Educational Research: Fundamentals for the

advertisement
Alternative measures of knowledge structure:
as measures of text structure
and of reading comprehension
May 14, 2012
BSI
Nijmegen, Nederland
Roy Clariana
RClariana@psu.edu
Clariana, R.B. (2010). Multi-decision approaches for eliciting knowledge structure. In D. Ifenthaler, P.
Pirnay-Dummer, & N.M. Seel (Eds.), Computer-Based Diagnostics and Systematic Analysis of
Knowledge (Chapter 4, pp. 41-59). New York, NY: Springer. link
1
Overview
• Introduction
• I am an instructional designer and a
connectionist, so my language may be a little
different, also slow me down if my accent is
difficult
• My intent today is to describe my research on
several approaches for measuring Knowledge
Structure (KS) and along the way, describe tools,
and maybe show extra ways of thinking about
text, knowledge, comprehension, and learning
2
KS: Encompassing theoretical positions
• Cognitive structures (de Jong & Ferguson-Hessler, 1986;
Fenker, 1975; Korz & Schulz, 2010; Naveh-Benjamin,
McKeachie, Lin, & Tucker, 1986; Shavelson, 1972)
• Conceptual networks (Goldsmith et al., 1991)
• Conceptual representations (Geeslin & Shavelson, 1975;
Novick & Hmelo, 1994); (McKeithen, Reitman, Rueter, &
Hirtle, 1981)
• Conceptual structures (Geeslin & Shavelson, 1975; Novick
& Hmelo, 1994)
• Knowledge organization and knowledge structures
(McKeithen et al., 1981)
• Semantic structures (Gentner, 1983; Riddoch & Humphreys,
1999).
3
KS: Encompassing theoretical positions
• Spatial knowledge (de Jong & Ferguson-Hessler, 1996;
Dunbar & Joffe, 1997; Jee, Gentner, Forbus, Sageman, &
Uttal, 2009; Korz & Schulz, 2010; Schuldes, Boland, Roth,
Strube, Krömker, & Frank, 2011)
• Categorical knowledge (Candidi, Vicario, Abreu, & Aglioti,
2010; Matsuka, Yamauchi, Hanson, & Hanson, 2005; Stone
& Valentine, 2007; Wang, Rong, & Yu, 2008)
• Conceptual knowledge (de Jong & Ferguson-Hessler, 1996;
Edwards, 1993; Gallese & Lakoff, 2005; Hallett, Nunes, &
Bryant, 2010; Rittle-Johnson & Star, 2009)
4
KS: My sandbox model
Our symbolic connectionist view:
• Knowledge structure (or structural knowledge) refers
to how information elements are organized, in people
and in artifacts
• A departure from most theories, we propose that
knowledge structure is pre-propositional, but that KS is
the precursor of meaningful expression and the
underpinning of thought
• Said differently, knowledge structure is the mental
lexicon that consists of weighted associations (that can
be represented as vectors) between knowledge
elements
5
KS is worth measuring
• Measures of content knowledge structure have been
empirically and theoretically related to memory, classroom
learning, insight, category judgment, rhyme, novice-toexpert transition (Nash, Bravaco, & Simonson, 2006) and
reading comprehension (Britton & Gulgoz, 1991; Guthrie,
Wigfield, Barbosa, Perencevich, Taboada, Davis, Scafiddi, &
Tonks, 2004; Ozgungor & Guthrie, 2004), and
• And findings for combining individual knowledge structures
to form group mental models (Cureeu, P.L., Schalk, R., &
Schruijer, S., 2010; DeChurch & Mesmer-Magnus, 2010;
Johnson & O’Connor, 2008; Mohammed, Ferzandi, &
Hamilton, 2010; Pirnay-Dummer, Ifenthaler, & Spector,
2010).
6
Applied to reading comprehension, KS
as a measure of the situation model
Ferstl & Kintsch (1999)
• Textbase (the text’s semantic content and
structure, van Dijk & Kintsch, 1983)
• Situation model (the integration of the
‘episodic’ text memory with prior domain
knowledge, van Dijk & Kintsch, 1983); also
called mental model of the text, the text
model, the discourse model
Ferstl, E.C., & Kintsch, W. (1999). Learning from text: structural knowledge assessment in the study of discourse comprehension. In
van Oostendorp and Goldman (eds.), The construction of mental representations during reading. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.
7
Visually
contingency
company
plan
classical
efficiency
leadership
humanistic
leadership
service
goal
focus
success
quality
TQM
success
environment
management
classical
individual
quality
work
humanistic
relationship
Text base
management
feelings
goal
environment
empowerment
concerns
productivity
needs
feelings
employee
empowerment
motivation
productivity
work
customers
relationship
company
customers
pay
pay
concerns
plan
contingency
focus
employee
TQM
efficiency
individual
measure
needs
motivation
Situation model
(pre list recall)
Updated situation
model
(post list recall)
contingency
TQM
leadership
classical
efficiency
quality
situation
customers
humanistic
goal
company
concerns
management
success
focus
work
plan
individual
employee
needs
measure
empowerment
motivation
feelings
productivity
pay
Ferstl, E.C., & Kintsch, W. (1999). Learning from text: structural knowledge assessment in the study of discourse comprehension. In
van Oostendorp and Goldman (eds.), The construction of mental representations during reading. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.
relationship
8
A KS measure of the situation model
• Ferstl & Kintsch (1999) used pre-and-post-reading listcued partially-free recall to elicit KS of the birthday story
(which obtains asymmetric matrices)
• Participants – 42 undergraduate students (CU Boulder)
• Pre-reading cued-association KS task: Students were
presented by computer a 60 word list of birthdayrelated terms to view one at a time (randomized), and
then were given the list on paper with 3 blanks beside
each list term and were asked to write in the 3 terms
from the list that come to mind
• Reading: Students then read the 600-word long
birthday story
• Post-reading cued-association KS task: i.e., same as
pre-task, fill in the list
Clariana, R.B., & Koul, R. (2008). The effects of learner prior knowledge when creating concept
maps from a text passage. International Journal of Instructional Media, 35 (2), 229-236.
9
Results
• Established that the KS cued association
paradigm was appropriate for assessing
background knowledge and text memory
• This KS approach facilitated interpretation,
depicting how the text ‘added to’ the post
reading situation model (see their figure 10.4,
p.260); provided a different or other way to think
about reading comprehension (p.268)
• Test-retest reliability may be a problem for this KS
approach
Ferstl, E.C., & Kintsch, W. (1999). Learning from text: structural knowledge assessment in the study of discourse comprehension. In
van Oostendorp and Goldman (eds.), The construction of mental representations during reading. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.
10
Another KS measure of the text base
(or situation model?)
