Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

advertisement
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
http://psp.sagepub.com
Social Vigilantism: Measuring Individual Differences in Belief Superiority and Resistance to Persuasion
Donald A. Saucier and Russell J. Webster
Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2010; 36; 19 originally published online Sep 23, 2009;
DOI: 10.1177/0146167209346170
The online version of this article can be found at:
http://psp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/36/1/19
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
Additional services and information for Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin can be found at:
Email Alerts: http://psp.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts
Subscriptions: http://psp.sagepub.com/subscriptions
Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav
Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Citations http://psp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/36/1/19
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
Social Vigilantism: Measuring Individual
Differences in Belief Superiority and
Resistance to Persuasion
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
36(1) 19­–32
© 2010 by the Society for Personality
and Social Psychology, Inc.
Reprints and permission: http://www.
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0146167209346170
http://pspb.sagepub.com
Donald A. Saucier1 and Russell J. Webster1
Abstract
Social vigilantism (SV) is an enduring individual difference that assesses the tendency of individuals to impress and propagate
their “superior” beliefs onto others to correct others’ more “ignorant” opinions. After establishing a reliable measure of
SV, three studies showed that SV was associated with greater expressions of belief superiority (whether reacting to others
holding dissimilar or similar beliefs) and greater resistance to persuasion (via increased rates of counterarguing and greater
attitude stability after persuasion appeals) even after controlling for relevant individual differences (narcissism, dogmatism,
psychological reactance, and need for cognition), as well as attitude importance and extremity. Thus, SV predicts expressions
of belief superiority and resistance to persuasion above and beyond characteristics of the attitude and individual difference
variables previously studied in the attitude literature. SV is a meaningful construct in increasing the understanding of persuasion,
attitude resistance, and attitude dissemination that can be applied in a variety of psychological domains.
Keywords
individual difference, persuasion, resistance, counterarguing, attitude change
Received September 30, 2008; revision accepted June 2, 2009
Everyone does have a right to their own opinions. I just
think you should know how ignorant you sound when
you voice them.
An anonymous participant
The individual who made the statement above had just
read a list of another person’s opinions. However, rather than
passively accepting that another person could hold contrasting viewpoints, the participant ­belittled the other person,
calling him or her “ignorant.” The participant likely believed
that his or her own beliefs were superior to the other person’s
and would have likely made an effort to both express that
superiority and to persuade the other person to alter those
beliefs. We believe that these are the behavioral manifestations of an enduring individual difference we term social
vigilantism (SV).
Vigilantes, as defined in the Merriam-Webster Collegiate
Dictionary, Eleventh Edition, are members “of a volunteer
committee organized to suppress and punish crime summarily.” We postulate that some individuals perceive the
expression of dissonant beliefs, attitudes, or opinions as a
social “crime” (i.e., an offense or a provocation) because
these beliefs are considered ignorant or irrational. Because
social vigilantes believe that their views are superior and
more accurate, these individuals feel responsible to impress
and propagate their beliefs onto others for the betterment of
society. In other words, we postulate that SV is an enduring
individual difference that captures the tendency some individuals have to assert their “superior” beliefs onto others to
correct others’ more “ignorant” opinions for the “greater
good.”
Accordingly, because SV is an enduring disposition, the
level of SV will habitually affect one’s interactions with and
adaptations to the environment (cf. Larsen & Buss, 2002).
Specifically, when approached with someone else’s opinions, people higher in SV will be inclined to propagate their
superior beliefs onto others to correct others’ beliefs and will
have developed defenses to resist persuasion attempts to
change their attitudes.
1
Kansas State University
Corresponding Author:
Donald A. Saucier, Department of Psychology, Kansas State University,
468 Bluemont Hall, Manhattan, KS 66506-5302
Email: saucier@ksu.edu or webster@ksu.edu
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
20
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(1)
The Foundations of SV: Belief Superiority
and Resistance to Persuasion
It has been argued that “resistance to persuasion has been a
relatively neglected phenomenon in social psychology”
(Jacks & Cameron, 2003, p. 145). Much of the literature on
resistance to persuasion has focused on the relations between
attitude resistance and the characteristics of the attitude targeted by a persuasive message. For example, attitude strength
is the tendency for an attitude to persist, be resistant to
change, to affect information processing, and to affect behavior (Krosnick & Petty, 1995). Attitude strength has been
attributed to several more specific attitude characteristics,
such as importance to the attitude holder or extremity of the
attitude (Krosnick, Boninger, Chuang, Berent, & Carnot,
1993; Visser, Krosnick, & Simmons, 2003). These attitude
characteristics have been shown to predict resistance to persuasive messages in several studies (e.g., Eagly & Chaiken,
1993; Jacks & Devine, 2000; Petty & Krosnick, 1995;
Zuwerink & Devine, 1996). However, we raise the possibility that resistance to persuasion can be more thoroughly
understood by considering the disposition of the individual
holding the attitude as well as characteristics of the attitude.
We argue that, more generally, individuals differ in their tendencies to accept or resist persuasive messages and that these
tendencies can be predicted by individuals’ levels of SV.
People higher in SV would resist persuasion attempts primarily by more aggressively counterarguing (i.e., provide
direct rebuttals of the message argument), one of the most
effective strategies used to repel persuasive messages (Abelson, 1959; Festinger & Maccoby, 1964; Jacks & Devine,
2000; Wellins, 1977)—provided that individuals are able to
derive their counterarguments themselves (Cameron, Jacks,
& O’Brien, 2002). By counterarguing, one may have the
opportunity to point out the shortcomings in others’ arguments but also to simultaneously impress one’s beliefs.
Accordingly, people higher in SV may use counterarguing as
a dual-purpose mechanism for both maintaining and disseminating their own superior attitudes and beliefs. Furthermore,
counterarguing is an effortful process and a direct form of
warding off persuasive messages; thus, counterarguing is an
investment that not all individuals will feel worthwhile in
making and may result in confrontations of the individual
delivering the messages. We propose that people higher in
SV will invest the energy to counterargue more than those
lower in SV.
Because people higher in SV believe that their views are
right, and will counterargue to disseminate these beliefs, we
predict that they should show little to no change in their
beliefs following a persuasive message. That is, the higher
the level of SV, the harder it will likely be to shift people’s
opinions. Furthermore, we posit that people higher in SV
will try to propagate their beliefs and resist persuasion
attempts regardless of whether they agree with a confronting
opinion; in short, people higher in SV will take any opportunity to impress their beliefs. Thus, because people higher in
SV are motivated to impress their beliefs and correct opinions regardless of the subject matter or of how ardently (or
halfheartedly) they feel about a particular topic, we hypothesize that SV will offer unique predictive validity above and
beyond characteristics of the attitude (e.g., the type of attitude, as well as attitude strength and extremity).
Potential Correlates of SV
Some of the cognitions and behavioral manifestations that
comprise SV could be partially accounted for by existing
individual difference measures. Specifically, we predicted
that SV would be positively correlated with exaggerated
egocentrism (e.g., narcissism), rigidity of beliefs (e.g., dogmatism), predispositions to counterargue (e.g., reactance),
and contemplative thinking (e.g., need for cognition). However, we assert that SV is a theoretically distinct construct
that would have predictive ability above and beyond these
other individual differences.
Narcissism
Clinically defined, an individual with narcissistic personality
disorder has characteristics that include a grandiose sense of
self-importance, a preoccupation with fantasies of power, a
tendency to exploit others, an unwillingness to identify with
the feelings and needs of others, and arrogant behaviors and
attitudes (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
­Disorders–Fourth Edition [DSM-IV]; American Psychiatric
Association, 1994). Narcissism is also measured as an individual difference among individuals on a continuum
regarding the extent to which they display these characteristics (e.g., Raskin & Terry, 1988).
