The Epistemology and Pedagogy of Education in High School

advertisement
University of Tennessee, Knoxville
Trace: Tennessee Research and Creative
Exchange
Senior Thesis Projects, 2003-2006
College Scholars
2004
The Epistemology and Pedagogy of Education in
High School Physics and Physical Science
Bennett Adkinson
Follow this and additional works at: http://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_interstp3
Recommended Citation
Adkinson, Bennett, "The Epistemology and Pedagogy of Education in High School Physics and Physical Science" (2004). Senior Thesis
Projects, 2003-2006.
http://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_interstp3/4
This Project is brought to you for free and open access by the College Scholars at Trace: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. It has been
accepted for inclusion in Senior Thesis Projects, 2003-2006 by an authorized administrator of Trace: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For
more information, please contact trace@utk.edu.
FORMC
COLLEGE SCHOLARS PROJECT APPROVAL
LJ~d;a I. f(~/E~
Scholar
Mentor
•.
,"
.
...
:,
.'. " . ' .. "
. .,.".
--. "
-
"
... :
.... ...
"
' .
'
. '",:
"COMMI,-TEEMEMS"ERS"".
" ·":(MinirnUrTl3.Require~) "
J"Pr·we
.
'"
.A
.,'
;
4--/
Ie:
t-y p rC/L-I~ r
PLEASE ATTACH A COpy OF THE SENIOR PROJECT TO THIS SHEET AND
RETURN BOTH TO THE PROGRAM DIRECTOR. THIS PAGE SHOULD BE
DATED AND COMPLETED ON THE DATE THAT YOUR DEFENSE IS HELD.
DATE COMPLETED "
5-6 - 0,<:1
Physics Education 1
-
-
-
The Epistemology and Pedagogy of Education in High School Physics and Physical
Science
Bennett Adkinson
University of Tennessee, College Scholars
-
-
Dr. Claudia Melear, faculty mentor
5/6/04
-
Physics Education 2
-
-
-
-
-
Table of Contents
3
Definitions
4
Introduction
5
Misconceptions
12
Addressing Misconceptions
17
Constructivism and Inquiry
21
Transmission vs. Constructivism
24
"Teachers Teach as They Were Taught"
28
Figures
29
References
-
Physics Education 3
-
-
-
Definitions
Conception
a constructed mental framework that can be either correct or not
.Constructivism an educational philosophy centered around the idea that students
actively construct all knowledge they possess
Inquiry an education concept where students through their own interest, direction, and
thought processes use observation and experimentation to actively learn science
Inservice Teacher
a practicing teacher in a primary or secondary school system
Misconception - a self-created scientific belief divergent from those generally accepted
by the scientific community
Preservice Teacher a college student in an education program with the specific goal of
being a certified, practicing teacher in the future
-
-
-
-
-
Transmission the traditional education model where the teacher lectures to the students
who passively absorb the information
Physics Education 4
.Introduction
-
When asked what she thought of physics in general, a ninth grade student
responded that she found physics boring and meaningless. "I don't like being forced to
memorize that something that falls down in a straight line has to be represented by a
crooked line rising to the right" (Nachtigall, 1990, p.2).
)-
-
-
-
-
-
This young learner is not alone in her feelings toward physics and physical
science. These courses are normally taken by twelfth graders and ninth graders
respectively in America, but are generally disliked by most Americans. To most people,
the physical sciences are perceived as an "inert body of information to be memorized"
(McDermott, Schaffer, and Constantinou, 2000).
Laypeople and high school students are not the only ones frustrated with physics
and physical science. Over the past thirty years, as physicists and science education
specialists have joined in, concern for the quality of physics education has steadily
increased (McDermott & DeWater, 2000; Mestre, 1991). Concern has mounted to the
point where some are claiming a "general crisis in physics education" (Nachtigall, 1990,
p. 1). Many researchers cite inadequate teacher preparation as the root of the problem
(Stein, 2001; Wilson, 1991; Nachtigall, 1990). Much research has been and is currently
being conducted into the value and effectiveness of science preparatory programs
(Adams & Krockover, 1997; Tibell, 2000).
Recognizing that there is a general discontent with the current status of physics
and physical science education, it becomes important to investigate the ways that physics
and physical science are learned and the ways that they are taught. This paper attempts
examine the roles of learning and teaching in physics and physical science for students
-
-
Physics Education 5
and their teachers. The ways physics and physical science are best learned often do not
match up with the ways they are most commonly taught on the secondary level (Trumper,
2001; Mestre, 1991).
-
-
Misconceptions
Students are bombarded daily with new information wherever they go. Whether
they are in the classroom, on the street, or at home in front of the television, students
cannot help but have ideas and concepts thrown at them. Students must actively construct
-
a personal understanding of this information in order to relate it to the rest of their world
as they understand it (Trumper & Gorsky, 1996). As new information is received,
students have to construct and deconstruct their current understanding so as to
-
-
-
accommodate the new information (Gonzalez-Espada, 2003). Whether the information is
scientific, pseudoscientific, or not particularly scientific at all, students will somehow
choose to respond and attempt to make sense of the situations by applying them to their
existing schemas. This construction of understanding is easily compared to Piaget's
assimilation and accommodation concepts and is often quite creative and seemingly
logical. Without proper scientific guidance, the frameworks constructed by student minds
to account for scientific concepts often diverge from those commonly agreed upon by the
-
scientific community (Gonzalez-Espada, 2003).
