JSM 2009 PowerPoint Presentation

advertisement
2009 Joint Statistical Meetings
Washington, DC
August 1-6, 2009
Interpreting Differential Effects in Light
of Fundamental Statistical Tendencies
James P. Scanlan
Attorney at Law
Washington, DC, USA
jps@jpscanlan.com
Oral at
http://www.jpscanlan.com/images/JSM_2009_ORAL.pdf
Summary
1. Factors that similarly affect two groups with different
baseline rates of an outcome will tend to show a larger
proportionate effect on the outcome for the group with
the lower base rate but a larger proportionate effect on
the opposite outcome for the other group
2. True subgroup effects can only be identified by
determining the degree to which a factor shifts each
group’s risk distribution
References

Measuring Health Disparities page on jpscanlan.com –
especially the Solutions tab

Scanlan’s Rule page on jpscanlan.com – especially
the Subgroup Effects tab

Can We Actually Measure Health Disparities? (Chance
2006)

Race and Mortality (Society 2000)

Divining Difference (Chance 1994)
An illogical expectation
There exists a tendency to regard it as
somehow normal that a factor that
similarly affects two groups’
susceptibilities to an outcome will cause
the same proportionate change in the
outcome rates for each group and to
regard anything else as a differential
effect (subgroup effect, interaction)
One reason the expectation is illogical


Where two groups have different base
rates of experiencing an outcome, a factor
that has the same proportionate effect on
each group’s rate of experiencing the
outcome necessarily has a different
proportionate effect on each group’s rate of
avoiding the outcome
E.g., Group A (10/90); Group B (20/80)
A logical expectation


A factor that similarly affects two groups
with different baseline rates of an outcome
will tend to show a larger proportionate
effect on the outcome for the group with the
lower base rate but a larger proportionate
effect on the opposite outcome for the other
group
Why is that logical?
Interpretive Rule 1/Scanlan’s Rule 1

When two groups differ in their
susceptibility to an outcome, the rarer the
outcome, the greater tends to be the
relative difference in experiencing it and the
smaller tends to be the relative difference in
avoiding it.
Fig 1. Ratio of (1) Disadvantaged Group (DG) Fail
Rate to Advantaged Group (AG) Fail Rate at Various
Cutoff Points Defined by AG Fail Rate
4
Ratio
3
(1) DG Fail Rate/AG Fail Rate
2
1
99
90
80
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
Cutoffs Defined by AG Fail Rate
1
Fig. 2. Ratios of (1) DG Fail Rate to AG Fail Rate and
(2) AG Pass Rate to DG Pass Rate at Various Cutoff
Points Defined by AG Fail Rate
4
3
Ratios
(1) DG Fail Rate/AG Fail Rate
(2) AG Pass Rate/DG Pass Rate
2
1
99
90
80
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
Cutoffs Defined by AG Fail Rate
1
Fig 3. Ratios of (1) Bl Rate of Falling below Various
Income Levels to Wh Rate of Falling below Level and
(2) Wh Rate of Falling above Level to Bl Rate of
Falling above Level
4
Ratios
3
(1) Bl Rate Bel/Wh Rate Bel
(2) Wh Rate Ab/Bl Rate Ab
2
1
0
600 500 400 300 250 200 175 150 125 100
Percent of Poverty Line
75
50
Fig. 4. Ratio of (1) Bl to Wh Rate of Falling
above Various SBP Levels and (2) Wh to Bl
Rate of Falling below the Level (NHANES
1999-2000, 2001-2002, Men 45-64)
5
Ratios
4
(1) Bl Rate Ab/Wh Rate Ab
Wh Rate Bel/Bl Rate Bel
3
2
1
110
120
130
140
150
160
170
180
Systolic Blood Pressure
190
Implications of SR1

As an adverse becomes less prevalent relative difference in experiencing it
tend to increase while relative differences in avoiding it tend to decline
(e.g., test failure, poverty, hypertension, mortality).

