Empathy and Neuropolitics: This is your brain on neoliberal culture

advertisement
Empathy and Neuropolitics: This is your brain on neoliberal culture. Any questions?
Gary Olson
Abstract
Mirror neurons, the brain cells believed to be the basis for empathy, have recently been
identified in the human brain. And yet we‟re left to explain the disjuncture between this deepseated, pre-reflective, moral intuition and the paucity of actual empathic behavior, especially in
certain cultures. I suggest that answers may be found in the bidirectional connection between
culture and brain development.
The political theorist William Connally has defined neuropolitics as “. . . the politics
through which cultural life mixes into the composition of the body/brain process. And vice
versa.” In this context, I hypothesize that the neo-liberal ideology justifying free market
capitalism is one of the most potent empathy “bracketing off” elements of that culture and hybrid
cultural/neurobiological imprinting can override the neurobiological traits that should bring
people together. The dominant culture‟s social engineering undermines and attenuates both the
acceptance and institutionalization of empathy on a grand scale, while channeling its expression
toward system maintenance behaviors.
There are outstanding exceptions, but too many cultural psychologists and other
subspecialists have followed too many anthropologists in failing to unpack the meaning of
culture itself. Following Gramsci, I argue that power and class realities have not received
sufficient attention in explaining what I‟ve described as a societal-wide cultural deficit disorder.
This pathological condition has structural roots in the socio-economic system which influence
1
the brain‟s mirror neuron network. Cross-cultural studies offer a promising avenue for aiding
our understanding of this process.
Introduction
“Mirror neurons,” the brain cells many neuroscientists believe are the basis for empathy,
were discovered in macaque monkeys in 1996 (Gallese, 1996; Iacoboni, 2008, 2009; Rizzolatti
and Sinigaglia, 2008). Located in area F5 of the premotor cortex, these neurons fired not only
when the monkey performed an action but also when it was watching the same action. The
monkey‟s neurons were “mirroring” the activity she was observing.
Later, the existence of mirror neurons in the human brain was strongly inferred by
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) but proof remained elusive. Now, for the first
time, we have direct recorded evidence for their presence. Roy Mukamel and colleagues
(Mukamel et al., 2010) recorded their data from the medial frontal and temporal cortices in 21
patients (with their consent) awaiting surgery for intractable epileptic seizures at UCLA‟s
Medical Center. The researchers “piggybacked” onto intracranial depth electrodes implanted
into the patient‟s brains as part of a search for a potential surgical treatment. The research team
recorded activity in 1,177 neurons in the 21 patients and concluded that “these findings suggest
the existence of multiple systems in the brain endowed with neural mirroring mechanisms for
flexible integration and differentiation of the perceptual and motor aspects of actions performed
by self and others.”
_______________
Gary Olson, Department of Political Science, Moravian College, Bethlehem, Pennsylvania.
Email: olson@moravian.edu
2
The mirror neurons in the same affective brain circuits are automatically mobilized upon
feeling one‟s own pain and the pain of others and this neural circuitry is the basis of empathic
behavior in which actions in response to the distress of others is virtually instantaneous.
Valayanur Ramachandran, director of the Center for Brain and Cognition at the University of
California at San Diego (UCSD) observes that “We used to say, metaphorically, that „I can feel
another‟s pain,‟ but now we know that my mirror neurons can literally feel your pain” (Slack,
2007). Ramachandran, who terms them “empathy neurons” or “Dalai Lama neurons,” writes
that “In essence the neuron is part of a network that allows you to see the world from the other
person‟s point of view, hence the name „mirror neuron‟” (Ramachandran, 2006). Where
comparable experience is lacking, “cognitive empathy” allows one to “actively project oneself
into the shoes of another person” by trying to imagine the other person‟s situation (Preston,
2002; Preston et al., 2007; Singer & Lamm, 2009). This “ability to perceive, appreciate, and
respond to the affective states of another” emerges as early as two years of age as the child
becomes aware of another‟s emotional experience (Decety and Michalska, 2009; Decety, 2008;
Decety et. al., 2008; Tomasello, 2009)).
The roots of prosocial behavior, including moral sentiments like empathy, precede the
evolution of culture and serve a critical evolutionary function. Mirroring was selected by
evolution because of its adaptive advantage in making some intersubjectivity possible, the
effortless and automatic access to other minds.