A list of commonly cited incorrect scientific concepts, created by the college
students of Wilson J. Gonzalez-Espada (2003), at Arkansas Tech University, and
gathered by means of a survey, includes:
• Seasons are created by the Earth's changing distance from the Sun.
-
-
Physics Education 6
• Meteors are falling stars.
• God and angels cause thunder and lightning.
• Batteries have "electricity" inside.
-
-
-
-
• The phases of the Moon are caused by the shadow of the Earth on the Moon.
• The pupil of the eye is a black object or spot on the surface of the eye.
• All metals are attracted to a magnet.
• All stars are the same size (or have the same brightness).
• Continents do not move.
• The Sun will never bum out.
(p.37)
Similar lists can be found in many sources (Beaty, 2001; Olenick, 2001; Crockett, 2004)
and can be quite informative into the thinking process of students.
-
-
-
-
-
-
These mental frameworks created with good intentions are often called
preconceptions or misconceptions, the earlier being all self-created beliefs of students and
the later being those beliefs that are generally accepted to be incorrect (Eryilmaz, 2002).
Other names for these include naIve conceptions, naIve theories, alternative frameworks,
alternative conceptions (Gonzalez-Espada, 2003), children's scientific intuitions,
common sense concepts, and spontaneous knowledge (Eryilmaz, 2002). Researchers
cannot seem to agree on a name for these, but the biggest concern lies in their existence
and the challenge of correcting them. The tendency for students to tenaciously hold on to
misconceptions is especially worrisome (Bisard, Aron, Francek, & Nelson, 1994; Mestre,
1991; Harrison, Grayson, & Treagust, 1999). Misconceptions have been an area of much
research in science education research. Studies concerning scientific misconceptions are
well documented (Harrison et ai., 1999) and span back to the 1940's (Blosser, 1987).
Research shows that students consistently embrace their misconceptions, even
-
when exposed to observations and insights that seemingly conflict with their
..
..
..
..
..
Physics Education 7
misconceptions (Mestre, 1991). Mestre accounts for this fight to maintain misconceptions
by stating that students "either view events through the myopic eye of the naIve theory or
make inconsequential modifications to their theories in ways that fail to resolve the
contradictions" (1991, p. 57). Mestre's explanation parallels the assimilation and
conceptual capture ideas of Harrison, Grayson, & Treagust in which students "simply
capture or add new information to their previous conceptions" (1999, p. 56) .
Misconceptions, especially those grounded in everyday experience, seem especially
resistant to change (Harrison et al., 1999). Because of this, unlearning misconceptions
can be exceptionally difficult, especially when the misconceptions "make sense" to the
students. As students desperately cling to their misconceptions, the difficult task of
unlearning these instead must fall on the teacher.
Studies have shown that physics and physical science misconceptions are not
necessarily specific to any certain type of people. Eryilmaz found that physics
misconceptions "show consistency across diverse samples of average students, honors
..
..
..
..
..
students, and even physics teachers" (2002, p. 1003). This means that most people tend to
have similar misconceptions, which may never be challenged, and if they are they may
never be changed.
Adults and children alike hold similar misconceptions. Implied within the fact that
adults often hold misconceptions is that science teachers can and do hold misconceptions
as well. This realization is not incorrect. The misconceptions and the implications therein
of preservice and inservice physical science and physics teachers are especially
interesting topics. The misconceptions of these teachers have been studied in great detail
for a variety of physical sciences topics.
-
-
-
Physics Education 8
A 2000 study by Trumper and Gorsky examined conceptions concerning force
held by physics students in a preservice high school teaching program. It showed that
physics majors, even into their fourth year of college, hold misconceptions worth noting
-
-
-
and correcting (Trumper & Gorsky, 1996). For example, the preservice teachers studied
held strong misconceptions concerning, and therefore did especially poorly when dealing
with, forces in dynamic situations and when balancing forces in all situations.
A 2002 study examined college students, including preservice teachers, as well as
inservice physical science teachers' understandings of heat and temperature (Jasien &
Oberem, 2002). The college students were divided into groups according to how many
physical science classes they had previously taken. The preservice teachers were
separated from these groups to form similar groups of their own. There was also a group
that separated out students in their second semester of organic chemistry. The inservice
teachers formed their own group, but it should be noted that they self-selected to attend a
summer enrichment physics course during which they were tested, and so, their results
-
-
-
-
-
are not indicative of all inservice teachers. All of the participants took a test about heat
and temperatures, and their scores were compared according to these groupings. The
inservice teachers did the best of all the groups, with 43% of their population getting all
of the questions correct, while their nearest competitors, the students who had taken fouror-more semesters of physical science, had only 22%), The preservice teachers scored the
lowest in all the categories compared to their non-education college student counterparts,
but only the preservice teachers who had taken only 1 semester of physical science did
significantly worse than the non-education college students with the same amount of
-
physical science coursework. The researchers note that the conceptions of the
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
Physics Education 9
science students and pre- and in-service teachers examined in this study [were]
lacking in the areas of thermal equilibrium, specific heat, and heat capacity.
Even among practicing physical science teachers at the middle- and high-school
level, the working knowledge in this topic [was] less than would be hoped for.
(Jasien & Oberem, 2002, p. 893)
Despite the fact that the researchers proved that there appears to be no correlation
between the number of physical science courses taken and performance on tests related to
thermal equilibrium, the researchers were genuinely not satisfied with the performance of
any of their participants.