Individuals higher in narcissism may perceive the presentation of opposing views as a form of negative feedback and
subsequently rate the presenter of those views as less competent (Kernis & Sun, 1994; Shrauger & Kelly, 1988; Swann,
Griffin, Predmore, & Gaines, 1987) or as possessing more
negative personality characteristics (Morf & Rhodewalt,
1993; Smalley & Stake, 1996). For narcissists, derogating
the dissenter could be a form of interpersonal self-regulation
with the goal of bolstering one’s own view of the self (Morf
& Rhodewalt, 2001); that is, social interactions are used to
garner attention and admiration, and narcissists may accordingly disengage from others who are unwilling to provide
this reinforcement (Campbell, 1999; Emmons, 1989).
By contrast, SV is a tendency some individuals have to
assert their superior beliefs onto others to correct others’
more ignorant opinions. Therefore, individuals higher in SV
are not motivated to gain admiration, or even social approval,
from their audiences; rather, they are motivated—almost feel
responsible—to impress information. Accordingly, we
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
21
Saucier and Webster
predict that SV will predict counterarguing in response to the
presentation of opposing (or even similar) viewpoints, while
narcissism would more likely predict avoidance of or withdrawal from the presenter of the opposing beliefs.
Dogmatism
Broadly, dogmatism is a closed way of thinking that is connected to an ideology of any content, an authoritarian life
perspective, intolerance toward those who disagree with
one’s chosen ideology, positive feelings toward those who
share one’s ideology (Rokeach, 1960), and a relative stability
of one’s beliefs and attitudes over time (Kemp, 1960). People
higher on dogmatism also have been shown to possess stronger beliefs and to deny opposing beliefs by labeling such
beliefs as irrelevant (Rokeach, 1954).
It would appear that dogmatism and SV would be related
because both constructs are associated with perceptions of
belief superiority and the aversion felt toward those who do
not agree with one’s beliefs. We believe that SV is conceptually distinct from dogmatism despite these similarities. We
do not believe it is necessary for individuals who are higher
in SV to have exceptionally strong attitudes and beliefs
across all subject matters; rather, we predict that social vigilantes will perceive their beliefs to be superior, whereas
dogmatism would be related to attitude strength. Furthermore, reactions to dissenting opinions would likely be
different between individuals higher in dogmatism and individuals higher in SV. Although dogmatism and SV may
relate to perceived superiority of one’s beliefs, we predict
that people higher in SV will be more likely to attempt to
impress their beliefs onto others. Conversely, people higher
in dogmatism will be more likely to summarily dismiss,
rather than engage, dissenters with efforts to persuade them
to change their beliefs.
Reactance
Psychological reactance theory states that individuals will
exhibit reactance when they feel that they are being restricted
from being able to express what had been “free behaviors”
(Brehm, 1966; Brehm & Brehm, 1981). The motivational
state of reactance may then produce efforts to reassert one’s
freedom through oppositional behavior (e.g., by resisting the
restrictions, exhibiting backlash effects, or aggressing
against the agent imposing restrictions; Brehm, 1966). When
individuals’ free behaviors are perceived to be restricted by
persuasion techniques, the individuals will often resist the
persuasive effort (Nimmer & Handelsman, 1992) or express
the opposing message as their own (Nail, Van Leeuwen, &
Powell, 1996). Furthermore, research has shown that individual differences in reactance exist and are relevant to
understanding of the production and effects of reactance
(Brehm & Brehm, 1981; Dowd, Milne, & Wise, 1991).
An individual’s tendency to experience psychological
reactance could predict counterarguing following a persuasive message. However, there is little in the theory to suggest
that individuals higher in reactance would feel it necessary to
reassert their restricted freedom following a persuasive message by doing no more than resisting the persuader. That is,
although individuals higher in psychological reactance may
counterargue following persuasive messages, we would
expect that they would not be motivated to “correct” the target’s opinions because the reassertion of their freedom is
achieved without doing so.
Need for Cognition
Need for cognition is the tendency for an individual to
engage in and enjoy thinking (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982).
Because counterarguing is a cognitively effortful response to
the presentation of others’ opinions, it is plausible that need
for cognition would predict resistance to persuasion. However, we believe SV and need for cognition are empirically
distinct. Need for cognition simply is a need to think more
deeply about ideas—that is, to be contemplative. Thus,
although need for cognition would likely predict processing
of a message, it will not predict the behavioral responses
(i.e., resistance to persuasion strategies) following such a
message. Conversely, people high in SV are motivated to
impress their beliefs onto others because they think dissemination of their beliefs and counterarguing will be worthwhile,
that is, will lead to changes in other people’s beliefs, regardless of whether their beliefs are in fact more “correct.”
General Overview of Studies
After constructing a reliable measure of SV (Study 1), we
predicted that because individuals higher in SV believe that
it is their responsibility to propagate their superior beliefs for
the greater good, such individuals would be more inclined to
impress their “superior” beliefs onto others and exhibit
greater counterarguing in response to extreme opinions
(Study 2). Moreover, we hypothesized that these effects hold
even after accounting for relevant individual difference variables (narcissism, dogmatism, reactance, and need for
cognition; Study 3), as well as participants’ level of attitude
extremity and importance (Study 4). We also predicted that
people higher in SV would exhibit greater attitude stability
after a persuasion appeal (Study 4).
Study 1: Construction and Reliability of the
Social Vigilantism Scale (SVS)
This study sought to identify reliable items to measure SV.
We intended to create a measure that would predict individual
differences in people’s reaction to and resistance of persuasive messages. Therefore, the SV measure would primarily
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
22
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(1)
assess people’s motivation to impress their superior beliefs
onto others, aversion to others’ ignorant beliefs, and motivation to correct others’ opinions. Furthermore, we created a
“moral stability” measure to control for individuals’ tendencies to believe that their beliefs are valid and relatively stable.
Accordingly, face valid items to assess this tendency were
administered in this study as well. Undergraduate participants
completed the initial list of items and reliable scales were created and administered to additional samples to assess the
scales’ internal consistency and test–retest reliability.
Method
Participants. Participants in each sample were undergraduate students in psychology classes participating in exchange
for course credit. Sample 1 consisted of 146 undergraduates
(46 men, 94 women, and 6 declined to report their gender).
Sample 2 consisted of 142 undergraduates (52 men, 79
women, and 11 declined to report their gender). Sample 3
consisted of 598 undergraduates (224 males, 367 females,
and 7 declined to report their gender). Sample 4 consisted of
67 undergraduates (16 males and 51 females).
Measures. For each of the items, participants indicated their
levels of agreement with the statements on a scale from 1
(disagree very strongly) to 9 (agree very strongly).
SV items. We wrote 29 items that tapped the different
facets of SV, including individuals’ belief superiority (e.g., “I
try to get people to listen to me, because what I have to say
makes a lot of sense”), aversion to others’ ignorant beliefs
(e.g., “Some people just believe stupid things”), and motivation/responsibility to correct others’ opinions (e.g., “I feel as
if it is my duty to enlighten other people”).
Moral stability items. We also wrote 14 face valid items to
assess the stability and validity of individuals’ moral beliefs
without regard for the actual content of the individuals’
moral beliefs (e.g., “I often change my mind”). These items
were written so that the construct of SV could be distinguished from one’s tendency to adhere to a set of valid and
relatively stable beliefs and values.
Procedure. Participants completed the questionnaire items
during their regularly scheduled class sessions in exchange
for extra credit or to partially fulfill their class research participation requirement. Samples 1, 2, and 3 completed the
measures once. Sample 4 completed the measures twice with
a 6-week interval separating the sessions. The questionnaire
was labeled “Personal Views, Ethics, and Morality” and participants were asked to complete the items honestly and
silently. All participants completed the questionnaires under
the conditions of anonymity.
