Physiology, Behavior, and Conservation

Physiology, Behavior, and Conservation
Author(s): Steven J. Cooke, Daniel T. Blumstein, Richard Buchholz, Tim Caro, Esteban
Fernández-Juricic, Craig E. Franklin, Julian Metcalfe, Constance M. O’Connor, Colleen
Cassady St. Clair, William J. Sutherland, and Martin Wikelski
Source: Physiological and Biochemical Zoology, Vol. 87, No. 1 (January/February 2014), pp. 114
Published by: The University of Chicago Press
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/671165 .
Accessed: 25/01/2014 20:44
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
.
The University of Chicago Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Physiological and Biochemical Zoology.
http://www.jstor.org
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
1
INVITED PERSPECTIVE
Physiology, Behavior, and Conservation*
Steven J. Cooke1,†,‡
Daniel T. Blumstein2
Richard Buchholz3
Tim Caro4
Esteban Fernández-Juricic5
Craig E. Franklin6
Julian Metcalfe7
Constance M. O’Connor8
Colleen Cassady St. Clair9
William J. Sutherland10
Martin Wikelski11
1
Fish Ecology and Conservation Physiology Laboratory,
Department of Biology and Institute of Environmental
Science, Carleton University, 1125 Colonel By Drive, Ottawa,
Ontario K1S 5B6, Canada; 2Department of Ecology and
Evolutionary Biology and Institute of the Environment and
Sustainability, University of California, 621 Charles E. Young
Drive South, Los Angeles, California 90095; 3Department of
Biology, University of Mississippi, University, Mississippi
38677; 4Department of Wildlife, Fish, and Conservation
Biology, University of California, 1 Shields Avenue, Davis,
California 95616; 5Department of Biological Sciences, Purdue
University, 915 West State Street, West Lafayette, Indiana
47907; 6School of Biological Sciences, University of
Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland 4072, Australia; 7Centre
for Environment Fisheries and Aquaculture Science,
Lowestoft Laboratory, Pakefield Road, Lowestoft, Suffolk
NR33 0HT, United Kingdom; 8Aquatic Behavioural Ecology
Lab, Department of Psychology, Neuroscience, and
Behaviour, McMaster University, 1280 Main Street West,
Hamilton, Ontario L8S 4K1, Canada; 9Department of
Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta
T6G 2E9, Canada; 10Conservation Science Group,
Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge,
Cambridge CB2 3EJ, United Kingdom; 11Max Planck
Institute of Ornithology, D-78315 Radolfzell, Germany, and
University of Konstanz, D-78467 Konstanz, Germany
Accepted 3/21/2013; Electronically Published 6/7/2013
* This paper was submitted as an Invited Perspective for a Focused Issue on
“Conservation Physiology.”
† Corresponding author; e-mail: steven_cooke@carleton.ca.
‡ Authorship alphabetical after first author.
Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 87(1):1–14. 2014. 䉷 2013 by The University of Chicago. All rights reserved. 1522-2152/2014/8701-3006$15.00.
DOI: 10.1086/671165
ABSTRACT
Many animal populations are in decline as a result of human
activity. Conservation practitioners are attempting to prevent
further declines and loss of biodiversity as well as to facilitate
recovery of endangered species, and they often rely on interdisciplinary approaches to generate conservation solutions. Two
recent interfaces in conservation science involve animal behavior (i.e., conservation behavior) and physiology (i.e., conservation physiology). To date, these interfaces have been considered separate entities, but from both pragmatic and
biological perspectives, there is merit in better integrating behavior and physiology to address applied conservation problems and to inform resource management. Although there are
some institutional, conceptual, methodological, and communication-oriented challenges to integrating behavior and physiology to inform conservation actions, most of these barriers
can be overcome. Through outlining several successful examples that integrate these disciplines, we conclude that physiology
and behavior can together generate meaningful data to support
animal conservation and management actions. Tangentially, applied conservation and management problems can, in turn, also
help advance and reinvigorate the fundamental disciplines of
animal physiology and behavior by providing advanced natural
experiments that challenge traditional frameworks.
Introduction
Global biodiversity and associated ecosystems services are
threatened by human activities and human-mediated environmental change (Butchart et al. 2005; Cardinale et al. 2012;
Hooper et al. 2012). Conservation practitioners devoted to
stemming the loss of biodiversity have had some success, but
generally biodiversity continues to decline while threats proliferate (Butchart et al. 2010). For example, 20% of vertebrates
are threatened, and that figure is increasing (Hoffmann et al.
2010). Populations of some amphibians (Stuart et al. 2004),
mammals (Schipper et al. 2008; Hoffmann et al. 2010), and
cartilaginous fishes (Dulvy et al. 2008) have seen dramatic declines, and many species in these groups are at risk of extinction.
There is both dire need and abundant opportunity for conservation scientists to understand the factors and processes responsible for population declines of animals and try to work
with management authorities and politicians to develop strategies to reverse such trends.
Conservation biology—or conservation science, to reflect its
multidisciplinarity—emerged as a crisis discipline in the 1980s
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
2 Cooke, Blumstein, Buchholz, Caro, Fernández-Juricic, Franklin, Metcalfe, O’Connor, St. Clair, Sutherland, and Wikelski
(Soulé 1985, 1986; Kareiva and Marvier 2012), with a goal of
trying to stem the loss of biodiversity. Yet because of the inherent complexity of environmental problems, conservation
science often requires an interdisciplinary approach, bringing
together disparate fields such as social science, biology, law, and
resource management (Soulé 1986). As a consequence, a number of new subdisciplines have emerged, some of which have
gained significant momentum, such as conservation genetics
(Hedrick 2001). An emerging interdisciplinary field is that of
conservation physiology (Wikelski and Cooke 2006; Cooke et
al. 2013), while another is a behavior-conservation interface
called conservation behavior (Sutherland 1998; Buchholz 2007;
Blumstein and Fernández-Juricic 2010). Conservation physiology is primarily concerned with animals and, more specifically, vertebrates (e.g., Carey 2005; Tracy et al. 2006; Wikelski
and Cooke 2006; Seebacher and Franklin 2012; Cooke et al.
2013; this issue).
Despite clear similarities, conservation physiology and conservation behavior have until now been considered separate
entities (although see Cooke et al. 2012; Metcalfe et al. 2012).
Yet there is merit in better integrating physiology and behavior
to address applied conservation problems. Conservation physiology excels in defining cause and effect relationships (Tracy
et al. 2006; Cooke and O’Connor 2010) but often requires
behavioral information for practical purposes. Conversely,
many targets of conservation behavior (e.g., captive breeding,
quantifying human impacts on wildlife, understanding how
populations respond to environmental change) would benefit
from the mechanistic understanding provided by physiological
data. Although a number of reviews have suggested an agenda
for both conservation physiology (Wikelski and Cooke 2006;
Cooke et al. 2013) and conservation behavior (Sutherland 1998;
Buchholz 2007), there are also those that identify a number of
challenges with the application of physiology (Cooke and
O’Connor 2010) and behavior (Caro 2007; Angeloni et al. 2008)
to conservation problems. As such, it seems prudent to explore
how aligning and even combining physiological and behavioral
approaches might help both contribute to meaningful conservation outcomes.
The review is organized as follows: we start by outlining the
development of conservation physiology and conservation behavior, focusing on their strengths and weaknesses (table 1).
Next, we consider how these two subdisciplines could benefit
from better integration and also provide examples of successful
integration. We conclude by considering how to overcome the
factors that hinder integration. In addition, we speculate that
applied problems can provide an opportunity to better integrate
and advance fundamental aspects of animal physiology and
behavior.
A Primer on Conservation Physiology
The term “conservation physiology” was first coined by Wikelski and Cooke (2006, p. 38) and defined as “the study of
physiological responses of organisms to human alteration of
the environment that might cause or contribute to population
declines.” Although there are no reference or textbooks on
conservation physiology per se, in 2013 a journal by the same
name was launched in recognition of the growing interest in
the topic (for inaugural paper in that journal, see Cooke et al.
2013). In addition, special issues on conservation physiology
were published in Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society
of London B in 2012 (see Seebacher and Franklin 2012) and
Physiological and Biochemical Zoology in 2013 (this issue). One
of the first examples of conservation physiology comes from
Rachel Carson’s Silent Spring (Carson 1962). The book is famous for highlighting physiological studies that helped to identify the cause-and-effect relationship between dichlorodiphenyl-trichloroethane and reproductive failure of raptors
(Pollock et al. 2001). Indeed, there are now many other examples of ecotoxicological studies that have used physiological
approaches (see Brouwer et al. 1990) and that led to regulations
that protect wildlife and ecosystems, including the listing of
species (Fossi et al. 1999). Many others have suggested integrating toxicology and conservation, and these form part of
conservation physiology also (e.g., Hansen and Johnson 1999a,
1999b). Yet conservation physiology has many other facets (Carey 2005; Tracy et al. 2006), including stress biology, particularly
the use of glucocorticoid stress hormones to characterize the
physiological costs of human activities (reviewed by Wingfield
et al. 1997; Busch and Hayward 2009). A further area centers
on the reproductive physiology of endangered organisms, for
example, by improving captive breeding (Wildt and Wemmer
1999). Finally, in the past decade, broadscale environmental
change—such as changing temperatures, levels of precipitation,
and ocean acidity—has initiated research using physiological
approaches to understand and predict the consequences of perturbations (reviewed in Pörtner and Farrell 2008; Seebacher
and Franklin 2012). Physiological knowledge is also increasingly
being used to inform resource management of exploited species
(Cooke et al. 2012; Metcalfe et al. 2012).