One study of common physical science and earth science misconceptions has
proven to be quite interesting and citation worthy (Trumper, 2001; Trundle, Atwood, &
Christopher, 2002). Bisard, Aron, Francek, & Nelson studied the misconceptions of a
wide range of children and college students in their 1994 study. The sample included 180
middle school students, 157 high school students, 236 college freshmen and sophomores,
56 college juniors and seniors in upper-division science courses, 52 science majors in
teaching methods classes, and 27 general-ed. majors in an elementary science methods
class. Each member of this diverse group received the same test concerning seasons,
temperature, moon phases, states of matter, light, and other commonly held
misconceptions. As the span of the sample was large and diverse, it certainly led to
interesting results. (See Figure 1.) As would be expected, older students generally did
better than younger students, with the students in upper-division science classes doing the
best with a score of 55.3%) correct. The preservice teachers did not however do as well as
their upper-division counterparts. The science education majors scored 51.4% correct,
while the general education majors did significantly worse with only 37.3% correct. The
-
Physics Education 10
researchers note that this is "approximately equivalent" (p. 40) to the score of the middle
school students (34.6%) and go on to comment:
-
-
-
-
-
This suggests that future general elementary teachers seem to have about as
many misconceptions concerning the topics covered in this survey as do typical
middle school students .... [The] tendency for middle schoolers to score
similarly on this survey to future general elementary teachers is alarming.
(Bisard et al., 1994, p. 40)
Multiple studies have involved preservice teacher understanding of basic
astronomy concepts. One study by Trumper (2001) of general astronomy concepts got a
correct response rate of 38.4% correct of the 433 preservice high school teachers tested.
This test was modeled after the Bisard et al. test described above, but with only the
astronomy components. Comparing these scores to the Bisard et al. scores, these preservice teachers did only a little better than the poorly performing pre-service elementary
teachers and a little worse than the high school students.
Other astronomy studies specifically study misconceptions about moon phases.
Moon phases are one of the most studied misconceptions, perhaps because moon phases
are a targeted concept in the National Science Education Standards (National Research
Council, 1996) clearly expected to be studied in grades K-8. They also might be studied
often because their common misconceptions are easily distinguished from the
scientifically correct conception. Misconceptions range from clouds blocking the light of
the Moon to the Earth's tilt to the Earth's rotation on its axis, but the most common
misconception is that moon phases are caused by the earth's shadow being cast on the
Moon (Trundle, Atwood, & Christopher, 2002). All of the above vary greatly from the
generally accepted concept that as the Moon orbits earth, half is always illuminated by
the Sun, but the relative position of the Sun, Moon, and Earth determines what part of
..
-
Physics Education 11
that halfwe can see. Studies have found that no groups do particularly well at describing
moon phases, but of most interest and concern are the science teachers who don't do
-
..
..
-
..
-
-
..
-
-
well. Before the sample was ever given specific moon phase instruction, Callison and
Wright (1993) found that of76 elementary pre-service teachers, only 6.6% held a
scientific concept of moon phases. A similar study found 18% understanding, while
Trundle et al. (2002) had only 3.2 % of their preservice teachers answer scientifically
before proper instruction. In summarizing all previous studies of preservice teacher
misconceptions of moon phases, Trundle et al. had this to say:
The results of these studies indicate that most pre-service teachers, like the
students they are preparing to teach, do not understand the cause of moon
phases.
(p.634)
The general conclusion from the above studies is that preservice and inservice
science teachers at all levels lack a solid understanding of the concepts and tenaciously
hold onto their science misconceptions. Herein lies a grave problem; the teachers are
themselves holding misconceptions concerning topics that they are expected to teach to
their students. The above can be inferred from the previously mentioned studies, but this
topic deserves proper discussion. Trundle et al., concerning their study and others
reviewed in the introduction of their study, address this topic directly, saying:
Because educators are charged with developing a scientifically literate society, a
potentially serious problem is presented by preservice and inservice teachers
who themselves hold alternative conceptions about concepts included in the
textbooks they use or that are targeted by the national science education
standards .... The potential for negative effects is clearly there.
(2002, p.634)
Blosser (1987), in her research on science misconceptions, states "If teachers do not
understand elementary physical science concepts, how can they teach their students"
..
Physics Education 12
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
-
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
-
(~12)?
To which Berg and Brouwer extend: "It is likely that some of those alternative
conceptions have been passed on to students, as they were related to phenomena
commonly discussed during a high school physics class" (1991). Bisard et ai., who led
the study where the general education teachers did incredibly poorly, are especially
bothered by what they call the "misconception cycle" (1994 p.42). As teachers either
teach their students the misconceptions they personally hold or at least affirm the
misconceptions that the students already hold, an endless circle of misconceptions starts .
This "misconception cycle" has the potential to persist, unless colleges and teacher
preparation programs appropriately address misconceptions to preservice and inservice
teachers of science.
Addressing Misconceptions
It is easy to say that science teachers must overcome the misconceptions that they
have in order to be properly trained in the science content that they teach, but overcoming
misconceptions is quite an undertaking. Ifmisconceptions are not adequately addressed
specifically, they are likely to persist. "Because misconceptions are so deeply ingrained, .
. . merely giving the correct explanations in class is inadequate" (Bisard et ai., 1994). If
they are only mentioned or slightly addressed, the subject is likely to listen to the new
information but resort back to the old misconception in the long run. Current ideas for
correcting misconceptions apply equally to both teachers and students. Therefore,
teachers should learn to correct their own misconceptions just as they should help their
students address their misconceptions. Considering the trend of teachers to teach as they
Physics Education 13
..