Results and Discussion
Internal Consistency in Sample 1. The participants’
responses to the SV and moral stability items were subjected
Table 1. Items for the Social Vigilantism Scale (SVS)
1. I feel as if it is my duty to enlighten other people.
2. I feel that my ideas should be used to educate others.
3. I feel a social obligation to voice my opinion.
4. I need to win any argument about how people should live
their lives.
5. Those people who are more intelligent and informed have a
responsibility to educate the people around them who are
less intelligent and informed.
6. I like to imagine myself in a position of authority so that I
could make the important decisions around here.
7. I try to get people to listen to me, because what I have to say
makes a lot of sense.
8. Some people just believe stupid things.
9. There are a lot of ignorant people in society.
10. I think that some people need to be told that their point of
view is wrong.
11. If everyone saw things the way that I do, the world would be
a better place.
12. It frustrates me that many people fail to consider the finer
points of an issue when they take a side.
13. I often feel that other people do not base their opinions on
good evidence.
14. I frequently consider writing a “letter to the editor.”
to reliability analyses during which items that lowered the
internal consistency as measured by Cronbach’s alpha were
deleted. Fourteen of the SV items survived this analysis to
comprise the SVS (alpha = .81; the items are listed in Table
1). These items assessed beliefs about belief superiority and
the responsibility of propagating one’s superior beliefs (e.g.,
“I feel as if it is my duty to enlighten other people”), beliefs
about how stupid or ignorant other people are (e.g., “Some
people just believe stupid things”), and beliefs about how
unreasoned people’s thinking is (e.g., “I often feel that other
people do not base their opinions on good evidence”). These
14 items captured different and multiple facets of the overall
construct of SV and were therefore averaged together to
create one mean SV composite score.
Ten of the moral stability items survived the reliability
analysis and formed a single-factor Moral Stability Scale
(MSS; alpha = .73).1 Examination of the correlation between
SVS and MSS scores indicates that the constructs measured
by these scales are independent, r = .13, p = .133.
Internal Consistency in Samples 2, 3, and 4. These
results indicated that the items comprising the SVS and the
MSS were internally consistent. The alpha values for the
SVS in Samples 2 and 3 were .83 and .83, respectively, indicating the SVS has a good degree of internal consistency.
The SVS was also internally consistent at each of the administrations for Sample 4, alphas = .85 and .88 at Times 1 and
2, respectively. The alpha values for the MSS in Samples 2,
3, and 4 (Times 1 and 2) were .66, .69, .66, and .70, respectively, indicating adequate levels of internal consistency for
the MSS. Consistent with the preceding, the magnitude of
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
23
Saucier and Webster
the correlations between the SVS and MSS indicate that the
scales measure independent constructs, rs = .08, .15, –.07,
and .16, ps = .33, < .001, .532, and .201 for Samples 2, 3, and
4 (Times 1 and 2), respectively.
Test–Retest Reliability in Sample 4. The correlation
between the scores participants received at Times 1 and 2 on
the SVS suggested that the scale has excellent test–retest
reliability, r = .87.
Sex Differences. Comparisons of the means for the SVS
and the MSS for male and female participants revealed that
male participants reported significantly higher means than
did female participants on both the SVS and MSS in Samples 1, 2, and 3, ts > 1.97, ps < .05. Sex differences in Sample
4 only reached significance for the SVS at Time 2, but this is
not surprising given that the proportion of males in this
sample was substantially smaller than in the previous samples. These results suggest that these sex differences must be
controlled for in any further statistical analyses.
Study 2: Reactions to Another Individual’s
Extreme Opinions
In Study 2, we predicted that individuals who were higher on
SV would perceive the expression of extreme views by
another person as an opportunity to advocate and impress
their own beliefs onto the other person. As such, we would
expect individuals higher on SV to be more likely to propagate their own superior beliefs and to counterargue relative
to the other person regardless of whether participants agreed
with the alleged target’s position. Accordingly, participants
completed the SVS and then read a page-length list of a target’s extremely right- or left-wing opinions. Participants
then responded to the target’s position in writing and
explained what their goals were in writing to the target. We
also had raters code participants’ reactions for their expressions of belief superiority and counterarguing.
Method
Participants. Undergraduates (N = 186; 73 men, 112
women, and 1 person declined to report his or her gender)
participated during a regular meeting of a social psychology
class. Participation was voluntary and participants were
compensated with extra credit in the course.
Procedure. Participants completed questionnaire packets
that contained the MSS and SVS. In addition, each packet
contained a one-page list of a target’s opinions. Although
these opinions were created for the purpose of this study, the
participants were not explicitly informed that the target presenting the beliefs was fictional.2 These opinions were
constructed to be extreme and reflective of either a predominantly right- or left-wing political ideology. For example, the
right-wing condition contained the statements “I am sick of
political correctness” and “I don’t hate the rich and I don’t
pity the poor.” The left-wing condition contained the parallel
statements “I am grateful for political correctness” and “I
hate the rich and I pity the poor.” The opinions were thus
created so that it would be unlikely that the participants reading them would agree or disagree entirely with the opinions
regardless of their own political ideology. In addition, the
last sentence of each opinion list was varied so that it relayed
the target’s opinion regarding the dissemination of his or her
own opinions. In the intrusive condition, the individual
stated “I believe that everyone else should agree with my
opinions.” In the nonintrusive condition, the individual
stated “I believe that everyone has a right to his or her own
opinions.”
Following the list of the fictional target’s opinions, participants were given the opportunity to write a response to
the target. Participants were then asked to rate from 1 (not at
all) to 9 (very much) how much they were persuaded by and
how much they agreed with the opinions, and how important
it was to express their own views, to tell the target that he or
she was wrong, to try to convince the target that they themselves were right, and to try to persuade the target to agree
with their own opinions.
Ratings of participants’ free responses. Three independent
raters who were unaware of the participants’ responses to
the other measures rated the responses that the participants
wrote to the fictional target. The raters rated the responses
from 1 (very little) to 9 (very much) on how much each
participant did the following in his or her response:
expressed superiority, tolerance, agreement that the fictional target was wrong/that the participant was right;
avoided conflict; discussed own views; and tried to convince the targets of participant’s own views. All qualities
were rated reliably, with the effective reliabilities (Spearman-Brown formula) of the independent raters exceeding
.66 in each case.
Results and Discussion
Reliabilities of and Relations Between the SVS and
MSS. The internal consistency for the SVS was acceptable,
alpha = .82, whereas the internal consistency for the MSS
was lower, alpha = .65. Consistent with Study 1, the scales
were independent, r = –.08, p = .268.
Data Reduction
Participants’ self-reported goals. Principal components analysis with varimax rotation revealed that two strong factors
(eigenvalues > 1) emerged that accounted for 68.59% of the
variance. The first factor was labeled Reported Counterarguing, included four items (i.e., present opinions, own opinions
are right, other’s opinions are wrong, convince other to
agree) that loaded well (>.71), and formed a reliable composite variable, alpha = .82. The second factor was labeled
Agree With Other, included two items (i.e., agree with other,
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
24
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(1)
persuaded by other) that loaded well (>.84), and formed a
reliable composite variable, alpha = .59.
