The pervasive influence of effort on neuropsychological tests

advertisement
Effort effects
1
The pervasive influence of effort on neuropsychological tests
Paul Green, Ph.D.
Neurobehavioural Associates, Edmonton, Canada
Communications should be addressed to Dr. Paul Green, 210 17010 103 Ave.,
Edmonton, Alberta, Canada, T5S 1K7 or via paulgreen@shaw.ca or
drpgreen@telus.net or www.wordmemorytest.com
Key words: Brain injury, effort, symptom validity, symptom exaggeration,
neuropsychological.
1
Effort effects
2
Abstract
Although it is intuitively obvious that people exerting a full effort on a test will score
higher than people making less effort, it is not obvious to what degree poor effort will
affect neuropsychological test scores. Nor is it self-evident how well scores on an effort
test based on memory will predict scores on non-memory tests. In this study, effort was
measured in 1,307 consecutive outpatients using the Word Memory Test (WMT, Green
& Astner, 1995, Green, Allen & Astner, 1996, Green, 2003). The mean WMT effort
scores were divided into six ranges, from satisfactory (91% to 100% correct) to very low
(50% correct or less). The tables show the mean scores on many commonly used
neuropsychological tests for each range of effort on the WMT. As effort decreases, scores
on most neuropsychological tests decrease significantly and systematically. In this
sample, the variable of effort has more impact on test scores than severe traumatic brain
injury.
2
Effort effects
3
The pervasive influence of effort on neuropsychological tests
Reitan (1974) has described a neuropsychological test as one whose scores are
differentially affected by brain disease, rather than environmental factors. In a study of
people with brain injuries, it was found that the greater the severity of traumatic brain
injury, based on time to follow commands, the lower were the Halstead-Reitan battery
test scores (Dikmen, Machamer, Winn & Temkin, 1995). These findings were replicated
by Rohling, Meyers & Millis (2003), using a different test battery, confirming the
differential sensitivity of neuropsychological tests to various levels of brain injury
severity. However, neuropsychological test scores are also affected by environmental
variables, one of which is the presence of incentives to perform well or poorly on testing.
A person capable of recalling ten words from a list could, in principle, decide to recall
only four words, thereby introducing major error into test results. It is an empirical
question whether brain injuries influence neuropsychological test scores more than
motivational factors or vice versa. Another question is whether only some
neuropsychological tests are affected by effort, as suggested by Nies and Sweet (1994) or
whether varying effort is a general phenomenon affecting most or all such tests. To
answer these questions, the effects of effort and brain injury on neuropsychological test
scores must be quantified and compared with each other, using data from actual patients.
It has been reported that the suppression of test scores by poor effort can be
greater than the effects of a severe traumatic brain injury in people claiming
compensation.
Green,
Rohling,
Lees-Haley
&
Allen
(2001)
converted
43
neuropsychological test scores to Z-scores relative to external norms in 904 outpatients. It
3
Effort effects
4
was found that effort explained approximately 50% of the variance in the
neuropsychological test scores, which was far more than that explained by brain injury
severity, education or age. The mean composite neuropsychological test score was 0.5
standard deviations below the normal mean in patients with the most severe brain
injuries, who passed the WMT effort subtests. Yet, in the patients with the most minor
head injuries, who failed the WMT, the mean composite neuropsychological test score
was 1.5 standard deviations below the normal mean. The same degree of suppression of
test scores was observed in patients of all diagnostic groups, who failed the WMT effort
measures. Thus, the effects of effort on neuropsychological tests can overshadow the
effects of severe traumatic brain injury, producing the misleading appearance of cognitive
deficits in cases with poor effort and potentially obscuring real group differences.
The acceptance of spurious deficits in neuropsychological test results as
representing valid impairment can have serious implications. Theories of brain disease
may be altered, depending on whether or not effort is measured. For many years, for
example, it was thought that neuropsychological deficits were greater in some cases of
psychogenic-nonepileptic-seizures (PNES) than in actual epileptic patients. The deficits
in PNES were thought to be indicative of presumed but undemonstrated brain disease.
However, Williamson, Drane, Stroup, Miller, & Holmes (2004) recently discovered that
more than half of PNES patients failed effort testing with the WMT. In comparison, the
WMT failure rate was very low in the patients with intractable seizures, who were due for
brain surgery. 50% of the variance in neuropsychological test scores was explainable by
fluctuating effort. The results suggested that, as a group, the PNES patients’
neuropsychological test data were invalid due to inadequate effort, such that they could
4
Effort effects
5
not be used to infer the presence or severity of underlying brain disease. These results
throw doubt upon the validity of test data from past studies of PNES patients, which did
not measure effort.
In a recent study of cases of mild head injury with compensation claims, it was
found that 47% of the variance in a summary score for the Halstead-Reitan battery (the
GNDS) was explained by effort measured by the Test of Memory Malingering
(Constantinou, Bauer, Ashendorf, Fisher & McCaffrey, 2005). Thus, three separate
studies have shown that effort explains approximately 50% of the variance in
neuropsychological test batteries. In two of these samples, there were financial incentives
for symptom exaggeration because they were involved in making compensation claims.
In the PNES study, however, the assessments were conducted to determine if brain
surgery was needed for epilepsy, although external incentives to appear impaired could
not be ruled out.
Important decisions rest on neuropsychological test data and, therefore, it is of
fundamental importance to understand further the extent to which test scores are affected
by diminishing effort. The tables in this paper provide information on neuropsychological
test results from 1,307 outpatients, who were clinically assessed in the private practice of
the writer. In most cases, there were financial incentives for disability, whether from
medical disability insurance, Workers’ Compensation or personal injury litigation. Scores
from twenty-three neuropsychological tests are tabulated according to ranges of effort
measured by the computerized WMT. The tables show how scores on tests of memory,
problem solving, fluency, manual skills, attention, and many other abilities decrease
systematically as effort declines, and to what degree.
5
Effort effects
6
Method
Participants
The sample of 1,307 cases, all of whom were tested by the current author,
included the 904 patients from the previous study of Green et al (2001), as well as 403
additional consecutive cases. There were 668 patients with head injuries, some with less
than one day of post-traumatic amnesia (n=520) and others with one day or more of posttraumatic amnesia (n=148). All were tested at least one month after the injury. 86% of
cases were tested at least four months post injury, the median being 15 months. There
were 130 neurological patients, suffering from a variety of brain disorders, including
strokes, aneurysms, multiple sclerosis, tumor, epilepsy, herpes simplex encephalitis, Von
Hippel-Lindau disease, hypoxic event, abscess, venous thrombosis and dorsal midbrain
hemorrhage. There were 126 patients with major depression, 23 with anxiety-based
disorders, 13 with bipolar disorder and 10 with other psychotic illnesses. Finally, testing
included 86 patients with orthopedic injuries, 34 with chronic fatigue syndrome, 78 with
pain disorder or fibromyalgia and 139 with various other conditions, such as alcoholism
or dementia. Excluded from the study were an additional 50 cases, given only the oral
Word Memory Test (WMT, Green & Astner, 1995) for various reasons, such as
blindness.
Referrals for assessment were made by the Workers’ Compensation Board in 41%
of cases, by insurance companies handling medical disability claims in 33% of cases and
by lawyers representing the plaintiff or the defense in personal injury claims in 18% of
cases. In a further 8% of cases, there was no direct involvement with a financial claim,
6
Effort effects
7
although, in principle, some might later be able to make claims. For example, a large
employer referred 40 people (3% of all cases) with questions about cognitive impairment
and emotional status impacting work performance. In the latter group, the very few
classified as disabled would go on to receive a medical disability pension but most were
highly motivated to carry on working. Some cases were privately referred for various
reasons, such as evaluation of suspected dementia.
Neuropsychological tests
1,307 consecutive cases were given tests of a comprehensive range of abilities
and the numbers of cases taking each test are noted in the tables. Most of the tests used
will be very familiar to neuropsychologists, such as the California Verbal Learning Test
(Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 1987) and tests referenced in the norms manual of
Heaton, Grant, & Matthews (1991), including the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Category
Test, Trail Making Test A & B, Thurstone Word Fluency Test, Grooved Pegboard, Hand
Dynamometer, Finger Tapping Test and Finger Tip Number Writing test (Reitan, 1969).