• Clariana & Koul (2008), we asked students to draw
concept maps (KS) of a text
• Participants – 16 graduate students in a science
instructional methods course (Penn State GV)
• First, students discussed concept maps in class
• Then working in dyads (8 pairs), students were given a
255 word passage on the heart and circulatory system
and were asked to create a concept map of it
• KS data sources
– 8 dyad concept maps of the text
– 1 expert concept map of the text
– A Pathfinder network (PFNet) map of the text automatically
formed by ALA-Reader software
Clariana, R.B., & Koul, R. (2008). The effects of learner prior knowledge when creating concept
maps from a text passage. International Journal of Instructional Media, 35 (2), 229-236.
11
Data
• 26 terms identified across all of the maps and text
• (Text  concept map), dyads’ concept map link lines
entered into a 26 x 26 half matrix
• Matrix analyzed using Pathfinder Knot
left atrium
Link Array
right ventricle
to the
pulmonary vein
moves through
pulmonary artery
passes into
to the
lungs
deoxygenated
oxygenated
a
b
c
d
e
f
g
left atrium
lungs
oxygenate
pulmonary artery
pulmonary vein
deoxgenate
right ventricle
a
0
0
0
1
0
0
b
c
d
e
f
g
1
1
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
0
0
0
-
(n2-n)/2 pair-wise comparisons
Clariana, R.B., & Koul, R. (2008). The effects of learner prior knowledge when creating concept
maps from a text passage. International Journal of Instructional Media, 35 (2), 229-236.
12
Data as percent overlap
• Percent overlap was calculated as links in
common divided by the average total links
e.g., Dyad PFNet
2
4
54
e.g., Expert PFNet
% overlap = 4 / ((6+8)/2)
% overlap = 4 / 7
% overlap = 57%
Clariana, R.B., & Koul, R. (2008). The effects of learner prior knowledge when creating concept
maps from a text passage. International Journal of Instructional Media, 35 (2), 229-236.
13
Data as percent overlap
Table 1. The average percent of agreement for each pair of concept map networks (the
number of network propositions are shown in parentheses).
Dyad 1 (18)
Dyad 4 (3)
Dyad 5 (6)
Dyad 7 (13)
Dyad 8 (9)
Dyad 2* (22)
Dyad 3* (11)
Dyad 6* (12)
ALA-Reader  Text (28)
Non-science majors
D1
D4
D5
D7 D8
-0%
-0% 22%
-7% 38% 11%
-0% 0% 0% 0%
-10% 0% 36% 11% 0%
7% 18% 24% 8% 0%
7% 0% 22% 8% 0%
13% 13%
Expert map (16) 12% 0%
* dyads with a science major
6%
9%
24% 5%
14% 8%
Science major
D2* D3* D6*
-61%
-65% 87%
Text
An aspect of
measurement
reliability and
validity
--
52% 46% 55%
58% 59% 64%
-71%
In the epigraph to Educational Psychology: A Cognitive View, Ausubel (1968) says, “The
most important single factor influencing learning is what the learner already knows.”
Clariana, R.B., & Koul, R. (2008). The effects of learner prior knowledge when creating concept
maps from a text passage. International Journal of Instructional Media, 35 (2), 229-236.
14
Percent Agreement with the 255-word text
The strong influence of
prior domain knowledge
80%
70%
Expert map
D6*
D3*
60%
D2*
50%
Only those with prior
domain knowledge
could adequately
‘capture’ the text
40%
30%
20%
D7
D5
10%
D8
D1
D4
0%
0
5
10
15
20
25
Number of Concept Map Propositions
Figure 3. The relationship between the number of propositions in the dyad
concept maps and the average percent agreement with the 255-word text
passage (* shows dyads with a science major).
Clariana, R.B., & Koul, R. (2008). The effects of learner prior knowledge when creating concept
maps from a text passage. International Journal of Instructional Media, 35 (2), 229-236.
15
ALA-Reader papers
ALA-Reader converts text  KS
Clariana, R.B., Wallace, P.E., & Godshalk, V.M. (2009). Deriving and measuring
group knowledge structure from essays: The effects of anaphoric
reference. Educational Technology Research and Development, 57, 725737.
Clariana, R.B., & Wallace, P. E. (2007). A computer-based approach for
deriving and measuring individual and team knowledge structure from
essay questions. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 37 (3), 209225.
Koul, R., Clariana, R.B., & Salehi, R. (2005). Comparing several human and
computer-based methods for scoring concept maps and essays. Journal
of Educational Computing Research, 32 (3), 261-273.
Clariana, R.B. (2010). Deriving group knowledge structure from semantic
maps and from essays. In D. Ifenthaler, P. Pirnay-Dummer, & N.M. Seel
(Eds.), Computer-Based Diagnostics and Systematic Analysis of
Knowledge (Chapter 7, pp. 117-130). New York, NY: Springer.
Also see HIMAT/DEEP software and Hamlet software
16
KS for influencing learning
• e.g., Trumpower et al. (2010) used knowledge
structure of computer programming represented as
network graphs to pinpoint knowledge gaps
• KS elicited as pair-wise
comparisons and datareduced to networks using
Pathfinder KNOT
• Learners’ networks then
compared to an expert
referent network
Trumpower, D.L., Sharara, H., & Goldsmith, T.E. (2010). Specificity of Structural Assessment of Knowledge. Journal of
Technology, Learning, and Assessment, 8(5). Retrieved from http://www.jtla.org.
17
KS for influencing learning
• The problems were intended to be complex enough
so that the solution depended on integration of
several interrelated concepts (relational)
• The presence of subsets of
links in participants’ PFnets
differentially predicted
performance on two types
of problems, thereby
providing evidence of the
specificity of knowledge
structure
Trumpower, D.L., Sharara, H., & Goldsmith, T.E. (2010). Specificity of Structural Assessment of Knowledge. Journal of
Technology, Learning, and Assessment, 8(5). Retrieved from http://www.jtla.org.
18
Protein structure as an analogy of knowledge
structure in reading comprehension
Christian Anfinsen received the Nobel Prize
in Chemistry in 1972:
• Linear sequence of amino acids 
enzyme structure  enzyme function
Is like:
• Linear sequence of words in a text 
knowledge structure  retrieval function
19
AA Linear sequence  enzyme structure  function
APRKFFVGGNWKMNGKRKSLGELIHTLD
GAKLSADTEVVCGAPSIYLDFARQKLDAKI
GVAAQNCYKVPKGAFTGEISPAMIKDIGA
AWVILGHSERRHVFGESDELIGQKVAHAL
AEGLGVIACIGEKLDEREAGITEKVVFQET
KAIADNVKDWSKVVLAYEPVWAIGTGKT
ATPQQAQEVHEKLRGWLKTHVSDAVAV
QSRIIYGGSVTGGNCKELASQHDVDGFLV
GGASLKPEFVDIINAKH
20
Triose Phosphate Isomerase: http://www.cs.wustl.edu/~taoju/research/shapematch-final.pdf
Read linear sequence of words in text
Figure 1, p.136
Hyona, J., & Lorch, R.F. (2004). Effects of topic headings on text processing: evidence
from adult readers’ eye fixation patterns. Learning and Instruction, 14, 131–152.