Predicting Dependent Measures
Analytic strategy. For each of the dependent measures,
hierarchical multiple regression was used to assess the
unique and incremental predictive value of several predictors, specifically to evaluate the unique predictive ability of
SV to predict the dependent measure after accounting for the
other variables. Accordingly, six steps were entered into the
analysis with all continuous predictors having been standardized before entry. Participants’ sex, dummy coded as 0
(male) and 1 (female), was entered into the first step to control for sex differences on the measures. The second step
added the ideological position of the opinions, dummy coded
as 0 (right-wing) and 1 (left-wing). The third step added the
intrusiveness of the last opinion, dummy coded as 0 (intrusive) and 1 (nonintrusive). The fourth step entered the
participants’ moral stability (MSS) scores. The fifth step
entered the participants’ SV (SVS) scores. The final step
entered the product term that carried the interaction between
SVS scores and intrusiveness level of the last opinion.3
Participants’ self-reported goals. The model for the prediction of the participants’ goals to Agree With Other was not
significant at any step of the analysis. However, as predicted,
the level of participants’ Reported Counterarguing was significantly predicted by SVS scores, R2 change = .239, F(1,
175) = 56.06, p < .001, as well as predicted by the product
term carrying the interaction between SVS scores and intrusiveness, R2 change = .022, F(1, 174) = 5.35, p = .022. First,
results showed that a positive relation existed between SV
and the participants’ goals to counterargue against the opinionated target, β = .497, p < .001. Second, simple slopes
analysis revealed that the relation between SV and Reported
Counterarguing was significantly positive regardless of
whether the opinion was intrusive; however, the relation
between SV was stronger when the last opinion was not intrusive: βs = .329 and .632, ps = .001 and < .001, for the intrusive
and nonintrusive conditions, respectively. These results are
Reported Counterarguing
Ratings of participants’ free response measures. The ratings
made by the three independent coders for each quality of the
free responses were averaged before entry into a principal
components analysis with varimax rotation that revealed the
emergence of two strong factors (eigenvalues > 1) that
accounted for 75.88% of the variance. The first factor was
labeled Actual Counterarguing, included three items (i.e.,
discuss own views, convince the individual, show oneself is
right) that loaded well (>.81), and formed a reliable composite variable, alpha = .90. The second factor was labeled
Actual Superiority, included five items (superiority, individual is wrong, tolerance reversed, avoided conflict reversed,
agreement reversed) that loaded well (>.62), and formed a
reliable composite variable, alpha = .83.
22.5
20.0
Intrusive
Nonintrusive
17.5
15.0
12.5
10.0
7.5
5.0
Low SVS
High SVS
Figure 1. Projected means for Reported Counterarguing of participants with high (1 SD above the mean) and low (1 SD below
the mean) Social Vigilantism Scale (SVS) scores against the fictional opinionated target who was intrusive or nonintrusive in the
presentation of his or her extreme opinions.
illustrated in Figure 1. In other words, participants higher on
SV were more likely to show a negative reaction toward the
nonintrusive target, in this case by being more likely to counterargue. Perhaps people higher in SV saw a greater
opportunity to impress their beliefs on a nonintrusive target.
Raters’ assessment of counterarguing and superiority in participants’ free responses. Consistent with the results predicting
the participants’ ratings of their goal to counterargue, the
model predicting the participants’ levels of Actual Counterarguing was significant only when the fifth step entered their
SVS scores, R2 change = .038, F(1, 179) = 7.08, p = .009. As
participants’ levels of SV increased, counterarguing
increased in their written responses to the opinionated target
as assessed by the raters, β = .197, p = .009.
The same pattern of results was found for the prediction
of the levels of Actual Superiority that the participants
showed in their written responses to the opinionated targets.
Only the addition of the fifth step containing participants’
SVS scores significantly improved the model, R2 change =
.049, F(1, 178) = 9.60, p = .002. Specifically, participants’
expressions of belief superiority, as assessed by raters,
increased as their levels of SV increased, β = .225, p = .002.
Interestingly, participants’ expressions of Actual Superiority mediated (Baron & Kenny, 1986) the relation between
their SVS scores and their levels of Actual Counterarguing.
When Actual Superiority was entered into the regression
model predicting Actual Counterarguing in the step following
the entry of SVS scores, the model was significantly improved,
R2 change = .300, F(1, 178) = 81.27, p < .001, indicating that
higher levels of superiority were associated with higher levels
of counterarguing, β = .577, p < .001. Furthermore, the
standardized regression coefficient for the prediction of counterarguing by SVS scores became nonsignificant, β = .062,
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
25
Saucier and Webster
p = .331. This indicates that, consistent with the predicted
nature of SV, individuals higher in SV may be more likely to
counterargue against those who express extreme opinions
because these participants believe that their own views are
superior.
In sum, consistent with our predictions, Study 1 showed
that in reaction to a target’s extreme position, participants
higher in SV expressed greater belief superiority and exhibited greater rates of counterarguing more than did individuals
with lower levels of SV. Moreover, mediational analysis
showed that when confronted by another individual’s opinions, individuals with higher levels of SV likely counterargued
more because of their exaggerated belief superiority.
Study 3: Unique Prediction of Reactions to
Another Individual’s Extreme Opinions
This study sought to replicate the effects of Study 2, such
that higher levels of SV predicted expressions of superiority
and counterarguing in response to an extremely opinionated
target. However, Study 3 intended to demonstrate that these
effects hold even after controlling for other measures that
may predict similar responses. Accordingly, the same procedure used in Study 2 was employed in Study 3, with the
addition that participants completed individual measures of
narcissism, dogmatism, reactance, and need for cognition so
that we could assess the unique predictive power of SV on
expressions of superiority and counterarguing after controlling for these individual differences.
Method
Participants. Undergraduates (N = 142; 67 men, 70 women,
and 5 declined to report their gender) voluntarily participated
during the regular class meeting of an experimental psychology in exchange for extra credit.
Procedure. Participants completed questionnaire packets
that contained the MSS, SVS, and lists of extreme right- or
left-wing opinions that included a final opinion that was
either intrusive or nonintrusive, and they were given the
opportunity to respond to and rate the author of the opinion
list. These manipulations and materials were identical to
those used in Study 2. The free responses were rated by two
independent raters for the same qualities and using the same
procedures as in the previous study, and the ratings were
again reliable (correlations between raters > .70 for all qualities). However, the following additions were made to the
questionnaire packets. Participants reported their agreement
to all items on a 1 (not at all) to 9 (very much) scale.
Dogmatism measure. Dogmatism, the tendency for individuals to be close-minded and driven by adherence to
ideology in their beliefs, was measured using the 20-item
version (Troldahl & Powell, 1965) of the Dogmatism Scale
(DS; Rokeach, 1960; Rokeach & Fruchter, 1956).
Reactance measure. Psychological reactance, the individual tendency to be oppositional when the individual feels he
or she is being restricted, was measured using the 28-item
Therapeutic Reactance Scale (TRS; Dowd et al., 1991).
Need for cognition measure. Need for cognition, the tendency for an individual to engage in and enjoy thinking, was
measured using the 34-item Need for Cognition Scale (NFC;
Cacioppo & Petty, 1982).
Narcissism measure. Individuals’ levels of narcissism—
summarily defined as an individual’s possession of a
grandiose sense of self-importance—was measured using
the 40-item Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin
& Terry, 1988).
Additional items about goals in responses. In addition to the
items included in Study 2 that assessed the participants’ goals
in making responses to the opinionated target, four items were
included to assess the participants’ belief that it was worth it to
try to change the opinionated target’s opinions in their
responses. The statements were that it was worth trying to
change the other person’s opinions, that they would be successful in changing the other’s opinions, that they could convince
the other person that they were right, and that they would spend
time trying to change the other person’s opinions.
Results and Discussion
Scale Reliabilities and Intercorrelations. The internal
consistencies for all measures were in the acceptable range:
MSS alpha = .69, SVS alpha = .87, DS alpha = .79, NFC
alpha = .93, TRS alpha = .87, and NPI alpha = .93. The correlations among the measures are shown in Table 2. SVS
scores correlated positively and significantly with scores on
the measures of dogmatism, reactance, and narcissism, but
not with the NFC.
Data Reduction
Participants’ self-reported goals. The ratings made for the
goals of participants’ responses to the opinionated target
were entered into a principal components analysis with varimax rotation, and three strong factors emerged (eigenvalues
> 1) that accounted for 72.97% of the variance. The first
factor was labeled Reported Counterarguing, included four
items (i.e., present opinions, own opinions are right, other’s
opinions are wrong, convince other to agree) that loaded well
(>.77), and formed a reliable composite variable, alpha = .85.
The second factor was labeled Agree With Other, included
two items (i.e., agree with other, persuaded by other) that
loaded well (>.79), and formed a composite variable with
relatively low internal consistency, alpha = .63. The third
factor was labeled Worth Arguing, included four items (i.e.,
successful at changing other’s opinions, could convince
other, how long spent changing other’s opinions, worth
trying to change other’s opinions) that loaded well (>.60),
and formed a reliable composite variable, alpha = .85.
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
26
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(1)
Table 2. Correlations Between Social Vigilantism, Moral Stability, Dogmatism, Need for Cognition, Reactance, and Narcissism
Measure
1
2
3
4
5
6
1. SVS
2. MSS
3. DS
4. NFC
5. TRS
6. NPI
—
.04
.49***
.02
.54***
.47***
—
–.19*
.40***
.10
.21*
—
–.27***
.25***
.28***
—
.15
.09
—
.53***
—
Note: SVS = Social Vigilantism Scale; MSS = Moral Stability Scale; DS = Dogmatism Scale; NFC = Need for Cognition Scale; TRS = Therapeutic Reactance
Scale; NPI = Narcissistic Personality Inventory.
*p < .05. ***p < .001.
Ratings of participants’ free response measures. The ratings
made by the two independent raters for the qualities of the
free responses were averaged and then entered into a principal components analysis with varimax rotation. Two strong
factors emerged (eigenvalues > 2) that accounted for 62.20%
of the variance onto which all but one (i.e., agreement with
other) of the qualities loaded well. This item was subsequently dropped from further analyses. The first factor was
labeled Actual Counterarguing, onto which three items (i.e.,
discuss own views, convince the individual, show oneself is
right) loaded well (>.83) and formed an internally consistent
composite variable, alpha = .88. The second factor was
labeled Actual Superiority, included the remaining four items
(i.e., superiority, individual is wrong, tolerance reversed,
avoided conflict reversed) that loaded well (>.49), and
formed a reliable composite variable, alpha = .71.
Predicting Dependent Measures
Analytic strategy. Hierarchical multiple regression was
used to assess the unique and additive prediction of the independent measures for each of the dependent measures. We
predicted that the addition of the participants’ SVS scores
would significantly improve the prediction of the participants’ reactions to the opinionated target upon its entry into
the model above and beyond the predictive value of the other
measures. The steps of the analyses were identical in structure to the analyses used in Study 2, except for the inclusion
of the additional measures (DS, TRS, NFC, NPI) in the step
before the entry of the SVS scores.
Participants’ self-reported goals. Consistent with the results
of Study 2, the prediction of the participants’ goal to Agree
With Other was not significant at any step of the analysis.
For the prediction of Reported Counterarguing, two steps
significantly improved the prediction offered by the model.
The addition of the fourth step improved the model, R2
change = .098, F(5, 119) = 2.63, p = .027. Examination of the
standardized regression coefficients at this step showed that
only participants’ DS scores offered significant unique prediction such that higher levels of dogmatism were associated
with higher levels of Reported Counterarguing, β = .267, p =
.008. The addition of SVS scores in the fifth step also
significantly improved the predictive value of the model, R2
change = .059, F(1, 118) = 8.33, p = .005, such that higher
levels of SV predicted higher levels of Reported Counterarguing, β = .318, p = .005. This showed that, consistent with
the results of Study 2 and with our hypotheses, SV is a significant predictor of individuals’ reports of counterarguing
above and beyond the other measures.
For the prediction of Worth Arguing, two steps improved
the predictive model significantly upon entry. Participants’
sex improved the model at the first step, R2 change = .032,
F(1, 127) = 4.14, p = .044, and indicated that male participants reported that it was worth arguing against the
opinionated target more than did female participants, β =
–.178, p = .044. Of greater theoretical import, the entry of
SVS scores in the fifth step significantly improved the
model, R2 change = .047, F(1, 119) = 6.63, p = .011, such that
as SV scores increased, so did the belief that it was worth
arguing against the opinionated target β = .286, p = .011.
Collectively, these results indicate that individuals who
are higher on SV are more likely to report trying to impress
their beliefs onto others and more likely to think they would
be successful in their attempts to impress their beliefs onto
others. We therefore thought it appropriate to test the ability
of Worth Arguing to mediate the relation between SV and
Reported Counterarguing. That is, would individuals higher
in SV be more likely to attempt to impress their beliefs onto
the opinionated target because they thought they would be
successful in doing so? To test this prediction, participants’
Worth Arguing scores were entered into the regression model
to predict Reported Counterarguing in the step that followed
the entry of SVS scores. The results showed that the relation
between SV and Reported Counterarguing was mediated by
the participants’ reported beliefs that they thought it was
worth arguing. The entry of Worth Arguing significantly
improved the model, R2 change = .144, F(1, 119) = 24.62, p
< .001, and showed that a positive relation existed between
Worth Arguing and Reported Counterarguing, β = .407, p <
.001. Furthermore, the unique prediction of participants’
levels of reported counterarguing was reduced from highly
significant to nonsignificant levels, β = .199, p = .057, upon
the entry of Worth Arguing. This mediation analysis
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
27
Saucier and Webster
indicates that individuals higher on SV may attempt to
impress their beliefs onto others because they believe that
they will be successful in their attempts.
Raters’ assessment of counterarguing and superiority in participants’ free responses. For the prediction of Actual
Counterarguing that the participants’ showed in their free
responses to the opinionated target, two steps significantly
improved the model. The entry of participants’ sex in the
first step improved the model significantly, R2 change = .056,
F(1, 129) = 7.68, p = .006, and showed that male participants
counterargued in their responses to the opinionated target
less than did female participants, β = .237, p = .006. This
result appears inconsistent with the results that show that
male participants find it more worth arguing and score higher
on the measures that have previously predicted reported and
actual levels of counterarguing in this and previous studies.
However, more consistent with our previous findings and of
greater theoretical import, the entry of participants’ SVS
scores in the fifth step again significantly improved the
model above and beyond the other predictors, R2 change =
.059, F(1, 121) = 8.41, p = .004, and showed that individuals
who scored higher on SV were more likely to show higher
levels of counterarguing in their responses to the opinionated
classmate, β = .319, p = .004. Furthermore, only the fifth
step, that is, entry of the SVS scores, approached significance in the prediction of Actual Superiority, R2 change =
.027, F(1, 121) = 3.728, p = .056. Specifically, levels of
superiority expressed increased as the participants’ SVS
scores increased, β = .217, p = .056.
Collectively, results indicated that SV predicted individuals’ reactions to a target’s extreme opinions. Specifically,
consistent with predictions, Study 3 showed that higher levels
of SV were associated with greater expression of belief superiority and counterarguing, as well as a more strongly held belief
that the counterarguing will successfully impress one’s beliefs
onto others even after controlling for individual differences in
narcissism, dogmatism, reactance, and need for cognition.
Study 4: Resistance to Persuasion Above
and Beyond Attitude Strength and
Extremity
In this last study, we predicted that individuals higher in SV
would exhibit greater rates of resistance to persuasion (including greater expression of belief superiority and counterarguing,
as well as lower rates of attitude change) after being exposed to
targets who disagreed with them, even after accounting for participants’ levels of attitude strength and extremity. This study
tested these predictions by exposing participants to a target’s
belief about sex education in public schools after they had
already reported their own level of agreement with the policy.
Participants were then given the chance to respond to the target
in writing, to evaluate the target on several adjectives, and to
report their subsequent level of agreement with teaching sex
education in public schools.