Other tests included Warrington’s Recognition Memory Tests for Words and Faces
(Warrington, 1984), the Ruff Figural Fluency Test (Ruff, 1988), Gorham’s Proverbs Test
(Gorham, 1956), Digit Span & Visual Memory Span subtests of the Wechsler Memory
Scale-Revised (1987), the Continuous Visual Memory Test (Trahan & Larrabee, 1988),
Rey Complex Figure Test (Meyers & Meyers, 1995), Benton’s Judgment of Line
Orientation Test and Benton’s Visual Form Discrimination Test (Benton, Hamsher,
Varney & Spreen, 1983). Intelligence was measured with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Scale-Revised
(Wechsler,
1981)
or
its
close
equivalent,
the
computerized
Multidimensional Aptitude Battery (Jackson, 1998). Some tests will be less familiar, such
7
Effort effects
8
as the Story Recall Test (Green & Kramar, 1983), the Emotional Perception Test (EPT;
Green & Seversen, 1986) and the Alberta Smell Test (AST; Green & Iverson, 2001;
Green, Iverson, Rohling & Gervais, 2003). The Story Recall Test involves immediate
recall of five short stories ranging from 10 to 25 items in length and recall of the stories
after a half hour delay. The Emotional Perception Test requires the person to judge the
emotions in the tone of voice of 45 sentences, each said in one of 5 emotions. The
Alberta Smell Test involves sniffing a scented felt marker while one nostril is closed and
then selecting the name of the odor from one of eight written on a sheet (e.g. orange,
lemon, mint). The score is the number correct out of ten and each nostril is tested
separately. The latter test was found to be more sensitive to the effects of a severe
traumatic brain injury than any of the conventional neuropsychological tests studied
(Green, Rohling, Iverson & Gervais, 2003).
Effort testing
To measure both effort and verbal memory, all cases were given the computerized
Word Memory Test (WMT) as part of 1.5 days of clinical neuropsychological testing,
conducted between 1996 and 2004. Cooperative clients often completed testing in only
one day but many were slow to perform. The first two out of six subtests of the WMT are
the immediate recognition (IR) and delayed recognition (DR) subtests, in which words
from a previously presented list must be identified, when presented individually with a
non-list foil word. They are the primary effort subtests. A third measure is derived from
the consistency of performance from the first to the second subtest.
The mean of the three WMT effort measures (IR, DR and Consistency) was
calculated for each person. The scores were broken down into six ranges, where 91% to
8
Effort effects
9
100% defines the top range. It may be noted that the mean score from healthy adults
tested with the WMT in a study by Suhr & Gunstadt (2005) was 99.5% correct (SD=1.6).
In the study by Gorissen, Sanz and Schmand (2005), the healthy adult mean was 96%
(SD=3), which similar to the healthy adult mean of 97.8% (SD=3) listed in the WMT
Windows program (Green, 2003). Hence, nearly all healthy adults would be expected to
score in the range 91% to 100% correct on the WMT effort subtests. The mean for
neurological patients in the Gorissen et al (2005) study was 93% (SD=10). Scores of 81%
to 90% make up the second range, scores in which may be described as “marginal or
failing”, although 42% of these scores were above the conservative cut-offs
recommended in the WMT test manual (Green, 2003). Successively lower ranges of
WMT effort scores were 71% to 80%, 61% to 70%, 51% to 60% and 50% or below.
In the tables, scores on other tests are presented for people scoring in each of six
ranges on their mean WMT effort scores. Other effort tests employed included the
Amsterdam Short Term Memory Test (Schmand, Lindeboom, Schagen, Heijt, Koene,&
Hamburger, 1998) and Computerized Assessment of Response Bias (CARB, Conder,
Allen & Cox, 1992).
Results
Every attempt was made to obtain optimal performance from patients and they
were all warned in advance that full effort was necessary to produce valid results.
Nevertheless, in this sample of 1,307 outpatients, 403 cases (31%) failed the WMT using
the clinically recommended cut-offs (82.5% or lower on IR, DR or consistency). In those
who failed the WMT, the mean WMT effort scores ranged from 88.3% to 36.6%, with a
mean WMT effort score of 71% (SD 13). In the 904 patients who passed the WMT, the
9
Effort effects 10
mean effort score was 96.2% (SD=3.5), which is almost identical to the value of 96%
(SD=3) found in healthy adults in the study of Gorissen, Sanz and Schmand (2005), using
the Spanish and French translations of the WMT.
Simulator profile found in those failing the WMT
It is not plausible that the profiles produced by the WMT failures were valid (i.e.
that they were reliable test scores, reflecting good effort) because there were important
internal inconsistencies between scores on the WMT subtests, similar to those found in
known simulators. The mean WMT scores of 25 patients with early dementia tested by
Brockhaus and Merten (2004) are contained within the WMT Windows program and are
shown in Figure 1. On the very easy WMT subtests (IR and DR), the 403 WMT failures
in the current study scored 74% (SD=16) and 71% (SD=16), whereas the latter dementia
patients scored higher than that (respectively, 85%, SD=11, and 82%, SD=15, see Figure
1). If valid, this would mean that those who failed the WMT in the current study were
more impaired than the dementia patients, which is not plausible, considering the
diagnoses and ages of these patients. For example, 176 of the WMT failures were cases
of mild head injury, with a mean GCS of 14.7 and a mean age of only 41 years.
Also, if they were making a valid effort but scoring lower than people with
dementia on the easy subtests, we would expect the WMT failures to show more
impairment than the dementia patients on the more difficult WMT subtests but just the
opposite was found. Whereas they scored lower than dementia patients on the very easy
WMT subtests, the WMT failures systematically scored higher than dementia patients on
the most difficult WMT subtests (Figure 1). On the MC subtest (Multiple Choice), the
WMT failures scored a mean of 51% (SD=18), compared with 43% (SD=20) in the
10
Effort effects 11
dementia patients. On PA (Paired Associate recall) the failures scored a mean of 47%
(SD=17), compared with 34% (SD=15) in the dementia patients. On FR (Free Recall of
the word list), the failures scored a mean of 29% (SD=13), compared with 21% (SD=16)
in the dementia patients.
----------- Insert figure 1 here --------------Such a pattern of (a) lower scores than dementia patients on easy WMT subtests
but (b) higher scores than dementia patients on harder WMT subtests is precisely the
pattern observed in studies of simulators (i.e. volunteers who were asked to fake memory
impairment). For example, highly educated volunteers who were asked to simulate
memory impairment scored means of only 71% and 67% correct on the WMT IR and DR
subtests and, therefore, lower than dementia patients on the easy subtests (Green, LeesHaley & Allen, 2002). Yet their mean scores on the harder subtests (MC=47%, PA=48%
& FR=35%) were all higher than those of the dementia patients discussed above (Figure
1). The marked similarity between the WMT profiles in those failing WMT effort tests
clinically and those of known simulators suggests that those failing the WMT clinically
were making a poor effort, if not actually trying to simulate memory impairment.
Neuropsychological test scores in WMT failures
Cases passing the WMT effort subtests were compared with cases failing the
WMT in terms of their mean scores on each of the neuropsychological tests shown in
tables 1 to 21. The differences were all strongly in the direction of poorer performances
in those failing WMT. The differences were significant at .0001 in all comparisons using
one-way ANOVA, with the exception of Grooved Pegboard left hand (p<.014), Finger
Tip Number Writing left hand (p<.006) and right hand (p<.001) and one non-significant
11
Effort effects 12
result on Ruff Figural Fluency perseverative responses (p<.4). The pervasive influence of
effort on almost all neuropsychological tests may be readily seen in the tables. The CVLT
will be used below to illustrate how effort affects test scores.
---------------- Table 1 about here please
-----------------
The effects of effort on CVLT memory test scores
The data in table 1 show a very strong correlation between scores on the effort
subtests of the WMT and CVLT recall scores. This relationship was duplicated in a very
large sample studied independently by Dr. Roger Gervais (table 5). The data suggest that
effort measured by the WMT recognition memory subtests (IR and DR) is an underlying
variable which strongly influences CVLT recall scores. The alternative explanation,
which is that the WMT effort scores correlate so strongly with CVLT scores in such large
samples because they tap memory ability, can be dismissed as implausible and contrary
to large amounts of data. Firstly, a strong relationship between WMT effort subtests and
other test scores may be seen in all of the tables in this paper but most of the tests are not
memory tests. Secondly, the quintessential measure of ability is intelligence but children
of higher intelligence scored no higher on WMT recognition subtests than children of
lower intelligence (Green & Flaro, 2003). Age invariably has an effect on ability tests in
children. However, in the latter study, children tested clinically and aged between 7 and
10 years did not score significantly lower on the WMT effort subtests than children aged
11 to 18 years. The children scored the same as adults seeking custody of their children,
consistent with the likelihood that WMT recognition subtests (IR & DR) primarily
measure effort, rather than ability.