Conservation physiology has several uses in conservation and
management. For example, conservation physiology is particularly effective at measuring immediate response of individuals
to environmental change or other stressors. Rapid responses
are often not evident in other endpoints, such as community
structure (Cooke and Suski 2008). Therefore, physiology can
provide a mechanistic basis to describe, elucidate, and predict
effects of environmental change and human disturbance (Wikelski and Cooke 2006; Seebacher and Franklin 2012). Additionally, many physiological endpoints are also directly relevant
to survival or fitness (e.g., ability to recovery from a stressor
enabling animals to avoid predators; Costa and Sinervo 2004).
With mechanistic understanding, managers can attempt to use
strategies that would alleviate or reduce a given stressor and
thus modify organismal behavior and/or population biology.
Moreover, through controlled experimentation, physiologists
are able to define and establish whether, when, and where there
is a direct causal link between a stressor and an organismal
condition, something that is rarely possible for behavioral ecologists (Tracy et al. 2006; Cooke and O’Connor 2010). Additionally, physiologists have the expertise to delve into the mech-
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Table 1: Summary of the key strengths and weaknesses of behavior and physiology, with specific reference to the application
of these approaches to conservation problems
Characteristics
Strengths
Weaknesses
Conservation behavior
Several frameworks (e.g., Tinbergian approach
and Berger-Tal et al. 2011) on which
conservation problems can addressed
Behavior can be a demographic mechanism
(Anthony and Blumstein 2000)
Behavior is an easily observed and immediate
metric of distress for individuals
Behavior integrates the response of an individual
to environmental change
Behavior is useful for understanding the success
or failure of concerted conservation efforts
where individuals are known
Behavior can provide an early warning to
population decline or habitat degradation
before numerical responses are evident
Observable behavior often engenders the human
empathy needed to support conservation
action
Fails to provide mechanistic explanations (e.g.,
linked to physiological status, tolerances, and
regulata) of observed behaviors
Population implications of individual behavior
remains tenuous
Some reliance on surrogate species
There is a need for better integration of
behavior in ecological models and
conservation/management plans
Conservation managers are often not trained in
behavior
There is a need for more success stories
Conservation physiology
Physiological experiments can be used to identify
causal relationships
Physiological information provides mechanisms
underlying observed patterns in behavior or survival
Physiology provides an objective characterization of
animal health and condition
Many physiological indicators are linked to stress and
fitness
Physiology can be used to predict organismal
responses to environmental perturbations and
change
Physiological responses are typically rapid (i.e.,
compared with changes in organismal abundance or
community structure)
Physiological responses may reveal fitness impacting
changes that are not evident from an animal’s
behavior (e.g., immobility)
Physiology can be used to characterize the diversity
among individuals, populations, and species
Because of its similarity to medical research, policy
makers may find physiological data more convincing
or objective than behavioral data alone
Physiology does not provide information on behavioral
responses that could mediate responses to
unfavorable environmental conditions or stressors
No common paradigm or underlying conceptual
framework for integrating physiology and behavior
(although see life history–physiology nexus and
performance framework developed by Arnold 1983;
also see fig. 2 for proposed framework)
There is a need to better understand links between
physiological indicators and survival/fitness
Population implications of individual physiology
remains tenuous
Strong reliance on surrogate species
There is a need for tools that enable physiology to be
studied in the wild
Some procedures are lethal
There is a need for better integration of physiology in
ecological models and conservation/management
plans
It is difficult to convey physiological concepts to the
general public
Conservation managers often not trained in physiology
There is a need for more success stories
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
4 Cooke, Blumstein, Buchholz, Caro, Fernández-Juricic, Franklin, Metcalfe, O’Connor, St. Clair, Sutherland, and Wikelski
anisms underlying certain behaviors, from the gathering of
sensory information to the processing of information in the
brain, which ultimately affects behavioral responses. This level
of mechanistic understanding may enhance the success of interventions, such as when managers try to manipulate the behavior of a species (e.g., attraction or repulsion from an area;
Sutherland 1998).
Conservation physiology has not been without critics. Cooke
and O’Connor (2010) noted that conservation actions tend to
focus on populations or species, whereas physiology tends to
focus on individuals, cells, or molecules, and they identified
the need to expand and validate the tools in the conservation
physiology toolbox, particularly those that enable the study of
animals in their natural environment (e.g., biotelemetry and
biologging, which also yield behavioral information; see Cooke
et al. 2004; Block 2005; Ropert-Coudert and Wilson 2005;
Cooke 2008; Metcalfe et al. 2012). They also recognized the
need to conduct research on threatened taxa rather than relying
on surrogates (also a critique of conservation behavior; Caro
et al. 2005).
A Primer on Conservation Behavior
Conservation behavior applies knowledge and methods from
animal behavior to solve wildlife conservation and management
problems (Curio 1996; Blumstein and Fernández-Juricic 2010).
It views behavior as a mechanism that influences demographic
processes and thus may identify levers that can be applied to
manage populations. In the same way that behavior is a mechanism of demography, physiology is a mechanism of behavior.
Thus, the integration of these fields is somewhat natural. Indeed, some aspects of physiology—such as endocrine-based
motivational changes, sensory neuron sensitivity, and other
proximate causes of behavior—have always been considered
one of the four behavioral questions posed by animal behaviorists (Tinbergen 1963). As a result, these aspects of physiology
were included in the concept of conservation behavior from
the very beginning (Clemmons and Buchholz 1997; Wingfield
et al. 1997).
The term “conservation behavior” was first used in a book
review (Blumstein 2001) of an edited volume that integrated
behavior and conservation (Gosling and Sutherland 2000). This
was just one of several edited volumes (Caro 1998; Festa-Bianchet and Apollonio 2003), substantive reviews (Strier 1997;
Sutherland 1998; Caro 1999; Reed 1999; Anthony and Blumstein 2000), as well as two special journal issues (Oikos, vol.
77; Environmental Biology of Fishes, vol. 55; Applied Animal
Behavior Science, vol. 102) that followed a pioneering edited
book by Clemmons and Buchholz (1997), which emerged from
a 1995 symposium at the Animal Behavior Society annual meeting in Lincoln, Nebraska. The first textbook on conservation
behavior was published in 2010 (Blumstein and FernándezJuricic 2010), and there have been additional reviews and conceptual frameworks published recently (e.g., Angeloni et al.
2010; Berger-Tal et al. 2011; Candolin and Wong 2012).
At one level, understanding and managing behavior has al-
ways been part of wildlife management (particularly that focused on repelling/attracting animals from certain areas and
dealing with captive breeding), but the key contribution in the
1990s was that behavioral biologists could generate novel conservation and management strategies to help manage our biodiversity crisis (Curio 1996). For instance, knowledge of conspecific attraction could be used to help encourage animals to
settle (or stay) in places where they were safe (e.g., Ward and
Schlossberg 2004). Knowledge of adaptive sex ratio variation
(Trivers and Willard 1973) could be used to distort the offspring
sex ratio in a desired manner (Robertson et al. 2006). Finally,
knowledge of how a species responds fearfully to humans (e.g.,
Blumstein 2007) could be used to develop setback zones to
reduce human impacts on animals (e.g., Fernández-Juricic et
al. 2005).
However, conservation behavior has also come in for criticism (Caro 2007). Conservation biology focuses on much larger
scales (landscape, ecosystem, biosphere) than the individual
focus of animal behaviorists. Caro (2007) suggested that the
different approaches of conservationists and behaviorists were
evidence of the failure of conservation behavior’s theoretical
insights to actually be applied to solve conservation problems,
and empirical examination concurs (Angeloni et al. 2008). He
suggested that conservation problems are driven by habitat destruction, exotics, exploitation, climate change, and pollution—
hardly the variables that behavioral biologists study. Buchholz
(2007) countered Caro’s (2007) criticism with concrete examples of how management has been improved by the integration of behavioral insights about mechanism, ontogeny,
adaptive function, and evolutionary history into conservation
planning. Interestingly, these relevant academic arguments are
sometimes not well rooted in the daily challenges many managers around the world face. Some of these challenges have a
central behavioral component because they involve the manipulations of the behavior of individual animals (e.g., lure them
to traps for relocation, detract them from breeding in areas
with high incidence of predation/brood parasitism). For the
correct questions at the proper scale, insights and knowledge
gained by those who study behavior can have profound impacts
on conservation and management outcomes. Nowadays, however, many recognize that captive breeding, translocation, and
reintroduction; creating effective deterrents; and managing and
understanding animals’ responses to growing populations and
urbanization are the areas where behavioral knowledge might
be most relevant to management problems (Blumstein and Fernández-Juricic 2010; Candolin and Wong 2012) rather than in
in situ conservation programs.