Method
Participants. Undergraduates (N = 117; 35 men, 76 women,
and 6 declined to report their gender) participated voluntarily
for extra credit during the regular class meeting of a large
social psychology class.
Procedure
Completion of measures. Participants received questionnaire packets that contained two sections in counterbalanced
orders. One section contained the SVS and MSS and was
entitled “Personal Views, Ethics, and Morality.” An additional section contained items regarding participants’ level of
support for the teaching of sex education in public schools, a
fictional classmate’s opinion about sexual education, and
opportunities for the participants to respond to and evaluate
the classmate. This section first asked the participants to
report their level of agreement with the statement “Sex education should be taught in the public schools of the United
States” from 1 (disagree very strongly) to 9 (agree very
strongly). Participants were also asked to rate how important
this issue is to them from 1 (not at all important) to 9
(extremely important). The participants were then asked to
imagine that a classmate communicated his or her4 beliefs
about sex education to them. This statement was manipulated so that half of the participants were randomly assigned
a statement that endorsed sex education and half of the participants were randomly assigned a statement that opposed
sex education. Following this statement, participants were
given the opportunity to write a response to the classmate in
a free response format and to rate how well each of 16 adjectives (e.g., unpleasant, awkward, tolerant, likable, friendly,
intelligent) described the classmate from 1 (not at all) to 9
(very much). Participants were also asked to report their level
of agreement with the original statement about sex education
after having been exposed to their classmate’s opinion.
Ratings of participants’ free responses. Two independent
raters who were unaware of the participants’ responses to the
other dependent measures evaluated the responses the participants wrote to the fictional classmate from 1 (very little)
to 9 (very much) on how superior, hostile, tolerant, friendly,
diplomatic, persuasive, intrusive, sophisticated, and rigid the
responses were. All qualities were rated reliably, with the
correlation between the ratings of the independent raters
exceeding .57 in each case.5
Results and Discussion
Reliabilities of and Relations Between the SVS and
MSS. The internal consistencies were acceptable for both
the SVS and MSS, alphas = .88 and .71, respectively. The
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
28
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(1)
scores on the SVS and MSS were not significantly correlated, r = .15, p = .119.
Data Reduction
Participants’ ratings of the classmate. The 16 adjective ratings for the classmate were entered into a principal
components analysis with varimax rotation. Four strong factors emerged (eigenvalues > 1) that accounted for 69.56% of
the variance. Five adjective ratings (unpleasant, awkward,
offensive, immoral, prejudiced) loaded (>.68) onto a Negative Ratings factor and formed a reliable composite variable,
alpha = .88. Six adjective ratings (likable, friendly, intelligent, hard-working, agreeable) loaded (>.55) onto a Positive
Ratings factor and formed an additional reliable composite
variable, alpha = .86. The remaining adjective ratings failed
to load onto factors that would produce composite variables
with acceptable levels of internal consistency and were
deleted from further analyses.
Ratings of participants’ free responses. The ratings by the
independent raters of the participants’ written responses to
the fictional classmate were averaged and then entered into a
principal components analysis with varimax rotation. Two
strong factors emerged that accounted for 79.20% of the
variance. Only the combination of the ratings that loaded
(>.48) on the first factor (superior, hostile, intrusive, rigid,
friendly reversed, diplomatic reversed, and tolerant reversed)
produced a reliable composite variable, alpha = .93, labeled
Aggressive Counterarguing, and the other ratings were not
subsequently used in later analyses.
Predicting Dependent Measures
Analytic strategy. For each dependent measure, hierarchical
multiple regression was used to assess the unique and incremental value of several predictors in accounting for variance
in the dependent measure. Specifically, these analyses were
constructed so that the ability of SV to predict reactions to
the classmate’s opinion regarding the teaching of sex education could be assessed after accounting for the prediction
offered by attitude extremity and attitude importance. Seven
steps were entered into the analysis, and all continuous variables were standardized before entry. The first step contained
participants’ sex to control for sex differences on the independent measures. The second step added the position that
the classmate took regarding the teaching of sex education.
Because 90.6% of the participants reported agreement with
teaching sex education in public schools by choosing a value
of 6 or greater on the 9-point scale, only these participants
are included in the regression analyses.6 The position taken
by the classmate was therefore rephrased as agreement (supported sex education) or disagreement (opposed sex
education) and dummy-coded 0 and 1, respectively, before
entry in the second step of the analyses. The third step added
the participants’ reports of how important the sex education
issue was to them and added an attitude extremity measure
that indicated how far from the midpoint the participants
reported that their initial agreement was with teaching sex
education (e.g., a value of 9 is 4 units away from the midpoint value of 5). The fourth step added the product terms
that carried the interactions between attitude importance and
agreement/disagreement and between attitude extremity and
agreement/disagreement. The fifth step added the participants’ moral stability (MSS) scores. The sixth step added the
participants’ SV (SVS) scores. The seventh step added the
product term that carried the interaction between SV and
agreement/disagreement.
Participants’ positive ratings of the classmate. The only step
of the analysis that reached significance was the second step
that entered agreement/disagreement into the model predicting the participants’ positive ratings of the classmate, R2
change = .081, F(1, 96) = 8.53, p = .004. Results indicated
that participants made less positive ratings of the classmate
when the classmate disagreed with the participants, β =
–.285, p = .004.
Participants’ negative ratings of the classmate. Three steps
offered significant improvements to the model predicting the
negative ratings given to the classmate by the participant.
The entry of agreement/disagreement significantly improved
the model at the second step of the analysis, R2 change =
.055, F(1, 96) = 5.63, p = .020, and indicated that participants rated the classmate more negatively when the classmate
disagreed with the participants, β = .235, p = .020. The entry
of participants’ moral stability scores improved the model
upon entry in the fifth step of the analysis, R2 change = .055,
F(1, 91) = 6.13, p = .015, and indicated that participants who
were higher in moral stability provided less negative ratings
of the classmate, β = –.244, p = .015. Finally, the seventh
step of the analysis in which the product term carrying the
interaction between SV scores and agreement/disagreement
was entered significantly improved the model, R2 change =
.039, F(1, 89) = 4.50, p = .037. Simple slopes analysis (Aiken
& West, 1991) revealed that although no significant relation
existed between SV scores and negative ratings in the agreement condition, β = –.084, p = .554, there was a significant
positive relation between SV scores and negative ratings in
the disagreement condition, β = .322, p = .020 (see Figure 2).
These results show that individuals who are higher in SV are
more likely than individuals who are lower in SV to dislike
those who disagree with them.
Aggressive counterarguing. Only two steps in the analysis
were significant upon entry in predicting the participants’
levels of aggressive counterarguing. Results for entry of the
second step indicated that the addition of agreement/disagreement did significantly improve the prediction, R2
change = .205, F(1, 95) = 24.58, p < .001. Expectedly, examination of the standardized regression coefficients indicated
that participants unsurprisingly showed higher levels of
aggressive counterarguing when the classmate disagreed
with them, β = .454, p < .001.
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
29
Saucier and Webster
22.5
Negative Ratings
20.0
Agreed
Disagreed
17.5
15.0
12.5
10.0
7.5
Low SVS
High SVS
Figure 2. Projected means for Negative Ratings for participants
with high (1 SD above the mean) and low (1 SD below the mean)
Social Vigilantism Scale (SVS) scores of the fictional classmate who
agreed or disagreed with participants about sex education policy.
Furthermore, results for the entry of the sixth step indicated that the addition of participants’ SV scores significantly
improved the model, R2 change = .047, F(1, 89) = 5.92, p =
.017. This showed that consistent with predictions, after controlling for the effects of attitude importance, attitude
extremity, and moral stability, higher levels of SV predicted
higher levels of aggressive counterarguing, β = .236, p <
.017. Interestingly, this effect was not qualified by an interaction between SV and agreement/disagreement when the
seventh step was entered, R2 change = .017, F(1, 88) = 2.22,
p = .140. This indicates that individuals higher in SV showed
the tendency to aggressively disseminate their own views to
other people even when those individuals already agree with
them.