..
..
..
..
..
were taught, properly addressing the misconceptions of teachers should adequately serve
to help stop the "misconception cycle."
Conceptual change is a fairly popular model for dealing with misconceptions
(Alparslan, Tekkaya, & Geban, 2003). The established conditions for conceptual change
are attributed to a paper by Posner, Strike, Hewson, and Gertzog concerning the
accommodation of scientific conceptions in 1982 (Eryilmaz, 2002; Harrison et al., 1999) .
This method calls for the teacher to help the student break the misconception
himselflherself (Posner, Strike, Hewson, and Gertzog, 1982). Conceptual change calls
for dissatisfaction, intelligibility, plausibility, and fruitfulness in the following manner:
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
a) There must be some level of dissatisfaction developed for the old conception
that cannot be remedied without radical changes in the conception .
b) The new conception must be intelligib Ie and make sense to the student.
c) The new conception must appear plausible, in that it should be capable of
solving problems that the old conception could not and it should cohesively
fit other conceptions and previous knowledge .
d) Lastly, the new conception should be fruitful, opening new doors for further
study and growth .
Nachtigall and Brouwer (as cited in Eryilmaz, 2002) provide a variation of
conceptual change designed to give students the "cognitive conflict necessary to help
them assimilate [new] conceptions into their everyday life" (p.1 003). Their method calls
for the teacher to:
a) help the students become aware of their misconceptions.
..
..
-
b) allow the students to define their misconceptions thoroughly and test them.
c) confront the students with situations where the misconceptions can not
adequately be used to explain or solve
d) let the students realize this conflict
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
Physics Education 14
e) help the student accommodate new ideas and conceptions
f) apply the new knowledge to familiar and new situations to validate the
power of the new conception over the old one.
g) give the students a feeling of progress and growth
h) test student understanding conceptually and quantitatively
(Nachtigal and Brouwer, 2002)
Eryilmaz used a variation of the above in a study of 396 high-school physics
students. The aim of the study was to see if conceptual change discussions, like the
Nachtigal and Brouwer model, and another concept called conceptual assignments, which
were homework assignments specifically involving physical phenomena from real life
instead of raw quantitative calculations, served to reduce misconceptions dealing with
force and motion. All of the students participated in regular physics classes and were then
divided into groups that did conceptual change discussions, conceptual assignments,
both, or neither. The students took tests to measure their pre-instruction misconceptions,
participated in the their assigned protocol for eight weeks, and were then tested for their
misconceptions again. The study concluded that conceptual change discussion was the
most effective means of reducing misconceptions and improving physics achievement.
There was not, however, much evidence that the conceptual assignments or the combined
protocol were particularly effective or in-effective in either regard. Despite their success,
the teachers of the conceptual change discussions did report added difficulty in
performing this protocol.
Bisard et al. (1994) offer another variation on Posner et al. 's conceptual change.
Their method is rooted in the exposure of the misconception, calling for the teacher to
specifically identify to the students that the conception of the students are
-
Physics Education 15
..
..
-
..
-
-
..
..
..
..
misconceptions. After this, the students are directly offered the correct scientific concept
in hopes of creating a "state of cognitive dissonance" (p.42). Class discussion furthers
this dissonance, forcing the students to relate and evaluate the conceptions, with the
intent of constructing "logical, coherent, and, most important, realistic knowledge of
science" (p.42). It should be noted that this option differs greatly from the Nachtigall and
Brouwer variation, because it does not allow for much, if any, personal discovery or
experimentation. Though this option seems logical and plausible but as a specific option
of its own it does not appear to have been tested.
Jose Mestre created his own method for helping students overcome
misconceptions (1991). Although he admits that there is no tried-and-true method for
such, he goes on to say that some approaches based on constructivism have shown to be
quite effective. His method, based on these and sounding fairly similar to the Nachtigall
and Brouwer method mentioned above, also claims to be constructivist, meaning that it
allows the students to construct their own knowledge. He first states that students will
accept a new conception over a misconception only if they fully understand the new
conception, it is believable and compatible with other conceptions, and it is useful for
interpreting and explaining other situations. Mestre's method consists of the teacher:
a) helping the students probe for the misconception
b) asking questions to clarify the beliefs of the students
c) suggesting events that contradict the beliefs of the students
-
..
-
d) encouraging debate and discussion to further ideas
e) guiding the students toward constructing scientific conceptions either
through synthesis or experimentation.
(Mestre, 1991)
-
Physics Education 16
...
...
...
...
...
...
...
...
...
...
-
...
In 2002, Trundle et at. decided to study a method of correcting misconceptions
based on Physics by Inquiry (McDennott, 1996), an established progressive physics
textbook. They state that their study used "an inquiry sequence of instructional activities
and strategies" (Trundle et at., 2002, p. 636). They used this method to counter
misconceptions of moon phases in preservice teachers. The students were interviewed at
the beginning of the study to detennine their misconceptions, but after that their
misconceptions were not discussed or mentioned. This is where this method varies from
the others mentioned. Instead, the students did research that slowly put them at odds with
their misconceptions. As the data came in, the students tried to justify their results by
constructing new conceptual models. The misconceptions were thus naturally confronted
by the students' desire to make sense of their observations. Much later in the semester
tenninology was added for greater understanding and the class completed exercises and
tests to verify their new understanding. This study found that at the end of the course
68.3% of the students had full scientific conceptions of moon phases. It should be noted
that their measure for detennining scientific conception was consisted of an interview
where the student explained the conception three-dimensionally using spheres and that
this measure was quite stringent. As low as a success rate of 68.30/0 sounds, it is still
significantly greater than all the other studies to correct moon phase misconceptions
mentioned in Trundle et al. 's study.
Key to all the methods of correcting misconceptions described above is the
...
-
concept th~t students must somehow be engaged with the thought that their initial
conception was indeed incorrect. Teachers must understand that the previous knowledge
of their students plays a major role in how they interpret and accommodate new
-
Physics Education 17
information and must be adequately addressed and belabored if change is to occur.
Teachers should examine and attempt to correct the misconceptions of their students, but
this can go too far. Mestre makes a special note that we should not "tum science
-
-
instruction into a witch-hunt for students' needs" (1991, p.S7). This is an important
concept, because misconceptions can easily steal time from laboratory investigation,
problem solving, and other important science skills. There are so many misconceptions in
so many areas of science that it can easily be overwhelming (Talanquer, 2002).
Constructivism and Inquiry
Based around an understanding of misconceptions is a contemporary view of
-
-
-
learning often called constructivism. Constructivism's formal title might be the
constructivist model of learning or constructivist epistemology, but central in all of these
terms is construction. Just as laborers physically construct a building, students "actively
construct the knowledge they possess" (Mestre, 2001, p.4S). Constructivism argues that
all of the knowledge an individual possesses is the result of having personally constructed
it. Jose Mestre, one of constructivist science education's greatest supporters, contends
that "constructing knowledge is a lifelong, effortful process requiring significant mental
engagement from the learner" (2001, p. 45). At any given time, a person's body of
knowledge is constructed in a way that makes sense to the current time and serves to
predict relevant events (Mestre, 1991).
-
Whether dealing with established science misconceptions, that need to be
deconstructed and then reconstructed, or science conceptions that students have never
Physics Education 18
-
thought about, that need their own construction, constructivism places a lot of emphasis
on the prior knowledge and sense making skills of the students. By taking advantage of
-
the already established conceptions and skills of students, teachers can adequately help
their students construct their own knowledge. Knowledge construction is often described
as an active process, so constructivist science calls for active student-driven learning.
-
-
-
"Hence," says Mestre, "instructional approaches where students are discussing physics
and doing physics, teaching each other physics and offering problem solution strategies
for evaluation by peers will facilitate the construction of physics knowledge" (Mestre,
2001, p. 46).
According to Haney, Lumpe, & Czerniak (2003), constructivist teaching includes
five specific components. These are scientific uncertainty, student negotiation, shared
control, critical voice, and personal relevance. Although these five are not directly
mentioned in all constructivist descriptions, they do offer insight into the constructivist
model of learning. According to Haney et al., "Teachers who believe and enact these
tenets of constructivism would present scientific knowledge as arising from human
experience and values, evolving and insecure, and culturally and socially determined"
(p.366). Students in a classroom like this would consider their ideas internally, listen to
-
have shared control in the 9utcomes of the class. Most importantly, Haney et al. express
-
meaningfully to their lives.
-
the ideas of their peers, and reflect on the relations and contradictions. Students would
that topics need to make use of the everyday experiences of the students and relate
One of the best ways for students to actively construct their own knowledge is
through a process called inquiry. Inquiry has been around in various forms since the early
...
..
Physics Education 19
1970's, but is fairly unified in concept. Central to inquiry is the idea presented in the
following quote by a student who was a part of an experimental inquiry-based classroom:
"Don't tell me the answer, I want help in finding out myself' (McDermott, 1974). The
National Science Education Standards, created in 1996 by the National Research Council,
defines inquiry in this fashion:
Inquiry is a multifaceted activity that involves making observations; posing
questions; examining books and other sources of information to see what is
already known; planning investigations; reviewing what is already known in
light of experimental evidence; using tools to gather, analyze, and interpret data;
proposing answers, explanations, and predictions; and communicating the
results. Inquiry requires identification of assumptions, use of critical and logical
thinking, and consideration of alternative explanations (p.23).
-
-
.-
In simpler terms, students use observation and experimentation along with scientific
reasoning and critical thinking to actively construct an understanding of science. Inquiry
can occur in laboratory-type experiments, conceptual group discussions, or many other
situations, but the key factor is always that the students evaluate and construct their own
knowledge themselves.
The National Science Education Standards define the goals of inquiry by its
ability to help students develop:
•
•
•
•
•
Understanding of scientific concepts.
An appreciation of "how we know" what we know in science.
Understanding of the nature of science.
Skills necessary to become independent inquirers about the natural
world.
The dispositions to use skills, abilities, and attitudes associated with
SCIence.
(National Research Council, 1996, p.1 05)
Understanding and skills, as mentioned above, are two key components of inquiry. By
trying to figure out scientifically sound conceptions on their own, students utilize the
-
Physics Education 20
skills and knowledge they already have to develop new skills that will help direct them
towards a greater understanding of science.
One way often used to approach inquiry is the Five-E method (Colburn, 1998).
Each of the five steps serves as a relevant path progression towards personal learning
through inquiry:
•
Engagement - teacher asks questions or gives statements, with very
little explanation, to cause students to think about their personal
knowledge.
•
Exploration
students actively learn through self-designed
experiments, activities, or discussions.
-
•
Explanation
students attempt to offer explanations for what they
have observed/studied, with teacher pointedly helping only when
necessary.
-
•
-
•
Extension - students attempt to apply their explanation in practical
terms.
Evaluation
teacher evaluates as desired, but should include a variety
of methods to account for different learning styles.
(Colburn, 1998)
Inquiry, as a leamer-centered educational experience, does not imply that the
-
students do whatever they wish. It is much more complicated than that. Teachers of
inquiry-based science have to do a lot of work. The teacher has to be more involved than
just giving knowledge. The teacher plays the role of facilitator rather than transmitter,
-
and therefore must be attentive and ready for questions with more questions and
situations. The teacher probes student understanding and helps them resolve conflicts
between scientific knowledge and their preconceptions. Thus, through inquiry and
constructivism, students do not "discover" everything for themselves. Instead, the teacher
-
..
Physics Education 21
-
-
must focus "on relating new knowledge both to previously learned knowledge and to
experiential phenomena so that students can build a consistent picture of the physical
world" (Mestre, 1991, p. 57).
Transmission vs. Constructivism
-
A teaching model in contrast to the constructivist/inquiry model is the prevalent
transmission model (Mestre, 1991). In the transmission model, students experience
learning through teacher-centered lectures, presentations, and readings. Assumed within
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
-
this model of learning is that the student actually absorbs what the teacher is trying to
transmit. Transmission is a teach-by-telling type of method, where student difficulties are
often attributed to an unclear or confusing presentation. This causation places the teacher
as the central player in the learning equation, as opposed to inquiry where the student is
the focus.
When most Americans think about school, they picture students sitting in little
desks in rows facing the front of the classroom, where the teacher stands at the
blackboard giving a lecture (Haney et al., 2003). This is the image of the American
classroom that most people have, because this is what they experienced, and what their
children are probably receiving. This is an image of the traditional classroom strictly
following a transmission model of learning. The transmission model works for almost all
subject areas, but is most easily associated with science and math classes. Conceptually
heavy subjects such as physics are most often taught using a transmission model
...
...
Physics Education 22
(Harrison et al., 1999), because of the perceived amount of content that must be learned
by the end of the school year (Tretter & Jones, 2003).
Despite the prevalence of the transmission model, one must remember that
frequency of use does not necessarily imply quality. In fact research shows that "students
...
...
...
...
learn best when actively engaged" (Mestre, 2001, pA8) and when "concrete experience
establishes the basis for the construction of scientific concepts" (McDermott & DeWater,
2000). Very little in the transmission model can be confused with being "actively
engaging. "
Often times the transmission model is defended by claims that its students often
achieve high grades on standardized tests. Studies have shown that students taught
physics problem-solving skills with the transmission model can in fact score quite well
on exams, but can still manage to display little understanding of the concepts underneath
...
-
-
(Mestre, 1991). There is no strong correlation between high grades and understanding
(Harrison et al., 1999). If the goal of science education is deeper conceptual
understanding of scientific principles, then the transmission model seems like a step
backwards.
When weighing transmission and constructivism, one really has to pick between
memorization and understanding, between breadth and depth (McDermott, 1974).
...
Transmission modeled classrooms often cover so many topics so quickly that little time
can actually be spent acquiring a true grasp of underlying concepts (McDermott et al.,
...
...
-
2000). Transmission physics classes all too often focus on quantitative skills at the cost of
qualitative reasoning .
-
-
Physics Education 23
Transmission model classrooms focus on algebraic formalism and definition
memorization, but their students can rarely relate these facts to real-life applications
(Nachtigall, 1990). Very few children can actually learn well this way. Except for the
fewer than one percent of high school students who might become physicists, one class in
physics is all most high school students will ever learn of physics, and such a teaching
style can actually have negative results (Nachtigall, 1990). Students do not need elegant
derivations and equations; they need relevance to their own lives and experiences.
Rushing through basic concepts can wear students out and tum them away from the joys
of science and physics. On the other hand, when students participate in creating their
-
-
-
knowledge, science can become an activity as opposed to a useless body of knowledge
(Etkina, 2000), and perhaps more than 1% of students will like physics enough to try it in
college.
Concerning standardized testing, a 2003 study in North Carolina found that there
was no significant difference in state proficiency exam scores between physical science
students who learned by a textbook-oriented approach and an inquiry-based approach
(Tretter & Jones, 2003). It should be noted that the standardized test studied and most
others are still aimed toward transmission-type learning, so it can be assumed that
inquiry-based learners might do significantly better if the tests were aimed towards
understanding instead of fact recitation. The researchers for this study also noted that the
inquiry group students had higher attendance rates and were more motivated.
The advantages of constructivism and inquiry-based learning seem to far
-
outweigh those of transmission. By this means, constructivism and inquiry have gained
much credibility in the eyes of science education specialists as viable frameworks for
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
-
Physics Education 24
teaching and learning models. Constructivism and inquiry, generally linked together,
have "become an intricate aspect of current educational reform and [are] included in
many of the national science education reform recommendations" (Haney et ai., 2003).
Inquiry plays an integral part in both Project 2061: Science for All Americans, an
initiative of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, and the National
Science Education Standards, written by the National Research Council and supported by
the National Science Teachers Association. The National Science Education Standards
believe so highly in inquiry that they go as far to say:
Inquiry is a critical component of a science program at all grade levels and in
every domain of science, and designers of curricula and programs must be sure
that the approach to content, as ~ell as the teaching and assessment strategies,
reflect the acquisition of scientific understanding through inquiry. Students then
will learn science in a way that reflects how science actually works.
