Characterising repetitive behaviours in young boys with fragile X syndrome

advertisement
bs_bs_banner
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
doi: 10.1111/jir.12234
54
VOLUME
60 PART 1 pp 54–67 JANUARY 2016
Characterising repetitive behaviours in young boys with
fragile X syndrome
A. Oakes, A. J. Thurman, A. McDuffie, L. M. Bullard, R. J. Hagerman & L. Abbeduto
MIND Institute, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA, USA
Abstract
Background Repetitive behaviours are frequently
observed in individuals with intellectual disability
(ID). The present study examined the profile, intercorrelations and predictive correlates of repetitive
behaviours in boys with fragile X syndrome (FXS),
the leading inherited cause of ID. Specific child
characteristics examined as predictors included anxiety, nonverbal cognition and autism social–affective
symptomatology.
Method Participants were 39 boys with FXS (aged
6–10 years). Repetitive behaviours were measured
using the Repetitive Behavior Scale – Revised
(RBS-R) – a 43-item caregiver-report measure
normed on individuals with ID.
Results Restricted Interests and Sensory Motor
behaviours were reported as most problematic for this
sample of boys, whereas Self-injurious behaviours
were less problematic. All subscales of the RBS-R
were significantly inter-correlated. Nonverbal IQ was
negatively related, whereas anxiety and social affective
symptoms of autism spectrum disorder were
positively related, to scores for Restricted Interests.
Anxiety was also positively related to scores for
Compulsive behaviours and Ritualistic Sameness
behaviours.
Correspondence: Ashley Oakes, MIND Institute, University of
California Davis, Sacramento, CA, USA (e-mail: ashley.
oakes1219@gmail.com).
Conclusions This study provides a preliminary
description of repetitive behaviours in boys with FXS,
which may form the groundwork for future research.
Keywords autism, behavioural phenotype, fragile X
syndrome, repetitive behaviours
Introduction
Repetitive behaviours refer to a broad heterogeneous
category of behaviours characterised by the repetition
of unvarying movements (Edwards et al. 2012),
rigidity and inappropriateness (Lewis & Kim 2009).
Typically developing infants and young children
commonly engage in repetitive behaviours and
activities. In infancy, these behaviours take the form
of banging, shaking, rocking and mouthing (Thelen
1981; Lifter 2000). Typically developing toddlers
frequently display action patterns characterised by
ordering objects, attachment to favourite objects and
preferred routines for engaging in everyday activities.
These types of ritualised behaviours, characterised by
insistence on sameness and things being ‘just right’,
commonly begin at about 14 months of age in
typically developing children and decline by the age of
4 years (Evans et al. 1997; MacDonald et al. 2007).
During this developmental period, these behaviours
are presumed to be functional by scaffolding the
precision and efficiency of motor movements (Thelen
1981), facilitating the development of mastery
motivation and self-regulation (e.g. Kopp 1982;
© 2015 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
VOLUME
60 PART 1 JANUARY 2016
55
A. Oakes et al. • Repetitive behaviours in FXS
Jennings 2002) or serving as a mechanism to alleviate
anxiety (Evans et al. 1997).
If repetitive behaviours persist later on in
development, there can come a time when the
presence of these behaviours begins to impede day-today functioning by creating a barrier to learning and
social interaction (Leekam et al., 2011). In fact,
repetitive behaviours are frequently observed in a
wide range of developmental [e.g. ID and
developmental disabilities and autism spectrum
disorder (ASD)] and psychiatric (e.g. schizophrenia
and obsessive–compulsive disorder) conditions
(e.g. Parkinson disease). Motor stereotypies occur
more frequently in younger and more
developmentally delayed individuals with autism,
whereas preoccupations, restricted interests and
obsessions are more often observed in individuals on
the spectrum with higher language and cognitive
abilities (Bishop et al. 2006; Richler et al. 2010).
Numerous behaviours are included in the broad
umbrella of repetitive behaviour, including
stereotypies, ritualistic behaviours, obsessive and
compulsive behaviours, restricted interests,
perseverations and self-injurious behaviours.
Furthermore, the likelihood of specific behaviours
varies across disorders (Moss et al. 2009). Numerous
categorisation approaches have also been utilised in
the investigation of repetitive behaviours. Some
researchers have argued for the use of broad
categorical classifications (e.g. motorically based
behaviours and/or sensory behaviours and higher
cognitively based behaviours), whereas others have
argued that these broad categories may be too
simplistic and mask more subtle, but important,
differences in the topographies and functions of
repetitive behaviours within and across disorders. For
example, there are likely a variety of mechanisms (e.g.
cognitive, communicative and anxiety) underlying the
presence of repetitive behaviours, which impacts the
management of these behaviours (Leekam et al.,
2011). That is, repetitive behaviours that serve as a
method of alleviating anxiety in one child likely
warrants a different management approach than
repetitive behaviours that serve as a method of
obtaining a preferred object.
The current study was designed to describe
patterns of repetitive behaviours in children with
fragile X syndrome (FXS), the leading inherited
cause of ID (Crawford et al. 2001). Clinical reports
suggest that repetitive behaviours are ubiquitous in
boys with the full mutation of this disorder, although
these behaviours have not been thoroughly described
or characterised. Given that the full mutation of
FXS in boys is also associated with high rates of ID,
anxiety and ASD symptoms (Hagerman 2006;
Harris et al. 2008; Hessl et al. 2009; Cordiero et al.
2011), the factors that drive the emergence of
repetitive behaviours in FXS remain poorly
understood. Clearly, there are many ways in which
repetitive behaviours can be categorised in
individuals with ID and development disabilities.
Thus, it is important to acknowledge that any
particular approach will affect the profile of the
repetitive behaviours obtained.
Fragile X syndrome
It is estimated that 1 in 4000 boys and 1 in 6000 to
8000 girls are affected with FXS [Centers for Disease
Control and Prevention (CDC) 2011]. FXS is caused
by a mutation of the fragile X mental retardation 1
gene (FMR1; Verkerk et al. 1991) on the X
chromosome, and this mutation interferes with the
production of the fragile X mental retardation protein
(Bell et al. 1991). The normal FMR1 allele is
comprised of 5–54 CGG repeats. Individuals with the
‘full mutation’ have expansions exceeding 200 repeats
and typically display the FXS behavioural phenotype
(Oostra & Willemsen 2003). Because it is an X-linked
condition, FXS is more common in boys than girls,
and boys are more severely affected given the
protective presence of an unaffected X chromosome
in girls (Crawford et al. 2001). Due to this difference
in phenotypic expression, the current study focused
only on boys with FXS.
Although there is variability in the expression of
behavioural symptoms in boys with FXS, a specific
behavioural phenotype is characteristically observed.