12
Effort effects 13
The latter argument is further supported by greater scores on WMT effort subtests
in a group of people with severe brain injuries than in a group with very mild head
injuries, an effect which is best explained by poor effort in the mild group (Green,
Iverson & Allen, 1999). Similar effects were found in compensation seeking cases using
another very easy effort test, called the Computerized Assessment of Response Bias
(CARB). Those with the most severe brain injuries scored higher on the CARB than
those with the most minor head injuries (Green & Iverson, 2001).
The WMT recognition memory subtests are extremely easy. In the original group
of 40 normal adult controls (Iverson, Green & Gervais, 1999), the median score was
100% on both IR and DR. Healthy adult volunteers in the study of Tan, Slick, Strauss and
Hultsch (2004) scored a mean of 99.5% correct on WMT IR (SD=2.3) and 99.5% correct
on WMT DR (SD=0.7). Mentally retarded adults in an institution in Germany scored a
mean of 96% (SD=5) on WMT DR (Brockhaus & Merten, 2004). Hence, someone who
fails WMT by scoring 82.5% or lower is scoring considerably lower than mentally
retarded adults. Similarly, almost perfect scores on WMT effort subtests are obtained
from adults or children with neurological diseases or severe traumatic brain injury, if they
make an effort (Green, Lees-Haley & Allen, 2002, Green, 2003). Neurological patients
with impaired verbal memory on the CVLT scored approximately 96% correct on WMT
recognition subtests, which was no lower than those with normal verbal memory (Green,
Lees-Haley & Allen, 2002). A group of neurological patients tested by Gorissen et al
(2005) scored a mean of 95% correct (SD 9.4) on the WMT Delayed Recognition subtest.
Children with various clinical conditions, such as fetal alcohol syndrome, obtained mean
13
Effort effects 14
scores well above 90% correct on WMT recognition subtests and the children tested
clinically performed just as well as adults seeking custody of their children (Green &
Flaro, 2003). These studies show that the WMT recognition subtests are very insensitive
to differences in levels of ability.
On the other hand, the WMT effort subtests are very sensitive to poor effort.
Highly educated volunteer simulators, mainly psychologists and physicians, were asked
to fake memory impairment but to avoid being detected. They scored only 71% on WMT
IR and 67% correct on the WMT DR subtest (Green, Lees-Haley & Allen, 2002). In an
independent study by Tan, Slick, Strauss & Hultsch (2004), volunteers who were asked to
simulate brain injury obtained mean scores of 65% correct on WMT IR (SD=18) and
64% correct (SD=18) on WMT DR. Using a score of 82.5% or lower on WMT IR, DR or
consistency to define poor effort, the WMT was found to be 100% accurate in classifying
good versus poor effort in the latter study. Similar reports of 99% to 100% accuracy in
classification of simulators versus good effort cases come from three other studies using
the WMT in German (Brockhaus & Merten, 2004), Turkish (Brockhaus, Peker, & Fritze,
2004) and Russian (Tydecks & Merten, personal communication).
The results of the latter studies support the notion that WMT recognition subtests
are very sensitive to poor effort but virtually insensitive to differences in ability. WMT
recognition scores are usually suggestive of poor effort if they are substantially lower
than those obtained by mentally retarded adults (96%, SD 5), by neurologically impaired
patients, who are known to have impaired verbal memory (95%, SD 5) or by children
with fetal alcohol syndrome (96%, SD 6). Scores on WMT effort subtests, which are
substantially lower than the mean scores from the latter groups suggest low effort,
14
Effort effects 15
especially where the clinical history would not suggest impairment as great as that of the
latter groups (e.g. cases of mild head injury in otherwise healthy adults). Exceptions
would include some people with dementia and children with less than a grade three
reading level.
In the current study, 6 cases of dementia, all in need of full time supervision and
guardianship, were given the computerized WMT and their mean scores on WMT were
IR, 85.4% (SD 14), DR 82% (SD 21) and consistency 82% (SD 17). While this is a very
small sample of dementia patients, the latter scores are similar to the WMT data from
adults with early dementia tested by Brockhaus & Merten (2004) using the German
WMT (Green, 2003). It is clear that some patients with dementia are unable to pass the
WMT IR and DR subtests even if they try their best to do so. To assist in the
interpretation of WMT scores in people who might genuinely be unable to pass the WMT
recognition subtests, the WMT Windows program contains mean scores and standard
deviations from all WMT subtests in groups of early and late dementia, as well as data
from 59 other comparison groups, with a total of more than 3,000 cases.
---------------- Tables 2 & 3 here please
-----------------
Effort explains more test variance than brain injury severity
In 421 cases of traumatic brain injury, for whom Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS)
scores were available, the correlation was only 0.17 between GCS and the mean of 7
CVLT scores (trials 1 & 5, short and long delayed free and cued recall and recognition
hits). Yet, in the same cases, the mean WMT effort scores correlated with mean CVLT
scores at 0.58. Hence, GCS explained 3% of the variance in CVLT scores, whereas effort
15
Effort effects 16
explained more than ten times the variance (34%). In non-brain injured cases, the
correlation between WMT effort and CVLT scores was 0.65.
Members of the head injury sample in table 4 all passed the WMT effort subtests.
Please note that the mean CVLT SD Free Recall score in the group with a GCS of 3 was
9.1, compared with a mean of 10.2 in the group with a GCS of 15. In contrast, the 745
cases scoring in the top range of WMT effort in table 1 obtained mean scores of 10.7 on
CVLT short delayed free recall, while those in the lowest effort range scored 4.4. Based
on these data, although brain injury has some impact, CVLT scores are influenced far
more by the effort being made by the person taking the test than they are by severity of
brain injury.
---------------- Table 4 here please
-----------------
Abnormal brain scan versus effort
The correlation between CVLT mean scores and the presence or absence of
abnormal CT or MRI brain scans in 456 cases of head injury was effectively zero (r=.01).
In 321 traumatically brain injured or neurological patients with normal CT or MRI brain
scan results, the mean of the CVLT short and long delayed free recall scores was 9.3 (SD
3.7). This was not significantly different from the mean score of 8.9 (SD 3.7) in 314 cases
with abnormal brain scans. However, poor effort obscured a true group difference. After
removing cases who failed the WMT, the mean CVLT free recall score in those with a
normal brain scan was 11.1 (SD 3.1, n=174) compared with 9.9 (SD 3.2, n=220) in those
with an abnormal scan (F 12.9, 1,392, p<.001). The mean difference of 1.2 points is
significant but it is minor in comparison with the effects of effort. In the whole sample,
16
Effort effects 17
the mean CVLT free recall score for those who passed the WMT (n=831) was 10.8 (SD
3.1), compared with 7.3 (SD 3.4) in those who failed the WMT effort subtests (n=370; F
303.5, 1,1199, p<.0001). The mean relative decrement of 3.5 CVLT free recall points
associated with failing the WMT is almost three times larger than the effect of a CT or
MRI brain abnormality.
Mild versus severe brain injury
Paradoxically, the mean WMT effort score of 87% (SD=14) in the mild head
injury cases (median GCS=15, median PTA=0, n=469) was significantly lower than the
mean score of 92% (SD=9.6) from cases with the most severe brain injuries (median
GCS=7, median PTA=336 hours, n=148; F=15, 1, 615, p<.0001). Using the usual cutoffs, 37.5% of the mild group failed the WMT but the failure rate in the cases with more
severe brain injuries was only 21.6%. In fact, the highest failure rate of 47% was in 223
people with mild head injuries, who were given a CT scan, which showed no brain
abnormality. These results are understandable in terms of poorer effort in the mild group.
They would not be explainable as an effect of actual impairment due to brain tissue injury
because those with the most severe head injuries had the highest scores on the WMT
effort subtests.
Given that effort is lower in mild versus severe brain injury groups, on average, it
is necessary to control for effort when comparing these groups on other tests. In order to
study group differences due to brain injury severity, we need to select groups of people
who are all making a full effort on tests. In fact, there was no significant difference
between mild and severe brain injury patients in their mean CVLT scores before WMT
failures were dropped. Including only those with presumed valid test results (i.e. passing
17
Effort effects 18
the WMT), there were 293 cases of mild head injury, compared with 103 cases of
moderate to severe brain injury. The mild group had a median GCS of 15 and a median
PTA of zero, whereas the more severe brain injury group had a median GCS of 9 and a
median PTA of 204 hours.
The mean CVLT free recall score was 10.8 (sd 3.0) in the mild group compared
with 9.9 (sd 3.1) in the more severe injury group, a mean difference of 0.9 points, which
is statistically significant (p<.02). On the other hand, it is quite small in comparison with
the difference of 6.3 points in short-delayed free recall scores between those in the top
range of effort on WMT in table 1 (mean 10.7, SD 3.2) and those in the lowest range
(mean 4.4, SD 2.6). It is important to note that, in the mild head injury group who failed
the WMT effort subtests, the mean CVLT short delay free recall score was 7.4 (SD=3).