We now have a wealth of evidence suggesting that empathy, the foundation for morality,
was not handed down from on high via social codes from religious authorities and philosophers
but constructed from the “bottom-up” (Green in Vedantum, 2007; de Waal, 2008, 2009;
3
Tomasello, 2009; Tangney, et al., 2007; and Iacoboni, 2009). And if morality is based in
biology, in the raw material for the evolution of its expression, the case can be made for a
fortuitous marriage of hard science and secular morality. I should quickly acknowledge that
mirror neuron research is not without its skeptics (Dinstein et al., 2008; Lippard, 2009; Virona,
2009, and Hickok, 2009) and the technical details supporting my assertions largely lie outside
this paper. But progress is proceeding at an exponential pace, new discoveries are persuasive,
and our understanding of empathy has increased dramatically in barely a decade (Gallese, Eagle
and Migone, 2007; Gallese, 2008; Iacoboni, 2008, 2009; Decety and Lamm, 2009). What
follows is some theoretical speculation that places empathy within the entwined context of neural
activity, culture and political economy.
Your Brain on Culture
I‟ve been pondering the nature of empathy for over two decades, initially as a
pedagogical challenge and later, given advances in neuroscience as a broader field of inquiry
(Olson, 1987, 2008). And for me, one of the most vexing questions that remains to be explained,
and the burden of this paper is to ask why, if “. . . we are not alone, but are biologically wired
and evolutionarily designed to be deeply interconnected with one another” (Iacoboni, 2008, p.
266), so little progress has been made in extending this empathic orientation toward distant lives,
to those outside certain in-group moral circles? Given a world rife with overt and structural
violence, one is forced to explain why our deep-seated moral intuition doesn‟t produce a more
ameliorating effect, a more peaceful world?
Echoing Dominguez (2006), I‟m proposing that reality is a social construction and
therefore “We should find that the brain would have some sort of bias acquired through exposure
to culture.” Neuroscientist Marco Iacoboni (2007, 2008, 2009), arguably the world‟s preeminent
4
authority on mirror neurons, suggests this disjuncture can be explained in part by massive belief
systems, including political and religious ones. I hypothesize below that the neo-liberal ideology
justifying global free market capitalism is one of the most powerful of these empathy-shaping
belief systems, especially as manifested in cultures like the United States. Over time, the culture
filters and influences how empathy evolves and is expressed (de Waal, 2007, p. 50). These
belief systems can override the automatic, neurobiological traits that should bring people
together, leaving selective moral amnesia in their wake.
Some twenty-five years ago, Lewontin, Rose and Kamin (1984) foregrounded a bidirectional link between culture and biology when they wrote, “humanity cannot be cut adrift
from its own biology, but neither is it enchained by it.” Prophetically, they foretold that “our
task . . . is to point the way toward an integrated understanding of the relationship between the
biological and the social” (cited by Wexler, 2006, p. 13; Tooby and Cosmides, 1992). It follows
that our approach must eschew privileging either brain or culture. In the first instance, Slaby
(2010) warns of the dangers inherent in neuronal reductionism, a sort of “brainhood ideology”
(Vidal, 2009) that essentializes the cerebral subject, while an exclusive focus on the social steers
us into the cul-de-sac of hyper-cultural reductionism. Cromby (2007) wisely points toward
“hybridity,” an appreciation of the intertwining of “mind-body-world” which mandates an
interdisciplinary inquiry.
Pioneers in the new fields of neuroanthropology (Downey and Lende, 2009; Dominguez
et al., 2009) and cultural neuroscience (Chiao, 2009; Chiao et al., 2009; Han & Northoff, 2008)
demonstrate in their recent work how a careful and critical synthesis of findings and approaches
can further our understanding of this complex subject.
5
Bearing this in mind, it‟s no longer debatable that culture has a measurable influence on
the brain. Work by Chiao and colleagues (2008) at Northwestern University and in Japan points
toward specific cultural priming (beliefs, values and practices) that modulates neural activity
within the anterior rostral portion of the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) and posterior
cingulated cortex (PCC). Initial findings, including some on empathy, are intriguing and at the
apex of neuroscience research. Recent studies using fMRI and magneto-ecephalography (MEG)
have established that cultural constructs shape the microstructure of the brain and this culturing
of the neural signature begins in early childhood and continues into adolescence and early
adulthood (Choudhury, 2009; Choudhury and Kirmayer, 2009; Turner and Whitehead, 2008).
This is complemented by a recent review of culture-in-the-brain studies (Dominguez et
al., 2009) from the aforementioned fledgling discipline of neuroanthropology which substantiates
that cultural experience influences virtually all critical brain areas; shapes and determines neural
patterns; affects brain structure; and modulates cognitive function. At least until early adulthood
(Wexler, 2006) our brains are shaping themselves in response to significant and repetitive
sensory stimulation from the surrounding environment. Thereafter the brain and mind seek to
create congruence between external realities and these newly existing internal structures and
there is more resistance to change. I‟m mindful not to caricature Hebb‟s rule (1949) that “The
neurons that fire together wire together” but his prescient emphasis on the roles of repetition and
synoptic plasticity draw our attention to the critical role of culture‟s neurobiological imprinting.