Approximately 85% of boys with full mutation FXS
have IQs less than 70 (Hagerman 2006; Hessl et al.
2009). The majority of boys with FXS also meet the
criteria for an anxiety disorder, including social
phobias, generalised anxiety and obsessive–
compulsive disorder (Hessl et al. 2008; Cordiero et al.
2011; Hall et al. 2012). In addition, boys with FXS are
likely to have delays, relative to age expectations, in
multiple domains of language, including vocabulary,
morphosyntax and the functional use of
© 2015 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
VOLUME
60 PART 1 JANUARY 2016
56
A. Oakes et al. • Repetitive behaviours in FXS
conversational language for social communication
(Abbeduto et al. 2007).
The limited evidence available suggests that the
FXS phenotype is associated with increased risk for
repetitive behaviours. Verbal perseveration is
frequently described in boys with FXS and
considered by some researchers to be a hallmark
feature of the behavioural phenotype of this disorder
(Sudhalter et al. 1990; Abbeduto et al. 2007; Roberts
et al. 2008). Compared with other syndromes,
researchers have found elevated levels of other types
of repetitive behaviours in boys with FXS, including
hand flapping, rocking and self-injury (Hagerman
2002).
Although repetitive behaviours have been reported
in FXS at a global level, there is limited research
characterising the relative rates of different types of
repetitive behaviours in FXS or exploring the
mechanisms that underlie their development. Such
research is important given that different
subcategories of repetitive behaviour may have
different neurobiological underpinnings and different
behavioural correlates, with these differences affecting
the potential efficacy of different behavioural or
pharmacological treatments. Recently, Wolff and
colleagues (Wolff et al. 2012) used the Repetitive
Behavior Scale – Revised (RBS-R; Bodfish et al.
2000) to examine the profile of repetitive behaviours
present in a group of 27 preschool-aged boys with
FXS. Results indicated that ratings were highest for
stereotyped and sameness behaviours and the lowest
for compulsive, ritualistic and self-injurious
behaviours.
Utilising the Repetitive Behavior Questionnaire
(RBQ), which shares some individual items with the
RBS-R, Moss & colleagues (2009) examined the
presence of repetitive behaviours across six groups of
individuals with IDs including boys with FXS who
ranged in age from 6 to 47 years. Participants with
FXS were reported to have significantly more
repetitive behaviours than at least two other
disability groups in the categories of compulsive
behaviour, insistence on sameness and repetitive
speech. In terms of individual behaviour items, Moss
& colleagues (2009) reported that boys with FXS
scored significantly higher than at least two other ID
groups in hand stereotypies, tidying up, lining up,
preference for routine, just right behaviours and
three types of verbal perseveration including
restricted conversation, repetitive phrases and
echolalia.
Because Wolff et al. examined repetitive behaviours
in a very young group of children and Moss et al.
included boys with FXS who ranged in age from
young childhood to adulthood, it is not yet clear
whether the profile of repetitive behaviour in boys
with FXS remains stable across development or if it
differs based upon an individual’s degree of delay.
Furthermore, there remains much that we do not
understand about the FXS behavioural phenotype,
including the extent to which the different types of
repetitive behaviours associated with FXS emerge
from the same or different underlying mechanisms.
The present study was designed to begin addressing
these gaps by investigating the profile and potential
predictors of repetitive behaviours in a group of
school-aged boys with FXS.
Factors relating to repetitive behaviours
Recent studies have shown that 25–60% of individuals
with FXS meet the criteria for ASD (e.g. Harris et al.
2008). Qualitatively, it is often the presence of
repetitive behaviours that suggests overlap in the
behavioural and neurobiological phenotypes of the
two disorders (Kau et al. 2004; Baranek et al. 2005;
Symons et al. 2010). However, there is growing
evidence that the same behavioural symptoms in FXS
and ASD may reflect different underlying
mechanisms (Gallagher & Hallahan 2012; Wolff et al.
2012; McDuffie et al. 2015). This distinction is
important given that behavioural and
pharmacological treatments may fail to be effective
across a range of disorders if underlying causes differ
between disorders.
Indeed, Moss et al. (2009) found that, whereas
individuals with FXS displayed the highest frequency
and largest number of different topographies of
repetitive behaviour relative to individuals with eight
other low-incidence neurodevelopmental disorders,
only one individual item from the Compulsive
subscale of the RBQ (Moss & Oliver 2008) was
correlated with ASD symptomatology scores as
measured by the Autism Screening Questionnaire
(Berument et al. 1999). This finding suggests that
many repetitive behaviours commonly seen in FXS
may not be associated with other symptoms of ASD.
Consistent with this conclusion, other studies have
© 2015 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
VOLUME
60 PART 1 JANUARY 2016
57
A. Oakes et al. • Repetitive behaviours in FXS
found many children with FXS present with
compulsive or self-injurious behaviours but show no
other symptoms associated with a diagnosis of ASD
(Hall et al. 2008; Symons et al. 2003). Furthermore,
Wolff et al. (2012) found that both boys with FXS and
boys with FXS + ASD had fewer compulsive and
ritualistic behaviours than did young boys with
nonsyndromic ASD. Taken together, these studies
suggest that repetitive behaviours in FXS and
nonsyndromic ASD may differ in extent and type and
that ASD symptomatology may not be the primary
risk factor for the development of repetitive
behaviours in FXS.
Thus, it is important to consider factors beyond
ASD when investigating the mechanisms underlying
repetitive behaviours in FXS. For example, cognitive
delays (Kover et al. 2013; Sansone et al., 2014) and
anxiety (Cordiero et al. 2011; Talisa et al. 2014) may
be important risk factors for the development of at
least some types of repetitive behaviours (Miguel
et al. 1997; Gabriels et al. 2005). In the case of FXS,
therefore, it is possible that high rates of repetitive
behaviours covary with lower levels of cognitive
functioning as well as with higher levels of anxiety
and are orthogonal to the social–affective
impairments at the core of an ASD diagnosis.
Taken together, these findings suggest that
individuals with FXS present with multiple risk
factors for the development of repetitive behaviours
and that there is a need to determine the correlates
of these behavioural symptoms as a first step
towards identifying underlying neurobiological
mechanisms.
Research questions
Given the cumulative impact of repetitive behaviours
on participation in learning activities, acceptance by
peers and family functioning (Boyd et al. 2012), and
the implications for treatment, the goal of the current
study was to develop a more nuanced description of
repetitive behaviours in school-aged boys with the full
mutation of FXS. To do this, we used the RBS-R to
examine the profile, item-level scores, subscale intercorrelations and longitudinal correlates of repetitive
behaviours. In particular, we were interested in
examining whether nonverbal IQ, anxiety and social–
affective symptoms of ASD were predictively
associated with the later presence of repetitive
behaviours in this sample of children. The study
addressed the following questions:
1.