Benefits of Integrating Physiology and Behavior
for Conservation Science
The thesis of this article is that the integration of behavior and
physiology would generate meaningful understanding to support conservation actions and do so synergistically. Often, questions arise from conservation behavioral studies that are physiological in nature, and vice versa for physiological studies.
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Physiology, Behavior, and Conservation 5
Integrating the disciplines would provide simultaneous data
and therefore better efficiency and speed in being able to understand causes of population declines and develop strategies
to reverse them. Collectively, physiology and behavior move us
toward a more complete understanding of individuals and how
different drivers could scale up to affect higher levels of biological organization. Even though physiologists and behaviorists
are often concerned with different levels of biological organization, they collectively address all of the ways in which stressors
can influence biological processes that ultimately will influence
population-level processes of great interest to conservation scientists (fig. 1).
Developing a more integrative understanding of target species will improve researchers predictions of individual responses
to environmental perturbations, on the basis of exposure and
sensitivity (Huey et al. 2012), and thus provide a mechanistic
and organismal understanding for changes in populations, distribution shifts of species, and extinctions (e.g., Seebacher and
Franklin 2012). Conservation practitioners need predictive demographic models that have specific mechanistic levers identified through physiology and parameterized with behavioral
information (e.g., Anthony and Blumstein 2000; Huey et al.
2012). Such knowledge could be used for identifying and applying various management actions that influence animal populations (e.g., leading to increasing populations of endangered
animals, reducing populations of invasive species). Extinction
probability models can also incorporate behavioral and physiological attributes that influence population persistence rather
than focusing solely on population biology (Reed 1999; Blumstein and Fernández-Juricic 2004). From a methodological
stance, if we knew more about the correlations between behavior and physiology, we could make more use of quick, inexpensive, and noninvasive behavioral assays to identify target
individuals for capture, protection, or aversive conditioning.
For example, the field of ethotoxicology uses behaviors as indicators of exposure to toxic chemicals (Parmigiani et al. 1998),
and behavioral assays are used to identify rabid individuals, and
the same might be true of much more nuanced health states
with more investment in the integration of these two fields.
With conservation triage inevitable (Bottrill et al. 2009), it
would also be worthwhile to identify how physiology and behavior could be used to identify practical conservation priorities. For example, with a growing number of species in need
of resource-intensive ex situ conservation, there is a need to
identify species/populations that will likely succeed in such environments, an area where the integration of physiology and
behavior has the potential to excel.
Examples of Successful Integration
to Advance Conservation
While advocating the integration of behavior and physiology
to improve conservation outcomes, we acknowledge that some
examples already exist. Here we summarize some successful
cases of integration of behavior and physiology that span environments, taxa, and conservation problems in an attempt to
Figure 1. Hierarchical levels of biological organization illustrating
where behaviorists, physiologists, and conservation practitioners typically focus their efforts. Note that environmental stressors affect multiple levels of biological organization and that failure to combine physiology and behavior to address conservation-oriented perspectives
would mean that there is an incomplete understanding of the biological
processes that ultimately determine the state of animal populations,
ecosystems, and conservation outcomes. A color version of this figure
is available online.
illustrate the benefits to advancing conservation. A common
feature of success is an interdisciplinary team or individuals
that walk the line between behavior and physiology (i.e., integrative biologists, behavioral physiologists). Successes are evident in documenting problems and developing conservation
solutions in both ex situ and in situ contexts.
Hoatzin (Opisthocomus hoazin) and Ecotourism
As noted above, the effects of human disturbance on wildlife
are often studied using either behavior or physiology, rarely
both. In South America and elsewhere, ecotourism operators
seek to provide tourists with opportunities to view birds and
other wildlife in their natural environment. Müllner et al.
(2004) studied hoatzin in areas with low and high ecotourism
and recorded adult flush distance, nest success, chick development, and glucocorticoid stress responsiveness (baseline and
maximal) of chicks. The behavior and fitness endpoints (survival) were alone meaningful and indicated that birds were
impacted by tourist activity. However, the physiological component identified the mechanism by which mortality was occurring (i.e., high levels of corticosterone, which impeded
growth), and identified that the impact was greatest on juveniles
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
6 Cooke, Blumstein, Buchholz, Caro, Fernández-Juricic, Franklin, Metcalfe, O’Connor, St. Clair, Sutherland, and Wikelski
before fledging. The notion of differential sensitivity to tourist
stress across developmental stages provides managers with potential opportunities for using temporal and/or spatial restrictions on ecotourism to protect sensitive species.
Ungulate Responses to Airplane Noise
Noise from anthropogenic activities is regarded as a form of
pollution. Weisenberger et al. (1996) studied the effects of simulated low-level military jet aircraft noise on mule deer (Odocoileus hemionus) and mountain sheep (Ovis canadensis) in outdoor desert enclosures. Heart rate telemetry and behavioral
observations both revealed evidence of disturbance. Heart rate
showed a positive relationship between cardiac response and
the decibels of the noise disturbance. Both heart rate and behavior, although altered, returned to predisturbance levels
within several minutes following the end of the disturbance.
Both metrics also showed evidence of habituation when exposed to repeated flyover simulations. Others have taken such
work further and modeled the energetic consequences of disturbance (e.g., on wandering albatross [Diomedea exulans] relative to human disturbance; Weimerskirch et al. 2002).
Pacific Salmon (Oncorhynchus spp.) and Climate Change
Climate change and associated warming river temperatures are
expected to have detrimental consequences on Pacific salmon.
Studies using biotelemetry to study survival, behavior (e.g.,
entry timing, migration speed), and physiology (e.g., tissue
samples of telemetered animals, parallel laboratory cardiorespiratory studies) reveal that there are stock-specific thermal
thresholds and clear relationships between impaired migration
behavior (including failed migration and subsequent death) and
physiological status (Cooke et al. 2012; Hinch et al. 2012). The
cardiorespiratory capacity of Pacific salmon is constrained such
that fish are unable to migrate beyond a given stock-specific
thermal threshold (Farrell et al. 2008; Eliason et al. 2011). The
behavioral and physiological disturbances are magnified when
other stressors are added, such as when fisheries interactions
(e.g., bycatch) coincide with high temperatures (e.g., Gale et
al. 2011). The combination of approaches (in lab and field,
behavior and physiology) used in this coordinated research
program (summarized in Cooke et al. 2012) have generated
data for management adjustment models (i.e., restricting harvest when water temperatures warm) allowing adequate numbers of adults reach spawning grounds, and they have also led
to the development of best handling practices for those that
capture and release salmon.
Kakapo (Strigops habroptilus) Supplemental Feeding
and Captive Breeding
The kakapo is a critically endangered bird endemic to New
Zealand. While early studies focused more on behavior and
demography of kakapo, aggressive conservation efforts have
begun to include supplementary feeding to increase chick sur-
vival and improve breeding frequency. This has led to a demand
for more knowledge of nutritional physiology. The research
team hypothesized that there was a minimum weight threshold
below which the birds would be incapable of reproducing (Harper et al. 2006) but also recognized that too much supplemental
feeding seemed to reduce breeding (Elliot et al. 2001). Detailed
nutritional physiology studies followed that examined the nutrient composition of kakapo diets in the field (Cottam et al.
2006), estrogenic activities of foods (Fidler et al. 2000), and
gut microbiology (Waite et al. 2012) to inform supplemental
and captive feeding. Experimental feeding with food formulations revealed that nutritional quality was the problem with
diets, not energy content (Houston et al. 2007). A recent thesis
(Cottam 2010) further explored the chemical compounds in
key fruits in an effort to identify triggers to reproduction. Research continues to unravel whether reproductive cues are visual or directly related to ingestion of compounds.
Ultraviolet B Effects on Amphibians
Amphibians are experiencing dramatic population declines and
extinctions. Although it is common for the causes of population
declines to be multifactorial, this concept is exemplified in the
amphibians in which ultraviolet (UV) radiation has synergistic
effects with a variety of environmental (e.g., water temperature,
pH, nitrates) and biotic (e.g., disease, predation) factors (see
Bancroft et al. 2008; Alton et al. 2010, 2012). After initial observations of deformities and population declines thought to
be mediated by UV light, both physiologists and behaviorists
became involved in attempting to understand the influence of
UV light on amphibians and identify its relevance to different
conservation strategies. One of the first studies to link physiological tolerances to behavior in a field environment revealed
that because of parental oviposition behavior, the embryos of
the most UV-sensitive species experienced the least UV-B radiation (Palen et al. 2005). Palen and Schindler (2010) combined surveys of the incubation timing, incident UV-B, optical
transparency of water, and oviposition depth and light exposure
of embryos at various sites to further reveal that exposure to
lethal levels of UV-B was minimal in a natural heterogeneous
landscape. Collectively, these studies suggest that maintenance
of natural heterogeneous habitats is essential for providing opportunities for amphibians to avoid UV-B. However, even when
UV-B is itself not lethal, in the presence of other stressors
(especially disease and predation pressures; Alton et al. 2011;
Blaustein et al. 2012), it is important to maintain good water
quality and minimize human disturbances associated with development. Much work is currently underway on understanding disease development and immune function (e.g., Richmond
et al. 2009), which is highly relevant to understanding the effects
of multiple stressors on amphibian populations.