The Cultural Regulation of Emotion
We can now begin to consider the mechanisms at the structural level of deep
enculturation or societal engagement that mediate changes in our plastic brain. Transcultural
neuro-imaging offers a promising avenue for aiding our understanding of how specific cultural
6
spaces are navigated (Malafouris, 2010) and cultural neuroscience reveals substantial variation
across cultures in terms of how individuals perceive social situations, understand themselves (as
selves) and others. The differences attributable to cultural mediation are significant (Chiao, et
al., 2010; 2009; Chiao, et al., 2008), Choudhury and Kirmayer, 2009; Molnar-Szakacs, et al.,
2007a, 2007b and Lieberman, 2007). For example, imaging studies (Hedden, et al., 2008; Han
and Northoff, 2008) show that East Asian and Westerners engage in different visual processing
activities and their cultural experience “sculps the perceptual brain.” It isn‟t that people from
different cultures perceive the world differently, “. . . but they think differently about what they
see” (Gabrielli, 2008). These differences also include variations in terms of encouraging and
sanctioning emotion—expressive behavior on the one hand and suppressing and otherwise
inhibiting that response on the other. Studies (Gazzaniga, 2005) suggest that when a person is
unwilling to act on a moral belief, the emotional part of her or his brain has not been activated.
As Butler (2007) and colleagues note, these habitual practices reflect dominant cultural values.
This is particularly germane for this discussion because human beings live in specific
cultural environments, settings neither of their own choosing nor, in C. Geertz‟s words,
“independent of time, place, and circumstances.” The fact that empathy is a universal hardwired response “in no way negates the cultural constitution of emotion” (Mesquita & Leu, 2007).
Put another way, the encultured brain moderates an individual‟s regulation of emotion, including
the very knowledge structures that are drawn upon in automatically reacting to various emotionevoking situations (Kitayama et al., 2004; Mauss et al., 2008; Sherman et al., 2009). A recent
collection of articles on emotion regulation adopts this definition:
7
The process by which individuals influence which emotions they have, when they
have them, and how they experience them and express these emotions (Gross,
1998, p. 275).
The editors are quick to acknowledge that emotion regulation is a bio-cultural process. How
emotions are expressed depends on socio-cultural context, on the requirements and demands
within a specific environment. Further, they argue that emotions are “already regulated prior to
their actual elicitation . . .” under conditions of “automatic emotion regulation” (p. 4).
Hochschild‟s (1979, 1983, 2003a, 2003b) path-breaking social theory of emotion asks us
to consider the estrangement, specifically the emotional costs to the self, inherent in the
management of emotions like empathy. For example, one cultural response under free-market
capitalism‟s ideology is to privatize “our idea of care” (2003b, p. 216). Elites shape the cultural
image of care/empathy toward minimizing the emotional needs of others and this closely
corresponds to the idea of American rugged individualism. Emotions are “impaired when the
private management of feeling is socially engineered and transformed into emotional labor for a
wage (2003, p. x). Of course, as Hochschild adds, even then “It takes a vigorous emotional
effort to repress the wish to care. . . .” (2003b, p. 221).
The dominant culture‟s social engineering allows for and even encourages individual
expressions of empathy, including volunteerism. And it‟s precisely because one-on-one
volunteerism—whether in shelters, soup kitchens or women‟s centers—only treats the symptoms
and not the sources, that it‟s a culturally sanctioned and channeled form of highly personal
empathic behavior. Charity would be another example. This bracketing off is entirely in
keeping with the dominant ideology, poses no threats, and functions to attenuate the acceptance
and institutionalization of social empathy on a grand scale.
8
To reiterate, ample evidence from numerous studies (Henrich and Henrich, 2007, pp. 27 31) demonstrates that cultural learning via imitation from modeled behavior is the most powerful
means through which both children and adults learn to practice altruistic behavior. But this vital
cultural transmission is generally limited to modeling individual acts of generosity.
Gramsci’s Politics and the Encultured Brain
Prefiguring the argument to follow, Poder (2008) perhaps comes as close as anyone in
highlighting the role of political power in the dynamics of emotional expression and regulation.
His specific focus is more limited and explores anger over reorganization within a corporate
culture, but in drawing upon Campbell‟s earlier work (1997) Poder states what should be
obvious but is too often ignored: “Individuals are not sovereign beings determining their own
feelings and how they can express themselves” (p. 295). A great deal depends on others‟
interpretation—invalidation or positive recognition—of one‟s emotional expression. Poder
reminds us that these “feeling rules” are being shaped by ideology and class.