21
Knowledge structure
Retrieval function
Retrieval structure
Imminent extinction
today
pandas
pandas
live
exclusively
linear
the climate
in the wild
Imminent extinction
the climate
A  B (propositional knowledge):
Where do pandas live? In the wild
A  B,C,D (relational knowledge):
What do we know about pandas today?
Pandas are heading towards extinction in
the wild due to climate change
22
Read  KS  Retrieval function
Retrieval structure
Retrieval function
A  B (propositional knowledge):
Where do pandas live? In the wild
Relational
A  B,C,D (relational knowledge):
What do we know about pandas today?
Pandas are heading towards extinction in
the wild due to climate change
23
Summary of the introduction
• KS cuts across theories, we support connectionist views
• KS is worth measuring, it correlates with many kinds of
performance
• KS can be measured in different ways
• KS has been used to visually represent the reading
comprehension situation model
• KS has been used to visually represent the text structure
• Specific KS structure leads to specific cognitive
performance
• Enzyme Analogy: linear chain  structure  function
24
Measuring knowledge structure
My foundation and trajectory for measuring KS:
• Vygotsky (in Luria, 1979); Miller (1969) card-sorting
approaches
• Deese’s (1965) ideas on the structure of association
in language and thought
• Kintsch and Landauer’s ideas on representing text
structure, and latent semantic analysis
• Recent neural network representations (e.g., Elman,
1995)
Jonassen, Beissner, and Yacci (1993) 
25
Dave Jonassen’s summary of KS
measures…
Trumpower, Sharara, & Goldsmith, 2010
similarity
ratings
Knowledge
elicitation
Ferstl & Kintsch, 1999
concept maps
written text
free
recall
Clariana & Koul, 2008
Knowledge
representation
Knowledge
comparison
Elicit responses  represent responses  compare response
26
Jonassen, Beissner, & Yacci (1993), page 22
Dave Jonassen’s summary …
similarity
ratings
card
sort
Knowledge
elicitation
hierarchical
clustering
quantitative
graph
comparisons
free
recall
written text
additive
trees
relatedness
coefficients
ordered
recall
graph
building
concept maps
To show different KR
let’s do an example …
word
associations
semantic
proximity
C of PFNets
qualitative
graph
comparisons
Knowledge
comparison
Knowledge
representation
Trees
scaling
solutions
expert/
novice
Dimensional
Networks
MDS – multidimensional scaling
ordered
trees
minimum
spanning
trees
link
weighted
Jonassen, Beissner, & Yacci (1993), page 22
Pathfinder
nets
principal
components
cluster
analysis
27
Knowledge Representation (KR)
• Multidimensional scaling (MDS) - Family of distance and
scalar-product (factor) models. Re-scales a set of dis/similarity
data into distances and produces the low-dimensional
configuration that generated them
(e.g., see: http://www.tonycoxon.com/EssexSummerSchool/MDS-whynot.pdf)
• Pathfinder Knowledge Network Organizing Tool (KNOT)
algorithms take estimates of the proximities between pairs of
items as input and define a network representation of the
items. The network (a PFNET) consists of the items as nodes
and a set of links (which may be either directed or undirected
for symmetrical or non-symmetrical proximity estimates)
connecting pairs of the nodes.
(See: http://interlinkinc.net/KNOT.html)
28
Pathfinder Network (PFNet) analysis
• Pathfinder seeks the least weighted path to connect all
terms, shoots for n-1 links if possible
• Pathfinder is a mathematical approach for representing
and comparing networks, see:
http://interlinkinc.net/index.html
• Pathfinder data reduction is based on the least weighted
path between nodes (terms), so for example, Deese’s 171
data points become 18 data points. Only the salient or
important data is retained.
• Pathfinder PFNet uses, for example:
– Library reference analysis
– Use google to search to see many more examples of
how Pathfinder can be used
Note that Ferstl & Kintsch (1999) used Pathfinder
29
bug
flower
yellow
fly
bird
wing
insect
moth
Deese (1965), free recall data (p.56)
moth
100 12 12 12 11
1
0
4
insect
12 100 9
9
17
1
1
33
wing
12
9 100 44 19
0
0
3
are shown
bird
12100 participants
9
44 100
21 a 1list of 0
3
time, and
fly
11related
17 words,
19 one
21 at a100
1
1
8
asked to free recall a related term
yellow
1
1
0
1
1 100 7
0
flower
0
1
0
0
1
7 100 2
bug
4
33
3
3
8
0
2 100
cocoon
11 10
2
2
6
0
0
7
color
0
1
0
1
1
17
3
0
Full array (n * n): 19 x 19 = 361
blue
0
1
0
1
2
23
7
0
Half array ((n – n)/2): ((19 x 19) –19 )/2 = 171
30
bees
2
3
10
10
6
2
2
5
Deese, J. (1965). The structure of associations in language and thought. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins Press, page 56
2
moth
insect
wing
bird
fly
yellow
flower
bug
cocoon
color
blue
bees
summer
sunshine
garden
sky
nature
spring
butterfly
butterfly
spring
nature
sky
garden
sunshine
summer
bees
blue
color
cocoon
bug
flower
yellow
fly
bird
wing
insect
moth
Deese (1965), free recall data (p.56)
100 12 12 12 11
1
0
4
11
0
0
2
2
5
1
1
1
1
15
12 100 9
9
17
1
1
33 10
1
1
3
0
0
0
0
1
0
12
12
9 100 44 19
0
0
3
2
0
0
10
0
0
0
0
3
0
13
12
9
44 100 21
1
0
3
2
1
1
10
0
1
0
1
5
0
12
11 17 19 21 100 1
1
8
6
1
2
6
0
3
0
2
4
0
11
1
1
0
1
1 100 7
0
0
17 23
2
2
7
5
2
4
3
5
0
1
0
0
1
7 100 2
0
3
7
2
1
6
18
2
6
2
6
4
33
3
3
8
0
2 100 7
0
0
5
0
0
0
0
2
0
4
11 10
2
2
6
0
0
7 100 0
0
4
1
1
1
0
2
0
22
0
1
0
1
1
17
3
0
0 100 32
0
0
2
0
8
0
0
0
0
1
0
1
2
23
7
0
0
32 100 1
2
4
4
46
3
2
2
2
3
10 10
6
2
2
5
4
0
1 100 1
2
3
0
4
2
7
2
0
0
0
0
2
1
0
1
0
2
1 100 5
2
0
1
10
0
5
0
0
1
3
7
6
0
1
2
4
2
5 100 2
3
2
15
4
1
0
0
0
0
5
18
0
1
0
4
3
2
2 100 0
4
4
2
1
0
0
1
2
2
2
0
0
8
46
0
0
3
0 100 0
1
0
1
1
3
5
4
4
6
2
2
0
3
4
1
2
4
0 100 2
3
1
0
0
0
0
3
2
0
0
0
2
2
10 15
4
1
2 100 2
15 12 13 12 11
5
6
4
22
0
2
7
0
4
2
0
3
2 100
Full array (n * n): 19 x 19 = 361
Half array ((n2 – n)/2): ((19 x 19) –19 )/2 = 171
31
Deese, J. (1965). The structure of associations in language and thought. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins Press, page 56
Using MDS in SPSS
• Start SPSS and open this Deese data file
• Under Analyze, select Scale, then select
Multidimensional Scaling (ALSCAL)…
1. Move Variable from left to right
2. Create distances from data
3. Model
How to - next page
4. Options
32
Select all of these
33
MDS of the Deese data
Derived Stimulus Configuration
Euclidean distance model
1,5
spring
garden
1,0
summer
sunshine
nature
bees
Dimension 2
flower
cocoon
0,5
butterfl
0,0
moth
-0,5
yellow
blue
-1,0
fly
insect
color
sky
wing
bird
bug
-1,5
-2
-1
0
1
Dimension 1
34
Side issue, the MDS obtains alternate visual
representations (e.g., enantiomorphism)
Eindhoven
Amsterdam
Utrecht
Nijmegen
The Hague
The Hague
Nijmegen
Utrecht
Amsterdam
Both are “correct solutions”.