Bycatch Reduction of Cetaceans and Turtles
Bycatch in marine fisheries is a global conservation problem.
Early studies focused on developing strategies to reduce bycatch
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Physiology, Behavior, and Conservation 7
and documented the behavior of nontarget taxa around fishing
gear. It became evident that where the target of the fishery and
the nontarget species overlapped, bycatch was inevitable. Researchers then began to incorporate knowledge and studies of
sensory physiology in an attempt to identify means of deterring
animals from approaching fishing gear. For example, pingers
(acoustic harassments devices) that are used to alert cetaceans,
especially dolphins, to nets and other fishing gear (e.g., Barlow
et al. 2003) have been developed according to physiological and
anatomical hearing capacities of cetaceans (e.g., Tyack et al.
2003; Nowacek et al. 2007). Similarly, visual, olfactory, and
auditory cues can be used as determents to bycatch for sea
turtles approaching baited long-line gear (Southwood et al.
2008). Spectral capacity of sea turtles differs from most target
species, and visual deterrents have been developed and tested;
in particular, use of light sticks and LEDs show promise (Wang
and Swimmer 2007; Wang et al. 2007). Laboratory studies combined with field trials using both behavior and physiology (for
examples, see Southwood et al. 2008) have already yielded tools
to reduce bycatch.
Impacts of River Damming on Bimodally Respiring Turtles
The damming of river systems can significantly alter the physiochemical conditions, including loss of pool-riffle sequences,
reduced water flow, and increased water depths; these changes
typically culminate in altered temperature profiles and decreased aquatic oxygen levels. In Australia, many of the river
systems that are dammed are home to species of freshwater
turtles that can respire aquatically via cloacal respiration, including some species that can acquire a significant amount
(170%) of oxygen via this route (Priest and Franklin 2002;
Gordos et al. 2007). Studies looking at the diving behavior and
physiology of bimodally respiring turtles, such as the Mary
River turtle (Elusor macrurus) and the Fitzroy River turtle
(Rheodytes leukops), found that elevated temperatures and reduced oxygen levels reduce submergence time and that these
species have little capacity to acclimate to these conditions
(Gordos et al. 2006; Clark et al. 2008, 2009). For hatchlings,
this may lead to an elevated predation risk as a consequence
of increased surfacing. These findings have resulted in greater
scrutiny of the impacts of dams on these vulnerable species,
including the cancellation of the Traveston Dam on the Mary
River.
Impacts of Diffuse Pollutants on Atlantic Salmon
A recent joint physiological and behavioral study of the impact
of the herbicide atrazine (a contaminant that salmon are exposed to in the wild and that modifies parr-smolt transformation) on Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) smolts during their
transition from fresh to sea water (Moore et al. 2008) showed
that exposure to low but environmentally realistic levels of
atrazine in freshwater over a 72-h period significantly reduced
gill Na⫹-K⫹-ATPase activity. Subsequent transfer to 33‰ saltwater resulted in 100% mortality after 72 h. No mortality oc-
curred in control fish on transfer to saltwater. On the other
hand, tracking studies showed that both atrazine-treated and
control fish successfully migrated across freshwater, out of the
estuary, and into marine coastal waters over similar periods of
time (Moore et al. 2008). In the tracking component of this
study, fish were held in freshwater for 6 d after atrazine exposure
before being released to the wild, so it may be that the fish had
at least partially recovered from the physiological effects of
atrazine exposure, suggesting that its physiological effects can
be reversed. This approach of using both physiological and
behavioral studies, together with the development and application of life-history models, has been used to assess the potential impact of diffuse pollutants on salmon populations in
the United Kingdom, and the results form the basis of advice
to the government.
Opportunities Lost?
Conservation Physiology
A number of opportunities to improve conservation and management outcomes have been lost because physiological studies
have not fully incorporated behavioral approaches. For example, the oxygen limitation hypothesis proposed by Pörtner
and Farrell (2008) postulates that organismal performance (activity metabolism and survival) will be impaired in the face of
stressors such as climate change that reduce aerobic scope.
However, field studies that combine physiology and behavior
are needed that directly test this idea and evaluate potential
thermoregulatory responses to mediate such effects. Indeed,
some preliminary work on that topic (i.e., Mathes et al. 2010)
suggests that fish may seek thermal refuge in deep, cooler lake
waters when possible. This is not intended to be critical of the
oxygen limitation hypothesis; rather, to date, field studies that
incorporate behavioral and physiological aspects have been limited yet are needed to test the validity of this interesting
hypothesis.
Stress physiology is also particularly pertinent in this regard
(Tarlow and Blumstein 2007). Hundreds of studies have examined the glucocorticoid response of a variety of vertebrates
to stress (reviewed in Walker et al. 2005; Busch and Hayward
2009), yet there are few studies that have integrated behavioral
aspects. For example, recent work on the effects of ecotourism
on glucocorticoid responses of orangutans (Muehlenbein et al.
2012) focused solely on physiological endpoints, but we do not
know whether or how these physiological stresses translated
into fitness consequences. Tarlow and Blumstein (2007) noted
that there is no single optimal method to quantify the effects
of anthropogenic stressors on wildlife, emphasizing the potential benefits of integrating behavioral, physiological, and ultimately demographic endpoints. Given the direct interaction
between hormones and behavior and the potential for animals
to mediate stress responses through behavioral alterations, it
would seem sensible to study both simultaneously. There are
exceptions where both behavior and hormones were measured
simultaneously, often leading to unexpected results (e.g., Butler
et al. 2009).
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
8 Cooke, Blumstein, Buchholz, Caro, Fernández-Juricic, Franklin, Metcalfe, O’Connor, St. Clair, Sutherland, and Wikelski
Conservation Behavior
Behavior has been applied successfully to understanding the
success or failure of concerted conservation efforts where individuals are known, such as in captive breeding programs,
relocation efforts, and monitoring wild individuals in the field.
But here too, as with conservation physiology, there still appear
to be some lost opportunities. For example, Wielebnowski
(1998) noted the need to integrate physiology and behavior in
captive breeding of endangered fauna. Specific examples include the testing of enrichment for breeding giant pandas,
where physiology could be used to test whether management
techniques can reduce glucocorticoid stress hormones (Swaisgood et al. 2000). Another example would be characterizing
the feasibility of captive-breeding Strombidae conch, which focused on trying to breed animals in captivity without understanding environmental and physiological tolerances and nutritional needs of the animals (Shawl and Davis 2004). For some
threatened species, physiological data could aid in manipulating
sex ratios (such as the Australian brush turkey and various sea
turtles; e.g., Robertson et al. 2006). Another suite of examples
of where physiology and behavior could together inform management decisions relates to animal-environment interactions
and sensory physiology (Appleby and Hughes 1993). These
include understanding how individuals decide which habitats
to use (Cooke 2008) and the consequences of doing so (Huey
1991), where to settle, crossing gaps in fragmented habitats
(Fahrig 2007), and deterring individuals from unsuitable habitats (Blumstein and Fernández-Juricic 2010). For instance,
when using nest boxes to increase the breeding densities of a
given species, researchers rarely consider the sensory attractiveness of the boxes. Incorporating physiology by designing
nest boxes that are attractive to the visual system of the target
organism (e.g., arboreal marsupial, cavity nesting birds) yet do
not attract predators could lead to higher breeding success. The
same sensory principles can be applied to artificial stimuli that
managers develop to repel birds from a given area, such as
airport runways, airplanes, wind turbines, or windows (e.g.,
Blackwell et al. 2012).
One particular example of a lost opportunity stands out.
Physiology could be combined with behavior to address key
questions related to the effects of human disturbance on wildlife
populations. First, how can we promote the spatial and temporal coexistence of recreationists and wildlife within protected
areas? Addressing this problem has obvious educational benefits
and requires a good understanding of how different species
respond behaviorally to different levels of recreationists (Blumstein et al. 2005; Rodrı́guez-Prieto and Fernández-Juricic 2005).
Second, can human disturbance negatively affect populations
or species? This necessitates understanding more than behavioral responses of a species to a given source of disturbance
(e.g., visual, auditory). Buchholz and Hanlon (2012) reviewed
studies of wildlife disturbance and found that most of them
did not link behavioral change to fitness indicators (but see
Beale and Monagham 2004), even though a framework for
doing so exists (Gill et al. 1996; Houston and McNamara 1999;
Frid and Dill 2002). They noted that integrating physiological
measures, such as glucocorticoids or heart rate, into studies on
whether human disturbance harms animal populations would
make results (whether positive or negative) more believable and
thus better support effective decision making (e.g., Bisson et
al. 2008). Indeed, many behavioral studies alone would fail to
meet the evidentiary requirements in a legal context if a regulator were to attempt to press charges related to some form
of human disturbance. However, with a physiological understanding that is worked out well, behavioral indicators could
be robust indicators of disturbance.