Therefore, the lowest CVLT recall scores were found not in the most severe brain injury
group but, on the contrary, in the group with the least severe brain injuries, who showed
poor effort.
Trail Making Test and Category Test in cases of mild versus severe brain injury
Similar findings were obtained when comparing the above mild and severe brain
injury groups on other tests, such as the Trail Making Test and the Category Test. There
was only a 20-second difference on Trail Making B between the groups with mild versus
severe brain injuries noted above, who passed WMT. Their mean scores were 69 and 89
seconds, respectively (SD=28 & 61). Yet, 314 cases who failed the WMT took 132
seconds (SD 106) on Trail Making B, which was 43 seconds longer than the severe brain
injuries and 54 seconds longer than the mean for all 733 patients who passed the WMT
(77.7 seconds, SD=48). Those in the top effort range in table 9 took 76 seconds on Trail
18
Effort effects 19
Making B, whereas those in the bottom effort range took 160 seconds (i.e. 84 seconds
longer). The mild head injury patients who failed the WMT took a mean of 128 seconds
to complete Trail Making B, which is considerably worse than the severe brain injury
patients passing WMT.
On the Category Test, the mild head injury group passing WMT made 51 errors
(SD 27), compared with the more severe brain injuries, with 59 errors (SD 31). This is a
significant difference in the expected direction but 8 points is a small difference
compared with the 33 point spread of Category Test errors as a function of effort shown
in table 16 (55 to 88). Also, it is small compared with the average effect of failing the
WMT. In 743 cases, who passed the WMT, the mean Category Test error score was 56
(SD 29) but in the 264 cases who failed the WMT, the mean error score was 74 (SD 30).
The latter mean difference due to effort (18 errors) is more than twice as large as the
difference between mild and severe brain injuries (8 errors). The mean Category Test
error score in mild head injuries who failed the WMT was 72 errors (SD=28), which is
greater than that of the above severe brain injury patients who passed the WMT (59
errors, SD=31).
The degree to which effort affected neuropsychological test scores in the current
study was so much greater than the degree to which brain injury affected these scores that
it would be prudent to measure effort in anyone given the CVLT, the Category Test, the
Trail Making Test or other neuropsychological tests. If we observe an impaired score on
any of these tests, one hypothesis to rule out is that poor effort explains the low score.
---------------- Table 5 here please -----------------
19
Effort effects 20
Independent replication
In table 5, the results are shown from an independent sample of 678 outpatients,
who were given the CVLT as part of psychological assessments by Dr. Roger Gervais in
Edmonton, Canada. They were predominantly sent for vocational assessment or general
psychological assessment after a work-related injury, with diagnoses including major
depression, orthopedic injury, PTSD, and various other conditions. The Gervais sample
contained very few cases with a primary diagnosis of head injury or neurological disease.
It may be seen that the Gervais sample also shows steady decreases in CVLT recall
scores as effort on the WMT decreases, strongly replicating the findings with the current
independent sample of 1,307 cases.
---------------- Table 6 here please
-----------------
The effects of effort on other neuropsychological test scores
In table 6, scores on Warrington’s Recognition Memory Tests for words and faces
are strongly related to WMT measured effort [(IR+DR+Consistency)/3]. The mean WMT
effort score correlated at r=0.73 with Warrington’s RMT for Words (n=465) and at
r=0.52 with Warrington’s RMT for Faces (n=1,025). Also affected by effort are
immediate and delayed story recall scores (table 7), memory on Rey’s Complex Figure
Test (table 8) and memory on the Continuous Visual Memory Test (table 21).
---------------- Tables 7 and 8 here please
---------------
20
Effort effects 21
The effects of effort are as clear on non-memory tests as they are on memory
tests, as shown in tables 9 to 21. Times taken to complete Trail Making A and B increase
steadily as WMT effort scores decrease (Table 9). Performance IQ and Verbal IQ scores
decline as effort declines (table 10). Even manual test scores reduce in a predictable way
as effort decreases, as shown in tables 11 to 14 (Finger Tip Number Writing, Grip
Strength, Finger Tapping Speed & Grooved Pegboard). In people with severe brain
injuries, who passed WMT, the mean right hand finger tap score was 49.6 (SD=7),
whereas in those with mild head injuries, who failed WMT, the mean score was 42.6
(SD=12). Once again, effort has more effect than brain injury.
---------------- Tables 9 to 16 about here please
---------------
Abstraction and problem solving tests are no exception to the general rule. The
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and Category Test are very susceptible to the influences of
effort (tables 15 & 16). Digit Span, Visual Memory Span, Thurstone’s Word Fluency and
Ruff Figural Fluency are also strongly affected by effort, as measured by the WMT
(tables 17 & 18). Scores on tests as diverse as proverb interpretation, judgment of
emotion in tone of voice, judgment of the angles of lines, visual memory and the ability
to identify odors are significantly affected by effort, as measured by the WMT (tables 19,
20 & 21). Although not shown in the tables, scores on WRAT reading, spelling and
arithmetic and on the PASAT were all significantly correlated with WMT effort scores.
---------------- Tables 17 to 21 about here please
---------------
21
Effort effects 22
Table 22 shows that effort, as measured by the WMT, is strongly linked with
effort measured by other symptom validity tests. The mean WMT effort score correlated
at 0.61 with CARB, 0.6 with the Amsterdam and 0.6 with the 21-Item Test. In the
Gervais sample shown in table 5, the correlation between the mean WMT effort score
and the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM trial two) was 0.68. WMT effort scores
were also significantly negatively correlated with all symptom self-rating scales,
including the SCL90-R positive symptom total (-0.26), Beck Depression Inventory (0.24), the MMPI-2 F scale (-0.22), the MMPI-2 D scale (-0.24) and the Memory
Complaints Inventory (-0.4).
WMT memory subtest scores by range of effort
The mean WMT DR score of those in the second effort range from the top was
88% and, of those in this range, 58% of cases failed the WMT using the standard clinical
criteria. In table 23, we can see that the group scoring in the second range on WMT effort
subtests obtained a mean score of 66.9% on WMT MC (Multiple Choice). When an MC
score that low occurs, the WMT Windows program (Green, 2003) issues a “caution”
because, in most patients, such a score would be too low to be valid. A score of 66.9% on
MC would be 4.25 standard deviations below the mean of 95.4% correct (SD 6.7) from
normal adult controls listed in the WMT Windows test manual and program (Green,
2003). Neurological patients, selected because they had impaired memory on the CVLT,
scored 81% (SD=11) on MC and a group with severe brain injuries and an average GCS
of 5 scored 88.2% correct (SD=18.4) on MC. Similarly, those who scored in the range
81% to 90% on the WMT effort measures also scored 61% on the Paired Associates
subtest, which is 3.3 standard deviations below the mean from normal adult controls
22
Effort effects 23
(table 24). An MC score of 66.9% or a PA score of 61% could be valid in a case of
dementia or in someone with a left medial temporal lobe brain tumor or lobectomy.
However, the vast majority of cases in the current study with WMT effort scores in the
second range from the top did not have dementia and did not need 24 hours a day care.
Hence, their mean scores of 66.9% on MC and 61% on PA would not be considered
valid.
When those in the top WMT effort range were compared with those in the second
range on all neuropsychological tests shown in the tables, the mean differences were
significant at 0.005 or lower in all cases, except for grip strength, grooved pegboard and
finger tip number writing. For example, the mean Trail Making B score was 76 seconds
in the top effort range but 105 seconds in the second range (table 9). To put that number
in perspective, people with head injuries and up to six days of coma took an average of
only 89 seconds on Trail Making B in the study of Dikmen et al (1995). Those scoring in
the second effort range from the top in table 1 scored a mean of 8.3 on CVLT short
delayed free recall, whereas the more severe brain injury group discussed earlier scored
9.9 (sd 3.1). This adds further support to the conclusion that mean WMT scores in the
second range imply poor effort, sufficient to affect most other test scores.
It is not surprising that mean scores of 81% to 90% on WMT effort subtests are
linked with declines in other test scores, when the scores of the following groups are
considered:- 1) Children with fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS) scored a mean of 95.5% (SD
5.8) on the Delayed Recognition (DR) subtest of the WMT (Green, & Flaro, 2003); 2)
Adults with neurological diseases, selected for having impaired verbal memory, scored a
mean of 95% (SD 5.5) on WMT DR (Green, 2003); 3) Mentally retarded adults scored a
23
Effort effects 24
mean of 95% (SD 5) on the oral form of the WMT (Brockhaus & Merten, 2004); 4)
Adults with severe brain injuries and a mean GCS of 5 obtained a mean score of 96.6%
(SD 4) on WMT Delayed Recognition (Green, 2003).