WARNING!!
Eindhoven
Like geographic data, for example, MDS may be oriented in different ways
35
(describe Ellen Taricani’s 2002 dissertation, handing out teacher maps post-reading is a bad idea)
How good is the MDS representation for
displaying the relationship raw data?
Derived Stimulus Configuration
• Many dimensions (in
this case 19)
reduced to 2
dimensions
• Check the “stress”
value to estimate
how strained the
results are
Euclidean distance model
1,5
spring
garden
Dimension 2
1,0
summer
sunshine
nature
bees
flower
cocoon
0,5
butterfl
0,0
moth
-0,5
yellow
blue
-1,0
fly
insect
color
sky
wing
bird
bug
-1,5
-2
-1
0
Dimension
1
MDS is an algorithmic, power, approach rather than based
on a distribution
model, so no assumptions about data structure are required…
1
36
PFNet of Deese data
sky
summer
blue
spring
sunshine
color
garden
yellow
flower
nature
butterfly
cocoon
moth
wing
bird
bees
fly
insect
bug
37
MDS and PFNet of the exact same data
from Deese
Derived Stimulus Configuration
Euclidean distance model
1,5
sky
summer
spring
blue
color
garden
1,0
garden
sunshine
yellow
flower
butterfly
cocoon
wing
bird
fly
nature
bees
cocoon
nature
0,5
butterfl
0,0
moth
moth
-0,5
bees
yellow
blue
-1,0
fly
insect
color
sky
insect
bug
sunshine
flower
Dimension 2
spring
summer
wing
bird
bug
-1,5
Pathfinder KNOT PFNet
(i.e., local structure, verbatim,
proposition specific)
-2
-1
0
SPSS 1MDS
Dimension
(i.e., global structure,
relational, fuzzy, gist)
1
38
MDS and PFNet of the exact same data
from Deese
Derived Stimulus Configuration
Blue
lines reproduce the PFNet links
Euclidean distance model
1,5
sky
summer
spring
blue
color
garden
1,0
garden
sunshine
yellow
flower
butterfly
cocoon
wing
bird
fly
nature
bees
cocoon
nature
0,5
butterfl
0,0
moth
moth
-0,5
bees
yellow
blue
-1,0
fly
insect
color
sky
insect
bug
sunshine
flower
Dimension 2
spring
summer
wing
bird
bug
-1,5
Pathfinder KNOT PFNet
(i.e., local structure, verbatim,
proposition specific)
-2
-1
0
SPSS 1MDS
Dimension
(i.e., global structure,
relational, fuzzy, gist)
1
39
MDS and PFNet data reduction
• MDS uses all of the raw data to reduce the dimensions
in the representation; if the stress is not too large,
global clustering is likely to be good but local clustering
less so, and the MDS distances between terms within a
tight cluster of terms are more likely to misrepresent
the relatedness raw data.
• Pathfinder uses only the strongest relationship data
(typically 80% of the raw data is discarded). Pathfinder
analysis provides “a fuller representation of the salient
semantic structures than minimal spanning trees, but
also a more accurate representation of local structures
than multidimensional scaling techniques.” (Chen,
1999, p. 408)
40
Dave Jonassen’s summary …
similarity
ratings
quantitative
graph
comparisons
free
recall
written text
additive
trees
relatedness
coefficients
ordered
recall
graph
building
hierarchical
clustering
distance
data
card
sort
Knowledge
elicitation
concept maps
Sabine Klois used …
word
associations
semantic
proximity
C of PFNets
qualitative
graph
comparisons
Knowledge
comparison
Knowledge
representation
Trees
scaling
solutions
expert/
novice
Dimensional
Networks
MDS – multidimensional scaling
ordered
trees
minimum
spanning
trees
link
weighted
Jonassen, Beissner, & Yacci (1993), page 22
Pathfinder
nets
principal
components
cluster
analysis
41
Poindexter and Clariana
• Participants – undergraduate students in an intro
Educational Psychology course (Penn State Erie)
• Setup – complete a demographic survey and how
to make a concept map lesson
• Text based lesson interventions – instructional text
on the “human heart” with either proposition
specific or relational lesson approach
• KS measured as ‘distances’ between terms in a
concept map (a form of card sorting) and also
concept map link data, but analyzed with
Pathfinder KNOT
Poindexter, M. T., & Clariana, R. B. (2006). The influence of relational and proposition-specific processing on structural
knowledge and traditional learning outcomes. International Journal of Instructional Media, 33 (2), 177-184.
42
Treatments
• Relational condition, participants were required to
“unscramble” sentences (following Einstein, McDaniel,
Bowers, & Stevens, 1984) in one paragraph in each of
the five sections or about 20% of the total text content
• Proposition-specific condition (following Hamilton,
1985), participants answered three or four adjunct
constructed response questions (taken nearly verbatim
from the text) provided at the end of each of the five
sections, for a total of 17 questions covering about 20%
of the total text content (no feedback was provided).
Poindexter, M. T., & Clariana, R. B. (2006). The influence of relational and proposition-specific processing on structural
knowledge and traditional learning outcomes. International Journal of Instructional Media, 33 (2), 177-184.