Here I‟m comfortable introducing what political theorist William Connally (2002)
describes as “. . . politics through which cultural life mixes into the composition of body/brain
process. And vice versa.” (To my knowledge Connolly was the first political scientist to employ
the term neuropolitics although he doesn‟t explore the mirror neuron/empathy link in his erudite
inquiry.) Choudhury and Kirmayer (2009, pp. 264-5) astutely and refreshingly extend this
notion by placing culture in the context of globalization. They propose a promising research
agenda with implications extending far beyond their immediate concerns with psychopathology
and strengthening scientific approaches to psychiatry: “How do culturally mediated
developmental experiences influence subsequent emotion regulation and expression?” This in
turn begs two additional and closely related questions, ones that neuroscience and its
9
proliferating spawn of neuro-subfields have failed to explore, namely: “How did the cultural
information get into the brain in the first place? (Losin et al., 2009, p. 175), and what are the
implications for our understanding of empathy?” An impressive body of evidence now supports
the proposition that the human mirror system is at the epicenter of this cultural learning and there
is every reason to assume that robust, cross-cultural (well-funded) studies collecting neuroimaging data will enhance an empirically informed theory about its operation.
By my reading, too many cultural psychologists have followed too many neuroanthropologists (and vice versa) in failing to unpack the meaning of culture itself. To the extent
that conventional anthropology has explained culture as consisting primarily of a self-sustaining,
neutral transmission of beliefs, values, mores and laws passed down through generations, it fails
to illuminate the conscious and active invention of culture by institutions serving particular
interests. Here the work of Antonio Gramsci, an early twentieth-century Italian Marxist, is the
essential primer and his classic analysis of cultural hegemony can be enormously helpful in
moving the investigation forward. Kate Crehan (2002), an anthropologist herself, takes pains to
clarify that for Gramsci culture includes, but is not limited to, how class realities are experienced
by members of a specific community, and how members of that cultural milieu come to
understand their world, “their lived experience.” 1
Gramsci is not a dogmatic, economic reductionist and consistently stresses the organic
nature of culture. However, he was insistent that “. . . ultimately the most important question is
_______________
1
This discussion relies heavily on Crehan‟s Gramsci, Culture and Anthropology (2002).
10
that of power: Who has the power and who does not? Who is the oppressor and who is
oppressed? And what are the specificities of the relation of oppression?” (p. 6). For Gramsci,
the dominant class culture embodies its worldview even as that perspective assumes the
everyday status of common sense. Given this reality, political scientist Michael Parenti (1999,
p. 13) cautions us that “. . . whenever anyone offers culturistic explanations for social
phenomena, we should be skeptical.” Why? Because cultural explanations are closer to
tautologies than explanations. Culture itself is what needs to be explained (Parenti, 2006).
However, it should be understood that these cultural narratives, while powerful, are not
hermetically sealed from challenges. Efforts to produce counter-narratives constitute contested
cultural terrain, and this was the ongoing struggle to which Gramsci devoted so much of his
life‟s work.
Finally, in that context, there would seem to be a cautionary note here for scientists as
intellectuals. Crehan argues “Gramsci‟s concern is always with the process by which power is
produced and reproduced or transformed and how intellectuals fit within this rather than with
individual intellectuals themselves” (p. 143). A cultural neuroscience or neuro-anthropology that
fails to account for class will have, at best, no explanatory value and, at worst, further obfuscate
reality under the guise of value-free scientific inquiry.
Through the Mind’s Mirror, Darkly
Again, the quandary is why there is such a paucity of real-world empathic behavior,
especially in the United States? If only some 4 percent of the U.S. population can be classified
as sociopaths—individuals utterly incapable of empathy—what accounts for a mass culture
characterized by an empathy disorder of virtually pathological proportions? (Studies reveal
11
substantially less incidence of sociopathy in some East Asian countries with percentages ranging
from 0.03 percent to 0.14 percent, conditions warranting a follow-up study of its own.)
I‟m proposing that future research pursue Goldschmidt‟s (1999) observation that
“Culturally derived motives may replace, supplement or override genetically programmed
behavior.” The mirror mechanism, a hard-wired biological mechanism, minus positive cultural
nurturing, is unlikely to flourish (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2006). For example, studies on
attachment theory and emotion regulation (Shaver, et al., 2008) suggests links between
attachment security and pro-social behavior, including self-transcendent values and empathy.
An enhanced sense of security correlates with being sensitive to other‟s needs and a
willingness to engage in pro-social responsive behavior. Conversely we know that empathy is
less likely to manifest itself under conditions of attachment insecurity because the individual is
more likely to be self-absorbed, personally distressed, and empathically unavailable. These
avoidant individuals fear being “sucked in” by empathy and compassion, not only because of the
“hassle” but because people in need bring out their own feelings of personal distress (Shaver, et
al., pp. 135-136). A study on the negative consequences of neo-liberal economic policy in Latin
America concluded that an empathic orientation may be crowded out when people are
preoccupied with personal needs, insecure, and fearful about tomorrow (Vilas, 1997). To me it
seems entirely plausible that culturally-driven psychological insecurity could weigh as heavily as
material deprivation. Ervin Staub, a pioneering investigator in the field, makes the case that even
if empathy is hardwired, people will not act on “. . . unless they have certain kinds of life
experiences that shape their orientation toward other human beings and toward themselves”
(Staub, 2002, p. 222).