Relative differences in adverse outcomes tend to be larger among
advantaged subpopulations while relative differences in favorable
outcomes tend to be smaller among such subpopulations (e.g. race
differences in health outcomes among the college-educated, race and SES
differences in health outcomes among the young, SES differences in health
outcomes among British Civil servants)

Factors that decrease/increase the prevalence of an outcome will tend to
have greater proportionate effect on group with lower base rate but greater
proportionate effect on the opposite outcome for the other group
Problems Arising from SR1


When we observe standard patterns of changes
in differences between group rates as
prevalence of an outcome changes, how do we
determine whether the difference between the
groups’ situations actually changed in a
meaningful way and how do we quantify the
difference at each point in time (in a way that is
unaffected by overall prevalence)?
How do we identify genuine differential effects?
Estimated Effect Size
Estimated effect size (EES) = estimated
difference, in terms of percentage of a
standard deviation, between means of
hypothesized underlying, continuouslyscaled normal distributions of factors
associated with experiencing an outcome,
derived from outcome rates of each group
(see JSM 2008 and Solutions and
Solutions Database tabs on jpscanlan.com)
Table 2 Illustration of the Solution (in terms of an
adverse outcome decreasing in overall prevalence)
Period
AG Rate
DG Rate
Yr 1
60%
77%
Yr 2
42%
60%
Measures of Difference
AdvRatio
1.16
FavRatio
1.74
1.43
1.45
EES (z)
.47
.50
Change Direction
Increase
Decrease
Decrease
Illustrations from Two Perspectives

Perspective 1: Identifying/evaluating
differential effects of factor on two groups

Perspective 2: Comparing the size of the
difference between the rates of two groups
according to the presence or absence of a
factor
Table 3. Comparison of Effects of Hypertension
Control on Heart Attack Risk of Women and Men with
Similar Risk Factor Profiles (A65,TC300,HDL50,NS,
NM) (Perspective 1)
Gender
SBP 120
Risk
SPB150
Risk
Adverse
Reduction
Favorable
Increase
EES
F
4.0%
7.0%
42.9%
3.2%
0.28
M
14.0%
19.0%
26.3%
6.2%
0.21
Table 4. Comparison of Gender Differences in Heart
Attack Risk for Women and Men with Similar Risk
Factor Profiles, by with and without Hypertension
Control (A65,TC300,HDL50,NS, NM) (Perspective 2)
SBP
Fem
Rate
Male
Rate
Adverse
Ratio
Favorable
Ratio
EES
120
4.0%
14.0%
3.5
1.12
0.68
150
7.0%
19.0%
2.7
1.15
0.60
Table 5: Comparison of Black-White Difference in
Lung Cancer among <11 and > 29 Cig Per Day
(Haimon NEJM 2006) (Perspective 2)
Cig/Day
Adverse
Ratio
Favorable
Ratio
White Rate
Black Rate
EES
<11
0.7%
2.2%
3.1
1.015
0.44
>29
3.1%
5.4%
1.7
1.024
0.26
Table 6. Comparison of Effects of Decreasing
Smoking from >29 to <11 Cig Per Day on
Whites and Blacks (Haimon NEJM 2006)
(Perspective 1) (cor. 9/20/14)
Race
<11 CPD
>29 CPD
Adverse
Reduction
Favorable
Increase
EES
W
0.7%
3.1%
77.2%
2.5%
0.60
B
2.2%
5.9%
63.4%
4.0%
0.42
Table 7. Comparison of Effects of Beta
Blockers on Mortality Among Heart Patients at
Different Ages (Gottlieb NEJM 1998)
(Perspective 1)
Age
Adverse
Beta Rate No Beta Rate Reduction
Favorable
Increase
EES
<70
11.3%
18.7%
39.6%
9.1%
0.34
>80
22.6%
33.1%
31.7%
15.7%
0.32
Table 8. Comparison of Age Differences in
Mortality Among Patients Treated and Not
Treated with Beta Blockers (Gottlieb NEJM
1998) (Perspective 2)
Treatment
Status
<70 Rate
>80 Rate
Adverse
Ratio
Favorable
Ratio
EES
Beta
11.3%
22.6%
2.0
1.15
0.48
NoBeta
18.7%
33.1%
1.8
1.22
0.45
Issues

Contrary literature regarding the patterns

Clinical significance issue

Statistical significance issues



Re spurious patterns
Re solutions approach
Absolute minimum issue
Download