43
DK and KS Posttests
• DK - Declarative Knowledge (Dwyer, 1976)
– Identification drawing test (20)
– Terminology multiple-choice items (20), declarative
knowledge, e.g., the lesson text states A  B, the
posttest asks A  ?(B, x, y, z) (explicitly stated)
– Comprehension multiple-choice items (20), inference
required, e.g., given A  B and B  C in the lesson
text, posttest asks A  ?(C, x, y, z) (implicit, not
stated)
• KS - Knowledge structure
– Concept map link-based common scores
– Concept map distance-based common scores
Poindexter, M. T., & Clariana, R. B. (2006). The influence of relational and proposition-specific processing on structural
knowledge and traditional learning outcomes. International Journal of Instructional Media, 33 (2), 177-184.
44
Note that declarative knowledge multiple-choice
posttest items are sensitive to the linear order of the
lesson text
If the lesson text is A  B, paraphrasing the stem (A’)
and/or transposing stem and response (B  A) to
create posttest questions influences performance.
posttest
When MC posttest is:
• Identical to lesson (A  B):
77%
• Transposed from lesson (B  A): 71%
• Paraphrased from lesson (A’  B): 69%
• Both T & P from lesson (B  A’): 67%
Bormuth, J. R., Manning, J., Carr, J., & Pearson, D. (1970). Children’s comprehension of between and within sentence
syntactic structure. Journal of Educational Psychology, 61, 349–357.
Clariana, R.B. & Koul, R. (2006). The effects of different forms of feedback on fuzzy and verbatim memory of science
principles. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76 (2), 259-270.
45
Recording link and distance data in a
concept map
(n2-n)/2 pair-wise comparisons
Distance Array
left atrium
right ventricle
to the
pulmonary vein
moves through
pulmonary artery
passes into
a
b
c
d
e
f
g
left atrium
lungs
oxygenate
pulmonary artery
pulmonary vein
deoxgenate
right ventricle
deoxygenated
oxygenated
Student’s concept map
b
36
84
102
42
102
c
d
e
f
120 114 138 54 84 144 138 42 114 120
g
-
Link Array
to the
lungs
a
120
150
108
73
156
66
a
b
c
d
e
f
g
left atrium
lungs
oxygenate
pulmonary artery
pulmonary vein
deoxgenate
right ventricle
a
0
0
0
1
0
0
b
c
d
e
f
g
1
1
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
0
0
0
-
46
Distance raw data reduction by
Pathfinder KNOT
(21 distance data points reduced to 6 link data points)
Distance Array
left atrium
right ventricle
pulmonary vein
pulmonary artery
a
b
c
d
e
f
g
left atrium
lungs
oxygenate
pulmonary artery
pulmonary vein
deoxgenate
right ventricle
a
120
150
108
73
156
66
b
36
84
102
42
102
c
d
e
f
120 114 138 54 84 144 138 42 114 120
g
-
Pathfinder Network
lungs
deoxygenated
oxygenated
Pathfinder network
(based on distances)
a
b
c
d
e
f
g
left atrium
lungs
oxygenate
pulmonary artery
pulmonary vein
deoxgenate
right ventricle
a
0
0
0
0
0
1
b
c
d
e
f
g
1
1
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
0
0
0
-
47
Example of link and distance PFNets
for the same concept map
left atrium
right ventricle
left atrium
to the
right ventricle
pulmonary vein
moves through
pulmonary artery
passes into
pulmonary vein
pulmonary artery
to the
lungs
deoxygenated
oxygenated
Student’s concept map
(i.e., link data)
lungs
deoxygenated
oxygenated
Pathfinder network
(from distance data)
48
Means and sd
Treatments
Posttests
Map-link
COMP
Map-prop
7.3
14.1
(5.4)
(4.6)
control
ID
15.1
(4.4)
TERM
12.3
(4.6)
Map-dist
Map-assoc
9.0
(3.6)
propositionspecific
16.3
(5.6)
14.6
(5.7)
13.8
(3.7)
16.5
(8.3)
11.5
(3.4)
relational
17.0
(2.6)
12.7
(3.5)
12.4
(3.0)
13.9
(9.4)
10.7
(4.6)
Poindexter, M. T., & Clariana, R. B. (2006). The influence of relational and proposition-specific processing on structural
knowledge and traditional learning outcomes. International Journal of Instructional Media, 33 (2), 177-184.
49
Analysis
• MANOVA (relational, proposition-specific, and control)
and five dependent variables including ID, TERM,
COMP, Map-prop, and Map-assoc.
• COMP was significance, F = 5.25, MSe = 17.836, p =
0.015, none of the other dependent variables were
significance.
• Follow-up Scheffé tests revealed that the propositionspecific group’s COMP mean was significantly greater
than the control group’s COMP mean (see previous
Table).
Poindexter, M. T., & Clariana, R. B. (2006). The influence of relational and proposition-specific processing on structural
knowledge and traditional learning outcomes. International Journal of Instructional Media, 33 (2), 177-184.
50
Correlations
ID (drawing)
TERM (MC)
COMP (MC)
Map-prop
Map-link
Map-distance
Map-assoc
ID
-0.71
0.50
0.56
0.45
Verbatim
AB
Inference
A  C
TERM
COMP
Map-link
Prop
-0.74
0.77
0.69
-0.53
0.71
-0.73
All sig. at p<.05
Compare to Taricani
& Clariana
next 
Poindexter, M. T., & Clariana, R. B. (2006). The influence of relational and proposition-specific processing on structural
knowledge and traditional learning outcomes. International Journal of Instructional Media, 33 (2), 177-184.
51
Compare the correlation results to a
related follow-up investigation
Poindexter & Clariana (2006)Term
Comp
Link data
0.77
0.53
Distance data
0.69
0.71
Taricani & Clariana (2006)
Term
Comp
Link data
0.78
0.54
Distance data
0.48
0.61
Taricani, E. M. & Clariana, R. B. (2006). A technique for automatically scoring open-ended concept maps.
Educational Technology Research and Development, 53 (4), 61-78.