Decline in test scores expressed in standard deviations
In table 24, it is evident that, on the eight neuropsychological tests shown, there
was an average drop in scores of half a standard deviation, in patients with WMT scores
in the second range from the top. Those scoring in the fourth effort range (mean
WMT=67%) showed a mean drop in neuropsychological test scores of 0.85 standard
deviations compared with the top effort group. Those scoring in the bottom effort range
showed an average drop of approximately two standard deviations.
For comparison with the effects of brain injuries on similar test scores, it may be
noted that, in cases with 7 to 13 days loss of consciousness studied by Dikmen et al
(1995), the mean deficit in Halstead-Reitan test scores compared with the normal adult
mean was 0.68 standard deviations (Rohling, Meyers & Millis, 2003). In those with more
than 28 days loss of consciousness (or time to follow commands), the mean overall
deficit in neuropsychological test scores was 2.3 standard deviations. Thus, the effects of
effort on test scores in the current study are roughly comparable to the reported effects of
severe brain injuries.
It may be noted also that the deficits reported by Dikmen et al (1995) were not
corrected for effort. No effort tests were employed. The possibility must be considered
that some of their cases of brain injury probably scored lower than they were actually
capable of achieving, magnifying the apparent effects of a severe brain injury on
neuropsychological test scores. Ideally, in similar studies in future, effort would be
24
Effort effects 25
measured objectively, allowing the effects of brain injury on neuropsychological test
scores to be separated from the effects of poor effort. Rohling et al (2003) did exclude
some suspected cases of poor effort, although not necessarily all of them. Perhaps this
partly explains why the mean deficit on many tests in their group with more than 28 days
of coma was 1.8 standard deviations, compared with 2.3 in the Dikmen et al study.
Sample of mild head injuries with very low effort scores
There were 48 cases in the current study with a mild head injury, no brain scan
abnormality and a median GCS of 15, who scored below 70% on WMT effort subtests.
Their mean WMT IR and DR scores were 58% and 56%. Yet, in the most severe brain
injury group, with PTA of one day or more and with an abnormal brain scan, there were
only five cases scoring lower than 70%. A WMT effort score of 56% is approximately 40
percentage points lower than the mean from adults with severe brain injuries, mentally
retarded adults or children with FAS. People who were specifically asked to fake memory
impairment on the WMT for experimental purposes and institutionalized patients
suffering from advanced dementia scored higher than the latter mild head injury patients
(Iverson, Green & Gervais, 1999, Brockhaus & Merten, 2004, Green, 2003). The mean
CVLT short delayed free recall score in the latter cases of mild head injury was 6 out of
16 (see table 1). Their mean CVLT recognition hits score was 10 (table 5). Their mean
Trail Making B score was 146 seconds (table 9) and they scored only 19 out of 30 on
Benton’s Judgment of Line Orientation test (table 21). These scores are lower than in the
more severe brain injury group making an effort and almost certainly do not provide a
valid reflection of the true capabilities of these 48 cases of mild head injury.
25
Effort effects 26
Discussion
Using the clinically recommended cut-offs, 30.8% of 1,307 cases in this study
failed the WMT effort subtests. There was a substantially higher failure rate on WMT in
the cases with mild head injury than in those with severe brain injuries. Also, the mean
WMT effort score was significantly lower in the mild than in the severe brain injury
group in this study. In those who failed WMT, the mean effort score was lower than that
from patients institutionalized with dementia. It was far lower than testable mentally
retarded adults performed on the WMT (Brockhaus & Merten, 2004, Green, 2003). These
findings are most consistent with the fact that the WMT effort subtests primarily measure
effort.
The tables show clearly that most neuropsychological test scores were
progressively reduced, to a degree corresponding with the level of effort indicated by the
WMT. When brain injury effects were weighed directly against effort effects in the
current study, the scales invariably tipped in the direction of effort having the greater
influence on test scores. It has been shown above that, within a large sample of patients,
scores on the CVLT, Trail Making Test and Category Test were suppressed more by
effort than by severe brain injury. On each of the latter tests, the patients with the most
mild head injuries, who failed the WMT effort subtests, scored much lower than those
with severe brain injuries who passed the WMT. The same conclusion holds true for most
of the other tests shown in the tables. In a single case, it might be argued that, despite
having suffered only a mild head injury, a certain unfortunate patient was the rare
exception and had suffered more brain impairment than most cases of severe brain injury.
However, it would be illogical to apply such an argument to the latter group data.
26
Effort effects 27
The average suppression of neuropsychological test scores by poor effort, as
shown in table 24, was of the same order of magnitude as reported in groups with various
degrees of brain injury severity, up to and including people who had not responded to
commands for more than 28 days. Rohling et al. (2003) reported that average
neuropsychological test scores were suppressed by about two standard deviations in the
group with the most severe brain injuries. The current tables show that poor effort can
suppress test scores to the same degree (table 24). The neuropsychological test scores of
people with mild head injuries, making a poor effort, were typically lower in the current
study than those of people with at least moderate to severe traumatic brain injuries,
making a full effort. This is the opposite of what would be expected if the test scores
were primarily affected by brain injury. The sensitivity of neuropsychological tests to
brain injury can thus be nullified by poor effort.
To the extent that these findings generalize to other samples, the results indicate
that objective effort testing is necessary to rule out deficits in test scores arising from
poor effort, as opposed to brain dysfunction. The data support the position of the National
Academy of Neuropsychology (Bush, Ruff, Troster, Barth, Koffler, Pliskin, Reynolds
and Silver, 2005) that effort needs to be measured in cases involved in any form of
compensation claim, including people who are already receiving compensation payments.
The results also suggest that, even if poor effort is not suspected, effort is such a powerful
variable that the objective measurement of effort is advisable. Assuming that we know
when effort is good without measuring it is a risky proposition. For many years,
neuropsychological studies of patients with PNES were performed without any attention
to effort measurement. However, when the assumption of good effort was put to the test
27
Effort effects 28
objectively, more than half of all such patients failed the WMT effort subtests, while
most of the actual seizure patients passed the WMT (Williamson et al., 2003). In the
latter study, if the WMT was failed, the odds were 20 to 1 that the patient was a case of
PNES and not a case of seizure disorder. Parents seeking custody of their children would
be expected to be motivated to perform well. Nevertheless, two out of 29 cases failed the
WMT (Green & Flaro, 2003) and, on questioning, they both admitted that they had
changed their minds and did not want the responsibility of having their children back in
their care. We might ask how many volunteers for normative studies make a full effort on
testing? Perhaps most of them do so but the honest answer is that we do not know
because effort was never measured objectively in past normative studies.
By omitting effort testing in clinical cases or group studies, we are assuming that
effort is uniformly good in all participants. That assumption may now be questioned
because poor effort in this study had a greater effect on test scores than severe brain
injury. Even when WMT effort scores were in the second range from the top (81% to
90%), the tables showed a uniform reduction in all neuropsychological test scores
compared with those in the top effort range. Except in dementia, we have good reason to
doubt the validity of neuropsychological data when a person scores below the cut-offs on
the WMT effort subtests. As in the case of the mild head injury subgroup, whose mean
WMT effort scores were below 70%, the logic is simple. If a person who is not demented
scores much worse on extremely easy tests than people with dementia, severe brain
injuries or mental retardation, then that person’s effort is insufficient to produce valid
data.
28
Effort effects 29
Rarely, if ever, have psychologists argued that subjective judgment, based on no
objective criterion, is superior to the use of methods based on objective measurement.
However, some neuropsychologists argue that they are able to detect incomplete effort by
casual observation alone and that clinical judgment is sufficient to identify poor effort. If
that were true, we would have known for many years that effort is a continuous variable,
as shown in the tables, but we did not know. If clinical intuition alone were sufficient, we
would have known for a long time that effort can affect neuropsychological test scores
more powerfully than severe brain injury but this has not been part of our collective
knowledge until very recently. If clinical judgment and effort testing with the WMT were
equivalent, we would probably all agree that there is insufficient effort to produce valid
test results in a substantial proportion of all compensation and disability cases. However,
those who rely on clinical judgment invariably argue that poor effort is so rare that there
is no need for objective tests of effort. Yet the same clinicians will often pay attention to
variables with far less substantial effects on test scores than effort, such as education, age,
mood or diagnosis. The National Academy of Neuropsychology Policy and Planning
Committee has now stepped into the debate and has published a position paper, in which
it is made clear that it is necessary to use objective methods for determining the amount
of effort applied to neuropsychological tests (Bush et al., 2005).