2.
3.
4.
What is the profile of repetitive behaviours for
boys with FXS?
What specific repetitive behaviours are most
frequent for boys with FXS?
What are the inter-correlations among the categories of repetitive behaviours?
Which participant characteristics predict the
various categories of repetitive behaviours later
in development?
Method
Participants and setting
Participants were 39 boys with FXS who ranged in
age from 6 to 10 years (M = 7.41 years) at the initial
time point and who participated in a larger,
longitudinal project on early word learning. Although
other papers have been published from this larger
project (McDuffie et al. 2012; McDuffie et al. 2013;
Oakes et al. 2013; Kover et al. 2014; McDuffie et al.
2015; Thurman et al. 2014), none has focused on the
questions of interest in the present study. Participants
were recruited nationally using a variety of sources,
including postings to internet listservs and websites,
newspaper advertisements, flyers at parent meetings
and a university research registry. Prior to
participation, documentation confirming a diagnosis
of the FMR1 full mutation through molecular genetic
testing was provided for each participant. In addition,
diagnosis was confirmed during the study through
molecular genetic testing of peripheral blood samples
(although testing was not performed for three
participants: two due to logistical reasons and one due
to participant refusal of the blood draw). All
participants met the following criteria: English as the
native language, speech as the primary means of
communication, no uncorrected sensory or physical
impairments that would affect participation in the
study, and no more than a mild hearing loss, all
determined from caregiver report. Written consent
was given by all caregivers prior to participation.
Within the larger project, testing was implemented at
two time points approximately 18 months apart. The
Time 1 (T1) visit took place over three consecutive
days and the Time 2 (T2) visit was completed in one
© 2015 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
VOLUME
60 PART 1 JANUARY 2016
58
A. Oakes et al. • Repetitive behaviours in FXS
day. All sessions were conducted at a university clinic.
All assessments were administered by trained
examiners.
For inclusion in the present study, participants
had relevant data available for both T1 and T2. At
T1, participants were administered measures of
nonverbal cognitive ability [i.e. Leiter International
Performance Scale – Revised (Leiter-R; Roid &
Miller 1997)], maladaptive behaviours [i.e. Anxiety,
Depression, and Mood Scale (ADAMS; Esbensen
et al. 2003)], and autism diagnostic assessments [i.e.
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS;
Lord et al. 1999)]. Two participants in the current
study were unable to complete the Leiter-R; for
these two participants, floor scores on the Leiter-R
at their chronological age level were used.
Participant characteristics are presented in Table 1.
The RBS-R (Bodfish et al. 2000) was administered
at T2.
Measures
The RBS-R (Bodfish et al. 2000) is a 43-item
caregiver-report measure normed on individuals with
ID. Each item is scored on a 4-point Likert scale
ranging from 0 (behaviour does not occur) to 3
(behaviour occurs and is a severe problem). Although
there are many approaches to the measurement of
repetitive behaviours in individuals with ID and
Table 1 Participant characteristics at time 1 (n = 39)
Chronological agea
Nonverbal IQb
Anxietyc
Autism symptom severityd
Caucasian
Mothers with college degreese
a
Mean
SD
7.41
59.26
5.52
5.90
2.03
14.90
3.26
2.11
Frequency
32
33
at T1;
brief IQ scores from the Leiter-R;
c
general Anxiety subscale from the ADAMS;
d
from the ADOS (Gotham, Pickles, & Lord, 2009);
e
missing for one participant.
Leiter-R, Leiter International Performance Scale – Revised; ADAMS, Anxiety, Depression, and Mood Scale; ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule.
b
development disabilities (i.e. RBQ; Moss et al.,
2009), we utilised the RBS-R (Bodfish et al. 2000).
The RBS-R has been widely used to characterise the
presence of repetitive behaviours, particularly in
individuals with autism symptomatology. Multiple
studies have empirically evaluated the validity of the
factor structure of the RBS-R (Lam & Aman 2007;
Mirenda et al. 2010; Bishop et al. 2013; Harrop et al.
2014). Recently, Bishop & colleagues (2013)
employed the largest sample to date in their
examination of the RBS-R factor structure. Thus, we
adopted the Bishop et al. (2013) 5-factor structure and
item distribution in the current study. The five
subscales included in the current study were as
follows: Sensory Motor, Restricted Interests, Selfinjury, Compulsive, and Ritualistic/Sameness. The
RBS-R was administered as the T2 outcome
measure.
The ADOS (Lord et al. 1999) is a semistructured, direct assessment of communication,
social interaction, play/imagination and repetitive
behaviours used to evaluate ASD symptoms.
The ADOS was administered during the T1 visit
by an examiner trained to research reliability
standards. Calibrated severity scores for the Social
Affective domain of the ADOS (Hus et al. 2012)
were used as a longitudinal predictor of repetitive
behaviours at T2.
The Leiter-R (Roid & Miller 1997) Brief IQ
screener (i.e. Figure Ground, Form Completion,
Sequential Order and Repeated Patterns subscales)
was collected at the T1 visit. These subtests measure
visualisation and fluid reasoning kills and yield a
nonverbal IQ score, age-equivalent score and growth
score. The Leiter-R is administered in an entirely
nonverbal manner and does not require any spoken or
written output from the participant. The examiner
uses pantomime and nonverbal cues to explain the
task and participants respond by pointing or placing
shapes or cards in a grid.
The ADAMS (Esbensen et al. 2003) is a 28-item
caregiver-report measure. Five subscales are
generated from the individual items:
Manic/Hyperactive, Depressed Mood, Social
Avoidance, General Anxiety, and
Obsessive/Compulsive. The General Anxiety subscale
of the ADAMS was used as the metric of anxiety for
the current study. This scale was completed by
caregivers during the T1 visit.
© 2015 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
VOLUME
60 PART 1 JANUARY 2016
59
A. Oakes et al. • Repetitive behaviours in FXS
Analysis plan
Mean subscale scores were used to address research
questions 1, 3 and 4. Because the data violated the
nonparametric assumption of normality,
nonparametric analyses were used in the present
project. A Friedman’s analysis of variance was used to
determine if repetitive behaviours differed as a
function of the RBS-R subscale. This analysis was
followed up with Wilcoxon rank sum tests to
determine whether any between subscale differences
were significant. We then descriptively examined
caregiver ratings on the individual items within each
subscale by graphing the percentage of caregivers who
endorsed the presence of individual behaviours as
being moderate to severe in nature. Finally, one-tailed
Spearman rank correlations were used to evaluate the
relations between the different subcategories of
repetitive behaviours and to examine the predictive
association between child characteristics at T1 and
repetitive behaviours at T2. We predicted a negative
association between T1 nonverbal IQ and T2
repetitive behaviours and positive correlations
between anxiety and social affective symptoms of
ASD at T1 and repetitive behaviours at T2.