52
Clariana and Marker (2007)
• Participants – 68 graduate students in INSYS intro
ISD course
• Computer-based lesson – text, graphics, and
questions on instructional design, either asked to
generate headings for each section or not
• Seven sections referred to as A through G, each
cover a component of the Dick and Carey model
• KS as a sorting task and a new listwise task
Clariana, R.B., & Marker, A. (2007). Generating topic headings during reading of screen-based text
facilitates learning of structural knowledge and impairs learning of lower-level knowledge. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 37 (2), 173-191. link
53
Posttests
• Declarative Knowledge – 30-item constructed
response terminology test, 15 items from lesson
sections B, D, and F (called “used”) and 15 items
from A, C, E, and G (called “not used”)

• Knowledge structure – Posttest focuses on 15
terms used in sections B, D, and F
– Listwise rating task agreement scores (compared to
linear and cluster referent)
– Sorting task agreement scores (compared to linear
and cluster referent)
List and sorting used by Sabine Klois, note: sorting not the same as card sorting
Clariana, R.B., & Marker, A. (2007). Generating topic headings during reading of screen-based text
facilitates learning of structural knowledge and impairs learning of lower-level knowledge. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 37 (2), 173-191. link
54
Listwise rating task …
(available at: www.personal.psu.edu/rbc4)
Clariana, R.B., & Marker, A. (2007). Generating topic headings during reading of screen-based text
facilitates learning of structural knowledge and impairs learning of lower-level knowledge. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 37 (2), 173-191. link
55
Sorting task …
Drag related terms closer together and unrelated terms further apart.
When done, click CONTINUE
CONTINUE
Preinstructional activities
Instructional strategy
Performance context
Learner analysis Goal analysis
Target population
Entry behaviors
Delivery system
Feedback
Job aid
Transfer
Intellectual skill
Concept
Verbal information
Psychomotor skill
Clariana, R.B., & Marker, A. (2007). Generating topic headings during reading of screen-based text
facilitates learning of structural knowledge and impairs learning of lower-level knowledge. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 37 (2), 173-191. link
56
An example student PFNet
Psychomotor
Skill B5
Concept
B3
Intellectual
Skill B4
Performance
Context
D4
Target
Population
D1
Learner
Analysis
D2
Verbal
Information
B2
Entry
Behaviors
.
D3
Goal
Analysis
B1
Instructional
Strategy F4
Show how to count linear and
nonlinear here …
Pre-instructional
activities F1
Delivery
system F2
Job aid
Clariana, R.B., & Marker, A. (2007). Generating topic headings during reading of screen-based text F3
facilitates learning of structural knowledge and impairs learning of lower-level knowledge. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 37 (2), 173-191. link
Feedback
F5
Transfer
D5
57
Means and standard deviations
Clariana, R.B., & Marker, A. (2007). Generating topic headings during reading of screen-based text
facilitates learning of structural knowledge and impairs learning of lower-level knowledge. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 37 (2), 173-191. link
58
Analysis
• The cued recall and sorting task posttest data were
analyzed by a 2 (Treatment: Headings vs. No Headings)
× 2 (Posttest: cued recall and sorting task) mixed
ANOVA. The first is a between-subjects factor and the
second is the within subjects factor.
• The Treatment main effect was not significant, F(1, 61)
= 0.220, MSE = 0.045, p = .94. The Posttest repeated
measure was significant, F(1, 61) = 18.874, MSE =
0.022, p < .001, showing that the mean cued recall test
score (M = 0.59) was greater than the mean sorting
task score (M = 0.47). Finally, the anticipated disordinal
interaction of Treatment and Posttest factors was
significant, F(1, 61) = 5.119, MSE = 0.022, p = .027
Clariana, R.B., & Marker, A. (2007). Generating topic headings during reading of screen-based text
facilitates learning of structural knowledge and impairs learning of lower-level knowledge. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 37 (2), 173-191. link
59
Generate headings when reading:
better ‘structure’ but worse ‘recall’
Declarative knowledge
Knowledge structure (KS)
Clariana, R.B., & Marker, A. (2007). Generating topic headings during reading of screen-based text
facilitates learning of structural knowledge and impairs learning of lower-level knowledge. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 37 (2), 173-191. link
60
Comparison of listwise and sorting KS
Listwise task
(more linear)
i.e., A1  A2
Sorting task
(more relational)
i.e., A1  A3 or A4 or A5
4.1
3.9
3.7
3.5
3.3
3.1
2.9
no Head
Headings
2.7
2.5
Linear
Non-linear
Linear
Non-linear
Clariana, R.B., & Marker, A. (2007). Generating topic headings during reading of screen-based text
facilitates learning of structural knowledge and impairs learning of lower-level knowledge. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 37 (2), 173-191. link
61 of 54
Correlations of interest
Table 1. The No Headers and Headers treatment group correlations (from Clariana & Marker,
2007).
A
B
C
D
E
No Header Treatment Group (N = 32)
A. CR Posttest (15 max.)
1
B. Sorting task (linear)
0.24
1
C. Sorting task (non linear)
-0.02
-0.37 *
1
D. Listwise task (linear)
0.62**
0.30
-0.21
1
E. Listwise task (non linear)
0.08
0.04
0.20
0.00
1
Header Treatment Group (N = 31)
A. CR Posttest (15 max.)
1
B. Sorting task (linear)
0.22
1
C. Sorting task (non linear)
0.49**
0.09
1
D. Listwise task (linear)
0.44*
0.36 *
0.39 *
1
E. Listwise task (non linear)
0.37*
0.30
0.30
0.04
1
p<.05; ** p<.01
Clariana, R.B., & Marker, A. (2007). Generating topic headings during reading of screen-based text
facilitates learning of structural knowledge and impairs learning of lower-level knowledge. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 37 (2), 173-191. link
62
Brain scans – in proficient readers, text with no headings
requires right hemisphere activity to achieve coherence (more
work), some students will not be able to form coherence
“Consistent with previous studies…the right middle temporal regions may be especially
important for integrative processes needed to achieve global coherence during discourse
processing.” (p.1317 St. George, Kutas, Martinez, & Sereno, 1999)
headings
RH
no headings
LH
RH
http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/reprint/122/7/1317
LH
63
Review - Generate headings when reading:
better ‘structure’ but worse ‘recall’
Declarative knowledge
Knowledge structure (KS)
Clariana, R.B., & Marker, A. (2007). Generating topic headings during reading of screen-based text
facilitates learning of structural knowledge and impairs learning of lower-level knowledge. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 37 (2), 173-191. link
64
Comments
• The better structured knowledge of the Headings group
(i.e., more like the author’s text schema) should allow the
learners to more flexibly use that knowledge (Jonassen &
Wang, 1993) which should influence the reader’s ability to
form inferences and comprehend the lesson text, but this
apparently comes at the expense of text details.
• These results are consistent with and help explain previous
investigations that have reported that learners who
generate headings score lower than no-headings control
groups on lower-order outcomes but score higher on
inference and comprehension tests (Dee-Lucas & DiVesta,
1980; Jonassen et al., 1985; Wittrock & Kelly, 1984). (These
papers are listed on the next screen)
65
Generative learning (relational lesson
tasks) DK KS reversal reference list
• Dee-Lucas, D. & DiVesta, F. F. (1980). Learner-generated
organizational aids: Effects on learning from text.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 72(3), 304-311.