Opportunities to Advance Fundamental Knowledge
If physiologists and behaviorists could work together to address
conservation problems, we believe that their disciplines would
thrive better as a result of the integration, potentially reinvigorating and advancing both disciplines. In recent years, there
have been more scientists combining physiological and behavioral techniques and theories (see Gilmour et al. 2005; Cabanac
2011), and conservation problems could serve as an opportunity to increase the level of integration further and would
help attract trainees. Possibilities for integration include (1) the
use of genomic physiology to understand physiological pathways and genomic connections between behavior and genes
(Ryder 2005) and (2) the integration of behavior and physiology
to yield measures or proxies of organismal fitness, such as
elevated cortisol being a marker for reproduction and fitness
in organisms subject to human activity.
In some ways, the rapid human-caused environmental
change serves as a large-scale experiment that provides opportunities to understand physiological and behavioral reactions
to environmental change (Carey 2007; Candolin and Wong
2012). Such work will also yield a better understanding of the
proximate and ultimate explanations behind how organisms
interact with each other and their physical environment, an
outstanding big question in ecology and one that demands
integration (May 1999). Referring to fundamental behavioral
questions, Owens (2006, p. 360) stated that “many of these
new [behavioral] questions are of a subtly different type and
require a more detailed understanding of the genetic and physiological mechanisms that underlay behavioral traits,” emphasizing the need for integration to advance fundamental scientific
questions. Similar perspective articles on the future of physiological ecology (Feder and Block 1991) and comparative physiology and biochemistry (Mangum and Hochachka 1998; Somero 2000) all identified the need and opportunity to further
embrace research outside of the laboratory, using new models
and, where relevant, integrating other techniques and disciplines, such as behavior. Collectively, these advances in fundamental knowledge would contribute to better predictive abilities in basic biology (e.g., developing predictive models of
mechanistic diversity that link to fitness) as well as integration
of information into large ecological and evolutionary models.
In terms of advancing fundamental knowledge, behavior and
physiology both require comparative data, yet disappearing spe-
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Physiology, Behavior, and Conservation 9
Figure 2. Adaption of the physiology–life history nexus (Ricklefs and Wikelski 2002) to incorporate Tinbergen’s concept that questions can
be approached in a complementary way using phylogenetic, ontogenetic, functional, and mechanistic approaches (Tinbergen 1963). This
framework emphasizes the connections among behavior, physiology, and population-level and evolutionary processes. A phylogenetic approach
can be used to understand variation in both behavior and physiology within and among species, while ontogenetic approaches can be used to
understand the influence of anthropogenic stressors in the developmental environment on behavioral and physiological phenotypes. Perhaps
the most common approach in conservation biology, a mechanistic approach can be used to understand the influence of anthropogenic stressors
in either the developmental or the immediate environment on physiological and behavioral aspects of performance. In this case, physiological
tools can also be used as a mechanistic tool to understand differences in behavioral performance. Finally, a functional approach to understanding
ultimate causes of variation in genotypes and phenotypes helps connect individual to evolutionary processes. Key to applying this framework
to conservation issues is to connect variation in performance to population-level processes whenever possible.
cies will increasingly prevent us from using the comparative
approach in physiology and behavior (Caro and Sherman
2011). For example, the gastric breeding frog Rheobatrachus
silus and Rheobatrachus villinus raised tadpoles in its stomach
but became extinct before the mechanism was identified. This
likely led to an irreversibly lost opportunity for treating peptic
ulcers, since these frogs secreted substances that inhibit acid
and pepsin secretions (Chivian and Bernstein 2008). Furthermore, as populations disappear, so do unique behaviors (Caro
and Sherman 2012). In other words, there is merit in behaviorists and physiologists contributing to conservation science
inasmuch as it also may help to ensure that they have the most
diverse suite of animals available for fundamental research.
Practical Challenges
Conceptual Challenges
Researchers tend to consider issues within a specific research
sphere and theoretical framework. The manner in which physiologists and behaviorists approach questions can be quite different. Behaviorists tend to work within the framework of Tinbergen’s four questions, even when addressing conservation
problems (Anthony and Blumstein 2000; Blumstein and Fernández-Juricic 2004; Berger-Tal et al. 2011). Yet this framework
provides an explicit entrée for the study of physiological mechanisms. While there is not a similar unifying theme or framework in physiology (or conservation physiology), the life history–physiology nexus (Ricklefs and Wikelski 2002; Young et
al. 2006; also see Spicer and Gaston 1999 for some discussion
of such integration) has strongly influenced many conservation
physiologists, and it provides an explicit entrée for the study
of the adaptive basis of some behavioral traits (fig. 2). Nonetheless, frameworks can be constraining.
Another conceptual challenge is that physiology and behavior
operate on different timescales, even though both are mechanisms underlying demography. Many physiological responses
occur much more slowly than (neurologically based) behaviors
(especially those related to seasonal acclimation, energetics,
growth, or reproductive preparedness), yet the consequences
of a seemingly ephemeral behavior (e.g., eating a toxic food,
winning a contest with a higher-ranking individual) can be long
lasting. There is a need for integrating these timescales.
These conceptual challenges would best be addressed by
training the next generation of integrative animal biologists not
only in the fundamentals of behavior and physiology but also
in their convergence and applicability to conservation. In reality, few academic programs cover behavior and physiology in
depth.
Methodological Challenges
Another significant challenge to the integration of behavior and
physiology is methodological. This is somewhat influenced by
the fact that for conservation problems, much of the research
needed is best done in the wild. Yet there are a growing number
of technological tools to integrate physiology and behavior in
field environments (see Costa and Sinervo 2004), particularly
related to biotelemetry and biologging, which incorporate var-
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
10 Cooke, Blumstein, Buchholz, Caro, Fernández-Juricic, Franklin, Metcalfe, O’Connor, St. Clair, Sutherland, and Wikelski
ious sensors (e.g., heart rate, temperature, pressure, acceleration; Cooke et al. 2004). Indeed, new technologies enable the
tracking of both behavior and physiology at the same temporal
scale and at a scale that is relevant to the animal. Unfortunately,
there are still many animals for which these tools cannot be
readily applied, given the small size of the animals (e.g., most
insects) or extreme environments (e.g., deep sea fishes).
Finding tools that themselves do not alter the behavior of
the animal being studied or impart physiological costs is a
common problem for behaviorists and physiologists and one
that influences data quality and ethical concerns (Cuthill 1991),
particularly when working with endangered animals (Putman
1995). Analytically, there can also be different expectations of
what good data are (e.g., treatment of outliers, biological significance) and the type of analyses involved. For example, and
in stark contrast to behavioral studies, there are few examples
of multivariate techniques or other advanced statistical approaches applied to physiological data aside from bioinformatics. Several books have been written on analyzing behavioral
data (e.g., Russo 2003; Blumstein and Daniel 2007; Martin and
Bateson 2007), and some animal behavior journals (e.g., Animal
Behaviour) provide authors with extensive guidelines of acceptable statistical practice. The integration between physiology
and behavior will increasingly require that researchers on both
sides acquire strong analytical skills and develop novel statistical
methods for a variety of data sets.
Communication and Institutional Challenges
There is still poor communication between behavioral and
physiological experts related to the different research traditions.
Joint meetings, integrative books and journals, and—most importantly—identifying questions or problems that simply demand integration (such as many animal conservation problems) could help to overcome these challenges. Communication
is increasing, and there have been several syntheses on the
behavior-physiology interface (albeit typically written by those
working more from physiological perspectives and in physiology journals, including Physiological and Biochemical Zoology;
see Gilmour et al. 2005); outlets such as Hormones and Behavior
and Physiology and Behavior are both positioned around the
interface and have been focused on fundamental research for
decades (albeit largely in a laboratory setting). Another barrier
is that funding structures do not overtly require integrating
input (knowledge and research capability) from managers, conservationists, ecologists, and behavioral and physiological biologists. A more interdisciplinary research approach where such
projects are encouraged would be useful.
Figure 3. Schematic diagram of the possible links between conservation, behavior, and physiology. We argue that there is inherent overlap
between behavior and physiology when either one is integrated with
conservation. A color version of this figure is available online.
servation outcomes (Caro 2007; Angeloni et al. 2008; Cooke
and O’Connor 2010; Cooke et al. 2013). This is particularly
the case for conservation physiology, which excels in defining
cause and effect relationships but often requires behavioral information related to how animals interact with their environment for results to be made relevant to the natural world
(Cooke et al. 2013). Clearly, an animal’s physiology modifies
its behavior and vice versa. Here we have highlighted some
examples where the two disciplines achieved meaningful conservation outcomes that benefited imperiled animal populations or improved resource management. Indeed, there are certainly examples of where physiology and behavior have been
integrated for some time to address basic and applied issues.
We also provided examples where there were opportunities lost
from applying physiology and behavior in isolation to conservation problems. There are many complementary aspects to
physiology and behavior (table 1) that collectively can lead to
conservation successes. Together, behavior and physiology can
generate meaningful data to support conservation actions, and
applied conservation problems are an opportunity to better
integrate and advance the fundamental disciplines of animal
physiology and behavior as well.