Stability of attitudes toward sex education. A chief objective
of this study was to examine how well SV could predict participants’ attitude stability. Accordingly, a main dependent
measure of this study was the difference between participants’ initial reports of their agreement with teaching sexual
education in public schools and their reports of agreement
with the same policy after being exposed to the opinion of a
fictional classmate. We had predicted that individuals who
were higher in SV would be less likely to alter their attitudes
than would individuals lower in SV, even after controlling
for the individuals’ reports of how important this issue was to
them and for the extremity of their initial attitude. The same
hierarchical multiple regression analytic strategy used in
each of the preceding analyses was used to test this prediction using the difference score between their initial agreement
and agreement after the classmate’s opinion as the dependent
variable.
Two steps improved the prediction of attitude change upon
entry into the model. The third step that contained the attitude
extremity and attitude importance variables significantly
improved the model upon entry, R2 change = .131, F(2, 95) =
7.41, p = .001. Examination of the regression coefficients
showed that attitude extremity, β = .297, p = .008, and attitude
importance, β = –.417, p < .001, each predicted unique portions of the variance in attitude change. However, the entry of
the sixth step that contained participants’ SV scores also
improved the model at near significant levels, R2 change =
.033, F(1, 91) = 3.83, p = .053, β = –.171. To better illustrate
the nature of SV scores with attitude stability, we used the
regression equation to predict the amount of attitude change
that would be projected for individuals whose SV scores were
1 SD above and below the mean. We found that although individuals who scored 1 SD below the mean were projected to
show a substantial amount of attitude change (difference
score = .408), individuals who scored 1 SD above the mean
were projected to show virtually no attitude change (difference score = .044). Solving the regression equation for
absolutely no attitude change (difference score = 0), we found
that this would be expected for participants who scored 1.24
SD above the mean on SV. These results support our predictions and show that individuals who score higher on SV,
compared to those who score lower, show more attitude stability and that this stability is predicted by SV above and
beyond the prediction provided by the individuals’ levels of
attitude importance and extremity.
Overall, the results of Study 4 supported our hypotheses
about the nature of SV. First, the higher the level of SV, the
more negatively participants rated the target who disagreed
with them. Higher levels of SV were also associated with
increased levels of counterarguing. Finally, higher levels of
SV were associated with higher levels of attitude stability,
even after controlling for attitude importance and extremity.
These results held regardless of whether the target’s position
aligned with the participants’. These results support our contention that SV is a unique individual difference that predicts
the evaluation and behavioral responses to people who not
only present extreme opinions, as in Studies 2 and 3, but to
those who generally disagree with them, as in Study 4. Moreover, Study 4 highlighted not only the aggressive
counterarguing exhibited by those higher in SV but also the
stability of their attitudes after a persuasion appeal.
General Discussion
SV is an enduring individual difference that assesses people’s tendency to impress and propagate their “superior”
beliefs onto others to correct others’ more “ignorant” opinions. After establishing a reliable self-report measure of SV
(Study 1), Study 2 demonstrated that higher levels of SV are
associated with greater expressions of belief superiority and
reported counterarguing toward a target expressing an
extreme ideological opinion. It is important to note that these
effects held even after controlling for participants’ ratings of
agreement. Study 3 showed that SV was again associated
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
30
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(1)
with greater expression of belief superiority and counterarguing even after accounting for the contribution of individual
difference measures of narcissism, dogmatism, reactance,
and need for cognition. Study 4 showed that people higher in
SV again expressed greater rates of counterarguing and evaluated dissenters more negatively but, more importantly,
exhibited less attitude change after a persuasion appeal.
Together, these studies demonstrate that SV is an important
individual difference variable that uniquely accounts for
variance above and beyond characteristics both of the individual and of the attitude previously studied in the social
psychological literature.
SV did correlate moderately with other individual difference measures (narcissism, dogmatism, psychological
reactance, and need for cognition) that would likely predict
similar perceptions of one’s own beliefs and similar reactions
to the expressions of others’ opinions. However, even after
controlling for these measures, SV remained an empirically
unique predictor of belief superiority and resistance to persuasion. That is, SV is related to these constructs with good
reason, and the nature and magnitude of the relations serves
as a convergent validity check of the measure of SV; however,
SV does not appear to be redundant with these measures.
The exploration of SV as an individual difference that
may predict resistance to persuasion is important in understanding the nature of persuasion and attitude resistance.
Essentially, SV may predict that some individuals will not be
successfully influenced by persuasion attempts. Our studies
indicate that counterarguing was initiated by individuals
higher in SV regardless of their agreement with the message
or by the intrusiveness of the persuader. This suggests that
these individuals may be more provoked by the mere presentation of opinions—and not by the opinions themselves—into
counterarguing, using their own beliefs as ammunition
against the persuasive message. By counterarguing, and by
believing that it is worthwhile to disseminate one’s own
beliefs, individuals higher in SV may be generally more
resistant to persuasion.
Furthermore, research on SV should address the efficacy
of various persuasion methods based on individuals’ levels
of SV to identify techniques that may be more effective in
circumventing the resistance to persuasion displayed by
those higher in SV. In addition, because the studies reported
here do not involve an actual confrontation by a persuader in
person, future studies will investigate the cognitive and
behavioral responses to the presentation of persuasive messages and extreme beliefs by others.
It may also be useful to further explore how SV has
evolved as an individual difference. We postulate that people
higher in SV have unusually strong confidence in the rightness of their beliefs, regardless of whether the beliefs can be
objectively falsified (confidence in one’s beliefs does not
necessarily correlate with accuracy or correctness). We posit
that feelings of belief superiority accumulate and strengthen
over time (starting in childhood) with repeated, multifaceted
interactions with assenters that confirm the rightness of one’s
beliefs as well as repeated interactions with dissenters in
which people higher in SV learn to “defend” the rightness of
their own beliefs when confronted with a persuasive message to change their attitudes (cf. Williams, Huang, & Bargh,
in press). We also reason that these feelings of belief superiority and rightness over time encompass not only deep-seated
and important attitudes but also any attitude held (for which
we have preliminarily received support in the set of current
studies). Yet, our studies do not speak directly to the development of SV. Our studies have demonstrated that we can
reliably and validly measure SV and that SV uniquely
accounts for variance above and beyond characteristics of
both the individual and the attitude previously studied in the
social psychological literature. Its developmental underpinnings, however riveting, will need to be carefully investigated
with further research.
Similarly, it would be fruitful to demonstrate more thoroughly the motivational nature and power of SV. For
example, would people higher in SV feel more frustrated if
the opportunity to impress their beliefs was impeded or
thwarted? Moreover, it would be fruitful to investigate the
lengths people higher in SV go to to fulfill their motivation.
Would they give up their time and money to have the opportunity to propagate their superior beliefs to correct someone’s
misguided attitudes? Would people higher in SV sacrifice
being liked by others to propagate their superior beliefs?
Although we would answer in the affirmative to these questions, only further research will be able to provide evidence
that people higher in SV will go “the extra mile” to have the
opportunity to propagate their beliefs and sway other people’s opinions. Furthermore, it would be intriguing to assess
whether the effects of SV are much like goal-directed behavior; that is, once people higher in SV fulfill their motivation
to impress their beliefs, would the motivation be satisfied
and thus less likely to occur second time (cf. Bargh, Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai, Barndollar, & Troetschel, 2001)?7
Lastly, while these studies did attempt to evaluate the relations with other variables that are associated with similar
attitudinal and behavioral responses, future work is needed to
address the relations with other individual difference measures,
such as different types of sociopolitical ideology (e.g., social
dominance orientation) and the nature or type of attitude (e.g.,
sex education vs. racism). Given the scope of the literature on
individual differences and attitude characteristics, we see
almost endless applications of SV in all fields of psychology.