12
The role of socio-cultural variables in influencing psychopathology (Marsella and
Yamada, 2007) is now accepted, and I‟m proposing here that it would be instructive to examine
whether certain pathogenic cultural factors explain the etiology of what I‟ve tentatively labeled a
societal empathy deficit disorder (SEDD). In their well-documented discussion of psychopathy
as a disorder characterized by callousness and lack of empathy, Blair and Blair (2009) discuss
the existence of a population that has been subject to insufficient moral socialization. Such
individuals reveal an absence of empathic response to the distress of others, an impaired reaction
to “moral transgressions.” What is striking here, at least to me, is the ascription of these
behaviors to a subset of outliers and not to the larger society, the implicit message being that the
latter‟s everyday behavior is well within the “normal” range.
That is, highly competitive societies optimize the behavior of genetically-based, primary
sociopaths. In her book, The Sociopath Next Door, psychologist Martha Stout argues that
American culture‟s celebration of extreme individualism and “me-first” thinking reinforces antisocial behavior in the United States, including an increasing incidence of primary sociopathy. If,
as suspected, cold and calculating individuals devoid of empathy are represented in higher
numbers at the upper levels of business, media, and politics, we can assume these values will
become the cultural norm. Therefore, under a pathological capitalist culture, psychopathy is a
successful adaptive behavior for secondary sociopaths intent on getting ahead in society
(Mealey, 1995).
Setting aside the genetic, permanent condition for the moment, I‟m drawing attention to
effective or secondary sociopaths whose empathy deficit is more a product of environmental
circumstances (Mealey, 1995). The terminology remains stubbornly imprecise but we might
extrapolate from what Damasio (1990, 1994, 2007) labeled an acquired sociopathic personality
13
when referencing individuals. Here I‟ve described it as an empathy-challenged personality
condition having structural roots in the socio-economic system. This incongruity between our
substrate of empathy and the external environment significantly contributes to the creation of
empathy-suppressed individuals because the culture virtually requires the methodical bracketing
off of empathy. It‟s less a foreclosure and more a question of to whom is empathy directed. As a
result, we habitually violate our biological moral compass (Tollberg, 2007; Johnson, 2005) and
secondary sociopathy not only becomes normal behavior but a necessary, rewarded adaptive
behavior under the aforementioned “feeling rules” (Lindsay, 2008; Miller, 1999, p. 45). The
primate scientist Frans de Waal succinctly captures the system-maintenance function of
contrived callousness when he asserts, “You need to indoctrinate empathy out of people in order
to arrive at extreme capitalist positions (de Waal, 2007). Miller (1999) goes a step further by
adding, “It may not be strictly necessary to be a sociopath in order to be in a position of power in
society, but the rules of the game require doing a good imitation of one.”
One could further surmise that this cultural programming causes psychological
discomfort when individuals feel called upon to act on their natural empathic impulses. Because
of the discordance with the dominant culture and even beyond any material deprivation, such
behavior may exact a psychic price. Further, empathy may be pre-empted within this ideological
framework because the “losers” are not only presumed to deserve their fate but encouraged to
voluntarily assume that role. As a bonus, this narrative permits privileged groups to embrace the
pathological delusion that their behavior is estimable (Johnson, 2005) and this “[S]ort of very
harsh political ideology is often sold as being congruent with how human nature operates. You
look at human nature as an extension of nature. Wall Street is a Darwinian jungle. But this is
not how human nature actually operates” (de Waal, 2007).
14
In sum, we should approach this topic as a heretofore largely unexamined area of
research. Is the brain‟s mirror neuron network significantly influenced by free-market
capitalism‟s contrived, orchestrated, and virtually unchallenged cultural narrative of hyperindividualism and personal identities constructed on market values. What role is played by a
system of beliefs that simultaneously cultivates a “common sense” bleak view of human nature
while denigrating any state role in promoting social solidarity? Again, I‟m suggesting that this
widespread social pathology would benefit from a detailed investigation focusing upon just how
certain socio-economic variables shape the culture. We should remain open to the possibility
that some cultures are fundamentally incompatible with the lived expression of empathy.
And if an ethos of empathy is an essential part of what it means to be human, then
empathically impaired societies should be found wanting. For example, one promising avenue
might be to devise and test empathy scales in hyper-individualist societies like the United States
and presumably more solidaristic, collective, empathic cultural environments in countries like
Denmark, Sweden, and The Netherlands. Given the universality of our biological predisposition
toward empathy, we now have both a potent baseline and the potential for accumulating robust
empirical evidence upon which to draw further conclusions about this critically important issue
within neuroscience. In the process we might create environments that enhance the flourishing
of empathy, the foundation of our moral sense.