Acknowledgments
Conclusion
Although conservation physiology and conservation behavior
have been considered separate entities, there is merit in better
integration in regard to applied conservation problems from
both pragmatic and biological perspectives (fig. 3). Both disciplines struggle to extend their findings to meaningful con-
S.J.C. was supported by the Canada Research Chairs Program
and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of
Canada. C.M.O. was supported by an E. B. Eastburn Postdoctoral Fellowship from the Hamilton Community Foundation.
C.E.F. is supported by funding from the Australian Research
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Physiology, Behavior, and Conservation 11
Council. R.B. is supported by the National Science Foundation
(NSF) and the University of Mississippi. D.T.B. and E.F.-J. are
supported by the NSF. W.J.S. is funded by Arcadia.
Literature Cited
Alton L.A., C.R. White, R.S. Wilson, and C.E. Franklin. 2012.
The energetic cost of exposure to UV radiation for tadpoles
is greater when they live with predators. Funct Ecol 26:94–
103.
Alton L.A., R.S. Wilson, and C.E. Frankln. 2010. Risk of predation enhances the lethal effects of UV-B in amphibians.
Glob Change Biol 16:538–545.
———. 2011. Small increases in UV-B increase the susceptibility of tadpoles to predation. Proc R Soc B 278:2575–2583.
Angeloni L., K.R. Crooks, and D.T. Blumstein. 2010. Conservation and behavior: introduction. Pp. 377–381 in M.D.
Breed and J. Moore, eds. Encyclopedia of animal behavior.
Vol. 1. Academic Press, Oxford.
Angeloni L., M.A. Schlaepfer, J.J. Lawler, and K.R. Crooks. 2008.
A reassessment of the interface between conservation and
behaviour. Anim Behav 75:731–738.
Anthony L.L. and D.T. Blumstein. 2000. Integrating behaviour
into wildlife conservation: the multiple ways that behaviour
can reduce Ne. Biol Conserv 95:303–315.
Appleby M.C. and B.O. Hughes. 1993. The future of applied
ethology. Appl Anim Behav Sci 35:389–395.
Arnold S.J. 1983. Morphology, performance and fitness. Am
Zool 23:347–361.
Bancroft B.A., N.J. Baker, and A.R. Blaustein. 2008. A metaanalysis of the effects of ultraviolet B radiation and its synergistic interactions with pH, contaminants, and disease on
amphibian survival. Conserv Biol 22:987–996.
Barlow J. and G.A. Cameron. 2003. Field experiments show
that acoustic pingers reduce marine mammal bycatch in the
California drift gill net fishery. Mar Mamm Sci 19:265–283.
Beale C.M. and P. Monaghan. 2004. Human disturbance: people
as predation-free predators? J Appl Ecol 41:335–343.
Berger-Tal O., T. Polak, A. Oron, Y. Lubin, B.P. Kotler, and D.
Saltz. 2011. Integrating animal behavior and conservation
biology: a conceptual framework. Behav Ecol 22:236–239.
Bisson I.A., L.K. Butler, T.J. Hayden, L.M. Romero, and M.C.
Wikelski. 2008. No energetic cost of anthropogenic disturbance in a songbird. Proc R Soc B 276:961–969.
Blackwell B.F., T.L. DeVault, T.W. Seamans, S.L. Lima, P. Baumhardt, and E. Fernández-Juricic. 2012. Exploiting avian vision
with aircraft lighting to reduce bird strikes. J Appl Ecol 49:
758–766.
Blaustein A.R., S.S. Gervasi, P.T. Johnson, J.T. Hoverman, L.K.
Belden, P.W. Bradley, and G.Y. Xie. 2012. Ecophysiology meets
conservation: understanding the role of disease in amphibian
population declines. Philos Trans R Soc B 367:1688–1707.
Block B.A. 2005. Physiological ecology in the 21st century:
advancements in biologging science. Integr Comp Biol 45:
305–320.
Blumstein D.T. 2001. Book review: Behaviour and conservation
(ed. by L.M. Gosling and W.J. Sutherland). J Wildl Manag
65:601–603.
———. 2007. Developing an evolutionary ecology of fear: how
life history and natural history traits affect disturbance tolerance in birds. Anim Behav 71:389–399.
Blumstein D.T. and J.C. Daniel. 2007. Quantifying behavior the
JWatcher way. Sinauer, Sunderland, MA.
Blumstein D.T. and E. Fernández-Juricic. 2004. The emergence
of conservation behavior. Conserv Biol 18:1175–1177.
———. 2010. A primer of conservation behavior. Sinauer, Sunderland, MA.
Blumstein D.T., E. Fernández-Juricic, P.A. Zollner, and S.C.
Garity. 2005. Inter-specific variation in avian responses to
human disturbance. J Appl Ecol 42:943–953.
Bottrill M.C., L.N. Joseph, J. Carwardine, M. Bode, C. Cook,
E.T. Game, H. Grantham, et al. 2009. Finite conservation funds
mean triage is unavoidable. Trends Ecol Evol 24:183–184.
Brouwer A., A.J. Murk, and J.H. Koeman. 1990. Biochemical
and physiological approaches in ecotoxicology. Funct Ecol
4:275–281.
Buchholz R. 2007. Behavioural biology: an effective and relevant
conservation tool. Trends Ecol Evol 22:401–407.
Buchholz R. and E. Hanlon. 2012. Ecotourism, wildlife management, and behavioral biologists: changing minds for conservation. Pp. 234–249 in B. Wong and U. Candolin, eds.
Behavioural responses to a changing world: mechanisms and
consequences. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Busch D.S. and L.S. Hayward. 2009. Stress in a conservation
context: a discussion of glucocorticoid actions and how levels
change with conservation-relevant variables. Biol Conserv
142:2844–2854.
Butchart S.H.M., A.J. Stattersfield, J. Baillie, L.A. Bennun, S.N.
Stuart, H.R. Akcakaya, C. Hilton-Taylor, and G.M. Mace.
2005. Using Red List Indices to measure progress towards
the 2010 target and beyond. Philos Trans R Soc B 360:255–
268.
Butchart S.H.M., M. Walpole, B. Collen, A. van Strien, J.P.W.
Scharlemann, R.E.A. Almond, J.E.M. Baillie, et al. 2010.
Global biodiversity: indicators of recent declines. Science 328:
1164–1168.
Butler L.K., I.A. Bisson, T.J. Hayden, M. Wikelski, and L.M.
Romero. 2009. Adrenocortical responses to offspringdirected threats in two open-nesting birds. Gen Comp Endocrinol 162:313–318.
Cabanac M. 2011. The place of behavior in physiology. Compr
Physiol 1523–1536.
Candolin U. and B.B.M. Wong. 2012. Behavioural responses
to a changing world: mechanisms and consequences. Oxford
University Press, Oxford.
Cardinale B.J., J.E. Duffy, A. Gonzalez, D.U. Hooper, C. Perrings, P. Venail, A. Narwani, et al. 2012. Biodiversity loss and
its impact on humanity. Nature 486:59–67.
Carey C. 2005. How physiological methods and concepts can be
useful in conservation biology. Integr Comp Biol 45:4–11.
Carey H. 2007. One physiology. Physiologist 50:48–54.
Caro T. 1998. The significance of behavioral ecology for con-
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
12 Cooke, Blumstein, Buchholz, Caro, Fernández-Juricic, Franklin, Metcalfe, O’Connor, St. Clair, Sutherland, and Wikelski
servation biology. Pp. 3–26 in T. Caro, ed. Behavioral ecology
and conservation biology. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
———. 1999. The behaviour-conservation interface. Trends
Ecol Evol 14:366–369.
———. 2007. Behavior and conservation: a bridge too far?
Trends Ecol Evol 22:394–400.
Caro T., J. Eadie, and A. Sih. 2005. Use of substitute species
in conservation biology. Conserv Biol 19:1821–1826.
Caro T. and P.W. Sherman. 2011. Endangered species and a
threatened discipline: behavioural ecology. Trends Ecol Evol
26:111–118.
———. 2012. Vanishing behaviors. Conserv Lett 5:159–166.
Carson R. 1962. Silent spring. Houghton Mifflin, Boston.
Chivian E. and A. Bernstein. 2008. Sustaining life: how our
health depends on biodiversity. Oxford University Press, New
York.
Clark N.J., M.A. Gordos, and C.E. Franklin. 2008. Thermal
plasticity of diving behavior, aquatic respiration, and locomotor performance in the Mary River turtle Elusor macrurus.
Physiol Biochem Zool 81:301–309.
———. 2009. Implications of river damming: the influence of
aquatic hypoxia on the diving physiology and behaviour of
the endangered Mary River turtle. Anim Conserv 12:147–154.
Clemmons J.R. and R. Buchholz. 1997. Linking conservation
and behavior. Pp. 3–22 in J.R. Clemmons and R. Buchholz,
eds. Behavioral approaches to conservation in the wild. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Cooke S.J. 2008. Biotelemetry and biologging in endangered
species research and animal conservation: relevance to regional, national, and IUCN Red List threat assessments. Endang Species Res 4:165–185.