Results
Profile of repetitive behaviour as measured by the
Repetitive Behavior Scale – Revised
Caregiver ratings significantly differed as a function of
RBS-R subscale, 003C72 (4) = 55.67, P < 0.001
(Fig. 1). Wilcoxon tests revealed that Restricted
Interests were rated as significantly more of a problem
than Self-injury, Compulsive or Ritualistic/Sameness
behaviours (Ps < 0.05). In addition, Sensory Motor
behaviours were rated as significantly more of a
problem than were Self-injurious or Compulsive
behaviours. No other significant differences were
observed across subscales.
Item-level examination of repetitive behaviours
We followed up these subscale-level findings with an
item-level examination of individual repetitive
behaviours (Fig. 2; Table 2). Seven items were
included within the Sensory Motor subscale. Of these
items, hand/finger stereotypies and sensory difficulties
were reported to be most problematic with
approximately 49% and 36% of caregivers rating these
behaviours as moderate to severe problems,
respectively. In contrast, whole body and head
stereotypies were reported to be the least problematic
behaviours for boys with FXS, with approximately 8%
and 0% of caregivers rating these behaviours as
moderate to severe problems, respectively. Two items
are included in the Restricted Interests subscale:
fascination/preoccupation with one subject or activity
and strongly attached to one specific object. These
items were relatively comparable with approximately
44% and 39%, of caregivers rating these behaviours as
moderate to severe problems, respectively. In terms of
Self-injury, 15% of caregivers reported hitting self
with a body part to be a moderate to severe problem
in their sons with FXS. All other behaviours in the
Self-injury subscale were seldom endorsed as a
moderate or severe problem (<8%).
Of the 10 items within the Compulsive subscale,
two behaviours emerged as problematic: 26% of
caregivers reported completeness (e.g. must have
Figure 1 Mean rating scores for the five
Repetitive Behavior Scale – Revised
subscales.
© 2015 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
VOLUME
60 PART 1 JANUARY 2016
60
A. Oakes et al. • Repetitive behaviours in FXS
Figure 2 Percentage of caregivers reporting behaviours to be a moderate to severe problem.
doors opened or closed and takes all items out of a
container or area) to be a moderate to severe problem
and 28% of caregivers reported sleeping/bedtime
rituals to be a moderate to severe problem. Finally, of
the 11 items within the Ritualistic/Sameness subscale,
26% of caregivers reported visiting new places as a
moderate to severe problem, and 36% of caregivers
reported resisting changing activities/difficulties with
transitions as a moderate to severe problem.
Inter-correlations across the Repetitive Behavior
Scale – Revised subscales
Spearman correlation coefficients were used to
examine associations between the five RBS-R
subscales. These analyses indicated that all of the
RBS-R subscales were significantly inter-correlated
(Table 3).
Relations between T1 participant characteristics
and repetitive behaviours at T2
Spearman correlations were also used to examine the
extent to which nonverbal IQ, anxiety and ASD
social–affective symptomatology at T1 were
predictively correlated with the RBS-R subscales at
T2 (Table 4). The ADAMS General Anxiety subscale
was significantly and positively related to the RBS-R
Restricted Interests (P = 0.02), Compulsive (P = 0.02)
and Ritualistic/Sameness (P = 0.003) subscales. The
Leiter-R Brief IQ was significantly and negatively
related to the RBS-R Restricted Interests subscale
(P = 0.03). Finally, ASD social–affective
symptomatology was significantly and positively
associated with the RBS-R Restricted Interests
subscale (P = 0.03).
Discussion
In this study, we sought to develop a more nuanced
understanding of repetitive behaviours in school-age
boys with FXS. To achieve this goal, we described the
subscale-level and item-level profiles of repetitive
behaviours reported by mothers and examined the
inter-correlations among different categories of
repetitive behaviours. Additionally, we evaluated
several child characteristics as potential longitudinal
predictors of different categories of repetitive
behaviours. Insight into the presence and correlates of
repetitive behaviours in FXS can help to further
elucidate the behavioural phenotype of this
neurodevelopmental disorder with the ultimate goal
of identifying appropriate pharmacological and
behavioural targets for intervention. Such insights are
also conceptually useful in deriving theoretical models
of repetitive behaviours in FXS that can then be
compared with profiles and predictors for other
neurodevelopmental disorders.
Using the RBS-R, a widely used informant report
measure of repetitive behaviours, we found that some
classes of repetitive behaviours were more
© 2015 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
VOLUME
60 PART 1 JANUARY 2016
61
A. Oakes et al. • Repetitive behaviours in FXS
Table 2 Repetitive Behavior Scale – Revised (RBS-R) items and
subscales according to Bishop et al.’s (2013) 5-factor structure
Subscale
Sensory Motor
Restricted
Interest
Self-injury
Compulsive
Ritualistic/
Sameness
Table 3 Concurrent associations across RBS-R subscale scores
Restricted SelfInterests
injury
Item
1. Whole body
2. Head
3. Hand/finger
4. Locomotion
5. Object usage
6. Sensory
43. Fascination, preoccupation with
movement
40. Fascination, preoccupation with one
subject or activity
41. Strongly attached to one specific object
7. Hits self with body part
8. Hits self against surface or object
9. Hits self with object
10. Bites self
11. Pulls
12. Rubs or scratches self
13. Inserts finger or object
14. Skin picking
15. Arranging/ordering
16. Completeness
17. Washing/cleaning
18. Checking
19. Counting
20. Hoarding/saving
21. Repeating
24. Sleeping/bedtime
25. Self-care (bathroom and dressing)
29. Insists that things remain in the same
place(s)
26. Travel/transportation
27. Play/leisure
30. Objects to visiting new places
31. Becomes upset it interrupted in what
he/she is doing
32. Insists on walking in a particular pattern
33. Insists on sitting at the same place
34. Dislikes changes in appearance or
behaviour of the people around him/her
35. Insists on using a particular door
37. Resists changing activities; difficulty
with transitions
38. Insists on same routine, household,
school or work schedule everyday
39. Insists that specific things take place at
specific times
problematic than others for boys with FXS.
Ritualistic/Sameness and Sensory Motor behaviours
were frequently endorsed as problematic, whereas
Sensory
Motor
Restricted
Interests
Self-injury
Compulsive
r = 0.56*
Ritualistic/
Compulsive Sameness
r = 0.55**
r = 0.38*
r = 0.65**
r = 0.32*
r = 0.39**
r = 0.59**
r = 0.35*
r = 0.36*
r = 0.65**
**P < 0.01,
*P < 0.05, one-tailed
RBS-R, Repetitive Behavior Scale – Revised.