Acknowledgements: I‟m grateful to K. Crehan, J. Cromby, D. Lende, K. Haddad, M. Iacoboni
and K. Kelly for insightful comments on an earlier draft. Thanks, per usual, to M. Ortiz.
15
References
Blair, R. J. R., and Blair, K. S. (2009) “Empathy, Morality, and Social Convention: Evidence
frm the Study of Psychopathy and Other Psychiatric Disorders,” pp. 139-152, in J.
Decety and W. Ickes (Eds.), The Social Neuroscience of Empathy. Cambridge, MA:
Bradford/MIT Press.
Butler, E. A., Lee, T. L., and Gross, J. J. (2007) “Emotion Regulation and Culture: Are the
Social Consequences of Emotion Suppression Culture-Specific?” Emotion, 7, 1, 30-48.
Chiao, J. Y. and Ambady, N. (2007) “Parsing Universality and Diversity across Levels of
Analysis.” In: Kitayama, S. and Cohen, D. (Eds.) Handbook of Culture Psychology.
NY: Guilford Press.
Chiao, J. Y. (2009) “Cultural neuroscience: Cultural influence in brain function.” Progress in
Brain Research. Elsevier Press.
Chiao, J. Y., Harada, T., Komeda, H., Li, Z., Mano, Y., Saito, D., Parrish, T.B., Sadato, N., and Iidaka, T.
(uncorrected proof). Dynamic Cultural Influences on Neural Representations of the Self.
Chiao, J. Y., Harada, T., Komeda, H., Li, Z., Mano,Y., Saito, D., Parrish, T. B., Sadato,N. and Iidaka, T.
(2009) “Neural Basis of Individualistic and Collectivistic Views of Self.” Human Brain
Mapping 00:000-000 (2009), pp. 1-8.
Choudhury, S. (2009) “Culturing the adolescent brain: What can neuroscience learn from
anthropology?” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, December 2, 2009, 1-9.
Choudhury, S. and Kirmayer, L. J. (2009) “Cultural neuroscience and psychopathology:
Prospects for cultural psychiatry.” In: J. Y. Chiao (Ed.), Progress in Brain Research,
178, 263-283, Elsevier Press.
Connolly, W. (2002) Neuropolitics. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
16
Crehan, K. (2002) Gramsci, Culture and Anthropology. Berkeley: University of California
Press.
Cromby, J. (2007) “Toward a Psychology of Feeling,” International Journal of Critical
Psychology, 21, 94-118.
Decety, J. and Lamm, C. (2009) “Empathy versus Personal Distress: Recent Evidence from
Social Neuroscience,” pp. 199-213, in J. Decety and W. Ickes (Eds.), The Social
Neuroscience of Empathy. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT Press.
Decety, J. and Michalska, K. J. (2009) “Neurodevelopmental changes in the circuits underlying
empathy and sympathy from childhood to adulthood,” Developmental Sciences, pp. 1-14.
DOI: 101111/i.1467-7687.2009.00940x
Decety, J., Mihalska, K. J. and Akitsuki, M. (2008) “Who caused the pain? An fMRI
investigation of empathy and intentionality in children,” Neuropsychologia, 46, 26072614.
Decety, J. (2006) “Mirrored Emotion.” Interview, The University of Chicago Magazine, 94, 4,
1-9.
de Waal, F. B. M. (2006) Primates and Philosophers: How Morality Evolved. Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
de Waal, F. B. M. (2005-06) “The Evolution of Empathy,” Greater Good, Fall-Winter, pp. 8-9.
de Waal, Interview (2007) “Frans de Waal,” Interview with Tamler Sommers, The Believer, 5,
7, n.p.
de Waal, F. B. M. (2008) “Putting the Altruism Back into Altruism: The Evolution of
Empathy,” Annual Review of Psychology, 59: 279-300.
de Waal, Frans (2009) The Age of Empathy. New York: Harmony.
17
Dinstein, I., Thomas, C., Behrmann, M., and Heeger, D. J. (2008) “A Mirror to Nature,”
Current Biology, 8, 1.
Dominguez, D. J. F., Turner, R., Lewis, E. D., and Egan, G. F. (2009) “The brain in culture and
culture in the brain: A review of core issues in neuroanthropology.” In: J. Y. Chaio (Ed.)
Progress in Brain Research, 78, 43-64.
Dominguez, D. J. F., Turner, R., Lewis, D. E., and Egan, G. (2009) “Neuroanthropology: A
humanistic science for the study of the culture-brain nexus,” Social Cognition and
Affective Neuroscience, 10.1093/scan/nsp023.