(National Research Council, 1996, p. 214)
All formality aside, perhaps the best endorsement comes from a group of secondary
science teachers. After taking a course in inquiry physical science, the nine in-service
-
teachers had this to say: "The best opportunity for establishing long-term conceptual
understandings in science lies in using the inquiry approach" (Kleine et ai., 2002, p. 39).
-
-
"Teachers Teach as They Were Taught"
Despite all the evidence in support of constructivism and inquiry, little is done in
real classrooms to conceptually change misconceptions or to introduce inquiry and
constructive learning. Inquiry may be the better method than transmission, but most
science teachers do not use inquiry as an educational method (Kleine et ai., 2002). There
-
are several reasons why most science teachers don't practice inquiry in their classrooms,
Physics Education 25
-
-
including fear that content moves too slowly and unwillingness to put forth the extra
effort, but the biggest deterrent from getting inquiry into all science classrooms is a lack
of teacher understanding of inquiry because few of them learned using inquiry.
-
Research verifies the cliche, "teachers teach as they were taught" (Stein, 2001;
McDermott, 1990). Whether intentional or not, teaching methods are learned by example
(McDermott & DeWater, 2000). Teachers tend to imitate their instructors in both style
-
-
-
and content (Nachtigall, 1990; McDermott, 1974). The transmission model thus pervades
classroom instruction mainly by default, because for most teachers it is the method by
which they were taught (Mestre, 1991). Having never seen an instructional method that
differs, secondary science teachers all but have to follow the transmission method. Thus
it persists with little theoretical justification, carried on the shoulders of tradition.
The pervasiveness of the transmission method in high schools can therefore be
attributed to the college science departments and education departments for not giving
preservice teachers opportunities to learn about inquiry or participate in inquiry. It is
common belief in physics departments that the quality of a high school teacher is
determined by the number and difficulty of physics courses taken (McDermott &
DeWater, 2000). By this measure, high school physics teachers might know conciderable
amounts of theoretical physics, but will not be able to convey it to their students in a way
that the students can understand. Although content knowledge is important, it does not
-
insure that a teacher will be able to teach as described above.
Physics and physical science teachers tend to learn their physics content
completely separate from instructional methodology, which decreases the effectiveness of
both (McDermott, 1990). Prospective teachers need to learn their elementary physical
Physics Education 26
-
concepts in depth for a sound understanding and the ability to explain the underlying
reasoning (McDermott, 1990). This requires a lot more time per topic than traditional
introductory physics classes allow, and would probably call for a separate class for
prospective teachers, delving deeply into a smaller number of topics (Zollman, 1994;
McDermott, 1990; Mestre 1991).
Just as science instruction for students has proven to be most effective when real
-
experiences are used to construct scientific thought, the same holds true for adults. Much
like the correction methods for dealing with misconceptions, studies have shown that
adults, including preservice and inservice teachers, learn best in the same situations that
..
are best for children (McDermott & DeWater, 2000). This means that not only would
teachers benefit pedagogically from an inquiry-based class, but that their content
••
knowledge would also dramatically benefit. To really be able to teach using inquiry, it
-
almost becomes a necessity that teachers participate in inquiry-based content specific
••
..
...
class.
Such a course should "emphasize the content the teachers are expected to teach"
(McDermott, 1990, p. 737), should present topics "in a way that is consistent with how
they [the preservice teachers] are expected to teach that material" (McDermott, 2000, p.
412), and should certainly be hands on and laboratory based (Wilson, 1991; McDermott,
2000). To make this class truly inquiry-based and meaningful to future teachers, the
college professor would have to teach as he/she was probably never taught and put in the
extra work to provide proper examples to challenge the teachers' misconceptions without
giving any of the answers away to the class easily, just as the teachers will have to do in
the high schools.
-
Physics Education 27
-
The only way to stop the misconception cycle and the transmission cycle is for
preservice physics and physical science teachers to be taught their physics content and
physics pedagogy in the same manner that science reform movements call for them to do.
-
Science Education faculty often swear by the National Science Education Standards and
Project 2061: Science for All Americans, but they rarely hold their students accountable
for the content of these. The content of these texts, being the proper pedagogy for both
students and teachers, should therefore be used in all levels of education. Science and
Science Education departments of all colleges should embrace The Standards and Project
-
..
..
2061 in their science classrooms to ensure that all future teachers receive the training they
will need to be better teachers. After taking classes that involve true inquiry and discuss
inquiry and pedagogy, teachers should be able to utilize the cliche. "Teachers teach as
they were taught" can actually take on a proper dignified meaning if tomorrow's teachers
are taught properly using the concepts of constructivism, inquiry, and conceptual change
for misconceptions today.
..
--
-
-
Physics Education 28
Figures
..
..
..
Figure 1:
PERCENT CORRECT IV GROUP
60
-
..
..
..
-
..
-
..
..
..
-
55
E
au so
II:C
~
(.)
i
I
45
~
~
w
40
M
35
30
GROUP
nSTl.O
from Bisard, Aron, Francek, & Nelson, 1994
..
..
..
-
..
-
Physics Education 29
References
Alparslan, C., Tekkaya, C., & Geban, 6., (2003). Using the conceptual change
instruction to improve learning. Journal of Biological Education, 37(3), 133-137.
Beaty, W. (2001). Children's misconceptions about science, The Science Hobbyist
webpage, Retrieved April 17, 2004 from the World Wide Web:
http://www.amasci.comimiscon/opphys.html
Berg, T. & Brouwer, W. (1991) Teacher awareness of student alternative conceptions
about rotational motion and gravity. Journal of Research in Science Teaching
003-18.