Table 4 Longitudinal associations between child characteristics at
time 1 and RBS-R subscale scores 18 months later
Sensory Motor
Restricted
Interests
Self-injury
Compulsive
Ritualistic/
Sameness
Nonverbal
IQa
Anxietyb
ADOS
social–
affective
domainc
r = -0.05
r = -0.33*
r = 0.20
r = 0.33*
r = 0.05
r = 0.31*
r = 0.09
r = 0.13
r = 0.007
r = -0.01
r = 0.36*
r = 0.44**
r = 0.03
r = 0.16
r = 0.05
**P < 0.01,
*P < 0.05, one-tailed;
a
brief IQ scores from the Leiter-R;
b
General Anxiety subscale from the ADAMS;
c
calibrated severity score from the ADOS
RBS-R, Repetitive Behavior Scale – Revised; Leiter-R, Leiter International Performance Scale – Revised; ADAMS, Anxiety, Depression, and Mood Scale; ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation
Schedule.
Self-injury was the least commonly reported type of
behaviour. This finding is consistent with the results
reported for 3 to 5 year olds with FXS by Wolff &
colleagues (2012) and suggests that the profile of
repetitive behaviours remains stable in FXS from
preschool through at least the middle school years.
When considering repetitive behaviours at the
individual item level, the four behaviours most
commonly reported by caregivers to be a moderate to
© 2015 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
VOLUME
60 PART 1 JANUARY 2016
62
A. Oakes et al. • Repetitive behaviours in FXS
severe problem were found in the Sensory Motor and
Restricted Interest categories. In the Sensory Motor
category, almost half of caregivers reported
hand/finger mannerisms (e.g. hand flapping) to be a
moderate to severe problem and more than a third of
caregivers reported sensory behaviours to be a
moderate to severe problem. The two items (i.e.
preoccupation with one subject or activity and strong
attachment to one specific object) comprising the
Restricted Interests category were reported as
moderate to severe problems by more than a third of
caregivers. The next most commonly reported
behaviours were found in the categories of
Compulsive and Ritualistic/Sameness behaviours.
More than a third of caregivers reported difficulty
with transitions as a moderate to severe problem and
between a quarter and a third of caregivers reported
sleeping/bedtime rituals, completeness (e.g. must
have doors opened or closed) and objecting to visiting
new places as moderate to severe problems. Once
again, at the individual level, Self-injurious
behaviours were reported to be least problematic
subcategory endorsed in our sample.
It is interesting that self-injurious behaviours were
least commonly endorsed as being problematic using
the RBS-R. In the Bailey et al. (2008) survey
completed by parents of 976 full mutation boys with
FXS over the age of 6 years, 41% of parents reported
self-injurious behaviour to be problematic for their
children. It seems possible that self-injurious
behaviours may emerge with age and become more
problematic in adolescence and young adulthood, but
the discrepancy between reports of self-injury using
the RBS-R and the Bailey et al. (2008) parent survey
should be noted. There are, however, several
potential differences between the ways in which
problem behaviours were classified/categorised
between our study and the report of the parent survey
by Bailey et al. (2008). First, the Bailey study queried
self-injurious behaviours as a global category and did
not report the percentage of parents who reported
individual types of self-injurious behaviours as
problematic. If we had added up the individual
categories of self-injury reported in the current study,
our findings are more comparable with theirs.
Additionally, the current study reported only the
percentage of children for whom self-injury was a
moderate to severe problem. Nevertheless, the mean
subscale score for the entire category of different self-
injurious behaviours in the current study was under 1,
suggesting these were not seen as problems by parents
for the most part.
In terms of the relations between the different types
of repetitive behaviours, significant positive
associations were observed between all subcategories
of repetitive behaviours. The finding that repetitive
behaviours seem to covary in FXS could suggest a
common underlying mechanism that predisposes the
emergence of several different topographies and types
of repetitive behaviours. Alternatively, this finding
could suggest that a great deal of overlap exists across
the category structure of the RBS-R. Thus, it may be
that an alternate factor structure would be more
helpful in considering the ways in which repetitive
behaviours are organised in FXS.
Regardless of the significant associations between
all of the examined categories of repetitive behaviour,
some authors may argue that self-injurious behaviours
represent a distinct category of behaviours that is not
accurately classified as a topography of repetitive
behaviour. This perspective rests on the argument
that self-injurious behaviours, unlike other categories
of repetitive behaviours, are often maintained by
environmental contingencies (Langthorne et al.
2011). However, other authors have suggested that,
over time, behaviours that begin as stereotyped or
repetitive may evolve into self-injurious behaviours as
social reinforcement from the environment replaces
the self-stimulating function of the original repetitive
behaviour. According to this viewpoint, nonharmful
levels of stereotypy may be maintained and shaped by
environmental contingencies, potentially changing in
function and topography into more problematic levels
of self-injury (Kennedy 2002).
In order to gain insight into the participant
characteristics that predict the various types of
repetitive behaviours, longitudinal associations
between nonverbal IQ, anxiety and social–affective
symptoms of ASD and parent report of repetitive
behaviours were examined. We found that multiple
participant characteristics at T1 predicted scores on
the Restricted Interest subscale measured 18 months
later. As predicted, a negative association was
observed between nonverbal IQ and Restricted
Interests subscale of the RBS-R. Additionally, a
positive association was observed between levels of
anxiety and the severity of social affective symptoms
of ASD with this same subscale of the RBS-R.
© 2015 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
VOLUME
60 PART 1 JANUARY 2016
63
A. Oakes et al. • Repetitive behaviours in FXS
Finally, anxiety was also positively associated with
scores on the Compulsive and Ritualistic/Sameness
subscales at T2.
These predictive associations suggest that multiple
types of repetitive behaviours may be driven by the
presence of anxiety in boys with FXS. In particular,
our data suggest that, for these young boys with FXS,
high levels of anxiety are associated with increased
difficulty with compulsive behaviours, restricted
interests and ritualistic behaviours. It may be that
these behaviours are serving as coping mechanisms to
deal with heightened levels of anxiety; that is, when an
individual is experiencing high levels of anxiety,
engaging in repetitive behaviours (e.g. talking about a
favourite topic of conversation and preoccupation
with moving a favourite object in a repetitive or
ritualistic fashion) may alleviate the physiological
arousal associated with anxiety. As a result of
engaging in these types of repetitive behaviours, the
child may spend less time actively exploring or
attending to the environment, thereby negatively
impacting opportunities to learn from ongoing
experiences. Our findings suggest that treating
symptoms of anxiety may lead to the reduction of
repetitive behaviours in FXS.
