ARTICLE REVIEW Περδικάκη Κατερίνα I.D. number

advertisement
ARTICLE REVIEW
Περδικάκη Κατερίνα
I.D. number: 1563200800215
PLANNING AND CONDUCTING RESEARCH
Elly Ifantidou
Date of Submission:
14th November 2011
Schweinle, William, Ickes, William, Rollings, Kathryn and Jacquot, Colette (2010)
“Maritally aggressive men: angry, egocentric, impulsive, and/or biased”.
Journal of Language and Social Psychology 29 (4): 399-424.
Research Topic: Violence through Language: How Innocent Are Words of
Everyday Speech?
The article is concerned with the relationship between the language used by
maritally aggressive men, other characteristics of them (anger, impulsivity, inferential
bias and attentional disorder or impairment) and their wife-directed aggression. The
authors aspire to shed light on the significance lying in the language employed by
such men in predicting their violent behaviour towards their wives. As far as the
hypotheses are concerned, these are explicitly stated: the authors expect to indicate
that men who describe their marriages by means of linguistic expressions conveying
anger are quite maritally aggressive and so are thought to be highly impulsive men.
They also attempt to show that attentional disorder/impairment results in increased
wife-directed aggression and, finally, that the same holds true as regards the strength
of the men’s critical/rejecting-overattribution bias. The study relies on findings of
previous research, which actually pointed to the aforementioned characteristics as
crucial indicators of maritally men’s aggression. Rather than simply confirming, the
article further examines the specific issue and concludes with additional factors that
play a significant part in predicting these men’s violent behaviour. In the end, it
comes up with two types of maritally aggressive men, but suggests further research
into the specific characteristics of each type. The cornerstone of the study involved is
the assumption that language is the medium through which people express aspects of
their personality and emotional life, even unintentionally. Besides, the importance of
linguistic word count analyses has been supported by previous researchers (Cohn,
Mehl, Pennebaker, Forgas and Tehani) to whom explicit reference is made.
The article is of particular interest to my research topic, which concerns the
way that violence is reflected on everyday speech; it can actually provide valuable
assistance as regards its investigation through examining the language used by
ordinary people who are yet to be labelled as violent, abusive or otherwise aggressive.
Moreover, it generated certain ideas as regards the questionnaire of my research. For
example, I could employ pictures presenting scenes of mild or more intense violence
2
and then ask participants to write down what comes to mind while looking at them.
Although the article examines only men’s aggressive behaviour, it may come as a
surprise that certain findings could apply to women as well, since participants of my
research are expected to be mainly female.
The material presented by the authors includes: findings stemming from the
Linguistic Inquiry Word Count software, self-report items, responses to items on the
Revised Dyadic Adjustment, the Conflict Tactics Scale, a measure of socially
desirable response bias, one of four temperament facets as well as a version of the
empathic accuracy paradigm. It is particularly helpful that detailed explanation of the
material and tables enabling a visual and statistical representation of the findings are
provided, which in turn facilitates the comprehension of the scientific procedure.
Nevertheless, technical symbols such as M, SD, r, p and terminology like z-scores and
standardized coefficients are not explained; thus, a reader lacking relevant knowledge
understands their meaning solely by inferring from context. In addition, all the
participants involved come from Texas and are married, which admittedly raises
questions about whether the sample is representative enough. What is more, only
married men are examined. Nonetheless, aggressive behaviour may precede marriage,
meaning that analysing the behaviour of men in romantic relationships on the whole
could point to additional factors predicting and leading to marital aggression. The
authors explain that they focus on men because previous research has indicated that
violence on their part is the most prominent issue and suggest further investigation
into women’s violent behaviour. In fact, my research could potentially show that
similar findings apply to women as well, as a widely-held assumption is that the
“weaker” sex has become more aggressive and masculine nowadays in both personal
and professional life.
All, in all, this study is ground-breaking in that it suggests that first-person
singular and first-person plural references are not correlated with wife-directed
aggression. Instead, the use of third-personal singular pronouns interestingly enough
points to Machiavellianism, meaning that employing pronouns such as “we”, “us” and
“our” – which are seemingly collectivistic – can actually imply a manipulative
behaviour. The article, finally, can prove useful in recognising signs of aggression –
even when it comes to our own behaviour – which we ought to take care of in light of
stable and more “peaceful” social relations.
3
Download