SV is a distinct and useful individual difference variable
that will allow for a more comprehensive understanding of
the nature of belief superiority, persuasion, and attitude resistance. It will also assist in the understanding of how
individuals perceive their own beliefs as superior relative to
those of others and the resultant need to perpetuate these
beliefs. This is an important domain that needs to be explored
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
31
Saucier and Webster
given the international conflicts that have arisen as a direct
result of one group or nation attempting to assert its own
beliefs (e.g., religious, political) onto other groups or nations
that do not share those beliefs. SV may have been a potential
factor in incidents ranging from the Spanish Inquisition to
the Israeli–Palestinian religious conflicts to the September
11 terrorist attacks. Understanding the individual propensity
for these cognitions and behaviors can serve to explain and,
hopefully, ultimately inform interventions designed to possibly circumvent these processes.
Authors’ Note:
We thank Jonathan Amburgey, Audrey Cawman, Sherise Davis, Marc Genzler, Carol Gordon, Melody Hunsinger, and Rachel Mitchell for their help in
collecting data, coding free responses, and offering suggestions, and Eric
Fisher for providing insight. We thank Mary Cain, Monica Harris, Richard
Smith, and the members of the Saucier Research Lab for their comments on
earlier drafts of this article.
Declaration of Conflict of Interest
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interests with respect to
their authorship or the publication of this article.
Financial Disclosure/Funding
The authors declared that they received no financial support for their
research and/or authorship of this article.
Notes
1. Items for the Moral Stability Scale are available from the authors
on request.
2. The fictional individual’s sex was not specified to the participants.
3. An alternative data analytic strategy was used to test all of the
two-, three-, and four-way interactions between SV scores,
participants’ sex, the ideological position of the opinions, and
the intrusiveness of the last opinion. None of these interactions, beyond the interactions reported in text, reached significance in improving the regression models used to predict each
of the dependent variables. Of note, the two-way interactions
between SV and the ideological position of the opinions failed
to improve the predictive models, suggesting that those higher
in SV had similar reactions to both the right- and left-wing ideological positions.
4. The fictional individual’s sex was not specified to the participants.
5. Raters also rated for the presence or absence of statements made
by participants that insulted the classmate, explicitly stated that
the classmate was wrong, and explicitly showed respect for the
classmate’s opinion. These statements were relatively infrequent. Interestingly, however, the participants who insulted the
classmate (n = 4) were significantly higher on the SVS than
those who did not, t(114) = 2.30, p = .023. In addition, the
participants who stated that the classmate was wrong (n = 16)
were significantly higher on the SVS than those who did not,
t(114) = 2.78, p = .006, and the participants who showed respect
for the classmate’s opinion (n = 46) were lower on the SVS,
t(114) = 1.81, p = .073, at marginally significant levels.
6. The excluded participants were not significantly different from
the included participants on the SVS.
7. We thank anonymous reviewers for suggesting these discussion
points.
References
Abelson, R. P. (1959). Modes of resolution of belief dilemmas.
Journal of Conflict Resolution, 5, 343-352.
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A. Y., Barndollar, K., &
Troetschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 81, 1014-1027.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator
variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.
Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. New
York: Academic Press.
Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. (1981). Psychological reactance: A
theory of freedom and control. New York: Academic Press.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116-134.
Cameron, K. A., Jacks, J. Z., & O’Brien, M. E. (2002). An experimental examination of strategies for resisting persuasion. Current Research in Social Psychology, 7, 205-224.
Campbell, W. K. (1999). Narcissism and romantic attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1254-1270.
Dowd, E. T., Milne, C. R., & Wise, S. L. (1991). The therapeutic
reactance scale: A measure of psychological reactance. Journal
of Counseling and Development, 69, 541-545.
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes.
New York: Harcourt Brace.
Emmons, R. A. (1989). Exploring the relations between motives
and traits: The case of narcissism. In D. M. Buss & N. Cantor (Eds.), Personality psychology: Recent trends and emerging
directions (pp. 32-44). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Festinger, L., & Maccoby, N. (1964). On resistance to persuasive
communications. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
68, 359-366.
Jacks, J. Z., & Cameron, K. A. (2003). Strategies for resisting persuasion. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 25, 145-161.
Jacks, J. Z., & Devine, P. G. (2000). Attitude importance, forewarning of message content, and resistance to persuasion. Basic and
Applied Social Psychology, 22, 19-29.
Kemp, C. G. (1960). Changes in values in relation to open-closed
systems. In M. Rokeach (Ed.), The open and closed mind
(pp. 335-346). New York: Basic Books.
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
32
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(1)
Kernis, M. H., & Sun, C. R. (1994). Narcissism and reactions to interpersonal feedback. Journal of Research in Personality, 28, 4-13.
Krosnick, J. A., Boninger, D. S., Chuang, Y. C., Berent, M. K., &
Carnot, C. G. (1993). Attitude strength: One construct or many
related constructs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1132-1151.
Krosnick, J. A., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Attitude strength: An overview. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength:
Antecedents and consequences (pp. 1-24). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Larsen, R. J., & Buss, D. M. (2002). Personality psychology: Domains
of knowledge about human nature. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Morf, C. C., & Rhodewalt, F. (1993). Narcissism and self-­
evaluation maintenance: Explorations in object relations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 668-676.
Morf, C. C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unraveling the paradoxes of
narcissism: A dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychological Inquiry, 12, 177-196.
Nail, P. R., Van Leeuwen, M. D., & Powell, A. B. (1996). The
effectance versus the self-presentational view of reactance:
Are importance ratings influenced by anticipated surveillance?
Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 11, 573-584.
Nimmer, J. G., & Handelsman, M. M. (1992). Effects of subject
pool policy on student attitudes toward psychology and psychological research. Teaching of Psychology, 19, 141-144.
Petty, R. E., & Krosnick, J. A. (1995). Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal components analysis of
the Narcissistic Personality Inventory and further evidence of its
construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 890-902.
Rokeach, M. (1954). The nature and meaning of dogmatism.
Psychological Review, 61, 194-204.
Rokeach, M. (1960). The open and closed mind. New York: Basic
Books.
Rokeach, M., & Fruchter, B. (1956). A factorial study of dogmatism
and related concepts. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 53, 356-360.
Shrauger, J. S., & Kelly, R. J. (1988). Global self-evaluation
and changes in self-description as a function of information
discrepancy and favorability. Journal of Personality, 56,
709-728.
Smalley, R. L., & Stake, J. E. (1996). Evaluating sources of egothreatening feedback: Self-esteem and narcissism effects. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 483-495.
Swann, W. B., Griffin, J. J., Predmore, S. C., & Gaines, B. (1987).
The cognitive-affective crossfire: When self-consistency confronts self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 881-889.
Troldahl, V. C., & Powell, F. A. (1965). A short-form dogmatism
scale for use in field studies. Social Forces, 44, 211-214.
Visser, P. S., Krosnick, J. A., & Simmons, J. P. (2003). Distinguishing the cognitive and behavioral consequences of attitude importance and certainty: A new approach to testing the
common-factor hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 39, 118-141.
Wellins, R. (1977). Counterarguing and selective exposure to persuasion. Journal of Social Psychology, 103, 115-127.
Williams, L. E., Huang, J. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (in press). The scaffolded mind: Higher mental processes are grounded in early
experience of the physical world. European Journal of Social
Psychology.
Zuwerink, J. R., & Devine, P. G. (1996). Attitude importance
and resistance to persuasion: It’s not just the thought that
counts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70,
931-944.
Downloaded from http://psp.sagepub.com at KANSAS STATE UNIV LIBRARIES on January 20, 2010
Download