Dominguez, D. J. F. (2006) Quoted in “Neuroanthropology: Culture on the Brain,” October 7,
2006. http://neurophilosophy.wordpress.com/2006/10/07.
Gabrielli, J. (2008) Interviewed by Clara Moskowitz in LiveScience, 18 January.
www.livescience.com/health/080118-culture-brain.html.
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996) “Action recognition in the premotor cortex.”
Brain, 1192: 593-609.
Gazzaniga, M. (2006) The Ethical Brain. New York: Dana Press.
Green, J. (2007) ini Vedantam, S. “If It Feels to be Good, It Might Be Only Natural,”
www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/article/2007/05/27.
Gross, J. J. (1998) “The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review,” Review
of General Psychology, 2, 271-299.
Han, S. and Northoff, G. (2008) “Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: A
transcultural neuroimaging approach,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience. August. Vol. 9,
pp. 646-654.
Hebb, D. O. (1949) The Organization of Behavior. New York: Wiley.
18
Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R., and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2008) “Cultural
Influence on Neural Substrates of Attentional Control,” Psychological Sciences, 19, 1,
12-17. (on line 26 Dec. 2007), DOI:10,1111/j1467-9280.2008.02038x.
Henrich, N. and Henrich, J. (2007) Why Humans Cooperate. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Hickok, G. (2009) “Eight Problems for the Mirror Neuron Theory of Action Understanding in
Monkeys and Humans,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 7, pp. 1229-1243.
Hochschild, A. R. (1979) “Emotion Work, Feeling Rules, and Social Structure,
American Journal of Sociology, 85, 3, 551-575.
Hochschild, A. R. (1983, 2003a) The Managed Heart. Berkeley and Los Angeles, California:
University of California Press.
Hochschild, A. R. (2003) The Commercialization of Intimate Life. Berkeley and Los Angeles,
California: University of California Press.
Iacoboni, M. (2009) “Imitation, Empathy, and Mirror Neurons,” Annual Review of Psychology,
60: 653-670.
Iacoboni, M. (2008) Mirroring People. New York: Farrar, Strauss and Giroux.
Iacoboni, M. (2007) “Neuroscience Will Change Society,” Edge, The World Question Center.
http:www.edge.org/q2007pp14-15
Johnson, C. (2005) “Narratives of Identity: Denying Empathy in Conservative Discourses on
Race, Class, and Sexuality,” Theory and Society, 34: 37-61.
Kitayama, S., Karasawa, M., and Mesquita, B. (2004) “Collective and personal processes in
regulating emotions: Emotion and self in Japan and the United States.” In: Philiippot and
R. S. Feldman (Eds.) The Regulation of Emotion (pp. 251-273). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
19
Knight-Jadezyk (2003) “Official Culture in America: A Natural State of Psychopathy?” Sign of
the Times, July, 2003.
Lewontin, R. C., Rose, S., and Kamin, L. J. (1984) Not In Our Genes. New York: Pantheon
Books.
Lieberman, M. (2007) “Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes,” Annual
Reviews in Psychology, 58, 259-89.
Lindsay, R. (2008) “Capitalism Normalizes Sociopathy,”
http://robertlindsay.blogspot.com/2008/5/capitalism.
Lippard, J. (2009) Mirror Neurons and the Study of Science.
http://lippard.blogspot.com/2009_09_01_archive.html.
Losin, E. A. R., Dapretto, M., and Iacoboni, M. (2009) “Culture in the mind‟s mirror: How
anthropology and neuroscience can inform a model of the neural substrate for cultural
imitative learning.” In: J. Y. Chaio (Ed.), Progress in Brain Research, pp. 175-190.
Elsevier Press.
Malafouris, L. (2010) “The brain-artifact interface (BAI): A challenge for archaeology and
cultural neuroscience,” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Advance Access,
January 19, 2010, 1-10.
Marsella, J. and Yamada, A. M. (2007) “Culture and Psychopathology: Foundations, Issues,
and Directions.” In: S. Kitayama and D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of Cultural
Psychology. Guilford Press.
Mauss, I. B., Bunge, S. A., and Gross, J. J. (2008) “Culture and Automatic Emotion
Regulations.” In: M. Vandekerckhove, C. von Scheve, S. Ismer, S. Jung, and S. Kronast
(Eds.), Regulating Emotions, pp. 39-60. London: Blackwell.
20
Mealey, L. (1995) “The Sociobiology of Sociopathy: An Integrated Evolutionary Model,”
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18: 523-599.
Mesquita, L. and Leu, J. (2007) “The Cultural Psychology of Emotion.” In: S. Kitayama and D.
Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of Cultural Psychology, pp. 734-759. New York: Guilford
Press.
Miller, W. (1999) “Social Change and Human Nature,” Monthly Review, 50, No. 9.