Limitations in nonverbal cognition and
impairments in social communication were also
observed to be predictively associated with a higher
degree of restricted interests in young boys with FXS.
Research indicates that more severe limitations in
cognitive ability are a risk factor for high levels of
repetitive behaviours (McClintock et al. 2003).
Additionally, it would be reasonable to speculate that
children who are preoccupied with strong and allencompassing nonsocial interests would be less open
to responding to the social bids of interactive partners
and in learning from these social encounters. In
addition, severity of ASD symptomatology is
negatively associated with nonverbal IQ in FXS
(Lewis et al. 2006). Thus, the relationship between
social impairment as measured by the ADOS and
restricted interests as measured by the RBS-R may be
mediated by the shared variance of these child
characteristics with nonverbal IQ. It should be
mentioned that the negative association found
between nonverbal IQ and restricted interests has not
been reported for boys with nonsyndromic ASD who,
on average, have higher levels of nonverbal cognitive
ability than do same-aged boys with FXS (Bishop
et al. 2006; Szatmari et al. 2006). This finding
suggests that, although restricted interests are a core
symptom present in children with ASD regardless of
their cognitive level, restricted interests in children
with FXS may be, in part, the result of higher levels of
cognitive impairment.
Limitations
As with all correlational studies, it is not possible to
determine the direction of causality or to rule out the
possibility that unmeasured variables explain the
resultant patterns of associations. The relatively small
sample precluded us from conducting a multiple
regression analysis that could simultaneously evaluate
the unique contribution of each potential predictor.
Additionally, we did not administer the RBS-R at the
initial T1 visit, which would have allowed us to
examine changes in the level of repetitive behaviours
over time and, additionally, to control for the level of
repetitive behaviours at T1. In addition, as the present
study did not include a comparison group of
individuals with other neurodevelopmental disorders
(e.g. ASD or Down syndrome), we are unable to
determine the extent to which the observed pattern of
findings is specific to FXS. Finally, as mentioned
earlier, all participants in the current study were
required to use speech as their primary means of
communication. This inclusionary criterion for the
current study, which required that participants use
three word phrases on a daily basis, may have resulted
in a sample that was not representative of the
population of boys with FXS and may account for the
lower levels and reduced severity of self-injurious
behaviours in our sample.
Future directions
Taken as a whole, these results contribute to the
literature examining profiles and predictors of
repetitive behaviours in FXS, the leading inherited
cause of ID. Future research should focus on
identifying additional child and environmental
characteristics that may have a causal relationship
with repetitive behaviours. Candidate variables
include executive functioning, attention, arousal,
sensory processing and levels of fragile X mental
retardation protein, the protein which is implicated in
neurological functioning in FXS.
© 2015 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
VOLUME
60 PART 1 JANUARY 2016
64
A. Oakes et al. • Repetitive behaviours in FXS
Research should also be directed towards
examining whether these same or different variables
influence the emergence of repetitive behaviours in
children with nonsyndromic ASD and how profiles of
affectedness differ between these two disorders. A
direct between-group comparison utilising a sample
of boys with nonsyndromic ASD (i.e. children who
meet the criteria for an ASD diagnosis but for whom a
diagnosis of the FXS full mutation has been ruled
out) could provide insights into possible differences in
the profile of repetitive behaviours. Additionally, such
a comparison would inform our understanding of the
strengths of association between child characteristics
and the presence of repetitive behaviours, providing
insights into similarities and differences in causal
mechanisms. This line of research is especially
pertinent given ongoing efforts to develop targeted
pharmacological and behavioural treatments for
individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders as it
may not be the case that treatments designed to
address repetitive behaviours in children with
nonsyndromic ASD will be equally effective for
children with FXS (Hall et al. 2010).
As mentioned previously, there are many
approaches to the measurement of repetitive
behaviours in individuals with ID and development
disabilities. Researchers have used different measures
that have been specifically developed to assess
repetitive behaviour (RBS-R; Bodfish et al. 2000;
RBQ; Moss et al. 2009) or have used subscales or
items from tests developed for other purposes, such
as autism diagnosis (i.e. ADI-R; Rutter et al. 2003).
Although the present study provides preliminary
insights into the presence of repetitive behaviours in
FXS, comparing results of different assessments
administered to the same sample of individuals with
FXS would be a productive direction for future
research. Additionally, other researchers have
utilised one set of items/behavioural characteristics
but have classified these items in different ways
depending on factor analyses conducted for
specific populations, particularly ASD (e.g. Bishop
et al. 2013). It may be that the phenotypic
characteristics associated with particular syndromes
warrant different organisations of individual
topographies of repetitive behaviours as a function of
disorder. To date, no FXS-specific factor analyses
have been conducted on any measure of repetitive
behaviours.
Finally, measures of repetitive behaviours vary in
terms of their focus on frequency of behaviours and
severity of behaviours or may collapse both
dimensions into a single rating scale. More work is
needed to examine both frequency and severity of
repetitive behaviours independently and the extent to
which either of these factors interferes with day-to-day
functioning.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by NIH grant R01
HD054764 and U54 HD079125 from the Eunice
Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health
and Human Development. A portion of the results
from the current study was presented at the 2012
International Fragile X conference. We would like
to thank the parents and children who participated
in this study. We would also like to thank the
following individuals who contributed to the
collection of data reported in this paper: Sara
Kover, Eileen Haebig, Claire Hauser, Sara Lifson,
David Benjamin, Susan Harris, Beth Goodlin-Jones
and Cecilia Compton.
References
Abbeduto L., Brady N. & Kover S. T. (2007) Language
development and fragile X syndrome: profiles, syndromespecificity, and within-syndrome differences. Mental
Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews
13, 36–46.
Bailey D. B., Raspa M., Olmsted M. & Holiday D. B. (2008)
Co-occurring conditions associated with FMR1 gene
variations: findings from a national parent survey. American
Journal of Medical Genetics Part A 146, 2060–9.
Baranek G. T., Danko C. D., Skinner M. L., Donald B., Jr.,
Hatton D. D., Roberts J. E. et al. (2005) Video analysis of
sensory-motor features in infants with fragile X syndrome
at 9–12 months of age. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders 35, 645–56.
Bell M. V., Hirst M. C., Nakahori Y., MacKinnon R. N.,
Roche A., Flint T. J. et al. (1991) Physical mapping across
the fragile X: hypermethylation and clinical expression of
the fragile X syndrome. Cell 64, 861–6.
Berument S. K., Rutter M., Lord C., Pickles A. & Bailey A.
(1999) Autism screening questionnaire: diagnostic
validity. British Journal of Psychiatry 175, 444–51.