• Jonassen, D. H., Hartley, J., & Trueman, M. (1985,
April). The effects of learner generated versus
experimenter-provided headings on immediate and
delayed recall and comprehension. Chicago: American
Educational Research Association (ERIC ED 254 567).
• Wittrock, M. C., & Kelly, R. (1984). Teaching reading
comprehension to adults in basic skills courses. Final
Report, Project No. MDA 903-82-C-0169). University of
California, Los Angeles.
66
MDS explanation:
Read with terms A I
Connectivity Matrix (Kintsch, 1998)
words
a
b
c
d
e
f
g
h
I
a
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
b
0
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
0
c
0
0
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
d
0
0
0
1
1
0
0
0
0
e
0
0
0
0
1
1
0
0
0
Link Array
(no color)
f
0
0
0
0
0
1
1
0
0
g
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
1
0
h
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
1
I
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
E
D
F
C
I
G
B
H
A
MDS
67
Same reading with terms A I,
but with section headings
Headings (i.e., color names)
red
blue
green
red
words
a
b
c
d
e
f
g
h
I
blue
red
green
a
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
0
0
b
0
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
0
0
c
0
0
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
1
0
0
d
0
0
0
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
1
0
e
0
0
0
0
1
1
0
0
0
0
1
0
Link Array
(with headings)
f
0
0
0
0
0
1
1
0
0
0
1
0
g
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
1
0
0
0
1
h
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
1
0
0
1
I
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
0
0
1
E
F
D
C
B
A
G
I
H
green
MDS
68
blue
MDS of connectivity matrices
No color names MDS
Color names MDS
red
E
E
D
F
F
D
tighter
clusters
C
C
I
B
A
G
B
H
A
G
I
H
green
?…. Context (like topic headings) may alter memory structure
in a regular way, and we can think about it visually.
69
blue
Explanation using Lawrence Frase’s matrix
multiplication to explain inference
context
Read A  B and B  C, model of the effects of context (as headings) on
verbatim and inference activation
A
B
C
context
A B C
0.9 0.3 0 0.3
0 0.9 0.3 0.3
0 0 0.9 0.3
0.3 0.3 0.3 1
no context
A 0.8 0.5 0.1
B 0 0.8 0.5
C 0 0 0.8
w context
A
B
C
context
A
0.9
0.1
0.1
0.6
B
0.6
0.9
0.1
0.7
C
0.2
0.6
0.9
0.7
0.7
0.7
0.6
1.3
control panel
try
context 1 name = context
column -> receives
0.3
reading A->B prop association strength = 0.3
0.4
context association strength = 0.3
0.9
term A association strength = 0.9
0.9
term B association strength = 0.9
0.9
term C association strength = 0.9
mmult
no context A B C
output
A 1 0.7 0.1
(no context) - (context)
B 0 1 0.7
verbatim A->B = -0.033 - means context better
C 0 0 1
inference A->C = -0.089 - means context better
w context A B C
A 1 0.7 0.2
B 0.1 1 0.7
C 0.1 0.1 1
row -> sends
(also notice B-A, C-A, and B-C activations)
Frase, L.T. (1969). Structural analysis of the knowledge that results from thinking about text. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 60 (6, monograph, part 2), 1-16.
70
Clariana and Prestera (2009)
• Background color as a weak context variable
• Participants – 80 graduate students in INSYS intro
instructional design course
• Computer-based lesson – text, graphics, and
questions with feedback on ISD, presented in 5
sections, each section covered a component of
the Dick and Carey model (items with feedback
should present STRONG AB effects)
• Intervention – lesson presented either with or
without a color band on the left margin (this use
of color should have WEAK relational effects)
Clariana, R.B., & Prestera, G.E. (2009). The effects of lesson screen background color on declarative and structural knowledge. Journal
71 ofof54
Educational Computing Research, 40 (3), 281 -293. link
Example lesson screen
Color
or
No color
Clariana, R.B., & Prestera, G.E. (2009). The effects of lesson screen background color on declarative and structural knowledge. Journal of72
Educational Computing Research, 40 (3), 281 -293. link
Posttests
• Declarative Knowledge vocabulary posttest – 18
constructed response items (fill in the blank) and 18
multiple choice items terminology test (strong AB)
• Knowledge structure posttest – sort the 36

vocabulary terms (same sorting task as Clariana &
Marker (2006) above)
• Results: The anticipated disordinal interaction of
Subtest and Lesson Color was significant, F(1, 71) =
5.008, MSe = 0.618, p = .028, with lesson color
enhancing structural knowledge scores and inhibiting
declarative knowledge scores.
Clariana, R.B., & Prestera, G.E. (2009). The effects of lesson screen background color on declarative and structural knowledge. Journal
73 ofof54
Educational Computing Research, 40 (3), 281 -293. link
Lesson and posttest means
Clariana, R.B., & Prestera, G.E. (2009). The effects of lesson screen background color on declarative and structural knowledge. Journal of74
Educational Computing Research, 40 (3), 281 -293. link
Another disordinal interaction of
declarative and structural knowledge
Clariana, R.B., & Prestera, G.E. (2009). The effects of lesson screen background color on declarative and structural knowledge. Journal of75
Educational Computing Research, 40 (3), 281 -293. link
Section summary
• Different measurement approaches are
better for prompting memory for linear or
cluster KS
• Linear lesson tasks establish linear KS and
relational (generative) lesson tasks establish
relational KS
• Models can account for verbatim and
inference outcomes
• Next section - Alternative measures of KS
76
For KS, more terms may be better
0.70
Predictive Validity
• Goldsmith et al. (1991) the
relationship between the
number of terms included
in Pathfinder network
analysis (elicited as pairwise) and the predictive
ability of the resulting
PFNets to predict end-ofcourse grades.
• But only if these are really
IMPORTANT terms
(Clariana & Taricani, 2010)
0.60
0.50
0.40
0.30
0.20
0.10
0.00
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
Number of terms
Goldsmith, T.E., Johnson, P.J., & Acton, W.H. (1991). Assessing structural knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83
(1), 88-96.
Clariana, R.B., & Taricani, E. M. (2010). The consequences of increasing the number of sterms used to score open-ended
concept maps. International Journal of Instructional Media, 37 (2), 163-173. link
77
Raw data reduction
by Pathfinder KNOT
500
450
Raw data (half array, (n2-n)/2 )
KNOT tries to form a
path with n-1 links
Terms = 20
Raw data = 190
PFNet = 19
PFNet as % of raw data = 10%
400
350
Terms = 10
Raw data = 45
PFNet = 9
PFNet as % of raw data = 20%
300
250
200
Terms = 30
Raw data = 435
PFNet = 29
PFNet as % of raw data = 7%
150
100
50
0
0
5
10
Methods that elicit pairwise association
fatigue with more then 20 to 30 terms)
15
20
Number of terms (n)
25
30
35
78
KS measurement
• More terms are better but the problem with eliciting KS
using pairwise comparisons (more than 20!)