Molnar-Szakacs, I., Wu, A., Robles, F. and Iacoboni, M. (2007a) “Do You See What I Mean?
Corticospinal Excitability During Observation of Culture Specific Gestures,” PLoS ONE,
2(7): e626, DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0000626.
Molnar-Szakacs, I. (2007b) Cited in “Culture Influences Brain Cells: Brain‟s Mirror Neurons
Swayed by Ethnicity and Culture,” Science Daily, University of California-Los Angeles
(2007, July 23).
Mukamel, R., Ekstrom, A. D., Kaplan, J., Iacoboni, M. and Fried, I. (2010) “Single-Neuron
Responses in Humans During Execution and Observation of Actions,” Current Biology.
DOI:10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.045.
Olson, G. (1987, reprinted 2007) “Execution Class,” Z Magazine, 20, 3, 200_.
Olson, G. (2008) “We Empathize, Therefore We Are: Toward a Moral Neuropolitics,”
neuropolitics.org/defaultnov08.asp
Parenti, M. (1999) “Reflections on the Politics of Culture,” Monthly Review, 50, No. 9.
Parenti, M. (2006) The Culture Struggle. New York: Seven Stories Press.
Poder, P. (2008) “The Political Regulation of Anger in Organizations.” In: M.
Vanderkerckhove, C. von Sheve, S. Ismer, S. Jung and S. Kronast (Eds.), Regulating
Emotions, pp. 291-311. London: Blackwell.
21
Preston, S. and de Waal, F. B. M. (2002) “Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases,”
Behavior and Brain Sciences, 25, 1-72.
Preston, S., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Grabowski, T., Stansfield, R., Mehta, S., Damasio, A.
(2007) “The Neural Substrates of Cognitive Humanity,” Social Neuroscience, 2, (3-4),
254-275.
Ramachandran, V. (2006) “Mirror Neurons and the Brain in the Vat,” Edge, January 10, 2006.
www.edge.org/3rdculture/ramachandran06/Ramachandran06index.html+jam.
Rizolatti, G. and Sinigalia, C. (2008) Mirrors in the Brain. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Shaver, P. R., Mikulincer, M. and Chun, D. S. (2008) “Adult Attachment Theory, Emotion
Regulation, and Prosocial Behavior.” In: M. Vandekerckhove, C. von Scheve, S. Ismer,
S. Jung, and S. Kronast (Eds.), Regulating Emotions, pp. 121-145. London: Blackwell.
Sherman, D. K., Kim, H.S. and Taylor, S. E. (2009) “Culture and social support: Neural bases
and biological impact.” In: J. Y. Chaio (Ed.), Progress in Brain Research, 178, pp. 227237. Elsevier Press.
Singer, T. and Lamm, C. (2009) “The Social Neuroscience of Empathy,” The Year in Cognitive
Neuroscience 2009. Ann. New York Academy of Science, 1156: 81-96.
Slaby, J. (2010) “Steps towards a critical neuroscience” (under review).
Slack, G. (2007) “I feel your pain,” www.Salon.com 2007/11/05.
Staub, E. (2002) Quoted in R. J. Davidson, and A. Harrington, A. (Eds.), Visions of
Compassion. New York: Guilford Press.
Stout, S. (2006) The Sociopath Next Door. New York: Broadway Books, 2006.
22
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J. and Mashek, D. J. (2007) “Moral Emotions and Moral Behavior,”
Annual Review of Psychology, 58: 345-72.
Tollberg, D. (2007) “From empathy to autism—how ignorance became the norm,”
gupea.ub.gu.se/space/bitstream/2007/17742/1/gupea2007.
Tomasello, M. (2009) Why We Cooperate. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.
Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L. (1992) “The Psychological Foundation of Culture.” In: Barkow, J.,
Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (Eds.), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the
Generation of Culture (pp. 19-136). New York: Oxford University Press.
Turner, R. and Whitehead, C. (2008) “How Collective Representations Can Change the
Structure of the Brain,” Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15, No. 10-11, pp. 43-57.
Vidal, F. (2009) “Brainhood: Anthropological figure of modernity,” History of the Human
Sciences, 22, (1) 6-35.
Vilas, C. (1997) “Inequality and the Dismantling of Citizenship in Latin America,” NACLA
Report on the Americas, 31, 1 (July-August), 57-63.
Virona, J. (2009) “Leaping from Brain to Mind: A Critique of Mirror Neuron Explanations of
Countertransference,” Journal of American Psychoanalytic Association, 57: 525-550.
Vrecko, S. (2010) “Neuroscience, power and culture: An introduction,” History of the Human
Sciences, Vol. 23, No. 1, 1-10.
Wexler, B. E. (2006) Brain and Culture. Cambridge, Mass.: Bradford/MIT Press.
23
Download