Bishop S. L., Hus V., Duncan A., Huerta M., Gotham K.,
Pickles A. et al. (2013) Subcategories of restricted and
repetitive behaviors in children with autism spectrum
© 2015 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
VOLUME
60 PART 1 JANUARY 2016
65
A. Oakes et al. • Repetitive behaviours in FXS
disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
43, 1287–97. DOI: 10.1007/s10803-012-1671-0.
Bishop S. L., Richler J. & Lord C. (2006) Association
between restricted and repetitive behaviors and nonverbal
IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders. Child
Neuropsychology 12, 247–67.
Bodfish J. W., Symons F. J., Parker D. E. & Lewis M. H.
(2000) Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism:
comparisons to mental retardation. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders 30, 237–43.
Boyd B. A., McDonough S. G. & Bodfish J. W. (2012)
Evidence-based behavioral interventions for repetitive
behaviors in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders 42, 1236–48.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2011) Fragile X
syndrome. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/Features/
FragileX/.
Cordiero L., Ballinger E., Hagerman R. J. & Hessl D. (2011)
Clinical assessment of DSM-IV anxiety disorders in fragile
X syndrome: prevalence and characterization. Journal of
Neurodevelopmental Disorders 3, 57–67.
Crawford D. C., Acuna J. M. & Sherman S. L. (2001) FMR1
and the fragile X syndrome: human genome epidemiology
review. Genetics in Medicine 3, 359–71.
Edwards M. J., Lang A. E. & Bhatia K. P. (2012)
Stereotypies: a critical appraisal and suggestion of a
clinically useful definition. Movement Disorders 27, 179–85.
Hall S. S., Lightbody A. A., Hirt M., Rezvani A. & Reiss
A. L. (2010) Autism in fragile X syndrome: a category
mistake. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Adolescent Psychiatry 49, 921–33.
Hall S. S., Lightbody A. A., McCarthy B. E., Parker K. J. &
Reiss A. L. (2012) Effects of intranasal oxytocin on social
anxiety in males with fragile X syndrome.
Psychoneuroendrocrinology 37, 509–18.
Hall S. S., Lightbody A. A. & Reiss A. L. (2008)
Compulsive, self-injurious, and autistic behavior in
children and adolescents with fragile X syndrome.
American Journal of Mental Retardation 113, 44–53.
Harris S., Hessl D., Goodlin-Jones B., Ferranti J., Bacalman
S., Barbato I. et al. (2008) Autism profiles of males with
fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental
Retardation 113, 427–38.
Harrop C., McConachie H., Emsley R., Leadbitter K.,
Green J. & PACT Consortium (2014) Restricted and
repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders and
typical development: cross-sectional and longitudinal
comparisons. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
44, 1207–19.
Hessl D., Nguyen D. V., Green C., Chavez A., Tassone F.,
Hagerman R. J. et al. (2009) A solution to limitations of
cognitive testing in children with intellectual disabilities:
the case of fragile X syndrome. Journal of
Neurodevelopmental Disorders 1, 33–45.
Esbensen A. J., Rojahn J., Aman M. G. & Ruedrich S.
(2003) Reliability and validity of an assessment instrument
for anxiety, depression, and mood among individuals with
mental retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders 33, 617–29.
Evans D. W., Leckman J. F., Carter A., Reznick J. S.,
Henshaw D., King R. A. et al. (1997) Ritual, habit, and
perfectionism: the prevalence and development of
compulsive-like behavior in normal young children. Child
Development 68, 58–68.
Hessl D., Tassone F., Cordeiro L., Koldewyn K.,
McCormick C., Green C. et al. (2008) Brief report:
aggression and stereotypic behavior in males with fragile X
syndrome – moderating secondary genes in a “single
gene” disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders 38, 184–9.
Gabriels R. L., Cuccaro M. L., Hill D. E., Ivers B. J. &
Goldson E. (2005) Repetitive behaviors in autism:
relationships with associated clinical features. Research in
Developmental Disabilities 26, 169–81.
Gallagher A. & Hallahan B. (2012) Fragile X-associated
disorders: a clinical overview. Journal of Neurology 259,
401–13.
Jennings K. D. (2002) Mastery motivation and the formation of
self-concept from infancy through early. Mastery Motivation:
Children’s Investigation, Persistence and Development 36.
Gotham K., Pickles A. & Lord C. (2009) Standardizing
ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum
disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
39, 693–705.
Hagerman R. J. (2002) The physical and behavioral
phenotype. In: Fragile X Syndrome: Diagnosis, Treatment,
and Research (eds R. J. Hagerman & P. J. Hagerman), third
edn, pp. 206–48. Johns Hopkins University Press,
Baltimore, MD.
Hagerman R. J. (2006) Lessons from fragile X regarding
neurobiology, autism, and neurodegeneration. Journal of
Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics 27, 63–74.
Hus V., Gotham K. & Lord C. (2012) Standardizing ADOS
domain scores: separating severity of social affect and
restricted and repetitive behaviors. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders 44, 2400–12.
Kau A. S. M., Tierney E., Bukelis I., Stump M. H., Kates
W. R., Trescher W. H. et al. (2004) Social behavior profile
in young males with fragile X syndrome: characteristics
and specificity. American Journal of Medical Genetics 126A,
9–17.
Kennedy C. H. (2002) Evolution of stereotypy into selfinjury. In: Self-injurious Behavior: Gene Brain–behavior
Relationships (eds S. R. Schroeder, M. L. Oster-Granite &
T. Thompson), pp. 133–43. American Psychological
Association, Washington, DC.
Kopp C. B. (1982) Antecedents of self-regulation: a
developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology 18, 199.
Kover S. T., Haebig E., Oakes A., McDuffie A., Hagerman
R. J. & Abbeduto L. (2014) Sentence comprehension in
boys with autism spectrum disorder. American Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology 23, 385–94.
© 2015 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
VOLUME
60 PART 1 JANUARY 2016
66
A. Oakes et al. • Repetitive behaviours in FXS
Kover S. T., Pierpont E. I., Kim J. S., Brown W. T. &
Abbeduto L. (2013) A neurodevelopmental perspective on
the acquisition of nonverbal cognitive skills in adolescents
with fragile X Syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology
38, 445–60.
Miguel E. C., Baer L., Coffey B. J., Rauch S. L., Savage C.
R., O’Sullivan R. L. et al. (1997) Phenomenological
differences appearing with repetitive behaviours in
obsessive–compulsive disorder and Gilles de la Tourette’s
syndrome. The British Journal of Psychiatry 170, 140–5.
Lam K. & Aman M. G. (2007) The repetitive behavior scalerevised: independent validation in individuals with autism
spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders 37, 855–66.
Mirenda P., Smith I. M., Vaillancourt T., Georgiades S.,
Duku E., Szatmari P. et al. (2010) Validating the
Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised in young children with
autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders 40, 1521–30.
Langthorne P., McGill P., O’Reilly M. F., Lang R.,
Machalicek W., Chan J. M. et al. (2011) Examining the
function of problem behavior in fragile X syndrome:
preliminary experimental analysis. American Journal of
Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 116, 65–80.
Leekam S. R., Prior M. R. & Uljarevic M. (2011) Restricted
and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders: a
review of research in the last decade. Psychological Bulletin
137, 562–93.
Lewis P., Abbeduto L., Murphy M., Richmond E., Giles N.,
Bruno L. et al. (2006) Cognitive, language and social–
cognitive skills of individuals with fragile X syndrome with
and without autism. Journal of Intellectual Disability
Research 50, 532–45.
Lewis M. & Kim S. J. (2009) The pathophysiology of
restricted repetitive behavior. Journal of
Neurodevelopmental Disorders 1, 114–32.
Lifter K. (2000) Linking assessment to intervention for
children with developmental disabilities or at-risk for
developmental delay: the developmental play assessment
(DPA) instrument. Play Diagnosis and Assessment 2, 228–61.
Lord C., Rutter M., DiLavore P. & Risi S. (1999) Autism
Diagnostic Observation Schedule – Generic. Western
Psychological Services, Los Angeles, CA.
MacDonald R., Green G., Mansfield R., Geckeler A.,
Gardenier N., Anderson J. et al. (2007) Stereotypy in
young children with autism and typically developing
children. Research in Developmental Disabilities 28,
266–77.
McClintock K., Hall S. & Oliver C. (2003) Risk markers
associated with challenging behaviours in people with
intellectual disabilities: a meta-analytic study. Journal of
Intellectual Disability Research 47, 405–16.
McDuffie A., Kover S. T., Abbeduto L., Lewis P. & Brown
T. (2012) Profiles of receptive and expressive language
abilities in boys with comorbid fragile X syndrome and
autism. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental
Disabilities 117, 18–32.
McDuffie A., Kover S. T., Hagerman R. & Abbeduto L.
(2013) Investigating word learning in fragile X syndrome:
a fast-mapping study. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders 43, 1676–91.
McDuffie A., Thurman A. J., Hagerman R. J. & Abbeduto
L. (2015) Symptoms of autism in males with fragile X
syndrome: a comparison to nonsyndromic ASD using
current ADI-R scores. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders 45, 1925–37.
Moss J. & Oliver C. (2008) The Repetitive Behaviour Scale,
Manual for Administration and Scorer Interpretation.
University of Birmingham, Birmingham.
Moss J., Oliver C., Arron K., Burbidge C. & Berg K. (2009)
The prevalence and phenomenology of repetitive behavior
in genetic syndromes. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders 39, 572–88.
Oakes A., Kover S. T. & Abbeduto L. (2013) Language
comprehension profiles of young adolescents with fragile
X syndrome. American Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology 22, 615–26.
Oostra B. A. & Willemsen R. (2003) A fragile balance: FMR1
expression levels. Human Molecular Genetics 12, 249–57.
Richler J., Huerta M., Bishop S. L. & Lord C. (2010)
Developmental trajectories of restricted and repetitive
behaviors and interests in children with autism spectrum
disorders. Development and Psychopathology 22, 55–69.
Roberts J. E., Chapman R. S., Martin G. E. & Moskowitz
L. (2008) Language of preschool and school-age children
with Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. In: Speech
& Language Development and Intervention in Down
Syndrome and Fragile X Syndrome (eds J. E. Roberts, R.
S. Chapman & S. F. Warren). Paul H. Brookes,
Baltimore, MD.
Roid G. & Miller L. (1997) Leiter International Performance
Scale – Revised. Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL.
Rutter M., Le Couteur A. & Lord C. (2003) Autism
Diagnostic Interview – Revised. Western Psychological
Services, Los Angeles, CA.
Sansone S. M., Schneider A., Bickel E., Berry-Kravis E.,
Prescott C. & Hessl D. (2014) Improving IQ
measurement in intellectual disabilities using true
deviation from population norms. Journal of
Neurodevelopmental Disorders 6, 16.
Sudhalter V., Cohen I. L., Silverman W. & Wolf-Schein E.
G. (1990) Conversational analyses of males with fragile X,
Down syndrome, and autism: comparison of the
emergence of deviant language. American Journal on
Mental Retardation 94, 431–41.
Symons F. J., Byiers B. J., Raspa M., Bishop E. & Bailey D. B.
(2010) Self-injurious behavior and fragile X syndrome:
findings from the national fragile X survey. American Journal
on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 115, 473–81.
Symons F. J., Clark R. D., Hatton D. D., Skinner M. &
Bailey D. B. (2003) Self-injurious behavior in young boys
© 2015 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
VOLUME
60 PART 1 JANUARY 2016
67
A. Oakes et al. • Repetitive behaviours in FXS
with fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Medical
Genetics 118A, 115–21.
Szatmari P., Georgiades S., Bryson S., Zwaigenbaum L.,
Roberts W., Mahoney W. et al. (2006) Investigating the
structure of the restricted, repetitive behaviors and
interests domain of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry 47, 582–90.
Talisa V. B., Boyle L., Crafa D. & Kaufmann W. E. (2014)
Autism and anxiety in males with fragile X syndrome: an
exploratory analysis of neurobehavioral profiles from a
parent survey. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A
164, 1198–203.
Thelen E. (1981) Rhythmical behavior in infancy: an
ethological perspective. Developmental Psychology 17,
238–57.
Thurman A. J., McDuffie A., Hagerman R. & Abbeduto L.
(2014) Psychiatric symptoms in boys with fragile X
syndrome: a comparison with nonsyndromic autism
spectrum disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities
35, 1072–86.
Verkerk A. J. M. H., Pieretti M., Sutcliffe J. S., Fu Y. H.,
Kuhl D. P. A., Pizzuti A. et al. (1991) Identification of a
gene (FMR-1) containing a CGG repeat coincident with a
breakpoint cluster region exhibiting length variation in
fragile X syndrome. Cell 65, 905–14.
Wolff J. J., Bodfish J. W., Hazlett H. C., Lightbody A. A.,
Reiss A. L. & Piven J. (2012) Evidence of a distinct
behavioral phenotype in young boys with fragile X
syndrome and autism. Journal of the American Academy of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 51, 1324–32.
Accepted 8 September 2015
© 2015 MENCAP and International Association of the Scientific Study of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and
John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Download