• So, we need a valid and efficient measure of KS … recall
from above that:
• Recall that Ferstl & Kintsch (1999) used a more efficient
cued-recall list approach (3 recalls for each term)
• Clariana & Marker (2007) added a ‘listwise’ approach, with
one recognition retrieval for each term and a ‘sorting’
approach (dragging all terms around on the screen at the
same time)
• Do ‘listwise’ and ‘sorting’ results compare with the more
traditional and accepted ‘pairwise’ approach? If yes, then
these two can handle large lists of terms.
79
Clariana and Wallace (2009)
• Compared pairwise, listwise, and sorting
• Participants – 84 undergraduate students in
business
• All students completed 3 computer-delivered KS
measures – listwise, pairwise, and sorting
(randomized) using the 15 major concepts of the
course
• Students grouped for analysis into high and low
groups based on a media split of their end-ofcourse multiple-choice exam
Clariana, R.B., & Wallace, P. E. (2009). A comparison of pairwise, listwise, and clustering approaches for eliciting
structural knowledge in information systems courses. International Journal of Instructional Media, 36, 139–143.
80 of 54
The three approaches
 pairwise
listwise 
sorting

Drag related terms closer together and unrelated terms farther apart.
When done. click CONTINUE.
Continue
computer literacy
internet
networks
WWW
applications
communications
ergonomics
input
output
system unit
CPU
operating system
ethics
privacy
Clariana, R.B., & Wallace, P. E. (2009). A comparison of pairwise, listwise, and clustering approaches for eliciting
structural knowledge in information systems courses. International Journal of Instructional Media, 36, 139–143.
81
Sorting and listing are faster than
pairwise
• Time to complete the tasks:
– pair-wise approach, X = 447.4 s (sd = 140.6)
– list-wise approach, X = 193.3 s (sd = 79.6)
– Sorting approach, X = 115.5 s (sd = 62.7)
• Concurrent / convergent validity: Do the
3 elicitation tasks obtain similar raw data
and PFNet data?
Clariana, R.B., & Wallace, P. E. (2009). A comparison of pairwise, listwise, and clustering approaches for eliciting
structural knowledge in information systems courses. International Journal of Instructional Media, 36, 139–143.
82
Individuals’ raw data arrays
were not similar (correlations)
Table 3. Relatedness correlations of individual and group average raw proximity data.
Group
Low (n = 41)
PxL
0.31
(.09)
Individuals
PxS
-0.21
(.15)
LxS
-0.30
(.14)
Group Average
PxL
PxS
LxS
0.68
-0.63
-0.79
na
na
na
High (n = 43)
0.31
(.16)
-0.25
(.19)
-0.29
(.13)
0.68
na
-0.67
na
-0.78
na
P – pair-wise, L – list-wise, S – sorting
Therefore, the 3 approaches do not elicit the same raw data
associations, individuals’ raw data seems to be idiosyncratic or
flaky or noisy; however the group average raw data are much
more alike (averaging within a group ‘smooths out’ idiosyncrasy)
Clariana, R.B., & Wallace, P. E. (2009). A comparison of pairwise, listwise, and clustering approaches for eliciting
structural knowledge in information systems courses. International Journal of Instructional Media, 36, 139–143.
83
% overlap based on ‘group average’
PFNet common scores (intersection)
pairwise
PL
PH
listwise
LL
LH
sorting
SL
SH
Pairwise low (PL)
-Pairwise high (PH) 64%
--
Listwise low (LL) 79%
Listwise high (LH) 71%
57%
79%
79%
--
Sort low (SL)
Sort high (SH)
57%
43%
43%
43%
71%
57%
64%
57%
64%
--
linear (lin)
non linear (nonlin)
50%
9%
29%
9%
36%
9%
36%
9%
29%
9%
29%
9%
referent
lin
nonlin
--
--
Clariana, R.B., & Wallace, P. E. (2009). A comparison of pairwise, listwise, and clustering approaches for eliciting
structural knowledge in information systems courses. International Journal of Instructional Media, 36, 139–143.
--
0%
--
84
Sabine’s experts
Expert_A
Expert_B
Expert_C
Expert_D
Expert_ave
one
0.46
0.27
0.43
0.71
one
0.44 one
0.57 0.42 one
0.56 0.52 0.54 one
each avg. = 0.47 0.51 0.41 0.49 0.58
All avg. = 0.49
one
0.60
0.67
0.53
0.79
one
0.73 one
0.53 0.53 one
0.79 0.79 0.58 one
one
0.43
0.29
0.43
0.43
Expert_ave
Expert_D
Expert_C
Expert_B
Expert_A
Expert_ave
Expert_D
Sorting
Expert_C
Expert_B
Expert_A
Expert_ave
Expert_D
Listwise
Expert_C
Expert_B
% overlap
Expert_A
Pairwise
one
0.43 one
0.21 0.36 one
0.57 0.64 0.36 one
0.65 0.66 0.68 0.54 0.74
0.39 0.41 0.43 0.34 0.50
0.65
0.41
85
Next directions for KS research?
• Continue to find valid and efficient KS
approaches
• And close with a few provocative
comments …
86
1st year undergraduate textbook in IST
an obvious ‘collage’
87
Web reading F-pattern?
Heatmaps from user eyetracking studies of three websites. The areas where users
looked the most are colored red; the yellow areas indicate fewer views, followed by the
least-viewed blue areas. Gray areas didn't attract any fixations.
http://www.useit.com/alertbox/reading_pattern.html
88
Gaze plot of the 4 main classes of web
search reading behaviornavigation-dominant
search-dominant
tool-dominant
http://www.useit.com/alertbox/fancy-formatting.html
successful
89
Sources of eye-tracking
• http://www.miratech.com/blog/eye-trackinglecture-web.html
• http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=X60VPJDL
AeM&feature=player_embedded
90
Altered reading due to web
experience?
• If students are not reading linearly, or are using (or
not using) headings and other text signals (color,
underline, highlights) differently, then the KS will be
different
• Specific KS can accomplish specific kinds of mental
‘work’ and other KS other work (the protein analogy)
• So determining how today’s students read hypertext
and web materials, and whether this transfer back to
paper-based text is an important question
• KS is one tool that can complement existing
measures and help explain this
91
Term activation across sentences
7
6
1
2
5
3
Axis Title
0.92
0.92
0.92
0.92
0.92
0.92
0.92
0.92
0.92
0.92
0.92
0.92
0.92
0.92
terms
knight
rode
forest
country
dragon
princess
kidnap
free (freed)
marry (married)
hurried
fought
death (killed)
armor
thankful
4
4
5
3
6
7
2
13
1
10
7
0
4
8
9
10
11
12
1
13
92
Download