Blosser, P. (1987). Science misconceptions: Research and some implications for
the teaching of science to elementary school students. ERIC/SMEAC
Science Education Digest, 1, Retrieved September 14, 2003 from
http://web5.silverplatter.comlwebspirs/showFullRecordContent.ws
Bisard, W.J., Aron, R. H., Francek, M. A., & Nelson, B. D. (1994). Assessing selected
Pphysical science and earth science misconceptions of middle school
through university preservice teachers: Breaking the science "misconception
cycle". Journal of College Science Teaching, 24(1), 38-42.
Callison, P.L. and Wright, E.L. (1993). The effect of teaching strategies using models
on peservice elementary teacher's conceptions about Earth-Sun-Moon
relationships. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Association
for Research in Science Teaching, April 1993.
Colburn, A. (1998). Constructivism and science teaching. Phi Delta Kappa Fastback,
435,7-44.
..
..
-
Crockett, C. (2004). What do kids know - and misunderstand - about science?
Educational Leadership, 61(5), 34-37.
Eryilmaz, A. (2002). Effects of conceptual assignments and conceptual change
discussions on students' misconceptions and achievement regarding
force and motion. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39(10),
1001-1015.
Etkina, E. (2000). Can we use the process of physics to guide physics instruction?
Submitted to the American Journal of Physics.
Gonzalez-Espada, W. (2003). A last chance for getting it right: Addressing
alternative conceptions in the physical sciences. The Physics Teacher,
41(1), 36-38 .
..
-
-
..
..
..
..
..
Physics Education 30
Haney,J.J., Lumpe, A.T., & Czerniak, C.M. (2003). Constructivist beliefs about the
science classroom learning environment: Perspectives from teachers,
administrators, parents, community members, and students. School Science and
Math, 103, 366 .
Harrison, A.G., Grayson, D. J., & Treagust, D. F. (1999). Investigating a grade
11 student's evolving conceptions of heat and temperature. Journal of
Research In Science Teaching, 36(1), 55-87 .
Jasien, P. G., & Oberem, G. E. (2002). Understanding of elementary concepts in
heat and temperature among college students and K-12 teachers .
Journal of Chemical Education, 79(7), 889-895.
Kliene, K., Brown, B., Harte, B.; Hilson, A., Malone, D., Moller, K., Niblett, A., Toole,
M., & Walker, B. (2002) examining inquiry. Principal Leadership, November,
36-39.
Mestre, J.P. (1991). Learning and instruction in pre-college physical science.
Physics Today, September, 56-62.
Mestre, J.P. (2001). Implications of research on learning for the education of
prospective science and physics teachers. Physics Education, 36, 44-51.
McDermott, L.C. (1974) Combined physics course for future elementary and secondary
school teachers. American Journal of Physics, 42, 668-676.
McDermott, L. C. (1990). A perspective on teacher preparation in physics and other
sciences: The need for special science courses for teachers. American Journal
of Physics 58, 734-742.
..
..
..
..
..
-
McDermott, L.C. (1996). Physics by inquiry. New York: Wiley.
McDermott, L.C. & DeWater, L. S. (2000). The need for special science courses for
teachers: Two perspectives. In J. Minstrell & E.H. van Zee (Eds.), Inquiring into
inquiry learning and teaching in science (pp. 241-257). Washington D.C.:
American Association for the Advancement of Science
McDermott, L.C., Shaffer, P.S., & Constantinou, C.P. (2000). Preparing teachers to
teach physics and physical science by inquiry. Physics Education, 35(6),
411-416.
Nachtigall, D.K. (1990). What is wrong with physics teacher's education? European
Journal of Physics, 11(#), 1-14.
..
Physics Education 31
National Research Council (1996). National science education standards. Washington
D.C.: National Academy.
Olenick, R.P. (2001). Helping students learn physics better: students' alternative
conceptions in introductory physics, C3P website, Retrieved April 17, 2004 from
http://phys.udallas.edulC3P/altconcp.htInl
..
..
..
..
..
..
..
Posner, GJ., Strike, K.A., Hewson, P.W., and Gertzog, W.A. (1982). Accomodation ofa
scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change. Science
Education, 66, 211-227 .
Talanquer, V. (2002). Minimizing misconception: Tools for identifying patterns of
reasoning. The Science Teacher, 69(8), 46-49 .
Tibell, G. (2000). An attempt at innovation in teacher training in Sweden. Physics
Education, 35(3), 160-163 .
Tretter, T.R. & Jones, M.G. (2003).Relationships between inquiry-based teaching and
physical science standardized scores. School Science and Math, 103, 345-350 .
Trumper, R., & Gorsky, P. (1996). A cross-college age study about physics students'
conceptions of force in pre-service training for high school teachers. Physics
Education, 31, 227-236 .
Trumper, R. (2001). A cross-college age study of science and nonscience students'
conceptions of basic astronomy concepts in preservice training for high-school
teachers. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 10, 189-195.
Trundle, K. C., Atwood, R. K., & Christopher, J. E. (2002). Pre-service elementary
teachers' conceptions of moon phases before and after instruction.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39(7), 633-658.
Yager, R.E. (Ed.). (1993, October) Salish proposal: Linking teacher preparation
outcomes and teacher performance. Unpublished grant proposal submitted to the
U.S. Department of Education .
..
..
..
-
Download