The relationships between effort and neuropsychological test scores, shown in
tables 1 to 24, are clear but they were neither obvious nor self-evident until the data were
gathered over an eight-year period. Although unexpected, the key finding is that
neuropsychological test scores in outpatients similar to those of the current study are
affected at least as much by effort as by brain injury. To most neuropsychologists, this is
29
Effort effects 30
initially a troubling conclusion because it gives us reason to pause and reconsider the
results of many past studies and clinical assessments, which did not involve effort testing.
As the PNES study by Williamson et al. (2003) illustrates, we need to revisit past studies,
in which it was assumed that effort was satisfactory in all patients.
In future, it is reasonable to expect that neuropsychologists, who do not measure
effort objectively risk failing to recognize the presence of poor effort and, hence, will
underestimate its impact on test scores. Low scores could easily be misinterpreted as a
sign of biologically based brain impairment, when the real cause is poor effort. Genuine
impairment in many brain-injured people could be overlooked because their
neuropsychological deficits seem minor compared with the very low test scores of those
with deficient effort. On the other hand, it is now possible to measure effort objectively.
We are therefore able to measure and control a major nuisance variable that affects our
data more than brain injury, impeding our understanding of the true effects of brain
disease. By removing data contaminated by poor effort, we are now able to see more
clearly than ever before the ways in which neuropsychological test scores actually reflect
underlying brain dysfunction.
30
Effort effects 31
References
Benton, A.L., Hamsher, K.de S., Varney, N.R. & Spreen, O. (1983) Contributions
to Neuropsychological Assessment, Oxford University Press, N.Y.
Brockhaus, R. & Merten, T. (2004), "Neuropsychologische Diagnostik
suboptimalen Leistungsverhaltens mit dem Word Memory Test", Nervenarzt,
75, (9), 882-887.
Brockhaus, R., Peker, Ö, Fritze, E. (2003): Testing effort in Turkish-speaking
subjects: Validation of a translation of the Word Memory Test (WMT).
Poster at annual convention of Gesellschaft der Neuropsychologie in Berlin.
Bush, S., Ruff, R., Troster, A., Barth, J., Koffler, S., Pliskin, N., Reynolds, C. &
Silver, C. (2005) Symptom Validity Assessment: Practice issues and medical necessity
(NAN Policy and Planning Committee). Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 419426.
Constantinou,
McCaffrey,
effort
R.J.
measures
M.,
(2005).
Bauer,
Is
indicative
poor
of
L.,
Ashendorf,
L.,
performance
on
generalized
poor
Fisher,
recognition
J.
&
memory
performance
on
neuropsychological tasks? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 2, 191-198
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. (1987). The California Verbal
Learning Test: Research Edition. New York: The Psychological Corporation.
Dikmen, S.S., Machamer, J.E., Winn, H.R., & Temkin, N.R. (1995).
Neuropsychological outcome at one year post head injury. Neuropsychology, 9, 80-90.
31
Effort effects 32
Gervais, R.O., Russell, A.S., Green, P., Allen, M., Ferrari, R. & Pieschl, S D.
(2001) Effort testing in patients with fibromyalgia and disability incentives. Journal of
Rheumatology, 28, 1892-1899.
Gorham, D.R., (1956) Proverb Interpretation Test, Psychological Test Specialists
(out of print).
Gorissen, M., Sanz, J.C., & Schmand, B. (2005). Effort and cognition in
schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Research, 78, 199-208.
Green, P. & Astner, K., (1995) Manual for the Oral Word Memory Test,
Cognisyst, N.C.
Green, P., Allen, L. & Astner, K., (1996) Manual for the Computerized Word
Memory Test (DOS version), Cognisyst, N.C.
Green, P., (2003) Green’s Word Memory Test for Windows: User’s manual,
Green’s Publishing, Edmonton, Canada.
Green, P. & Flaro, L. (2003) Word Memory Test Performance in Children, Child
Neuropsychology 19, 3, 189-207
Green, P. Manual for the Emotional Perception Test. (1986, revised 1997), Green’s
Publishing, Edmonton
Green, P. Manual for the Story Recall Test (ACT). (1983, revised 1996) Green’s
Publishing, Edmonton
Green, P., Iverson, G. & Allen, L. (1999) Detecting malingering in head injury
litigation with the Word Memory Test. Brain Injury,13, 10, 813-819
32
Effort effects 33
Green, P. & Iverson, G.L.
exaggeration
on
olfactory
(2001)
deficits
in
Effects of injury severity and cognitive
head
injury
compensation
claims.
Neurorehabilitation, 16, 237-243
Green, P. and Iverson, G. (2001) Validation of the Computerized Assessment of
Response Bias in litigating patients with head injuries. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
15 (4), 492-497.
Green, P., Rohling, M.L, Iverson, G. & Gervais, R. (2003) Relationships between
olfactory discrimination and head injury severity. Brain Injury, 17, 6, 479-496
Green, P., Rohling, M, Lees-Haley, P. & Allen, M. (2001) Effort has a greater
effect on test scores than severe brain injury in compensation claimants, Brain Injury, 15,
12, 1045-1060.
Green, P., Lees-Haley, P.R. & Allen, M. (2002) The Word Memory Test and the
validity of neuropsychological test scores. Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, 2, 3,
97-124
Iverson, G., Green, P. & Gervais, R. (1999) Using the Word Memory Test to
detect biased responding in head injury litigation. The Journal of Cognitive
Rehabilitation, 17, 2, 4-8.
Jackson, D.N. (1998), Multidimensional Aptitude Battery-II. Sigma Assessment
Systems, Port Huron, Michigan
Meyers, J., & Meyers, K. (1995). The Meyers Scoring System for the Rey
Complex Figure and the Recognition Trial: Professional Manual. Psychological
Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL.
33
Effort effects 34
Reitan, R. M. (1974) Methodological problems in clinical neuropsychology. In R.
M. Reitan & L. A. Davison (Eds) Clinical Neuropsychology: Current Status and
Applications. (pp. 19-46) V. H. Winston & Sons: Washington, DC
Reitan, R. M. (1969). Manual for Administration of Neuropsychological Test
Battery: Theory and Interpretation. Indianapolis, IN: Neuropsychological Press.
Rohling, M.L., Meyers, J.M., & Millis, S.R. (2003) Neuropsychological
Impairment Following Traumatic Brain Injury: A Dose-Response Analysis. The Clinical
Neuropsychologist, 17, 3, 289-302
Rohling, M.L., Green, P., Allen, L.M. & Iverson, G.L. (2002) Depressive
symptoms and neurocognitive test scores in patients passing symptom validity tests.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 3, 205-222
Ruff, R. (1988) Ruff Figure Fluency Test. Neuropsychological Resources, San
Diego, CA.
Schmand B, Lindeboom J, Schagen S, Heijt R, Koene T, Hamburger HL. (1998)
Cognitive complaints in patients after whiplash injury: the impact of malingering. Journal
of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 64, 339-43.
Suhr, J. & Gunstadt, J. (2005) Further exploration of the effect of “diagnosis
threat” on cognitive performance in individuals with mild head injury. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 23-29.
Tombaugh, T. N., Test of Memory Malingering (1996). Multi-Health Systems,
Toronto.
Trahan, D.E., & Larrabee, G.J. (1988). Continuous Visual Memory Test. Manual.
Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL.
34
Effort effects 35
Tydecks, S. & Merten, T., (2005, personal communication). The Word Memory
Test and the One-In-Five-Test in an Analogue Study with Russian Participants.
Warrington, E. K. (1984). Recognition Memory Test Manual. Windsor, NFERNelson.
Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. Psychological
Corporation, New York.
Wechsler, D. (1987) Wechsler Memory Scale - Revised. Psychological
Corporation, New York.
Williamson, D. J., Drane, D. L., Stroup, E. S., Miller, J. W., Holmes, M. D., & Wilensky, A.
J. (2004). Detecting cognitive differences between patients with epilepsy and patients
with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: Effort matters. Epilepsia, 45 (Suppl. 7), 179.