Cooke S.J., S.G. Hinch, M.R. Donaldson, T.D. Clark, E.J. Eliason, G.T. Crossin, G.D. Raby, et al. 2012. Conservation
physiology in practice: how physiological knowledge has improved our ability to sustainably manage Pacific salmon during up-river migration. Philos Trans R Soc B 367:1757–1769.
Cooke S.J., S.G. Hinch, M. Wikelski, R.D. Andrews, T.G. Wolcott, and P.J. Butler. 2004. Biotelemetry: a mechanistic approach to ecology. Trends Ecol Evol 19:334–343.
Cooke S.J. and C.M. O’Connor. 2010. Making conservation
physiology relevant to policy makers and conservation practitioners. Conserv Lett 3:159–166.
Cooke S.J., L. Sack, C.E. Franklin, A.P. Farrell, J. Beardall, M.
Wikelski, and S.L. Chown. 2013. What is conservation physiology? perspectives on an increasingly integrated and essential science. Conserv Physiol 1, doi:10.1093/conphys/cot001.
Cooke S.J. and C.D. Suski. 2008. Ecological restoration and
physiology: an overdue integration. BioScience 58:957–968.
Costa D.P., and B. Sinervo. 2004. Field physiology: physiological
insights from animals in nature. Annu Rev Physiol 66:209–
238.
Cottam Y. 2010. Characteristics of green rimu fruit that might
trigger breeding in kakapo. PhD diss. Massey University,
Palmerston North.
Cottam Y., D.V. Merton, and W. Hendriks. 2006. Nutrient com-
position of the diet of parent-raised kakapo nestlings. Notornis 53:90–99.
Curio E. 1996. Conservation needs ethology. Trends Ecol Evol
11:260–263.
Cuthill I. 1991. Field experiments in animal behaviour: methods
and ethics. Anim Behav 42:1007–1014.
Dulvy N.K., J.K. Baum, S. Clarke, L.J. Compagno, E. Cortés,
A. Domingo, A., S. Fordham, et al. 2008. You can swim but
you can’t hide: the global status and conservation of oceanic
pelagic sharks and rays. Aquat Conserv 18:459–482.
Eliason E.J., T.D. Clark, M.J. Hague, L.M. Hanson, Z.S. Gallagher, K.M. Jeffries, M.K. Gale, D.A. Patterson, S.G. Hinch,
and A.P. Farrell. 2011. Differences in thermal tolerance
among sockeye salmon populations. Science 332:109–112.
Elliott G.P., D.V. Merton, and P.W. Jansen. 2001. Intensive management of a critically endangered species: the kakapo. Biol
Conserv 99:121–133.
Fahrig L. 2007. Non-optimal animal movement in humanaltered landscapes. Funct Ecol 21:1003–1015.
Farrell A.P., S.G. Hinch, S.J. Cooke, D.A. Patterson, G.T. Crossin, M. Lapointe, and M.T. Mathes. 2008. Pacific salmon in
hot water: applying aerobic scope models and biotelemetry
to predict the success of spawning migrations. Physiol
Biochem Zool 81:697–709.
Feder M.E. and B.A. Block. 1991. On the future of animal
physiological ecology. Funct Ecol 5:136–144.
Fernández-Juricic E., M.P. Venier, D. Renison, and D.T. Blumstein. 2005. Sensitivity of wildlife to spatial patterns of recreationist behavior: a critical assessment of minimum approaching distances and buffer areas for grassland birds. Biol
Conserv 125:225–235.
Festa-Bianchet M. and M. Apollonio, eds. 2003. Animal behavior and wildlife conservation. Island, Washington, DC.
Fidler A.E., S. Zwart, R. Pharis, S.B. Lawrence, G. Elliott, D.
Merton, and K.P. McNatty. 2000. Screening foods of the
endangered kakapo parrot (Strigops habroptilus) for oestrogenic activity using a recombinant yeast bioassay. Br Poult
Sci 41:48–49.
Fossi M.C., S. Casini, and L. Marsili. 1999. Nondestructive biomarkers of exposure to endocrine-disrupting chemicals in endangered species of wildlife. Chemosphere 39:1273–1285.
Frid A. and L.M. Dill. 2002. Human-caused disturbance stimuli
as a form of predation risk. Conserv Ecol 6:11.
Gale M.K., S.G. Hinch, E.J. Eliason, S.J. Cooke, and D.A. Patterson. 2011. Physiological impairment of adult sockeye
salmon in fresh water after simulated capture-and-release
across a range of temperatures. Fish Res 112:85–95.
Gill J.A., W.J. Sutherland, and A.R. Watkinson. 1996. A method
to quantify the effects of human disturbance on animal populations. J Appl Ecol 33:786–792.
Gilmour K.M., R.W. Wilson, and K.A. Sloman. 2005. The integration of behaviour into comparative physiology. Physiol
Biochem Zool 78:669–678.
Gordos M.A., M. Hamann, C.S. Schauble, C.J. Limpus, and
C.E. Franklin. 2007. Diving behaviour of Elseya albagula
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Physiology, Behavior, and Conservation 13
from a naturally flowing and hydrologically altered habitat.
J Zool 272:458–469.
Gordos M.A., C.J. Limpus, and C.E. Franklin. 2006. Response
of heart rate and cloacal ventilation in the bimodally respiring freshwater turtle, Rheodytes leukops to experimental
changes in aquatic Po2. J Comp Physiol B 176:65–73.
Gosling L.M. and W.J. Sutherland, eds. 2000. Behaviour and
conservation. Vol. 2. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Hansen L.J. and M.L. Johnson. 1999a. Conservation and toxicology: integrating the disciplines. Conserv Biol 13:1225–1227.
———. 1999b. Conservation and toxicology: the need to integrate the disciplines. Environ Toxicol Chem 18:2121–2122.
Harper G.A., G.P. Elliott, D.K. Eason, and R.J. Moorhouse.
2006. What triggers nesting of kakapo (Strigops habroptilus)?
Notornis 53:160–163.
Hedrick P.W. 2001. Conservation genetics: where are we now?
Trends Ecol Evol 16:629–636.
Hinch S.G., S.J. Cooke, A.P. Farrell, K.M. Miller, M. Lapointe,
and D.A. Patterson. 2012. Dead fish swimming: a review of
research on the early migration and high premature mortality
in adult Fraser River sockeye salmon Oncorhynchus nerka. J
Fish Biol 81:576–599.
Hoffmann M., C. Hilton-Taylor, A. Angulo, M. Böhm, T.M.
Brooks, S.H.M. Butchart, K.E. Carpenter, et al. 2010. The
impact of conservation on the status of the world’s vertebrates. Science 330:1503–1509.
Hooper D.U., E.C. Adair, B.J. Cardinale, J.E. Byrnes, B.A.
Hungate, K.L. Matulich, A. Gonzalez, J.E. Duffy, L. Gamfeldt,
and M.I. O’Connor. 2012. A global synthesis reveals biodiversity loss as a major driver of ecosystem change. Nature
486:105–108.
Houston A.I. and J.M. McNamara. 1999. Models of adaptive
behaviour: an approach based on state. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Houston D., K. Mcinnes, G. Elliott, D. Eason, R. Moorhouse,
and J. Cockrem. 2007. The use of a nutritional supplement
to improve egg production in the endangered kakapo. Biol
Conserv 138:248–255.
Huey R.B. 1991. Physiological consequences of habitat selection. Am Nat 137:91–115.
Huey R.B., M.R. Kearney, A. Krockenberger, J.A. Holtum, M.
Jess, and S.E. Williams. 2012. Predicting organismal vulnerability to climate warming: roles of behaviour, physiology
and adaptation. Philos Trans R Soc B 367:1665–1679.
Kareiva P. and M. Marvier. 2012. What is conservation science?
BioScience 62:962–969.
Mangum C.P. and P.W. Hochachka. 1998. New directions in
comparative physiology and biochemistry: mechanisms, adaptations, and evolution. Physiol Biochem Zool 71:471–484.
Martin P. and P. Bateson. 2007. Measuring behaviour: an introductory guide. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Mathes M.T., S.G. Hinch, S.J. Cooke, G.T. Crossin, D.A. Patterson, A.G. Lotto, and A.P. Farrell. 2010. Effect of water
temperature, timing, physiological condition and lake thermal refugia on migrating adult Weaver Creek sockeye salmon
(Oncorhynchus nerka). Can J Fish Aquat Sci 67:70–84.
May R. 1999. Unanswered questions in ecology. Philos Trans
R Soc B 354:1951–1959.
Metcalfe J.D., W.J.F. Le Quesne, W.W.L. Cheung, and D.A.
Righton. 2012. Conservation physiology for applied management of marine fish: an overview with perspectives on
the role and value of telemetry. Philos Trans R Soc B 367:
1746–1756.
Moore A., D. Cotter, V. Quayle, G. Rogan, R. Poole, N. Lower,
and L. Privitera. 2008. The impact of a pesticide on the physiology and behaviour of hatchery-reared Atlantic salmon,
Salmo salar, smolts during the transition from fresh water to
the marine environment. Fish Manag Ecol 15:385–392.
