversal because
the treatment is not under the experimenter’s control because of carryover
effects and changes in the participants, themselves
Maturation—changes in people over time because of growth or other historical
variables


History/Period effects—important historical effects/events that can affect
subjects (e.g., wars, 9/11/01, the depression)


E.g., cognitive development
These are assumed to affect everyone that lives through these
Birth Cohorts—specific events that occur during specific times of people’s lives
(e.g., the depression) that make this cohort different than others

These are generation-specific—a child had different experiences in the depression
than did an adults
Chapter 11 continued:

To overcome threats to internal validity for natural
treatments, we can use:

Nonequivalent control groups—a control group that is not
determined by random assignment but is usually selected
after the fact and is supposed to be equivalent to the
naturally treated group

For example, if we were looking at a new third-grade reading
curriculum (ABA), we could compare this class ex post factor with
another third grade class that was similar to the one we studied but
did not receive the new curriculum

Because we did not randomly assign subjects to groups, though, we
could have induced selection bias
Chapter 11 continued:

To overcome threats to internal validity for one-shot case
studies (AB design), we can use:

Deviant-case analyses—when we take an individual as similar as
possible to our case except for a crucial missing treatment and then
determine how the individuals are different from each other



The similar individual is a nonequivalent control because the case was
selected and not randomly assigned to the “control” condition
The best the researcher can hope for from this design is to search for
potential causal variable
P.Z. was a famous scientist who abused alcohol and had a severe memory
loss


Butters and Cermak (1986) compared him to a colleague of similar age and
background except the colleague did not drink
Because the colleague did not show severe memory deficits, the researchers
concluded that P.Z.’s memory loss was due to his heavy drinking
Chapter 11 continued:

One-shot quasi-experiments can also be useful in
providing descriptive information and, perhaps, even
causal; information

In the Munich airport study, researchers looked at two
experimental groups of children—one who already had
been exposed to airport noise at an old airport, and
another who was about to be at a new airport



Two control groups were not exposed to airport noise
Wave 1 was 6 months before the opening of the new airport, Wave
2 was one year later, and Wave 3 was two years later
The results showed that memory improved after the noise was
removed
Chapter 11 continued:

Threats to validity in time series studies

Interrupted times series design—when you have an
experimental group that you follow over time (before and
after a naturally occurring treatment)

E.g., the relation between school achievement and introduction of
fluoride into drinking water—school achievement increased after
fluoride was introduced


However, you need to control for other possible IVs (e.g., better health
care, better food, more time spent on school)
You can use nonequivalent controls and multiple dependent variables to
increase validity
Chapter 11 continued:

Designs employing subject variables: age as a variable—is it
due to maturation, history, or cohorts?


Cross-sectional
Longitudinal




Regression to the mean, selective attrition
Time-lag—look at the effect of time of testing while holding age
constant (cohort is confounded with time of testing)
Cross-sequential—testing two or more age groups at two or more times
(age is confounded)
Cohort-sequential--
Chapter 12: Conducting Ethical Research


Research—Applied to ethics is a class of activities designed to develop or contribute to
generalizable knowledge (Levine, 1986)
The Belmont Report (1979) formed the foundation of research ethics in the US—it has three
core components:

Respect for Persons—this resulted in a requirement in most situations that research participants consent
to participate (informed consent)

this typically requires:





Beneficence—this is the idea that researchers need to justify their research benefits in the context of
their costs/risks


Disclosure of risks by researchers
Understanding of the study by participants
Voluntariness of participants (e.g., you cannot force a person to participate in medical research or face a loss of care)
Competence—the participant needs the ability to make informed consent (e.g., severe dementia)
All research should involve a cost (or risk)/benefit analysis
Justice—this is the idea that society, in general, should not only benefit in research equally, they should
participate in it equally

E.g., in is inappropriate to force prisoners, institutionalized children, and economically impoverished individuals to
participate in risky research
Chapter 12: Ethics Continued:

Research with human subjects: APA’s 10 principles:





during the planning stage of a study, the investigator has the
responsibility to make a careful evaluation of its acceptability. This
should involve a “cost-benefit analysis”
Determine whether subjects will be “at risk” or at “minimal risk”
The principal investigator holds primary responsibility—even for
collaborators and other employees or assistants.
Except when approved by an IRB (institutional review board), it is
required that subjects read and sign a consent form in which they agree
to participate after being informed of the procedures.
When deception is necessary (and is justified after a cost-benefit
analysis), it is important to debrief subjects afterwards (except in rare
circumstances when such debriefing would only hurt the subjects—
although this determination must be made by the IRB—not the
investigator).
Chapter 12: Ethics continued
Research with human subjects: APA’s 10 principles continued:
Subjects have the right to decline participation without being penalized. This includes
ending participation partially through the experiment.
Protection from harm—risk is minimalized, and participants should be informed of how to
contact the investigator if they have any questions or concerns at a later time. Also,
subjects need to be informed of an IRB contact in case they questions about their rights as
a research subject.
Subjects should be debriefed after completing the study. If debriefing is not the best
option as determined by the IRB, it is still the experimenter’s responsibility to protect the
subject from long-term negative consequences.
Research results obtained from a given subject are anonymous or confidential unless
otherwise agreed upon before participation. Typically results are reported in group form,
or in a case study, names are changed to protect the true identity of the subject (e.g.,
H.M.).






Anonymous—when there is no link between the data and the identity of a subject (this includes linklists)
Confidential—when the experimenter knows the identity of the subjects, but keeps this information
secret from all non-qualified personnel
Chapter 12 continued:

Key components of ethical issues with human
subjects:







Informed consent
Deception and debriefing
Freedom to withdraw from research without penalty
Protection from harm
Debriefing, in general
Removing harmful consequences (e.g., following up on
subjects)
Confidentiality or anonymity
Chapter 12 continued:

Examples:








Milgram and obedience to authority
Watson and Little Albert (conditioned fear and failure to remove
harmful consequences)
Zimbardo and post-hypnotic suggestion (again, failure to debrief)
The Tuskegee Syphilis study (no justifiable rationale—this one fails all
three Belmont Criteria)
Alzheimer’s research and informed consent
Genetic testing research and HIPA
Cloning
Psychologists participating in torture-like interventions
Chapter 12 continued:
Ethics in Animal research

arguments against animal research by animal rights activists:





animals feel pain
destroying or harming any living thing is dehumanizing the human scientist
claims about scientific progress being helped by animal research are a form of racism
speciesism—neglecting the rights and interests of other species
arguments in favor of research with animals (most experimental psychologists
disagree with the last three conclusions)




while animals feel pain, a cost-benefit analysis must be carried out and approved before
animals can be put into painful conditions
given that humans are primarily carnivores, this second point is difficult. Again, a costbenefit analysis must be carried out before any animal can be hurt.
With regard to speciesism, much animal research actually helps animals. However, a
certain level of speciesism is probably necessary unless humans are willing to have a
higher death rate from many diseases.
Chapter 12 continued:
Guidelines for use of animals in research:






The acquisition, care, use, and disposal of all animals must be in compliance
with the current federal, state or provincial, and local laws and regulations
(e.g., air circulation and temperature regulation)
A psychologist in charge must be qualified to carry out the research methods
and oversee the care of laboratory animals.
The psychologist in charge is responsible to ensure that all project personnel
have instruction in experimental methods, and in the care, maintenance, and
handling of the species being used.
Psychologists take every effort to minimize discomfort, illness, and pain of
animals. Subjecting animals to pain, stress, or deprivation is used only after
cost-benefit analysis justification. Surgical procedures are performed under
appropriate anesthesia, and techniques to avoid infection and minimize pain
are followed during and after surgery.
When it is necessary to terminate an animal’s life, it should be done rapidly
and painlessly.
Chapter 12 continued:

Scientific fraud: deliberate bias by scientists





falsification—reporting incorrect data that have been
collected
fabrication—making up uncollected data
plagiarism—using others’ work as your own and not
attributing this work to others
idea plagiarism—stealing another person’s idea without
giving them credit (can be unconscious)
intellectual property
Chapter 13: Interpreting the Results of
Research:
Interpreting specific results:

Scale attenuation—when performance exceeds the limitations of the
measurement scale (ceiling or floor effects)
To fix scale attenuation, we can add other dependent variables (e.g., RT to
the memory study), or we can examine data points that do not exhibit
scale attenuation—e.g., 3, 6, and 9 seconds of retention interval in the
Scarborough (1972) study.
Regression artifact (regression toward the mean)—the tendency for
extreme measures on some variable to be closer to the group mean when
re-measured, due to unreliability of measure.
A large change in performance due to regression to the mean does not
mean that true-score performance has changed, but, rather, that you have
measurement error that inflated or deflated some scores.





random assignment to different conditions helps minimize regression toward the
mean because it equates your groups efficiently as long as your sample is
sufficiently large.
Chapter 13 continued:
Interpreting patterns of research:

reliability and replication



test reliability—is your test consistent?
Experimental reliability—the ability to replicate your
results
Chapter 13 continued:

three types of replications: direct, systematic, and
conceptual



direct replication: simply repeating an experiment as closely as
possible with as few changes as possible in the methods.
systematic replication: repeating an experiment while varying
numerous factors considered to be irrelevant to the phenomenon to
see if it will survive these changes.
Conceptual replication—attempting to demonstrate an experimental
phenomenon with an entirely new paradigm or set of experimental
conditions.
Chapter 13 continued:
Converging operations—procedures that validate
a hypothetical construct used to explain behavior
by eliminating alternative explanations (Garner,
Hake, & Eriksen, 1956.


use Allen, Sliwinski, and Bowie (2002) as an
example—do older adults have consistent peripheral
deficits for semantic and episodic memory (compared
to younger adults), but no deficit for semantic slopes
and a large deficit for episodic slopes?
Chapter 14: Presenting Research Results:
How to write a research report:

develop an outline


use APA format: title page, abstract, introduction,
method, results, discussion, references, author notes,
tables, figure captions, figures
Chapter 14 continued:

It is important to remember that writing a paper is also a
compromise between achieving scientific precision and
expressing creativity. Also, you will be faced with the
dilemma of precision versus parsimony.



Writing is a craft that takes practice.
Most journal submissions take many drafts before they are ready for
submission.
It helps to have at least one other person read your paper and to
provide you with comments before you submit it.
Chapter 14 continued:

In the Introduction, the toughest portion of a paper to write,
you state why a particular issue is of interest, what other
investigators have found, and what variables you will be
examining, and how this will extend previous knowledge.



You should review relevant research that helps you set up your own
research question. At the end of your Intro, you should provide an
overview of your new experiment(s).
Specify your hypotheses explicitly and outline your predictions
derived from the theories that you have already discussed.
After reading your Introduction, it should be clear how your new work
is extending our knowledge in a given area or areas.
Chapter 14 continued:

How to publish an article in a peer-reviewed journal:



submit several copies via mail or electronically submit an article (e.g.,
using a PDF format)
Your article will be sent out to review by the journal editor (or
associate editor that serves as the action editor on your manuscript).
Usually your manuscript will be sent to 2-4 outside reviewers. This is
termed peer review. The reviewers will evaluate your paper on the
basis of its scientific merit.
When the editor receives the reviews back, then he or she must
decide whether to reject without a request for resubmission, request
a revision, or accept your manuscript. Keep in mind that rejection is
the norm.
Chapter 14 continued:

Publishing Continued:




If your paper is rejected, you take the comments from the reviewers
and the editor into consideration in the revision process.
It frequently will require additional data collection to fix problems
brought up by the reviewers.
Once you have addressed these concerns, you typically will submit
your revised paper to another journal—until you find a journal that
finally accepts your paper.
It is common for psychology professors to go through their entire
career and obtain 20 peer-reviewed publications, or less.

So, this is a grueling process that requires a lot of patience and
perseverance.
Chapter 14 continued:

Publishing continued:



Once your paper is accepted for publication (you will receive a
letter from the journal editor informing you of this considerable
accomplishment), you still will have at least the galley proof stage of
revision (and maybe a copy edit version before the galley proof
stage—this is the case for most APA publications).
After a paper has been accepted for publication, there is usually a
6-month to 2-year lag before the paper actually appears in print in
the journal.
All told, the publication process from initial submission to the
appearance of a paper in print is usually 2-3 years (although your
professor is, hopefully, just completing one project that has been
ongoing for 10 years!).
Download