35
Effort effects 36
Table 1: Mean California Verbal Learning Test recall scores at each level of effort
Mean
CVLT Short
CVLT SD
CVLT
CVLT
WMT
Delay Free
Free
Long Delay Free LD Free
effort
N
Recall Mean
SD
Recall Mean
SD
91-100%
745
3.2
3.2
10.7
11.2
81-90%
206
3.2
3.2
8.3
8.9
71-80%
105
3.5
3.4
7.8
8.2
61-70%
61
3.0
3.3
7.4
7.3
51-60%
50
2.9
3.1
5.8
5.5
<=50%
34
2.5
3.0
4.4
3.3
Table 2: CVLT cued recall scores by level of WMT effort
Mean
CVLT SD
CVLT SD
CVLT LD
WMT
Cued Recall
Cued Recall
Cued Recall
effort
N
Mean
SD
Mean
91-100% 745
2.8
11.8
11.9
81-90%
206
3.0
9.7
9.8
71-80%
105
3.4
9.5
9.2
61-70%
61
3.0
8.9
8.5
51-60%
50
3.0
7.0
6.5
<=50%
34
3.1
5.4
4.5
CVLT LD
Cued Recall
SD
2.9
3.0
3.2
3.2
3.2
3.1
SD=short delay, LD=long delay
Table 3: CVLT learning trial scores and recognition hits by level of WMT effort
Mean
CVLT CVLT CVLT CVLT CVLT
CVLT
WMT
N
Trial 1 Trial 1 Trial 5 Trial 5 Rec.Hits Rec.Hits
effort
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
91-100% 745
2.3
2.5
1.6
7.4
12.4
14.6
81-90%
206
2.1
2.7
2.2
6.4
10.6
13.5
71-80%
105
2.2
3.1
2.9
6.0
10.2
12.8
61-70%
61
2.3
2.6
2.8
6.0
9.7
11.9
51-60%
50
1.6
2.8
3.2
4.9
8.7
10.7
<=50%
34
2.1
2.9
3.7
4.5
7.0
7.6
CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test, version 1; Rec.Hits = Recognition Hits score
36
Effort effects 37
Table 4: CVLT by Glasgow Coma Scale scores in 258 cases of head injury, who
passed WMT effort subtests (i.e. data presumed valid)
N
CVLT
CVLT
CVLT
CVLT
GCS
SD Free Recall SD Free Recall LD Free Recall LD Free Recall
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
3
11
3.1
3.2
9.1
10.3
5
7
2.4
3.1
8.0
7.6
6
8
3.3
2.1
7.9
8.3
7
9
4.1
5.5
8.5
9.6
8
6
3.1
2.8
8.5
9.3
9
5
3.9
2.9
10.7
11.0
10
6
3.1
3.2
11.4
11.6
11
8
3.5
3.4
10.6
11.6
12
7
2.7
2.2
10.1
10.7
13
9
3.2
3.4
11.2
11.6
14
39
3.2
2.9
10.6
11.3
15
152
3.2
3.2
10.2
10.7
SD=short delay, LD=long delay
Table 5: Independent replication of the effect of effort on CVLT scores in 678 cases
tested by Dr. Roger Gervais: Scores on CVLT short and long delayed free recall
and Recognition Hits and on TOMM trial two.
Mean
WMT
Effort
91100%
8190%
7180%
6170%
5160%
<=50%
N of CVLT CVLT CVLT CVLT CVLT CVLT TOMM TOMM
cases
SD
SD
LD
LD
Rec.
Rec.
Trial
Trial
FREE FREE FREE FREE Hits
Hits
2
2
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
365
3.0
3.1
1.6
.9
11.0
11.4
14.8
49.8
128
8.8
3.4
9.2
3.2
14.0
2.0
49.0
2.5
58
8.9
3.4
8.7
3.7
13.5
2.3
46.5
4.9
46
6.6
2.8
6.6
2.8
11.5
3.7
43.7
8.0
23
6.6
2.4
6.3
2.9
11.6
3.0
38.8
9.4
13
3.8
2.5
3.5
2.7
9.2
4.4
28.9
10.9
SD=short delay, LD=long delay; Rec.Hits = Recognition Hits score
37
Effort effects 38
Table 6: Warrington’s Recognition Memory Test (RMT) for words and faces by
level of WMT effort.
Mean
WMT
effort
91-100%
81-90%
71-80%
61-70%
51-60%
<=50%
N
645
176
85
51
37
31
WRMT
Faces
Mean
41.8
39.8
37.1
36.2
32.3
26.1
WRMT
Faces
SD
5.1
5.8
6.6
6.5
8.1
8.8
N
301
76
35
25
13
15
WRMT
Words
Mean
46.0
41.7
39.8
36.1
29.1
23.0
WRMT
Words
SD
4.3
5.7
6.7
7.3
6.9
5.4
Table 7: Immediate and 30-minute Delayed Story Recall by level of WMT effort.
Mean
WMT
effort
91-100%
81-90%
71-80%
61-70%
51-60%
<=50%
N
773
207
105
58
45
31
Immediate
Story Recall
Mean
47.1
42.3
40.9
38.6
34.8
28.5
Immediate
Story Recall
SD
9.5
9.4
10.3
8.9
11.6
10.6
N
766
203
104
57
45
31
Delayed
Recall
Mean
36.3
27.0
26.9
23.5
17.8
13.8
Delayed
Recall
SD
12.2
11.7
12.3
9.9
10.7
8.8
Note: Scores are out of a maximum of 80 for the five-story set on immediate and delayed recall, where 50
(SD 7) is the normal mean for immediate recall in adults of average IQ.
Table 8: Rey Complex Figure Test (Meyer method) by level of WMT effort.
Mean
WMT
effort
91-100%
81-90%
71-80%
61-70%
51-60%
41.1%
Rey
Rey CFT
N
CFT
Immediate
Immediate
Recall
Recall
SD
(%ile)
Mean
813
30.6
37.7
218
28.2
26.4
119
23.8
22.6
63
18.5
14.3
55
14.9
10.9
39
16.7
9.5
Rey
CFT
Delayed
Recall
(%ile)
Mean
35.9
23.3
20.5
12.4
10.4
5.4
Rey
CFT
Delayed
Recall
SD
Rey
CFT
Copy
(Raw)
Mean
Rey
CFT
Copy
SD
31.1
26.4
25.2
16.6
15.0
9.4
33.1
32.1
31.4
30.5
27.5
27.0
3.0
3.4
4.7
4.1
6.5
5.8
Note: The Rey CFT score is the raw score for the copy trial. Otherwise, the Rey CFT scores and the Digit
Span scores are expressed as a percentile rank relative to age, gender and education.
38
Effort effects 39
Table 9: The Trail Making Test by levels of effort on WMT
Mean
WMT
effort
N
91-100%
81-90%
71-80%
61-70%
51-60%
<=50%
813
218
119
63
55
39
Trail
Making A
(seconds)
Mean
30.4
38.6
42.1
44.9
60.3
65.2
Trail
Making
A
SD
12.6
17.3
20.2
24.9
46.1
45.1
Table 10: PIQ and VIQ by levels of effort on WMT
Mean
PIQ
PIQ
VIQ
WMT
N
Mean
SD
Mean
effort
91-100%
761 104.7
12.9 101.7
81-90%
199
14.1 94.8
98.3
71-80%
98
12.9 95.5
94.2
61-70%
56
13.3 94.6
93.1
51-60%
43
13.0 87.7
86.3
<=50%
24
16.3 86.0
84.0
Trail
Making B
(seconds)
Mean
76.5
105.0
112.7
139.7
183.1
159.6
Trail
Making
B
SD
48.1
77.0
78.5
96.9
157.5
141.1
VIQ
SD
13.2
13.1
13.4
13.5
13.2
15.8
Table 11: Finger Tip Number Writing by levels of effort on WMT
Mean
FTNW FTNW FTNW FTNW
WMT
N
Errors Errors Errors Errors
effort
Left
Left
Right
Right
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
91-100%
272
2.3
2.3
1.5
1.8
81-90%
65
2.3
2.2
1.7
2.1
71-80%
24
2.8
2.8
2.4
3.1
61-70%
16
2.5
2.1
2.4
2.1
51-60%
11
3.1
3.5
3.3
4.1
<=50%
4
7.9
8.3
7.2
8.5
39
Effort effects 40
Table 12: Grip strength by level of WMT effort.
Mean
WMT
effort
N
91-100%
81-90%
71-80%
61-70%
51-60%
<=50%
813
218
119
63
55
39
Grip
strength
Right (Kgs)
Mean
41.4
39.4
36.9
38.0
33.6
31.7
Grip
strength
Right
SD
14.2
13.6
16.4
16.6
14.6
14.2
Grip
strength
Left (Kgs)
Mean
38.1
38.0
35.3
36.7
32.5
31.6
Grip
strength
Left
SD
13.6
13.3
14.8
16.0
14.2
12.5
Table 13: Finger tapping speed by level of WMT effort.
Mean
WMT
effort
91-100%
81-90%
71-80%
61-70%
51-60%
<=50%
N
529
134
63
32
26
15
Finger tapping speed
Right
Mean
48.8
45.3
43.3
44.9
38.0
34.7
FT
Right
SD
8.9
11.2
11.1
10.6
11.9
15.4
Finger tapping speed
Left
Mean
45.3
42.7
40.7
42.8
37.4
36.0
Table 14: Grooved Pegboard by level of WMT effort.
Mean
Grooved
Grooved
Grooved
WMT
N
Pegboard
Pegboard
Pegboard
Effort
Right (secs)
Right
Left (secs)
Mean
SD
Mean
91-100% 813
24.4
72.2
81.3
81-90% 218
22.2
80.3
86.2
71-80% 119
51.2
92.6
90.5
61-70%
63
18.9
82.8
90.5
51-60%
55
55.4
109.6
116.2
<=50%
39
55.1
108.4
123.6
FT
Left
SD
8.6
9.3
9.2
10.0
12.0
13.8
Grooved
Pegboard
Left
SD
32.9
24.3
29.6
18.5
61.4
96.3
40
Effort effects 41
Table 15: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and scores by levels of effort on WMT
Mean
WMT
effort
N
WCST Perseverative
Errors
Mean (% ile rank)
91-100%
81-90%
71-80%
61-70%
51-60%
<=50%
813
218
119
63
55
39
51.3
42.1
38.7
33.4
26.9
11.2
WCST
Categories
Mean
5.2
4.5
4.4
4.2
3.3
2.1
WCST
Categories
SD
1.5
1.9
1.9
2.0
2.3
2.3
Note: The WCST perseverative error score is expressed as a percentile rank relative to age, gender and
education, using Heaton’s norm tables.
Table 16: Category Test errors by levels of effort on WMT
Mean
Category
Cat
WMT
N
Test
Test
effort
Errors
Errors
Mean
SD
91-100%
674
29.5
55.1
81-90%
164
28.9
66.2
71-80%
77
28.2
73.3
61-70%
44
28.2
70.7
51-60%
30
28.4
91.1
<=50%
18
33.8
88.4
Table 17: Digit Span and Visual Memory Span by level of WMT effort.
Mean
WMT
Effort
N
91-100%
81-90%
71-80%
61-70%
51-60%
<=50%
675
178
95
54
42
30
Digits
Forward
(%ile)
Mean
Digits
Backward
(%ile)
Mean
49.3
41.4
27.1
31.7
20.6
11.5
58.7
51.2
37.6
35.9
33.3
14.2
N
630
166
88
49
36
21
Visual
Memory
Span
Forward
(% ile)
56.8
46.7
38.8
32.8
20.4
17.7
Visual
Memory
Span
Backward
(% ile)
65.6
53.1
46.9
41.3
32.1
29.4
Note: Digit Span and Visual Memory Span scores are expressed as a percentile rank relative to age, gender
and education, using Heaton’s norm tables.
41
Effort effects 42
Table 18: Thurstone Word Fluency and Ruff Figural Fluency Test by level of WMT
effort.
Mean
Thurstone
Thurstone
Ruff FFT
Ruff FFT
WMT
N
Word Fluency
Word
N
Total Designs Total Designs
Effort
Mean
Fluency
Mean
SD
SD
91513
17.8
611
22.6
51.1
76.8
100%
81-90% 105
16.4
145
23.6
44.0
65.2
71-80% 49
15.5
64
21.9
38.5
61.7
61-70% 23
17.9
35
24.2
43.3
63.2
51-60% 19
18.6
27
17.7
35.5
61.7
<=50% 10
17.2
14
21.5
37.0
52.6
Note: Thurstone scores are percentile ranks for age, gender and education using Heaton’s norms.
Table 19: Gorham’s Proverb Interpretation and Emotional Perception
Test by level of WMT effort.
Mean
Emotional
Emotional
WMT
N
Gorham’s
Gorham’s
Perception Test Perception Test
Effort
Proverbs
Proverbs
(errors/45)
(errors/45)
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
91648
5.1
4.1
11.1
9.0
100%
81-90% 149
4.4
4.4
8.6
10.7
71-80%
77
4.9
4.3
9.0
10.4
61-70%
37
4.5
4.9
8.3
11.5
51-60%
26
5.7
5.4
9.5
14.3
<=50%
15
4.6
6.4
7.5
13.5
Table 20: Alberta Smell Test Scores by level of WMT effort.
Mean
Smell test Smell test Smell test Smell test
WMT
N
(Right)
(Right)
(Left)
(Left)
effort
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
91-100% 813
2.6
2.7
5.2
5.6
81-90%
218
2.5
2.6
4.5
4.6
71-80%
119
2.9
2.9
4.4
5.0
61-70%
63
2.8
2.6
3.6
4.1
51-60%
55
2.6
2.7
3.6
3.7
<=50%
39
2.4
3.2
3.5
3.5
Note: Scores are number correct out of 10 per nostril
42
Effort effects 43
Table 21: Benton Judgment of Line Orientation (BJLO) and
Continuous Visual Memory Test (CVMT) by level of WMT effort.
Mean
CVMT CVMT
WMT
N BJLO BJLO Delay
Delay
effort
Mean
SD
Mean
Mean
91-100% 653 24.7
4.2
1.5
4.2
81-90%
169 23.8
4.4
1.6
3.6
71-80%
87
4.4
1.5
22.7
4.2
61-70%
49
5.8
1.5
21.7
2.8
51-60%
43
5.1
2.0
18.5
2.0
<=50%
32
6.7
1.3
15.3
1.5
Table 22: Amsterdam Short Term Memory Test (ASTM), 21-item test and
Computerized Assessment of Response Bias (CARB) by level of WMT effort.
Mean
ASTM
21 Item
21Item
CARB
WMT
N
Mean
SD
N Recognition
Rec.
N
Mean
effort
Mean
SD
91163
2.9
89
2.2
788
87.2
17.5
98.0
100%
81-90% 48
4.3
16
2.3
210
84.7
16.6
95.5
71-80% 21
6.1
10
2.6
115
82.5
14.9
91.1
61-70%
8
5.9
9
3.0
60
80.0
15.9
85.6
51-60%
7
6.6
4
3.7
54
79.4
10.2
76.1
<=50%
7
16.5
3
.58
38
69.6
8.3
64.1
SD
5.2
7.2
10.8
14.2
22.0
25.3
Note: ASTM scores are out of 90. 21-item test scores are out of 21. CARB scores are % correct.
Table 23: WMT memory subtests by level of WMT effort
Mean
WMT
effort
91-100%
81-90%
71-80%
61-70%
51-60%
<=50%
WMT
DR
Mean
97.5%
88.1%
77.0%
67.1%
54.5%
41.1%
N
813
218
119
63
55
39
WMT
MC
Mean
90.4
66.9
54.5
47.8
36.0
26.8
WMT WMT WMT WMT WMT
MC
PA
PA
FR
FR
SD
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
10.8
85.8
14.4
52.9
52.9
14.1
16.0
35.6
61.4
35.6
12.6
50.2
14.9
31.5
31.5
11.3
44.9
14.7
27.8
27.8
13.0
34.5
13.2
23.7
23.7
15.6
27.4
14.1
19.9
19.9
DR=Delayed Recognition, MC=Multiple Choice, PA=Paired Associates
43
Effort effects 44
Table 24: Average drop in performance on eight tests for each level of effort on
WMT: Scores expressed in terms of standard deviations below the mean for those
in the top range of effort.
Mean CVLT
WCST Trail
Ruff
Finger
Immed. Mean
WMT
SD
WRMT Categ- Making
FFT
Tap PIQ Story
of 8
effort FREE Faces
ories.
A
Designs Right
Recall Tests
91100%
8190%
7180%
6170%
5160%
<=50%
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
-0.8
-0.4
-0.5
-0.7
-0.5
-0.4
-0.5
-0.5
-0.54
-0.9
-0.9
-0.5
-0.9
-0.7
-0.6
-0.8
-0.7
-0.75
-1.0
-1.1
-0.7
-1.2
-0.6
-0.4
-0.9
-0.9
-0.85
-1.5
-1.9
-1.3
-2.4
-0.7
-1.2
-1.4
-1.3
-1.46
-2.0
-3.1
-2.1
-2.8
-1.1
-1.6
-1.6
-1.2
-1.94
Note: For Trail Making A, the signs have been reversed to make the table consistent, because longer times
imply poorer performance
Figure 1: Scores from 403 cases failing WMT when tested clinically resemble those from
a group of volunteer simulators: They score lower than dementia patients on easy subtests
and higher on harder subtests.
44
Download