Muehlenbein M.P., M. Ancrenaz, R. Sakong, L. Ambu, S. Prall,
G. Fuller, and M.A. Raghanti. 2012. Ape conservation physiology: fecal glucocorticoid responses in wild pongo Pygmaeus
morio following human visitation. PLoS ONE 7:e33357.
Müllner A., E.K. Linsenmair, and M. Wikelski. 2004. Exposure
to ecotourism reduces survival and affects stress response in
hoatzin chicks (Opisthocomus hoazin). Biol Conserv 118:549–
558.
Nowacek D.P., L.H. Thorne, D.W. Johnston, and P.L. Tyack.
2007. Responses of cetaceans to anthropogenic noise. Mamm
Rev 37:81–115.
Owens I.P. 2006. Where is behavioural ecology going? Trends
Ecol Evol 21:356–361.
Palen W.J. and D.E. Schindler. 2010. Water clarity, maternal
behavior, and physiology combine to eliminate UV radiation
risk to amphibians in a montane landscape. Proc Natl Acad
Sci USA 107:9701–9706.
Palen W.J., C.E. Williamson, A.A. Clauser, and D.E. Schindler.
2005. Impact of UV-B exposure on amphibian embryos: linking species physiology and oviposition behaviour. Philos
Trans R Soc B 272:1227–1234.
Parmigiani S., P. Palanza, and F.S. vom Saal. 1998. Ethotoxicology: an evolutionary approach to the study of environmental endocrine-disrupting chemicals. Toxicol Ind Health
14:333–339.
Pollock C.G. 2001. Silent spring revisited: a 21st-century look
at the effect of pesticides on wildlife. J Avian Med Surg 15:
50–53.
Pörtner H.O. and A.P. Farrell. 2008. Physiology and climate
change. Science 322:690–692.
Priest T.E. and C.E. Franklin. 2002. Effect of water temperature
and oxygen levels on the diving behaviour of two freshwater
turtles: Rheodytes leukops and Emydura macquarii. J Herpetol
36:555–561.
Putman R.J. 1995. Ethical considerations and animal welfare
in ecological field studies. Biodivers Conserv 4:903–915.
Reed J.M. 1999. The role of behavior in recent avian extinctions
and endangerments. Conserv Biol 13:232–241.
Richmond J.Q., A.E. Savage, K.R. Zamudio, and E.B. Rosenblum. 2009. Toward immunogenetic studies of amphibian
chytridiomycosis: linking innate and acquired immunity.
BioScience 59:311–320.
Ricklefs R.E. and M. Wikelski. 2002. The physiology/life-history
nexus. Trends Ecol Evol 17:462–468.
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
14 Cooke, Blumstein, Buchholz, Caro, Fernández-Juricic, Franklin, Metcalfe, O’Connor, St. Clair, Sutherland, and Wikelski
Robertson B.C., G.P. Elliott, D.K. Eason, M.N. Clout, and N.J.
Gemmell. 2006. Sex allocation theory aids species conservation. Biol Lett 2:229–231.
Rodrı́guez-Prieto I. and E. Fernández-Juricic. 2005. Effects of
direct human disturbance on the endemic Iberian frog Rana
iberica at individual and population levels. Biol Conserv 123:
1–9.
Ropert-Coudert Y. and R.P. Wilson. 2005. Trends and perspectives in animal-attached remote sensing. Front Ecol Environ 3:437–444.
Russo R. 2003. Statistics for the behavioural sciences: an introduction. Psychology, New York, NY.
Ryder O.A. 2005. Conservation genomics: applying whole genome studies to species conservation efforts. Cytogenet Genome Res 108:6–15.
Schipper J., J.S. Chanson, F. Chiozza, N.A. Cox, M. Hoffmann,
V. Katariya, J. Lamoreux, et al. 2008. The status of the world’s
land and marine mammals: diversity, threat, and knowledge.
Science 322:225–230.
Seebacher F. and C.E. Franklin. 2012. Determining environmental causes of biological effects: the need for a mechanistic
physiological dimension in conservation biology. Philos
Trans R Soc B 367:1607–1614.
Shawl A.L. and M. Davis. 2004. Captive breeding behavior of
four Strombidae conch. J Shellfish Res 23:157–164.
Somero G.N. 2000. Unity in diversity: a perspective on the
methods, contributions, and future of comparative physiology. Annu Rev Physiol 62:927–937.
Soulé M.E. 1985. What is conservation biology? BioScience 35:
727–734.
———. 1986. Conservation biology: the science of scarcity and
diversity. Sinauer, Sunderland, MA.
Southwood A., K. Fritsches, R. Brill, and Y. Swimmer. 2008.
Sound, chemical, and light detection in sea turtles and pelagic
fishes: sensory-based approaches to bycatch reduction in
longline fisheries. Endang Species Res 5:225–238.
Spicer J.I. and K.J. Gaston. 1999. Physiological diversity and
its ecological implications. Blackwell Science, Oxford.
Strier K. 1997. Behavioral ecology and conservation biology of
primates and other animals. Adv Study Behav 26:101–158.
Stuart S.N., J.S. Chanson, N.A. Cox, B.E. Young, A.S. Rodrigues,
D.L. Fischman, and R.W. Waller. 2004. Status and trends of
amphibian declines and extinctions worldwide. Science 306:
1783–1786.
Sutherland W.J. 1998. The importance of behavioural studies
in conservation biology. Anim Behav 56:801–809.
Swaisgood R.R., D.G. Lindburg, X. Zhou, and M.A. Owen.
2000. The effects of sex, reproductive condition and context
on discrimination of conspecific odours by giant pandas.
Anim Behav 60:227–237.
Tarlow E.M. and D.T. Blumstein. 2007. Evaluating methods to
quantify anthropogenic stressors on wild animals. Appl Anim
Behav Sci 102:429–451.
Tinbergen N. 1963. On aims and methods of ethology. Z Tierpsychol 20:410–433.
Tracy C.R., K.E. Nussear, T.C. Esque, K. Dean-Bradley, L.A.
DeFalco, K.T. Castle, L.C. Zimmerman, R.E. Espinoza, and
A.M. Barber. 2006. The importance of physiological ecology
in conservation biology. Integr Comp Biol 46:1191–1205.
Trivers R.L. and D.E. Willard. 1973. Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring. Science 179:
90–92.
Tyack P., J. Gordon, and D. Thompson. 2003. Controlled exposure experiments to determine the effects of noise on marine mammals. Mar Technol Soc J 37:41–53.
Waite D.W., P. Deines, and M.W. Taylor. 2012. Gut microbiome
of the critically endangered New Zealand parrot, the kakapo
(Strigops habroptilus). PLoS ONE 7:e35803.
Walker B.G., J.C. Wingfield, and P.D. Boersma. 2005. Age and
food deprivation affects expression of the glucocorticosteroid
stress response in Magellanic penguin (Spheniscus magellanicus) chicks. Physiol Biochem Zool 78:78–89.
Wang J.H., L.C. Boles, B. Higgins, and K.J. Lohmann. 2007.
Behavioral responses of sea turtles to lightsticks used in longline fisheries. Anim Conserv 10:176–182.
Wang J.H. and Y. Swimmer. 2007. Preliminary field trials in
Baja California, Mexico with lightsticks and shark shapes.
Pp. 18–19 in Y. Swimmer and J.H. Wang, eds. 2006 sea turtle
and pelagic fish sensory workshop, September 12–13, 2006.
National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration Technical
Memorandum NMFS-PIFSC-12.
Ward M.P. and S. Schlossberg. 2004. Conspecific attraction and
the conservation of territorial songbirds. Conserv Biol 18:
519–525.
Weimerskirch H., F. Bonadonna, F. Bailleul, G. Mabille, G.
Dell’Omo, and H.P. Lipp. 2002. GPS tracking of foraging
albatrosses. Science 295:1259.
Weisenberger M.E., P.R. Krausman, M.C. Wallace, D.W. De
Young, and O.E. Maughan. 1996. Effects of simulated jet
aircraft noise on heart rate and behavior of desert ungulates.
J Wildl Manag 60:52–61.
Wielebnowski N. 1998. Contributions of behavioral studies to
captive management and breeding of rare and endangered
mammals. Pp. 130–162 in T.M. Caro, ed. Behavioral ecology
and conservation biology. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Wikelski M. and S.J. Cooke. 2006. Conservation physiology.
Trends Ecol Evol 21:38–46.
Wildt D.E. and C. Wemmer. 1999. Sex and wildlife: the role of
reproductive science in conservation. Biodivers Conserv 8:
965–976.
Wingfield J.C., K. Hunt, C. Breuner, K. Dunlap, G.S. Fowler,
L. Freed, and J. Lepson. 1997. Environmental stress, field
endocrinology, and conservation biology. Pp. 95–131 in J.R.
Clemmons and R. Buchholz, eds. Behavioral approaches to
conservation in the wild. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge.
Young J.L., Z. Bornik, M. Marcotte, K. Charlie, G.N. Wagner,
S.G. Hinch, and S.J. Cooke. 2006. Integrating physiology and
life history to improve fisheries management and conservation. Fish Fish 7:262–283.
This content downloaded from 128.97.244.96 on Sat, 25 Jan 2014 20:44:50 PM
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions