Document 10973476

advertisement
 RELATIONS AMONG INTERPARENTAL CONFLICT, PARENTING PRACTICES, AND
EMOTION REGULATION DURING EMERGING ADULTHOOD
A DISSERTATION
SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL
IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS
FOR THE DEGREE
DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY
BY
XIAOPENG GONG
DR. SHARON PAULSON - ADVISOR
BALL STATE UNIVERSITY
MUNCIE, INDIANA
JULY 2013
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I would like to express my ultimate gratitude to my chair, Dr. Sharon Paulson, for her dedication
and guidance on the dissertation; without her mentorship and devotion, the work would not have
been completed. My gratitude also extends to the committee members, Dr. Jerrell Cassady, Dr.
Winnie Mucherah, Dr. Holmes Finch, and Dr. Christopher Airriess, for their support, time and
energy throughout the process.
ii
DEDICATION
To my husband —
For his years of love, support and encouragement!
To my daughter —
For her presence and smile, she is so precious and wonderful!
To my parents —
For their love and inspiration!
iii
ABSTRACT
DISSERTATION: Relations Among Interparental Conflict, Parenting Practices, and Emotion
Regulation During Emerging Adulthood
STUDENT: Xiaopeng Gong
DEGREE: Doctor of Philosophy
COLLEGE: Teachers College
DATE: July, 2013
PAGES: 149
This study examined the influence of parenting-related factors to emerging adults’
emotion regulation, especially in the context of interparental conflict. Specifically, the purpose of
the study was to examine how interparental conflict, parenting (defined as parental psychological
control, autonomy support, and behavioral control), and parent-child relations (defined as parentchild attachment) are related to emerging adults’ emotion regulation. In addition, do parenting
behaviors (psychological control, autonomy support, and behavioral control) and parent-child
attachment mediate the relations between interparental conflict and emotion regulation? A total
of 361 college students reported their perceptions of interparental conflict, their parents’
parenting practices, parent-child attachment, as well as their emotion regulation capabilities. The
majority of the participants were females (n = 292), and Caucasians (n = 322) with an average
age of 20.23 (SD = 1.39) years. In general, the participants reported moderate interparental
conflict, relatively low psychological control and behavioral control, moderate levels of parental
autonomy support, and high parent-child attachment, along with relatively high emotion
regulation capabilities. With regression analyses, the results showed that emerging adults who
reported higher levels of resolution of interparental conflict, moderate levels of parental
behavioral control, greater attachment communication, and lower levels of alienation from
iv
parents were associated with better emotion regulation. Path analyses were used to test the role
of parenting and attachment in mediating the relations between interparental conflict and
emotion regulation. Results demonstrated that parental behavioral control, autonomy support,
and parent-child attachment partially mediated the role of resolution of interparental conflict on
emotion regulation. In addition, parental behavioral control and autonomy support partially
mediated the impact of resolution of interparental conflict on emerging adults’ alienation from
parents. In particular, resolution of interparental conflict was the strongest predictor of emerging
adults’ emotion regulation capability, even when parenting practices and parent-child attachment
were controlled.
v
TABLES OF CONTENTS
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS..........................................................................................................ii
DEDICATION............................................................................................................................ iii
ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................................ iv
TABLE OF CONTENTS ............................................................................................................ vi
LIST OF TABLES....................................................................................................................... ix
LIST OF FIGURES ...................................................................................................................... x
CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................. 1
Statement of Purpose ........................................................................................................ 1
Rationale ........................................................................................................................... 3
The Current Study .......................................................................................................... 12
CHAPTER II: REVIEW OF LITERTURE ................................................................................ 16
Emotional Development in Infancy and Toddlerhood ................................................... 16
Emotional Development during Childhood and Adolescence ....................................... 20
Emotion Regulation ........................................................................................................ 25
Family Influences on Emotion Regulation ..................................................................... 32
Parent-Child Relations: Attachment ................................................................... 32
Parenting Behaviors: Parental Control and Autonomy Support ......................... 34
Distinguishing Parent-Child Relations and Parenting Behaviors ....................... 42
Interparental Conflict ...................................................................................................... 43
Emerging Adulthood ...................................................................................................... 55
CHAPTER III: METHOD .......................................................................................................... 58
Participants ..................................................................................................................... 59
Measures ......................................................................................................................... 60
Demographics ..................................................................................................... 60
Emotion Regulation ............................................................................................ 60
Interparental Conflict .......................................................................................... 65
vi
Parental Psychological Control .......................................................................... 66
Parental Autonomy Support ............................................................................... 67
Parental Behavioral Control ............................................................................... 68
Parent-Child Attachment .................................................................................... 68
Procedure ........................................................................................................................ 69
Data Analysis .................................................................................................................. 70
Preliminary Analysis .......................................................................................... 70
Regression Analysis ........................................................................................... 70
Path Analysis ...................................................................................................... 71
CHAPTER IV: RESULTS ......................................................................................................... 74
Preliminary Analysis ...................................................................................................... 74
Research Question One .................................................................................................. 77
Research Question Two .................................................................................................. 87
Post-hoc Analysis ........................................................................................................... 93
CHAPTER V: DISCUSSION .................................................................................................... 96
Direct Relations Between Family Factors and Emotion Regulation .............................. 97
Indirect Effects of Interparental Conflict with Emotion Regulation ............................ 100
Importance of the Current Study .................................................................................. 105
Limitations of the Study ............................................................................................... 107
Directions for Future Study .......................................................................................... 108
Conclusion .................................................................................................................... 110
REFERENCES ......................................................................................................................... 111
APPENDICES .......................................................................................................................... 132
Appendix A................................................................................................................... 132
Appendix B ................................................................................................................... 134
Appendix C ................................................................................................................... 136
Appendix D................................................................................................................... 137
vii
Appendix E ................................................................................................................... 138
Appendix F ................................................................................................................... 139
viii
LIST OF TABLES
Table 1
Demographic Information of Participants .......................................................... 59
Table 2
Fit Indices for Four Confirmatory Factor Analysis Models of SREIT Scale
............................................................................................................................ 62
Table 3
Mean, Standard Deviations, and Reliability of Variables .................................. 75
Table 4
Inter-Correlations Among Variables .................................................................. 76
Table 5
Simultaneous Regression Analysis Predicting Emotion Regulation from
Interparental Conflict, Parenting Practices, and Parent-Child Attachment
............................................................................................................................ 85
Table 6
Results of Path Analyses: Parenting Practices and Parent-Child
Attachment as Mediators Between Interparental Conflict and Emotion
Regulation ........................................................................................................... 89
Table 7
Total and Specific Indirect Effect of Frequency, Intensity, and Resolution
of Interparental Conflict on Emotion Regulation ............................................... 91
ix
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 1
Confirmatory Factor Analysis of SREIT Model One ......................................... 63
Figure 2
Confirmatory Factor Analysis of SREIT Model Two ........................................ 64
Figure 3
Proposed Model for Research Question One ..................................................... 72
Figure 4
Proposed Model for Research Question Two ..................................................... 73
Figure 5
Scatterplot for Checking Homoscedasticity Assumption ................................... 78
Figure 6
P-P Plot for Checking Normality Assumption ................................................... 78
Figure 7
Histogram for Checking Normality Assumption................................................ 79
Figure 8
Linearity Relationship Checking: Frequency of IPC with ER ........................... 79
Figure 9
Linearity Relationship Checking: Intensity of IPC with ER .............................. 80
Figure 10
Linearity Relationship Checking: Resolution of IPC with ER ........................... 80
Figure 11
Linearity Relationship Checking: Psychological Control with ER .................... 81
Figure 12
Linearity Relationship Checking: Autonomy Support with ER ......................... 81
Figure 13
Linearity Relationship Checking: Behavioral Control with ER ......................... 82
Figure 14
Linearity Relationship Checking: Attachment Trust with ER............................ 82
Figure 15
Linearity Relationship Checking: Attachment Communication with ER .......... 83
Figure 16
Linearity Relationship Checking: Attachment Alienation with ER ................... 83
Figure 17
Results of Multiple Regression Analysis............................................................ 86
Figure 18
Path Analysis Model with Parenting Practices and Parent-Child
Attachment as One Level of Mediators .............................................................. 92
Figure 19
Path Analysis Model with Parenting Practices and Parent-Child
Attachment as Two Levels of Mediators ............................................................ 95
x
1
CHAPTER I
Introduction
Statement of Purpose
Over the past two decades, there has been a renewed interest in emotion regulation,
defined as “extrinsic and intrinsic processes responsible for monitoring, evaluating, and
modifying emotional reactions, especially their intensive and temporal features, to accomplish
one’s goals” (Thompson, 1994, pp. 27-28), as a psychological construct worthy of further
investigation (Cole, Martin, & Dennis, 2004). Almost ten years ago, a special section of a
leading journal in psychological science was devoted to discussing and clarifying emotion
regulation as an important psychological construct, particularly its importance to understanding
children’s and adolescents’ development (Langlois, 2004; see Child Development, March/April
2004 for details). Research about the development of emotion regulation has continued to grow,
particularly with regard to the contextual, specifically family, factors that influence its
development. In addition, emotional development research historically has focused on young
children, with studies about adolescents and emerging adults needed (Gullone, Hughes, King, &
Tonge, 2010; Yap, Allen, & Ladouceur, 2008).
There is much consistency in how emotion regulation is defined in the literature, and its
characteristics are well captured within the construct of emotional intelligence. First proposed by
Salovey and Mayer (1990), emotional intelligence consisted of a set of four abilities that
included perception, appraisal, and expression of emotion with self and others; accessing and/or
generating feelings that foster thought; understanding of emotion and emotion-related
knowledge; and regulation of emotions to facilitate personal emotional and intellectual growth
(Mayer & Salovey, 1997; Salovey & Mayer). The four areas of emotion-related abilities
represented a series of psychological processes with perception, appraisal, and expression of
2
emotion considered the lowest level ability and regulation of emotion the highest. According to
Salovey and Mayer’s theory, emotion regulation as an important part of emotional intelligence
has two components: regulation of emotion in the self and regulation of emotion in others.
Specifically, emotion regulation can be divided into individuals’ capabilities of staying open to
both positive and negative feelings, engaging or detaching certain emotions based on their utility,
monitoring emotions relating to self and others, and managing one’s own and others’ emotions
adaptively (e.g., fostering pleasant ones and lessening unpleasant ones) without suppression or
exaggeration. These abilities emerge at different times with openness to emotions developing
first and conscious, reflective emotion regulation developing last (Mayer & Salovey, 1997). For
the current study, emotion regulation is used only to refer to an individual’s conscious regulation
of emotions in oneself (e.g., making efforts to maintain or dampen a good mood, changing or
improving a bad mood, or simply leaving a certain mood unattended) to achieve personal
adaptive functioning and growth (Mayer & Salovey, 1997).
Family as the main context where emotional socialization occurs has the most important
influence on individuals’ emotional regulation development (Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers, &
Robinson, 2007). In addition, an ecological systems approach would suggest that to understand a
person’s emotional development, family contexts such as marital relations and parent-child
interactions must be taken into account (Bronfenbrenner & Crouter, 1983). In a widely cited
review and conceptual article, Morris et al. (2007) proposed a theoretical model to explain how
family might influence children’s and adolescents’ emotion regulation. Specifically, the overall
family emotional climate including interparental relations, parent-child attachment, and
parenting style were highlighted as significant contributors to emotion regulation. Indeed,
numerous empirical research studies have confirmed the role of parenting behaviors and parent-
3
child relations in the development of children’s emotion regulation (Chan, 2011; Cummings,
Braungart-Rieker, & Du Rocher Schudlich, 2003; Gilliom, Shaw, Beck, Schonberg, & Lukon,
2002; McDowell, Kim, O’Neil, & Parke, 2002; Moilanen, 2007; Strayer & Roberts, 2004;
Wolfradt, Hempel, & Miles, 2003). However, given the changes in family relationships and
parenting practices that occur during adolescence and particularly changes in attachment and
autonomy during emerging adulthood when children leave home (Arnett, 2007), an urgency to
study family influences on emotional development among adolescents was expressed (Morris et
al.). The purpose of the current study is to extend what is known about family influences on
emotion regulation during childhood and to examine the relations of interparental conflict,
parenting practices, and parent-child relations, particularly transitions in attachment, and emotion
regulation during late adolescence/emerging adulthood.
Rationale
Parents as important socialization agents of emotion have a significant impact on
fostering or undermining their children’s emotion regulation. Parents assume the responsibility
of regulating their children’s emotions and behavior when they are young (Kopp, 1989). Early
parenting is closely associated with and indicated by attachment between parents and children.
Children with warm, responsive parents or caregivers are more likely to build positive
expectations that their needs would be attended to when needed (Cassidy, 1994) thus they
become more competent and ready to turn to parents for assistance when a need rises; whereas
children who encountered cold, neglectful parents tend to form expectations that their parents
might neglect or only selectively attend to their signals (Cassidy, 1994). In other words, emotion
regulation is facilitated by the existence of beneficial (or subjective desired) outcomes, and if
such outcomes are absent no matter what has been tried, emotion regulation is thus undermined
4
(Thompson, 1994). Consequently, parent-child attachment would be indicative of whether
parents were emotionally available to children, especially in times of distress.
Infants and children use different emotion regulation responses to their caregivers’
behaviors, which function as ways to maintain their relationships with caregivers (Cassidy,
1994). For example, in one observational study, Malatesta and colleagues (Malatesta, Culver,
Tesman, & Shepard, 1989) found that insecurely attached toddlers showed more positive affect
during reunion with mothers when emotional distress should have been expressed, indicating that
they had exercised more constraints and compression over their emotions (through facial
expressions). In addition, avoidance of mothers also was observed. The abnormal onset of
positive affect together with avoidance of reunion was interpreted as insecurely attached
children’s emotional regulation strategies to control their anger. Securely attached children, on
the other hand, engaged in more communication with mothers (e.g., eye contact) and expressed
more distress, demonstrating that they were openly expressing their frustration (Malatesta et al.,
1989). Cassidy (1994) argued that children engaged in various emotion regulation behaviors with
the aim to preserve and protect their relations with caregivers. Insecurely attached children’s
adoption of emotion suppression reflected their understanding and expectation based on previous
contact and experience with caregivers that their parents would rather not be bothered.
Attachment continues to serve as a strong foundation for adaptive emotional functioning
during adolescence (Hill, 1983; Hill & Holmbeck, 1986; Morris et al., 2007). Attachment
between adolescents and parents has been conceptualized and measured through trusting
relations, open communication, as well as feelings of closeness between parent and child
(Armsden & Greenberg, 1987; Gomez & McLaren, 2007), and has been associated with
adolescents’ emotional autonomy and self-regulation (Gallarin & Alonso-Arbiol, 2012).
5
As children grow, parental emotional availability becomes demonstrated through the
styles that parents use to interact with their children. The parenting literature has featured various
typological approaches to the study of parenting style. Most commonly used is the typological
approach proposed by Baumrind (1971) and transformed by Maccoby and Martin (1983).
Baumrind originally identified three types of parenting: authoritarian, authoritative, and
permissive. Using two-dimensions of parenting identified as demandingness (control) and
responsiveness (warmth), Maccoby and Martin redefined the three types of parenting into four
types: authoritarian, authoritative, indulgent, and neglectful, based on parents’ relative position
on the demandingness and responsiveness dimensions. This approach was popular for about two
decades, but gradually declined because of its fundamental flaws. With a typology approach, it
was difficult to distinguish the impact of the individual dimensions used to define the types on
children’s outcomes. In other words, although it has been widely accepted that authoritative
parenting is the most adaptive to children and neglectful parenting the least functional (Lamborn,
Mounts, Steinberg, & Dornbusch, 1991), it has not been understood how and why these
outcomes occurred (Darling & Steinberg, 1993). Additionally, research studies about four
parenting styles (authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent and neglectful) and their impact on
children’s and adolescents’ educational, social, and emotional outcomes in ethnic groups other
than European Americans have demonstrated some alternative findings. For example,
authoritarian parenting that was generally believed to be detrimental was associated with high
educational engagement among Hispanic adolescents; whereas among African-American
students, authoritative parenting was not related to academic achievement (Steinberg, Lamborn,
Dornbusch, & Darling, 1992). It is true that to understand parenting styles better, researchers
6
have come to consider different dimensions of parenting that could be examined separately in
terms of their impact on children’s and adolescents’ functioning (Darling & Steinberg, 1993).
This second approach to parenting styles divided parenting into three distinct dimensions:
parental psychological control, behavioral control, and autonomy support granting. These
constructs were first proposed by Schaefer (1965) in his analysis of children’s reports of parental
behaviors. Over the past two decades, several studies have assessed the distinction among the
factors, particularly during adolescence, and provided a clearer understanding of their role in
parenting and adolescents’ outcomes. Steinberg (1990) found distinct differences between
psychological and behavioral control, and proposed that adolescents would be negatively
impacted by psychological control, defined as the intrusion on the development of individuals’
emotional autonomy and personal self reliance, and positively influenced by the presence of
behavioral control, defined as the provision of behavioral guidance or demandingness. Barber,
Olsen, and Shagle (1994) continued to study the distinction between these two constructs and
demonstrated with empirical studies that high psychological control was related to more
internalized problems (e.g., depression), whereas moderate to high behavioral control was
associated with fewer externalized problems (e.g., delinquency) when the other construct was
controlled (Barber et al., 1994). Barber (1996) defined psychological control as parental
“attempts to intrude into the psychological and emotional development of the child (e.g.,
thinking processes, self-expression, emotions, and attachment to parents)” (p. 3296) and
developed a self-report measure to assess parental psychological control, thus establishing the
conceptual and research foundation for the construct.
The literature has been inconsistent in terms of the nature of parental autonomy support
granting. Although some early research studies suggested that autonomy granting was the
7
opposite of psychological control (e.g., Steinberg, 1990), more recent researchers have viewed it
as a distinct construct (Silk, Morris, Kanaya, & Steinberg, 2003; Wang, Pomerantz, & Chen,
2007), defined as parents’ facilitation of adolescents’ independent thinking and expression, as
well as behavioral decision-making (Silk et al., 2003). The work of Soenens and Vansteenkiste
(2010) provided some insights into this conceptual dispute. They argued that autonomy granting
was mainly defined in two distinct ways: the first viewed autonomy granting as the promotion of
independence whereas the second conceptualized the construct as promotion of volitional
functioning, and the choice of definition leads to different conclusions about the relations
between autonomy granting and psychological control. For the current study, psychological
control is viewed as distinctively different from autonomy granting (Silk et al., 2003; Wang et
al., 2007). Such speculation also has been supported by other empirical studies. For example,
Skinner, Johnson, and Snyder (2005) found that a two-factor model (psychological control and
parental autonomy support) fit much better than a one-factor model with data collected from a
large sample of adolescents. Moreover, psychological control and parental autonomy support
were only weakly correlated with each other, and demonstrated different association patterns
with adolescents’ internalized problems, problem behavior, and self-concept. In another study,
Skinner and colleagues found that the unipolar (multiple) dimensions of parenting were
significantly better than a bipolar model with self-reported data from parents and adolescents. To
be specific, autonomy support and parental psychological control (coercion in their term) were
found to be distinct constructs rather than opposite ends of one parental dimension (Skinner et
al.).
Parental behavior control has been found to be positively associated with preschoolers’
emotion regulation whereas negative control like psychological control has been related to less
8
use of adaptive emotion regulation strategies and more dysfunctional means for regulation
(Calkins, Smith, Gill, & Johnson, 1998). Children whose mothers were supportive had the
capacities of generating more coping strategies (Hardy, Power, & Jaedicke, 1993). Parental
psychological control has been found to be associated with emotion regulation problems among
early adolescents as well (Manzeske & Stright, 2009; McEwen & Flouri, 2009), whereas
maternal warmth was associated with adolescents’ emotion regulation (Walton & Flouri, 2010).
It is important to note that although parenting practices and parent-child relations are all
part of the overall family domain, they are generally considered distinct contributors to
children’s and adolescents’ developmental outcomes (Scott, Briskman, Woolgar, Humayun, &
O’Connor, 2011). Whereas parenting practices generally designate concrete parenting behavior
(e.g., what parents do to control children’s emotions and behavior), parent-child relations (e.g.,
parent-child attachment) reflect how children feel about their interpersonal relations with parents
(Karavasilis, Doyle, & Markiewicz, 2003). In fact, research has shown that parenting behaviors
influence adolescents’ affective attachment with parents, which in turn impacts their
developmental outcomes (Scott et al., 2011). For example, in recent studies, negligent and
unresponsive parenting predicted insecure attachment (Brenning, Soenens, Braet, & Bal, 2012;
Karavasilis et al., 2003), whereas parental autonomy support was associated with higher levels of
adolescents’ trust towards parents and feelings of being loved (Shpigel, Diamond, & Diamond,
2012). In addition, attachment dimensions (e.g., trust, communication, and alienation) were
found to mediate the relations between parenting practices and late adolescents’ emotional
dysfunction (e.g., aggression) (Gallarin & Alonso-Arbiol, 2012), and between parental warmth
and self-esteem along with externalizing problems among adolescents (Doyle & Markiewicz,
2005).
9
In addition to dimensions of parenting that directly impact emotional development,
negative relationships between parents (i.e., interparental conflict) can also influence children
and adolescents’ emotion regulation (Morris et al., 2007). It has been hypothesized that
interparental conflict is related to children’s emotion regulation both directly and indirectly by
undermining parenting practices and parent-child relationships (i.e., parent-child attachment)
(Erel & Burman, 1995; Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000). The first generation of research on
interparental conflict focused on exploring its direct impact on children’s and adolescents’
maladjustment (Fincham, 1994). Emotion regulation could be learned through observation of
parents as models. Parents’ emotional display and regulation provided children important
information about what is accepted and encouraged in the family. The interactions among family
members, especially between parents, implicitly conveyed messages to children about how they
should manage their emotions, especially negative emotions in their current and future life
(Morris et al., 2007). Indeed, interparental conflicts were found to be associated with children’s
emotional regulation problems. Children from families characterized by hostile interaction and
high conflicts exhibited more aggressive behaviors (Emery, 1982) and had problems in
managing their negative emotions such as anger (Clark, 2004). Research studies have shown that
overt interparental conflict and physical violence were the most relevant to children’s
dysfunction (Fincham, 1994).
The second generation of interparental conflict research shifted attention from direct
impact to examining the processes or mechanisms of interparental conflict’s impact on children’s
and adolescents’ development (Cummings & Davies, 1994, 2010; Fincham, 1994). In particular,
several hypotheses and models in the literature proposed parenting as an important mechanism.
One commonly accepted hypothesis, the spill-over effect, explicitly proposed that the negative
10
emotions and behaviors resulting from interparental conflict would spill into the parent-child
subsystem and undermine parenting practices and parent-child attachment (Erel & Burman,
1995; Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000). Parents drained by marital discord might become less
responsive and emotionally available to their children. In addition, they might become less
consistent in parenting practices (Erel & Burman, 1995). Empirical studies generally supported
the spill-over effect with moderate effect sizes recorded (ranging from .49 to .62 between
interparental conflict and negative parenting) (Erel & Burman, 1995; Krishnakumar & Buehler,
2000). For example, a national study testing the spill-over effect indicated that interparental
conflict was associated with parental use of harsh discipline as well as low involvement among
preschoolers, children, and adolescents (Buehler & Gerard, 2002). In a recent study, responsive
parenting was found to buffer the negative effects that interparental conflict had on children. In
addition, secure attachment with parents was linked with less distress experienced by children in
times of conflict (DeBoard-Lucas, Fosco, Raynor, & Grych, 2010).
Another well-known hypothesis, the emotional security hypothesis, articulated the
relations between interparental conflict and their children’s emotion regulation. The hypothesis
proposed that the primary goal for children exposed to conflicts between parents was to preserve
their emotional security by regulating their emotions via various means: expressiveness of
emotional reactivity or arousal (e.g., fear, distress, and vigilance), regulation of exposure to
interparental conflict (avoidance or involvement), and internal representations of parental
conflict (constructive or destructive) (Cummings & Davies, 1994, 2010). Originally proposed to
be the mechanism between interparental conflict and children’s and adolescents’ maladjustment,
most researchers have examined how three regulatory processes (emotional reactivity, regulation
of exposure to interparental conflict, and internal representations of parental conflict) accounted
11
for variances between interparental conflict and their children’s development in both concurrent
and longitudinal studies (see Cummings & Davies, 2010). For example, one’s emotional security
system mediated the relationship between interparental conflict and children’s learning
engagement and emotional problems at school (Grych, Wachsmuth-Schlaefer, & Klockow,
2002; Sturge-Apple, Davies, Winter, Cummings, & Schermerhorn, 2008), and between
interparental conflict and children’s difficulties sleeping and academic performance even when
attachment insecurity was considered (El-Sheikh, Buckhalt, Keller, Cummings, & Acebo, 2007).
In particular, some studies found that emotional reactivity and internal representations were
stronger mediators between interparental conflict and children’s outcomes (e.g., internalized
problems), than were parenting behaviors (Davies & Cummings, 1998).
Similar to the emotional security hypothesis, Grych and Fincham (1990) provided a
contextual-cognitive framework to illustrate how children’s cognitive appraisal and coping with
interparental conflict thus offered an explanation for the detrimental impact of conflict on
children’s adjustment. The model proposed that interparental conflict as a stressor imposed on
children would lead to two cognitive processes. When exposed to interparental conflict, children
would first encounter affective distress or arousal (primary processing), such arousal then would
lead to children’s reflection on their attributions of the conflicts as well as how to cope with them
(secondary processing). Emotional arousal in the first process would impact cognitive
interpretation and coping in the second process, which in turn would ease or magnify the
emotional distress generated. Both processes were influenced by contextual factors and the
developmental stage of children. In particular, children’s perceived emotional climate within the
parent-child domain was conceptualized as a significant contributor to the affect and cognition
12
involved in the two cognitive processes. Good parent-child relationships could buffer the
negative impact of interparental conflict (Emery, 1982; Grych & Fincham).
Both the emotional security hypothesis and the cognitive-contextual model articulated the
idea that individuals’ emotion regulation might be influenced by interparental conflict. The
objective of the current study, however, focused on examining the mechanisms between
interparental conflict and their children’s emotion regulation. To be specific, it was anticipated
that parenting (including parenting behaviors and parent-child relations) functioned as important
mediators between interparental conflict and children’s emotion regulation. However, instead of
measuring emotion regulation in the specific context of interparental conflict, general emotion
regulation capabilities were assessed.
The Current Study
The accumulation of literature on emotion regulation among children including the model
proposed by Morris and colleagues (2007) highlights the family processes through which
emotion regulation develops, in particular, how interparental conflict, parent-child relations, and
parenting practices are related to emotion regulation. The aim of the current study is to explore
the relative contribution of parenting-related factors to emotion regulation, especially in the
presence of interparental conflict. In addition, the spillover effect will be tested to examine
whether or not parenting practices (psychological control, autonomy support, and behavior
control) and parent-child relations (attachment) mediate the relations between interparental
conflict and emotion regulation. In children and adolescents, the existing research would suggest
that interparental conflict and negative aspects of parenting (psychological control) would have
negative relations with emotion regulation whereas positive aspects of parenting (autonomy
support, and moderate behavioral control) and parent-child attachment would foster emotion
13
regulation. It also might be expected that parenting and parent-child relations would mediate the
relations between interparental conflict and emotion regulation.
For the purposes of this study, emotion regulation is defined as individuals’ awareness
and conscious control over their emotions to achieve adaptive functioning. Interparental conflict
is defined as the overt conflicts between mother and father perceived by their emerging adult
kids. Parental psychological control includes the parental “attempts to intrude into the
psychological and emotional development of the child (e.g., thinking processes, self-expression,
emotions, and attachment to parents)” (Barber, 1996, p. 3296), whereas parental behavior
control refers to parental management and regulation of adolescents’ behaviors. Parental
autonomy support is viewed as parents’ facilitation of adolescents’ independent thinking and
expression, as well as decision-making (Silk et al., 2003). Parent-child attachment is defined as
the emotional bond between parents and their adolescent children reflected in three main
dimensions: trust, communication and feelings of alienation (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987).
The studies about emotion regulation mostly have focused on infants and young children,
and little is known about late adolescents and emerging adults’ emotion regulation (Galambos &
Costigan, 2003; Morris et al., 2007). The onset of adolescence usually is accompanied by mild
conflicts and decreased closeness between parents and their adolescent children (Larson &
Richards, 1991), which often lead to a more egalitarian, autonomous parent-adolescent
relationship (Steinberg, 1990). However, research on early adolescents, although scarce, has
shown that the processes through which parent-child relations influence emotion regulation are
similar to those found in younger children. In contrast, in late adolescents, now more commonly
referred to as emerging adults (Arnett, 2000), socialization influences from parents become more
voluntary as most have moved out of their parents’ house for either educational or independent
14
purposes. They are granted greater autonomy though parents still exert assistance/influence on
their emotion-regulation development in more flexible ways (Arnett, 2007). For example, parents
exert less psychological control and behavior control on their emerging adult children, while
maintaining autonomy support. In response, the attachment relationship remains secure. Healthy
development of emotion regulation depends on these transitions to occur without disruption.
Despite the potential increased physical distance between parents and their emerging adult
children, disturbances in the parental relationship and subsequent influences on parenting and the
parent-child relationship may still have an impact on emotional well-being. The current study
will explore how emotion regulation is shaped in the context of family, particularly among
emerging adults and within the context of varying degrees of perceived interparental conflict. To
the researcher’s knowledge, no study has explored the relations among interparental conflict,
parenting practices, emerging adults’ attachment relations, and emotion regulation in one frame.
In addition, almost all studies in the interparental conflict literature use emotional constructs as
mediators, thus ignoring the possibility that emerging adults’ abilities of emotional regulation
could be negatively impacted and associated with the conflict-ridden environment within the
family, and various parenting-related behaviors.
Question 1
How are interparental conflict, parenting (defined as parental psychological control,
autonomy support, and behavioral control), and parent-child relations (defined as parent-child
attachment) related to emerging adults’ emotion regulation?
Question 2
15
Do parenting behaviors (psychological control, autonomy support, and behavioral
control) and parent-child attachment mediate the relations between interparental conflict and
emotion regulation?
Hypothesis 1
Interparental conflict, parental psychological control will be negatively associated
emerging adults’ emotion regulation whereas behavior control, autonomy support, and parental
attachment will be positively related to emotion regulation.
Hypothesis 2
Parental psychological control, autonomy support, behavioral control, and parent-child
attachment will mediate the relations between interparental conflict and emerging adults’
emotional regulation.
16
CHAPTER II
Review of Literature
Emotional development has long been neglected in the study of psychological concepts.
Two factors could account for this: one is the conviction that emotions are epiphenomenal in
nature and another is the difficulty with the measurement of the construct (Saarni, Mumme, &
Campos, 1998). However, in the past 25 years, a revival has taken place and a large amount of
evidence has accumulated to support the scientific validity of emotions as a construct (Sarrni et
al., 1998; Cole et al., 2004) and advances have been made in measurement.
Emotional Development in Infancy and Toddlerhood
Like many areas of study during infancy, the study of emotions is quite challenging
because infants cannot verbally describe or express their own feelings. Emotional development
during the early years of life was recognized mainly through observing the interactions between
infants and their primary caregivers or parents, and emotions at this time can be viewed as “a
sensitive barometer of early developmental functioning in the child-parent system” (Emde &
Easterbrooks, 1985, p. 80). Research findings have provided evidence that people from various
cultures and backgrounds understand and appreciate photographs of emotional expression
similarly (Ekman, Friesen, & Ellsworth, 1972). The social context where emotional exchange
takes place during infancy is within families where parents or caregivers engage in face-to-face
interactions with their young children (Thompson, Easterbrooks, & Padilla-Walker, 2003). These
affective interactions provide infants important information about emotion-related knowledge
such as rules of communication, accustomed interactive pace and intensity, and ways of dealing
with negative emotions. Other developmental skills such as self-regulation and adjusting
mismatched interactions could be learned through such social exchanges (Gianino & Tronick,
1988; Thompson et al., 2003).
17
Starting from the first several months, infants also learn how to express their emotions
and what emotions and expressions are encouraged and viewed as appropriate. Socially and
culturally approved ways of expression are reinforced through parents’ imitating their infants’
positive feelings such as happiness and surprise, whereas some negative emotions are
discouraged by parents’ conscious unresponsiveness to such emotional expressions. Gender and
cultural differences start to show with infant boys receiving more emotion-related training than
girls (Weinberg, Tronick, Cohn, & Olson, 1999) and babies from some Asian countries such as
Japan and China being less likely to express feelings (whether it is smiling or crying) due to the
cultural expectations (Camras et al., 1998).
Infants are not just passive recipients during their interactions with parents. Instead, they
actively participate in eliciting parental response and contributing to the shaping of the
environment (Thompson et al., 2003). Infants’ emotional expressions are evident in interactions
with adult partners through the imitation of emotional expression of adults, gazing, and
approaching things they are interested in. Adults take the lead by scaffolding their initiatives to
match with infants’ readiness to respond. Infants also detect and change their feelings through
emotional contagion (i.e., becoming happy or sad by noticing others’ emotions as such)
(Thompson et al., 2003). They also could gradually form social expectations about others’
responses contingent on their own behavior. For example, by four months, infants anticipate their
social partners to respond likewise when they smile or vocalize (Rochat, Striano, & Blatt, 2002).
They respond positively to partners who are responsive yet turn away from those who do not
respond to their initiatives (Murray & Trevarthen, 1985). By the end of the sixth month, they
have learned some basic rules of social interaction and become aware about how they could
induce responses from others (Thompson et al., 2003).
18
As they grow physically, infants start to crawl and walk later in the first year. Many
parents have noted that such changes bring new challenges into the parent-child interaction,
which have an impact on young children’s socio-emotional development (Biringen, Emde,
Campos, & Appelbaum, 1995). This period is accompanied by parents’ report of an increased
parental emotional expression towards their children, including anger and frustration (Campos et
al., 2000). The self-motivated and produced locomotion make it compelling that parents monitor
the movement of their children, and increase the use of limitations or prohibitions when
necessary. Such changes provide some catalyst to infants’ early understanding of others’
emotional states, as well as the fact that others’ emotions might be distinct from their own
(Thompson et al., 2003).
Research studies have provided a lot of evidence about how infants use parental
emotional cues as ways to regulate their own emotions (Mumme, Fernald, & Herrera, 1996;
Murray et al., 2008; Walden, 1991). Social referencing as another important way of
understanding others’ emotion emerges gradually during this time as well. In events that are
novel and ambiguous, infants will turn to adults, usually their caregivers, to seek emotionally
related information (Saarni et al., 1998). For example, how infants respond to strangers largely
depends on caregiver’s emotional expression in that setting (Repacholi, 1998). Social referencing
is important for infants’ emotional development, for it allows them to incorporate others’
emotional information into their own responses and events, and facilitates their awareness and
understanding of other people’s subjective mental states (Thompson et al., 2003). It also lets
children compare their emotional responses with those of others so that they can understand that
others may have different emotional reactions from their own.
19
Human beings have primary and secondary emotions. Four basic emotions including
happiness, anger, sadness, and fear have been widely studied. Self-conscious feelings such as
guilt, shame, embarrassment, envy and pride appear in the later part of the second year with the
onset of self-awareness and social information provided by adults (Lewis, 1995; Izard,
Ackerman, & Schultz, 1999). Cultural differences exist in how certain emotional expressions are
encouraged or disapproved. For example, it is common that children are taught to be proud of
their achievement in western cultures whereas such practices are generally discouraged in some
eastern cultures such as China and Japan, based on the belief that it is embarrassing to draw
others’ attention to one’s personal success (Akimoto & Sanbonmatsu, 1999).
Infants’ capabilities of regulating their emotions gradually develop during the first years
of life, although in the first several months parents take major responsibility in managing their
infants’ emotion by intervening directly to pacify or sooth crying irritable babies, or by altering
the physical environment of home to suit their infants’ needs (Thompson et al., 2003). Emotion
regulation requires effortful control and management of emotions, and improves slowly as the
young brains mature biologically since the capabilities to inhibit impulsivity are constrained
within certain regions of the brain (Diamond, Werker, & Lalonde, 1994). As young children
grow cognitively, effective ways of regulating emotions can be taught through verbal instruction.
Young children realize that emotions can be managed through conscious efforts such as shifting
attention from emotionally arousing situations, or self-assurance talk (Calkins & Johnson, 1998;
Grolnick, Bridges, & Connell, 1996).
The ways parents respond to their infants have a significant impact on their emotional
development. Infants whose parents sensitively respond to their emotional needs tend to be easily
soothed whereas the slow intervention and neglecting parents tend to promote intense distress of
20
infants. Maternal sensitivity at 10 months significantly predicted their infants’ future social and
emotional development (Page, Wilhelm, Gamble, & Card, 2010). Maternal negative control,
however, was related to less effective emotion regulation among toddlers (Calkins et al., 1998).
Caregivers’ neglectful regulation of stressful situations for their babies could result in a failure of
development in the brain areas that manage stress, leading to permanent regulation problems for
children (Crockenberg & Leerkes, 2000). Eisenberg and colleagues (Eisenberg et al., 2001,
Eisenberg, Morris, & Spinrad, 2005) proposed that children whose parents were warm and
supportive were more likely to comply with parental demands and socialization including
appropriate self-regulation requirements; whereas, when parents displayed heightened levels of
negative emotions, especially when such emotions targeted children, children tended to believe
that they were not concerned therefore less likely to comply with their parents’ requirements.
Emotional Development during Childhood and Adolescence
Preschool and school-aged children’s emotion expression becomes more complex and
context-based. For example, in the second year of life, with the onset of self-mobility as well as
young children’s increasing demand for autonomy, negative emotions such as anger and
frustration could be observed more (Gilliom et al., 2002). At this time, positive socialization
practices used by parents such as warmth and verbal guidance help to facilitate the normative
development of emotion regulation (Kopp, 1989; Gilliom et al., 2002). Secure attachment also
works as a facilitator in developing adaptive emotion-regulation strategies among toddlers
(Gilliom et al., 2002). During interactions with peers, anger and happiness are expressed more
than other feelings and could be observed in one setting (Denham, 1986; Cole, 1985). In
addition, with the development of cognitive and language abilities, children’s understanding of
emotions and self-regulation improves so that expressions of negative feelings decline with age
21
(Cole, Mischel, & Teti, 1994). They become more adept at understanding and evaluating both
their own and others’ feelings (Cummings et al., 2003). Preschool-aged children still focus on
external rather than internal factors when appreciating others’ feelings (Levine, 1995), and are
capable of discussing the causes, outcomes, and behavioral signs of emotion (Stein & Levine,
1999). Children become more aware of emotional display rules, with parents as their major
sources of influence. Concurrent and longitudinal studies indicated that children whose parents
frequently engaged in emotion-related discussion were more advanced in understanding and
judging others’ emotions (Denham & Kochanoff, 2002; Dunn & Brown, 1993).
Language becomes an important tool for young children to regulate their own impulses,
behaviors, and emotions. Parents usually encourage their children to speak about their feelings
instead of bursting into tears or acting aggressively to express their anger or frustration (Cole et
al., 1994). Several strategies that are usually used include talking to themselves, shifting
attention, changing goals, problem-solving, support-seeking, and distancing-avoiding
(Thompson, 2000; Saarni, 2000).
With family as the primary context where emotion socialization occurs, it is reasonable to
believe that parents contribute significantly to emotion-regulation abilities of their children and
parental emotion expression becomes very important. The modeling hypothesis has argued that
by observing how parents handle their feelings, children can learn strategies about how to
regulate their own emotions. The hypothesis has been supported by numerous research findings.
For example, toddlers’ emotion regulation has been closely related to maternal emotional
expressiveness (Garner, 1995). In addition, maternal reports of expressed positive emotions
predicted toddlers’ self-regulated behavior such as self-soothing, whereas maternal reports of
negative emotions (i.e., sadness) in the family were negatively related to their young children’s
22
emotion self-regulation (Garner, 1995). Moreover, when mothers expressed heightened levels of
negative emotions such as sadness, they tended to become less responsive to young children’s
emotional expression (Biringen & Robinson, 1991) thus making children lose opportunities to
learn about how to regulate their negative emotions (Garner, 1995). Mothers who used a lot of
negative emotion-socialization had children with problems in regulating their emotions during
interactions with peers (Denham, 1989).
In another line of research, children whose mothers were depressed created a negative
emotional environment for their children as well as provided some negative models of emotion
regulation. Young children of depressed mothers were more likely to regulate their emotions
passively (wait passively instead of actively distract themselves) (Silk, Shaw, Skuban, Oland, &
Kovacs, 2006). Similarly, in middle and late childhood, children of depressed mothers had poor
emotion regulation abilities compared with non-depressed mothers’ children (Garber, Braafladt,
& Zeman, 1991). When expressing frequent heightened levels of negative emotion, parents set
models for their children to imitate such dysfunctional behavior (Cole et al., 1994).
School-age children’s interpretation of emotion shifts from external to internal states
(Flavell, Flavell, & Green, 2001). They gradually realize they could experience multiple
emotions or even conflicting ones at times. Such awareness might appear at five to six years of
age (Stein & Trabasso, 1989) or not until later childhood (Harter & Whitesell, 1989). Young
adolescents could even integrate two seemingly conflicting emotions in one setting (Saarni,
Campos, Camras, & Witherington, 2006). Such increasing awareness about one’s complicated
feelings helps to gain insights about others’ emotional states and how they feel. Children realize
that people may not express their true feelings and that in some situations masking how one feels
might be desired. In a classic study in which children received a present they disliked, children’s
23
responses indicated that, as children grow older, they become better at hiding their true feelings
for they realize how others might be influenced by their expression (Saarni, 1984). Such age and
individual differences are indicators of social competence. In another study with elementary
students, children’s use of appropriate display rules was associated with social competence
reported by teachers and peers (McDowell, O’Neil, & Parke, 2000).
School-age children’s abilities to regulate emotion develop significantly as well. They
can actively use various forms of coping alternatives to engage in emotion regulation to achieve
their goals. As they grow, they become better at adopting cognitive oriented strategies to deal
with situations that are difficult to control (Compas, Malcarne, & Fondacaro, 1988). Lazarus and
Folkman (1984; Lazarus, 1999) proposed a dichotomy structure of coping: problem-focused and
emotion-focused coping. Some researchers have argued that whether a coping strategy is
adaptive or not depends largely on the specific context (Hardy et al., 1993). When situations are
appraised as changeable, it is adaptive for children to engage in problem-focused coping;
whereas when little could be done to improve the overall situation, employing emotion-focused
or avoidant strategies could be effective as well to control and manage internal distress.
Emotional self-efficacy, a collective feeling of having control over one’s emotional experience,
also appears among school age children along with their cognitive and emotional development
(Saarni, 2000), and could further foster children’s abilities to face emotional-laden situations.
Adolescents’ understanding of basic emotions develops further (Smrtnik Vitulic, 2009).
Adolescence is a time when individuals experience dramatic physical, cognitive, and social
changes. G. Stanley Hall (1905) hypothesized that adolescence was filled with emotional turmoil
induced by hormone changes, but the hypothesis has not been supported by research findings
(Galambos & Costigan, 2003). In one study, for example, both adolescents and preadolescents
24
were asked to report their moods on an hourly basis for a week, but there was no sign of rapidly
changing moods among adolescents compared with preadolescents though there were more
reports about experiencing mildly negative feelings and fewer extreme positive ones among
adolescents (Larson & Lampman-Petraitis, 1989). Similarly, in a recent study, researchers
examined whether the onset of puberty was followed by more complex mixed feelings, no
differences were found between late childhood and adolescent groups (Stephanie, Stephanie,
Geoffrey, & Sarah-Jayne, 2011). However, emotion-regulation during adolescence does show
some interesting changes. Of the few studies comparing the normative development of emotion
regulation in children and adolescents, Gullone et al. (2010) found age differences between the
uses of two emotion-regulation strategies: cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression.
Adolescents used less expressive suppression compared with their young peers, indicating the
general trend of a healthier emotion regulation development as individuals grow (John & Gross,
2004). Unexpectedly, adolescents also reported less use of cognitive reappraisal than children.
During this time, family continues to play an important role in adolescents’ emotion
regulation development (Sheeber, Allen, Davis, & Sorensen, 2000). Adolescents whose mothers
responded negatively to their positive emotion expression used more maladaptive emotion
regulation strategies and reported more depressive symptoms (Yap et al., 2008) and adolescents
whose mothers exhibited more emotion-related coping had better negative emotion (such as
anger) regulation (Shortt, Stoolmiller, Smith-Shine, Eddy, & Sheeber, 2010). Warm, supportive
parenting was associated with active coping among adolescent children whereas harsh,
authoritarian parenting was related to heightened levels of maladaptive forms of coping
strategies (Chan, 2011; Wolfradt et al., 2003). Maternal warmth was associated with better
emotion regulation among adolescents, which negatively predicted conduct problems.
25
Contextual risks measured as adverse life events such as “parents separated” also were
significant predictors of difficulties in emotion regulation and externalizing problem behaviors
(Walton & Flouri, 2010). Parenting such as maternal warmth and mother-child attachment could
contribute to individual differences in emotion regulation (Brody & Ge, 2001; Contreras, Kerns,
Weimer, Gentzler, & Tomich, 2000; Walton & Flouri, 2010). Parental control predicted poor
emotion regulation among school age children and adolescents (McDowell et al., 2002;
Moilanen, 2007; Strayer & Roberts, 2004). Maternal use of psychological control was related to
lower levels of emotion regulation among young adults (Manzeske & Stright, 2009). In
particular, during adolescence and young adulthood, when young adults are striving for
independence and emotional autonomy, psychological control was found to be detrimental
(Manzeske & Stright, 2009).
However, research about emotional development mainly focuses on young children, and
studies about emotion regulation of adolescents and beyond are scarce (Gullone et al., 2010; Yap
et al., 2008). Some theorists have proposed indicators of adaptive emotion regulation such as
self-esteem and psychosocial maturity, arguing that adaptive emotional functioning could be
inferred from sound development of these constructs (Galambos & Costigan, 2003). Although
such statements could be taken as legitimate, it indicates some urgency to study emotion
regulation with older participants.
Emotion Regulation
Researchers and theorists have made many attempts to define the construct of emotion
regulation (ER) (see Eisenberg, Fabes, & Guthrie, 1997; Eisenberg & Morris, 2002; Eisenberg &
Spinrad, 2004; Grolnick et al., 1996; Kopp, 1989; Thompson, 1994). It is generally accepted that
emotion regulation consists of “internal and external processes involved in initiating,
26
maintaining, and modulating the occurrence, intensity, and expression of emotions” to achieve
one’s goals (Morris et al., 2007, p. 363) and it includes several characteristics. First, emotion
regulation involves efforts to maintain and/or enhance certain emotional arousal, and inhibit
and/or suppress others based on the cultural values. Secondly, emotion regulation encompasses
both self-regulated and other-regulated elements. In other words, ER is not just self-regulated
behavior that comes from the self. It also involves regulation from external sources. Parental
active monitoring and regulating young children’s emotions to promote their well being could be
viewed as a good example of extrinsic emotion regulation (Zerman, Cassano, Perry-Parrish, &
Stegall, 2006). Thirdly, emotion regulation in most cases refers to changing the intensity of
certain affect, instead of altering and/or inhibiting the discrete emotions experienced by
individuals. Lastly, emotion regulation has functional purposes and the ultimate goal of
regulation is to enhance individuals’ adaptive development and accomplish personal goals in
specific situations (Thompson, 1994). Emotion regulation could be best accomplished when
individuals have the freedom or feel no pressure to express negative, and sometimes even
positive affects, and when there exists a wide range of choices for expressing affects (Thompson,
1994).
Eisenberg and her colleagues (Eisenberg & Spinrad, 2004; Eisenberg et al., 2005) pointed
out that it was important to differentiate emotion-regulation that was extrinsic from intrinsic and
that was other-regulated from self-regulated; and emotion regulation should include both
external regulation and self-regulation. They defined emotion self-regulation as individuals’
consciously “initiating, avoiding, inhibiting, maintaining, or modulating the occurrence, form,
intensity, or duration of internal feeling states, emotion-related physiological states, attentional
processes, and motivational states of emotion” to accomplish personal goals (Eisenberg et al.,
27
2005, p. 424). Recent research has suggested that it is more useful to focus on the process of
emotion regulation than the amount of emotion experienced or regulated (Eisenberg & Sulik,
2012).
John and Gross (2004) proposed a framework for researchers to study emotion
regulation. According to the framework, there are two strategies that individuals often use during
emotion regulation: cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. Cognitive reappraisal as an
antecedent-focused strategy focuses on changing one’s way of thinking about certain emotional
stimuli. Expressive suppression is response-focused emotion regulation that targets suppressing
or changing one’s way of responding to emotionally-laden situations. Reappraisal occurs in the
early stage before the full generation of emotional response, so it takes fewer cognitive
resources. Suppression comes later when negative emotions have been induced, therefore
requiring more psychological and cognitive resources. In addition, suppression often introduces a
sense of discrepancy between internal feelings and external expression, which might result in
more negative feelings and cause further problems in social interactions.
Research studies have demonstrated that the consequences of reappraisal and suppression
are distinctly different (Hofmann, Heering, Sawyer, & Asnaani, 2009; Memedovic, Grisham,
Denson, Moulds, 2010; Ortner & Koning, 2013). Reappraisal decreased negative feelings and
behavioral expression whereas suppression did not alter the amount of negative emotions felt by
subjects, though behavior expression was reduced. In addition, greater physiological activation
was observed among individuals who used suppression (Gross, 1998). Suppression also resulted
in memory impairment during social interaction because of its use of enhanced effortful control
(Richards & Gross, 1999, 2000). Reappraisal and suppression also were different in social
consequences. Because suppression took up a lot of cognitive resources, individuals who adopted
28
the strategy usually were unable to absorb information from social partners, thus failed to
respond properly in social situations, leading to communication problems and disruption in
social interactions (John & Gross, 2004). Therefore, in general, individuals who used reappraisal
to regulate their emotions experienced more positive and less negative feelings, tended to
function better in social settings, and had better well-beings than those who adopted suppression
(Gross & John, 2003).
The characteristics of emotion regulation, particularly those proposed by Eisenberg and
colleagues, have been well captured in the research on emotional intelligence. A group of
theorists and researchers represented by Salovey and Mayer (1990) viewed emotions as one of
the three fundamental mental subsystems (the other two are motivation and cognition) and
differentiated them from moods, as emotions were shorter and more intense. Salovey and Mayer
proposed the theoretical framework of emotional intelligence as a subset of social intelligence,
composed of three aspects: to appraise and express self and others’ emotions through verbal and
non-verbal ways; to regulate emotions in self and others, and to utilize emotions in adaptive
ways to achieve personal goals (e.g., flexible planning, creative thinking). Understanding what
emotions the self is experiencing is the first step to express one’s own emotions effectively to
others, and understanding what emotions others are feeling ensures appropriate affective
responses to other people. Then individuals have the abilities to regulate their own emotions
through various means. One can create or enter into situations that might bring positive affect,
and avoid those associated with negative affect. One also can regulate emotions by choosing
whom to associate with. Another way to alter moods is through cognitive reevaluation of
negative affect. If situations that introduce negative affect are evaluated as under control and
changeable, individuals could be more persevering in time of distress, and more ready to enter
29
into new situations that might bring positive affect. Emotion regulation also includes changing
and modifying emotions of others to meet certain goals. A third aspect of emotional intelligence
is individuals’ abilities to use emotions to solve problems, including flexible planning, creative
thinking, mood redirected attention, and motivation (Salovey & Mayer).
In their later work, Mayer and Salovey (1997) revised and updated the definition of
emotional intelligence. They kept the first two aspects of emotional intelligence and took out the
utilization of emotions. Instead, another two aspects were introduced into the framework:
accessing and generating feelings that facilitate thinking, and employing emotional knowledge.
The four areas of emotional intelligence were then proposed to range from basic to higher or
more complicated psychological process. The first level; perception, appraisal, and expression of
emotion; concerned individuals’ accurately identifying emotions in self and others and making
distinctions between various emotional states. It involved abilities to identify specific emotions
in self, other people, artwork and designs, through different means (e.g., language, sound,
appearance, behavior etc.). It also involved abilities to express emotions accurately, and express
needs that might relate to specific emotions. This aspect of emotional intelligence also includes
abilities to make distinctions between honest and dishonest feeling expressions. The second
level, emotion’s facilitation of thinking, dealt with emotion’s function on intellectual processing
and everyday tasks. It directs one’s attention to important things. For example, a student stressed
about a coming examination might start to study for it. Similarly, a husband overwhelmed by
tense marital relations might seek external counseling to provide constructive suggestions to
improve it. Emotion’s facilitation of thinking also demonstrated as generation of emotions
needed to better understand how others feel and assisting individuals to make personal decisions,
and/or considering multiple options. In addition, certain emotions might facilitate thinking
30
directly (e.g., happiness enhances reasoning and creativity; negative feelings such as anger
sometimes motivate people to excel in certain fields). A third level of emotional intelligence
concerned understanding and analyzing emotions and using emotional knowledge. This involved
abilities to understand the complex relations among different emotions, and to understand that a
person could simultaneously have two or more seemingly opposite feelings (e.g., feelings of hate
and love towards the same object/person), and to be able to recognize transitions between
emotions. The final level, which is viewed as the most advanced in emotional intelligence,
relates to individuals’ reflective regulation of emotions to promote personal growth. Early stages
of the level involve openness to internal feelings, either pleasant or unpleasant, and abilities to
engage in or detach from certain feelings based on utility. As individuals become mature, they
are able to mentally monitor emotions as to how influential and reasonable they are, and to
consciously regulate emotions such as moderating or altering negative feelings and maintaining
and enhancing positive ones, or sometimes to detach from certain feelings (Mayer & Salovey).
To sum up, emotional intelligence could be defined as a set of capabilities “to accurately
perceive emotions, to access and generate emotions so as to assist thought, to understand
emotions and emotional knowledge, and to reflectively regulate emotions so as to promote
emotional and intellectual growth” (Mayer & Salovey, p.5).
Measurement of EI has followed two main approaches: standardized maximum
performance tests and self-reported scales. Among all the available measures, the MayerSalovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2002), the
Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i) (Bar-On, 1997), and the self-report EI test (SREIT)
(Schutte et al., 1998) are probably the most-widely used (Brackett & Mayer, 2003). Whereas the
MSCEIT followed a rigid standardized procedure of testing individuals’ intelligence, the SREIT
31
has drawn more and more attention and has tended to be used as a measure of emotional
intelligence by numerous researchers. It was developed by Schutte et al. (1998) based on the
model proposed by Mayer and Salovey (1990). A total of 62 self-report items were created and
33 items were retained after a factor analysis. The 33 items loaded onto one factor that was
consistent with Mayer and Salovey’s conceptualization of EI. Petrides and Furnham (2000),
however, found that the scale could be divided into four factors (optimism and mood regulation,
appraisal of emotions, social skills, and utilization of emotion) and recommended that
researchers conduct their own exploratory factor analysis of the SREIT (see Appendix A for
SREIT items).
Researchers have generally agreed that there exist two types of emotional intelligence:
trait EI and ability EI, based on the measurement method used (Petrides & Furnham, 2003). Trait
EI (or trait emotional self-efficacy) assesses self-perceived and reported emotion-related
capabilities and is measured through self-report measures. Ability EI, on the other hand, tackles
emotional informational processing abilities that are measured through standardized tests
(Mavroveli, Petrides, Rieffe, & Bakker, 2007). Trait EI was strongly correlated with traditional
personality measures and therefore was viewed as a dimension of personality. Ability EI, in
contrast, differentiated from personality and well-being constructs, and shared moderate
association with general intelligence (Brackett & Mayer, 2003; Alberto, 2011). It is noteworthy
that trait EI and ability EI were only weakly correlated with each other (Brackett & Mayer, 2003;
O’Connor & Little, 2003).
Since its introduction, numerous studies have examined the relations of emotional
intelligence to various other developmental factors. Emotional intelligence has been found to be
associated with peer relations among children (Petrides, Sangareau, Furnham, & Frederickson,
32
2006), depression and anxiety (Russo et al., 2012), quality of social interactions with friends
among college students (Lopes et al., 2004), coping styles (Mavroveli et al., 2007), and socioemotional competence (Frederickson, Petrides, & Simmonds, 2012).
Family Influences on Emotion Regulation
Morris and colleagues (2007) proposed a theoretical model to explain the impact of
family on children’s emotion regulation. Based on the model, there are mainly three ways that
family influences children’s emotion regulation. First, emotion regulation could be learned
through observation, including parental modeling, social referencing, and emotion contagion.
Second, emotion regulation could be influenced by parenting behaviors associated with emotion
socialization such as parental emotion coaching and parental reactions to emotions. Third, the
overall emotional climate of the family could affect children’s emotion regulation with the
emotional climate including such factors as parent-child attachment, parenting style, and marital
relations, all of which may project parents’ own expression of emotions. Morris et al. suggested
that when examining the family factors that influence children’s emotion regulation, it is
important that researchers explore particularly the impact of interparental conflict, parenting
behaviors, and parent-child relations.
Parent-child relations: attachment. Parents play important roles in socializing and
fostering children’s emotion regulation. When children are young, parents take major
responsibility in regulating children’s emotions by soothing them or changing the physical
environment to suit their young children’s needs (Kopp, 1989). Responsive caregivers
appropriately read infants’ signals, directly intervene to minimize the distress, or pacify irritating
children and elicit positive feelings (Calkins & Hill, 2007). Moreover, a secure relationship with
33
caregivers provides children strong emotional backup to explore new environments and regulate
their own emotions.
The attachment theory proposed by Bowlby (1982) hypothesized that caregiving
practices impact young children’s emotion regulation through the attachment relationship
between caregivers and children. Through the daily experience of interacting with caregivers,
children develop an expectation or “internal working model” that serves as a guide about what to
expect about caregivers responses and behaviors and whether they can be relied on as secure
sources of comfort and assistance. The relationship between young children and caregivers is
very important for their emotional development, because secure attachment provides young
children information that caregivers give the support necessary to reduce their fear and anxiety in
unfamiliar or challenging settings. Consequently, children become confident enough to explore
the new environment. In addition, young children become more competent and more ready to
seek assistance when caregivers sensitively respond to their needs (Thompson et al., 2003).
Attachment could be viewed as whether parents are emotionally available and responsive enough
to young children’s various needs. Securely attached children understand that their needs will be
responded to whereas insecure children expect that parents might neglect or only selectively
attend to their signals (Cassidy, 1994). In one study, secure attachment with parents at age three
and a half years together with warm supportive parenting was associated with more use of
effective emotion regulatory strategies (Gilliom et al., 2002). Insecurely attached children
showed more positive affect when they should not and suppressed their emotions more (through
face expressions) (Malatesta et al., 1989).
Relational influences on emotional development become more salient during middle
childhood with family as the most important source of such influences (Cummings, Davies, &
34
Campbell, 2000). A functional perspective places particular emphasis on relationships in social
contexts as they relate to emotion development (Cummings et al., 2003). For example, parentchild relationships become a foundation for emotional functioning in social settings. Parents are
highly influential for children’s regulation of affect. As with infants, a less secure relationship
with parents in middle childhood is associated with more fluctuating and unpredictable feelings,
posing threats to emotion regulation (Cummings et al., 2003). Parent-child attachment continues
to provide the foundation for emotional functioning during adolescence (Hill, 1983; Hill &
Holmbeck, 1986; Morris et al., 2007). Adolescents’ perceptions of their relationships with their
parents are characterized by acceptance, trust, and communication (Armsden & Greenberg,
1987; Gomez & McLaren, 2007) and are related to the development of emotional autonomy and
self-regulation (Gallarin, & Alonso-Arbiol, 2012). In other research, a secure attachment with
parents has been associated with adolescents’ emotion regulation and less aggressiveness
(Gomez & McLaren, 2007; Morris et al., 2007).
Parenting behaviors: parental control and autonomy support. Specific parenting
behaviors have received much attention for their relations with emotional development. One area
of parenting believed to impact children’s emotions is parental control, namely psychological
control and behavior control (Barber & Harmon, 2002). Concepts of parental control were first
introduced in the pioneering work of Schaefer (1965) in an analysis of children’s reports of
parental behaviors. Three dimensions of parenting were found in the analysis: acceptance versus
rejection, psychological autonomy versus psychological control, and firm control versus lax
control. The psychological autonomy versus psychological control dimension was defined
through subscales of intrusiveness and parental control through guilt. In particular, this
dimension was given the name for its suggested parental use of covert psychological methods to
35
manipulate children and inhibit them from developing their individual selves. In contrast, the
dimension of firm vs. lax control represented aspects of physical and behavioral control of
children’s behaviors. The concepts of parental control (both psychological and behavioral) could
also be found in Becker’s work (1964) in which he pointed out that there existed two general
ways of parental discipline: love-oriented and power-assertive. Love-oriented techniques
included positive methods (e.g., praise and reasoning) and negative methods (e.g., withdrawal of
love, showing disappointment); whereas, power-assertive techniques included physical
punishment and forceful commands. Using the parallel terminology, psychological control could
be viewed as parental use of love-oriented techniques to discipline children whereas behavior
control was the use of power-assertive techniques.
The constructs of psychological control and behavioral control were then neglected for
over two decades because another line of parenting research became the focus. Proposed by
Baumrind (1971) and revised by Maccoby & Martin (1983), the typological approach divided
parenting into four distinctive types according to two factors, responsiveness and
demandingness. Originally, Baumrind proposed three types of parenting: authoritative,
authoritarian, and permissive parenting. Authoritative parenting encouraged verbal give and take,
and respected children as autonomous individuals. In addition, clear expectations and behavioral
rules were expressed with reason and firmness. The basic emotional tone between parents and
their children was love and responsiveness. Authoritarian parents exerted overt control over their
children. With various methods such as physical punishment, yelling, shouting, parents required
complete obedience from their children without any reasoning. At the same time, authoritarian
parents provided little emotional and psychological support to their children. Permissive
parenting was characterized by parental high emotional involvement with children, but without
36
providing clear behavior rules and regulations that were adaptive for children’s development.
Based on Baumrind’s typological model, Maccoby and Martin made transformations and
developed a two-dimensional framework and added another type of parenting: neglectful
parenting. According to Maccoby and Martin, parenting could be distinguished into four
different styles according to two dimensions: responsiveness, the presence of love, warmth, and
child-centered practices, and demandingness, the presence of behavior expectations and
consequences. Parenting that was high in both responsiveness and demandingness could be
viewed as authoritative whereas high demandingness coupled with low responsiveness
characterized authoritarian parenting. Permissive/indulgent parenting was high in responsiveness
but low in demandingness whereas neglectful parenting was low in both dimensions. By typing
parenting using two dimensions, parental styles were transformed from qualitative differences to
quantitative distinctions (Darling & Steinberg, 1993).
Despite the popularity and strength of Baumrind’s typological parenting approach, the
model had some inherent problems. One problem was that although ample evidence was
accumulated that authoritative parenting was the most adaptive to children’s and adolescents’
emotional development, it was not clear how and why (Darling & Steinberg, 1993). The fact that
a typological approach included a constellation of parenting qualities made it really difficult to
tell which aspect of parenting produced positive outcomes (Darling & Steinberg, 1993). By the
early 1990s, researchers had started to look for another way to measure parenting, particularly
among adolescents, whose emotional outcomes seemed to be particularly susceptible to
distinctions between behavioral and psychological parenting.
The concepts of psychological and behavioral control were revived in the 1990s when
Steinberg made distinctions between psychological and behavioral control and Barber’s (1994,
37
1996) contributions of both conceptual and empirical studies related these two constructs.
Steinberg (1990) posited that adolescents would be negatively impacted by psychological
control, the opposite of psychological autonomy, yet positively affected by behavioral control,
the presence of demandingness. He argued that adolescents need supervision and guidance,
without which they may be exposed to danger and temptation, yet must be granted psychological
autonomy, the absence of which would lead to psychological dependency and hinder their
psychological and emotional development. Barber (1996) redefined psychological control as
parental “attempts to intrude into the psychological and emotional development of the child (e.g.,
thinking processes, self-expression, emotions, and attachment to parents)” (p. 3296). In
particular, his research demonstrated that psychological control was mostly associated with
internalized problems whereas behavioral control was related to externalizing behaviors (Barber
et al., 1994, Barber, 1996).
Barber and Harmon (2002) well summarized the features characterizing psychological
control. According to their description, parents who are psychologically controlling use methods
that are psychological to manipulate and control their children’s behavior. Psychological control
is intrusive as it steps into the “child’s person” (Crockenberg & Litman, 1990), and denies their
“self-discovery opportunities” (Barber & Harmon; Constanzo & Woody, 1985). According to
this research, psychological control is manipulative in that parents use tactics such as guilt
induction, withdrawal of love, and introduction of anxiety to influence and mold children’s
behavior to be in line with the parents’ wishes. Psychological control is possessive in that parents
treat children as their own property, and infantilize children, encouraging children to depend on
parents’ to make decisions and using parental agreement as criteria of choice-making.
Psychological control is dominating in that it puts children in a subordinate role so that they will
38
conform to parental wishes (Barber & Harmon; Shoben, 1949). Barber and Harmon also found
that psychological control is enmeshing, or “blurring individual psychological boundaries in
favor of a family identity” (p. 24), in that there are no clear boundaries between parents and
children psychologically, and dependency is greatly encouraged. Finally, psychological control
involves constraining children’s verbal expression. It deals with parents’ denial or
discouragement of the child’s talking about “anxieties, conflicts, hostilities, and disagreements
with parental policies” (Schaefer & Bell, 1958; p. 347).
Characterized by non-responsive socialization of children’s psychological and emotional
needs, psychologically controlling parenting restrains children’s self-expression as well as
autonomy (Barber & Harmon, 2002), and psychological control has been associated with
individuals’ feelings of guilty and non-expressive aggression (Becker, 1964), and baffled selfregulation (Baumrind, 1991). The research findings about psychological control have been very
consistent in showing that it is a significant predictor of youth problem behavior and low selfesteem (Litovsky & Dusek, 1985), internalized behaviors such as depression and isolation
(Barber et al., 1994; Barber, 1996; Bean, Barber, & Crane, 2006), difficulties in making
committed choices (Luyckx, Soenens, Vansteenkiste, Goossens, & Berzonsky, 2007), and
antisocial behavior such as delinquency (Barber et al., 1994; Barber, 1996). Many researchers
have pointed out that psychological control is a pattern of ineffective parenting that undermines
children’s competency and self-value, and parents’ responsiveness to children’s needs
(Cummings & Davies, 2010; Doyle & Markiewicz, 2005; Pettit & Laird, 2002). In particular,
psychological control was related to less use of adaptive emotion regulation strategies and more
dysfunctional means for regulation (Calkins et al., 1998). In contrast, children with more
supportive mothers were able to generate more coping strategies (Hardy et al., 1993).
39
Parental behavior control, on the other hand, refers to parents’ regulation and guidance of
children’s behavior and rule setting in the physical world (Barber, 1996; Barber & Harmon,
2002; Pomerantz & Wang, 2009). It provides structure and guidance necessary to children’s
behavior and higher levels of behavioral control are associated with adaptive outcomes among
adolescents (Bean, Barber, & Crane, 2006). Although termed traditionally as a form of parental
control, behavioral control is not intrusive or dominant in nature and has actually been associated
with positive psychological outcomes among children and adolescents (Bean et al., 2006;
Pomerantz & Wang, 2009). Adolescents need moderate regulation and guidance to ensure their
appropriate growth and development and lower levels of behavior control might contribute to
maladaptive outcomes such as externalized problems (Barber, 1996). Moreover, behavior control
has been found to be positively associated with children’s and adolescents’ emotion regulation
(Calkins et al., 1998).
The distinctions between psychological control and behavioral control could be made as
such. Psychological control is intrusive with the aim to promote dependency, and manipulate the
child’s behavior and psychological world as parents wish. It impedes individualization and
identity formation, the outcomes of which are incompetent individuals with detrimental selfworth and confidence who are more likely to turn inward and withdraw when confronted with
setbacks in social interactions. Behavioral control, on the other hand, deals with parental
provision of sufficient regulation to ensure that the child learns rules and structures guiding the
functional interpersonal interactions in social settings (Barber et al.,1994). Simply put,
psychological control hurts children as it impedes the development of children’s self whereas
behavioral control is beneficial for it provides guidance necessary for development without
intruding into children’s psychological world (Wang et al., 2007). Research findings have
40
supported the distinctions between the two types of parental control. Barber and colleagues
(1994) demonstrated that psychological control and behavioral control were independent family
constructs as evidenced by their contrasting effects on youth outcomes.
Another aspect of parenting of particular interest during adolescence is autonomy
support. Historically, parental autonomy support has been viewed as the opposite of
psychological control. Schaefer (1965) labeled the two constructs as representing the opposite
ends of a parenting continuum. Similarly, Steinberg (1990) in his widely referenced conceptual
work viewed psychological control as the absence of autonomy support. However, both of these
assertions were based purely on conceptual guessing. Recent researchers, however, have
questioned such conceptualization and proposed that the absence of psychological control is not
equal to the presence of autonomy support. Although psychological control refers to parental
intrusion into children’s emotional and psychological self, the essence of autonomy granting is to
enhance individual expression and self-determination (Wang et al., 2007). Parents who do not
use psychological methods of control may also be low on autonomy granting. On the other hand,
parents encouraging independence and choice-making may also adopt psychological control to
manipulate their children (Silk et al., 2003).
The notion that psychological control and autonomy support are discrete dimensions of
parenting has been supported with empirical studies. For example, Silk and colleagues (2003)
explored the relations between psychological control and autonomy support with a large sample
of middle to late adolescents. With confirmatory factor analysis, the results revealed that parental
psychological control and autonomy support were distinct constructs instead of the opposite ends
on one dimension of parenting. In addition, these two constructs were found to be only weakly
associated and related differently to adolescents’ internalized problems (Silk et al.). Likewise, in
41
another study with parental report data, autonomy support and psychological control were
reported as two unipolar dimensions rather than one bipolar dimension. In other words, they
were found to be distinct from each other (Skinner et al., 2005). Further evidence also has been
found in a cross-cultural longitudinal study, in which psychological control predicted less
adaptive emotional functioning, whereas autonomy support predicted improved emotional and
academic functioning (Wang et al., 2007). Parental autonomy support is more than the absence
of psychological control and refers to parenting that is demographic, allowing free selfexpression and making decisions, encouraging exploring for identity, and articulating of ideas or
preferences. As such, optimal parenting could be defined as the combination of behavioral
control/regulation, autonomy support, and the absence of psychological control (Skinner et al.,
2005).
In summary, empirical research findings have suggested that parental psychological
control and behavioral control are related to children’s and adolescents’ emotion regulation.
Maternal control, especially higher psychological control, has been found to be associated with
lower levels of emotion regulation among college students (Manzeske & Stright, 2009). In
another study examining father’s parenting and adolescents emotional regulation, paternal
psychological control, but not behavioral control was related to difficulties in emotion regulation
among a group of early- to late-adolescents (McEwen & Flouri, 2009). Maternal warmth also
was related to adolescents’ emotion regulation, which in turn was predictive of lower levels of
conduct problems (Walton & Flouri, 2010).
Family expressivity is another way of socializing emotions to children. Parental
expression of positive emotions has been related to positive socio-emotional development among
children whereas expression of negative emotions have been associated with dysfunctional
42
development outcomes, though there have been inconsistent findings (Morris et al., 2007).
Parents who experience and express a lot of positive emotions are more likely to be warm and,
responsive to their children whereas parents with too much negative emotion tend to be less
responsive, or even harsh or hostile towards their children (Morris et al., 2007). According to
Gottman, Fainsilber-Katz, & Hooven (1996), parents have different philosophies or ideas
towards their own as well as children’s expression of emotion. Parents who are emotioncoaching encourage children to express and discuss emotions with them for they view it as
excellent opportunities to help their young children and promote parent-child intimacy. They
would encourage their children to speak out the specific emotions, and then teach them how to
use adaptive emotion regulation strategies to deal with negative emotions (e.g., problem solving).
Emotion-dismissing parents, however, view negative emotions as inappropriate and harmful, and
try to get rid of expression of negative emotion for children by ignoring or dismissing it
(Gottman et al.). They avoid openly discussing negative feelings, and do not instruct their child
how to deal with it.
Distinguishing parent-child relations and parenting behaviors. An important yet
emerging research direction is to distinguish parent-child attachment and parenting practices and
to examine their separate roles in children’s adjustment (Scott et al., 2011). Conceptually
speaking, parenting practices generally reflect concrete parenting behavior (e.g., what parents do
to control children’s emotional and psychological self, or to regulate their behavior) whereas
parenting parent-child relations (e.g., parent-child attachment) describe how children feel about
their interpersonal relations with parents (Karavasilis et al., 2003). Individuals’ development of
attachment style was based on their interparental experiences with parents (Bowlby, 1982; 1988;
Brenning et al., 2012). When children enter into adolescence, different forms of parenting
43
behaviors might impact their felt security in the parent-child realm, which in turn influence their
developmental outcomes (Scott et al., 2011). In one study, parental warmth, autonomy granting,
and behavioral control were predictive of a better and securer parent-child attachment whereas
negligent parenting predicted insecure attachment (Karavasilis et al., 2003). In other research,
maternal provision of autonomy granting also was associated with adolescents’ reports of closer
parent-child relations including higher levels of trust and feelings of being loved (Shpigel et al.,
2012). Further, parental unresponsiveness and low autonomy support were associated with
adolescents’ insecure attachment styles (Brenning et al., 2012). Parent-child attachment has been
linked with behavioral maladjustment (e.g., antisocial behavior) over and above parenting
behaviors such as behavioral control (Scott et al., 2011), and the dimensions of attachment (e.g.,
trust, communication, and alienation) functioned as full mediators between the relations of
parenting practices and late adolescents’ aggression (Gallarin & Alonso-Arbiol, 2012).
Attachment also has been found to mediate the relations between parental warmth and selfesteem as well as externalized problems among adolescents (Doyle & Markiewicz, 2005).
Therefore, it is important for researchers to examine various dimensions of parental control (e.g.,
psychological control and behavioral control) and to explore their relations with parent-child
attachment (Karavasilis et al., 2003).
Interparental Conflict
Interparental conflict as a multidimensional construct is composed of various dimensions
of properties including frequency, intensity and resolution characteristics that are important to
the children’s outcomes (Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000; Grych, Seid, & Fincham, 1992).
Marital conflict was a better predictor of maladjustment than martial status (Amato, 1986) and
conflicts that were hostile and aggressive endangered children in great developmental problems
(Grych & Fincham, 1990). Interparental conflict could be both overt and covert in style, and
44
overt conflict has been demonstrated as the most detrimental to children adaptive functioning
(Grych & Fincham, 1990) and has been the focus of most studies in the field (Krishnakumar &
Buehler, 2000).
Grych and Fincham (1990) proposed that to study interparental conflicts several
dimensions needed to be considered, such as frequency, intensity, and resolution, which were
most related to children’s outcomes when interparental conflicts became a stressor. Frequency
refers to how frequent parents disagree with each other regarding various family issues (Buehler,
Krishnakumar, Anthony, Tittsworth, & Stone, 1994). It designates children’s increased exposure
to interparental conflict (e.g. I often see my parents arguing) (Grych & Fincham; Grych et al.,
1992) and frequent conflicts between parents would make children become more sensitized to
future conflicts (Cummings & Davies, 2010). However, frequency when examining alone served
only as a weak predictor of negative parenting (Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000). Interparental
conflict also varies by the degree of its intensity, from quiet disagreement to violent physical
aggression. Children reacted more negatively to conflicts with physical aggression than to other
levels of conflicts in laboratory studies (Laumakis, Margolin, & John, 1998; O’Brien, Margolin,
John, & Krueger, 1991). Resolution referred to how parents resolved their disagreements or
conflicts and their post-conflict behaviors and expressions (Cummings & Davies, 2010; Grych &
Fincham) and was concerned with whether parents could handle their conflicts effectively.
Constructive resolution greatly reduced children’s distress and could even be beneficial for their
later development as children perceived conflicts as unavoidable yet manageable, whereas failure
of resolution often led to maladaptive outcomes among children (Cummings & Davies, 2010).
The relationship between interparental conflict and child maladjustment has been well
documented (Cummings & Davies, 2010) with decades of research showing that interparental
45
conflict is associated with externalizing and internalizing problems, dysfunctional social skills
and relationships, and lower academic performance (see Cummings & Davies, 2010 for details).
Research in this field at first targeted the direct impact of interparental conflict on various
outcomes of children, but the focus has gradually shifted to examine the underlying mechanisms
and processes (Fincham, 1994). In particular, both theory and empirical studies have focused on
how marital conflicts might relate to or undermine children’s outcomes through the parent-child
relationship (Cox, Paley, & Harter, 2001). Notably, several main hypotheses in the area
discussed or directly dealt with such mechanisms. For example, the cognitive and contextual
model proposed that the family emotional environment, defined as the parent-child relationship,
would impact how children develop and feel in marital conflict contexts. Warm, supportive
parent-child interactions would lessen the detrimental effects of a conflicting background
whereas cold, unresponsive relations between parent and child would exacerbate the problems
(Grych & Fincham, 1990).
A commonly accepted hypothesis, the spill-over effect, directly theorized that the
negative emotions and behaviors sparked in conflicts between spouses would spill into the
parent-child relationship (Erel & Burman, 1995; Katz & Gottman, 1996). Drained by high
emotional arousal and energy consumption resulting from marital conflicts, parents were less
likely to spend enough time with their children, and notice their developing children’s various
needs. In addition, the negative emotions generated in the conflicts would easily be brought into
parent-child interactions, leading to harsh or inconsistent parenting. Some parents might involve
their children in the conflicts, for example, making them take sides. In two of most cited metaanalytic review studies, Erel and Burman (1995) recorded an effect size of .49 between overt
interparental conflict and ineffective parenting, whereas Krishnakumar and Buehler (2000)
46
documented an overall effect size of .62 of the association. In other words, a moderate positive
relation between interparental conflict and ineffective parenting existed. In particular, the
association was stronger in intact families than in divorced ones, stronger for EuropeanAmerican families than for mixed- or other-ethnic samples, stronger for parenting dimensions
such as harsh punishment and acceptance, and stronger during middle childhood and
adolescence. In addition, child-perceptions of interparental conflict provided the most reliable
evidence compared with other- or multiple-reporters (Krishnakumar & Buehler).
The spill-over hypothesis has been widely supported by empirical studies. In a study
testing the spill-over hypothesis in a group of European- and African-American families (pre-,
early-, and late-adolescents), parental monitoring, maternal acceptance, and parent-youth conflict
were found to mediate the relations between interparental conflicts and externalized problems,
and maternal psychological control and parent-child conflict mediated the relations between
interparental conflicts and internalizing problems in European-Americans (Krishnakumar,
Buehler, & Barber, 2003). However, the spill-over effect was not found in African-American
families. The findings were consistent with the Krishnakumar and Buehler (2000) meta-analysis
in that the spill-over effect was generally supported in European-American families, but not in
other ethnic groups. In another study based on national surveys on families, the relations between
marital conflict, ineffective parenting (defined as harsh discipline, low parental involvement),
and children’s and adolescents’ maladjustment were examined. Spill-over effects were found
among preschoolers, middle childhood children, and adolescents. In other words, marital conflict
was associated with negative parenting, which in turn led to children’s and adolescents’
maladjustment (Buehler & Gerard, 2002). Hostility between spouses was associated with
paternal harsh, negative parenting and maternal and paternal rejection towards children (Katz &
47
Gottman, 1996; Shelton & Harold, 2008). Disrupted marital quality was associated with more
use of intrusive control among fathers (Belsky, Youngblade, Rovine, & Volling, 1991). In
another longitudinal study with adolescence, researchers found that parenting fully mediated the
relations between marital conflict at time one and internalized and externalized problems, and
self-esteem at time two (Doyle & Markiewicz, 2005). Parenting warmth/rejection also
functioned as a mediator between interparental conflict and children’s depression in a diverse,
urban, low-income population (O’Donnell, Moreau, Cardemil, & Pollastri, 2010). In addition,
parenting and attachment also influenced children’s appraisal of interparental conflicts. Negative
parenting was associated with higher-levels of distress such as self-blame whereas supportive,
responsive parenting could buffer the negative effects of that interparental conflict had on
children. In addition, secure attachment with father was reported to be linked with less distress
experienced by children in time of conflicts (DeBoard-Lucas et al., 2010).
Another less noticed effect, the “compensatory” hypothesis, offered a seemingly different
perspective. Based on the hypothesis, some parents in the face of high marital conflict might
consciously offset children’s vulnerability to the distress by becoming highly involved in the
parent-child relationship (Erel & Burman, 1995; Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000). Although such
expressions might be viewed as parental warmth at first sight, researchers reminded us that this
actually functioned as a compensatory process where parents who felt rejected by the spouse,
dissatisfied with a lack of affectionate and supportive relationship in the marriage, shifted their
attention to their responsibilities as parents. More specifically, such parental warmth might
actually represent an enmeshed, intrusive parenting pattern (parental psychological control) that
characterizes many high-conflict families and become a potential risk that mediates the
relationship between marital conflict and children’s development (Cummings & Davies, 2010).
48
In addition, compensatory mechanisms might introduce triangulation and cross-generational
coalitions into the family, thus causing further problems between parents and children (Erel &
Burman, 1995). Erel and Burman (1995) pointed out that “it is questionable whether a close
relationship fueled by a parent’s negative relationship with his or her spouse is truly positive” (p.
110).
Despite the different parental responses to children, the “compensatory” (over-involved,
intrusive parenting) and “spillover” hypotheses (negligent or unresponsive parenting) both
documented forms of ineffective parenting. However, research studies generally supported the
spillover effect over the compensatory hypothesis (Erel & Burman, 1995; Krishnakumar &
Buehler, 2000). The study conducted by Krishnakumar and Buehler (2000) did not support the
compensatory hypothesis because they concluded that the compensatory behaviors should be
positively related to effective parenting. In contrast, Cummings and Davies (2010) concluded
that parents who try to compensate for the high-conflicts are in fact likely to engage in
ineffective parenting and become psychological controlling parents who attempt to intrude into
their children’s subjective world. In other words, both the spillover effect and compensatory
hypothesis should direct towards the same direction that interparental conflicts lead to ineffective
parenting. In one of the few studies examining the compensatory hypothesis, Belsky et al (1991)
found that maternal supportive behavior towards their young children during play was associated
with marital problems with spouses, and intrusive paternal behavior towards children. However,
the researchers cautioned a full illustration of the compensation hypothesis evidenced by the
findings. They proposed the possibility that fathers’ intrusive behavior might be caused by
mothers’ involvement with children. Moreover, such maternal supportive parenting might
introduce more problem into father-child relationship (e.g., alliance with mothers) therefore
49
endanger instead of buffer children’s well-being (Belsky et al). In particular, chronic
interparental conflict was associated with insensitive parenting, which in turn predicted an
insecure parent-child attachment, even for young infants (Owen & Cox, 1997).
Parent-child attachment may serve as an important mechanism between interparental
conflict and individuals’ emotion regulation (Owen & Cox, 1997). Strong interparental conflict
might be perceived as frightening by children thus making them less likely to approach parents
for comfort and solutions (Owen & Cox, 1997). The consequence of a high conflict presents
children “a paradoxical problem-namely, an attachment figure who is at once the source of and
the solution to its alarm” (Main & Hesse, 1990, p. 163). Maternal reports of the quality of
marriage (e.g., high satisfaction, good communication, and low conflict) predicted children’s
security of attachment (Howes & Markman, 1989). In a longitudinal study, parenting behaviors
(e.g., parental warmth) and attachment were found to be mediators between marital conflict and
various maladjustment outcomes among adolescents (Doyle & Markiewicz, 2005). Marital
conflict was a predictor of parent-child relations among adolescents and children (Amato, 1986).
According to the results from Erel and Burman’s (1995) meta-analytic review, good parent-child
relations were difficult to achieve with poor marital relations between father and mother. Indeed,
parents low in conflict and high satisfaction with their marriage were more likely to have
children with secure attachment styles (Grych, 2002).
Cummings and Davies (1994) proposed the emotional security hypothesis (EST), which
offered another important process mediating the link between marital conflict and child
adjustment. The theory was based on a functionalist perspective of emotion regulation in which
emotions guide individuals’ appraisal of events, and provide motivation for behaviors (Davies &
Cummings, 1998). It posited that in the context of parental conflicts, a primary and salient goal
50
for children is to preserve their emotional security, which motivates and explains children’s
various behaviors and responses towards the conflict. Davies and Cummings (1998) suggested
that the emotion security theory could be viewed as an extension of attachment theory.
Attachment theory emphasizes achieving emotional security as a prominent goal for children in
their relationships with parents in the family context. The emotion security theory expanded the
ways emotion security might be enhanced or undermined, proposing that children’s emotional
security could be impacted by parent-child relationships, specifically attachment, as well as the
family emotional environment manifested in interparental relationships. In the theory, the
interparental relationship does not just serve as a context that influences parent-child attachment.
Rather, the security from interparental relationship is distinct from what is obtained in parentchild context. In other words, a child might feel insecure emotionally about the interparental
relations but secure in their relations with parents or vice versa (Davies, Forman, Rasi, &
Stevens, 2002).
Emotion security is conceptualized to be a multi-dimensional latent construct that is
composed of three distinct components or processes: emotional reactivity, regulation of exposure
to parental conflict, and internal representations. These three processes serve as mechanisms to
assist in the attainment of a felt security (Davies et al., 2002). First, emotional reactivity
demonstrated as heightened fear, distress, and vigilance among children reflects emotional
insecurity. Although such arousal might energize children’s physiological and psychological
resources to cope with distress, high levels of negative emotionality might become precursors of
later internalized and externalized problems (Davies & Cummings, 1998; Saarni et al., 1998).
Second, guided by the goal of preserving emotional security, children become motivated to
regulate their exposure to parental negative affect and conflicts. They might choose to avoid the
51
parental disputes or fights, or become overregulated by actively intervening into parental
conflicts, making efforts to cease the disputes or conflicts. Over-involvement into interparental
conflicts might reflect children’s emotion insecurity in that children, based on their past
experience, conclude that such conflicts would not be constructively resolved. A third
component or process of emotional security involves children’s internal representations of
parental conflicts, as to what conflicts might mean for themselves and their families. Children
from high-conflict homes are at greater risk of developing insecure internal representations. Such
representations might lead to fear of escalation of fight or violence, a concern for self-security,
family future (divorce or not), and parental negative affect spilling into the parent-child
relationship (Davies & Cummings, 1998).
The emotional security hypothesis could be viewed as a framework regarding how
emotion regulation might be influenced by interparental conflict. Based on its original
conceptualization, the three aspects of emotion regulation engaged by children were mostly
examined as the mechanisms between the relations of interparental conflict and children’s and
adolescents’ maladjustment. For example, marital conflict has been found to have a negative
impact on children’s functioning in school, including emotional problems and learning
engagement in classroom, through children’s emotional security system (Grych et al., 2002;
Sturge-Apple et al., 2008). In another study, marital conflict was associated with children’s
difficulties sleeping and academic performance through children’s insecurity about parental
relations, but not through the attachment insecurity (El-Sheikh et al., 2007). Lower levels of
constructive representations and avoidance among adolescents mediated the relations between
marital hostility at time one and internalized problems at time two (Buehler, Lange, & Franck,
2007). Emotional security also mediated the link between interparental conflict and security
52
about parenting, which in turn, predicted children’s psychological problems (Harold, Shelton,
Goeke-Morey, & Cummings, 2004). In a study with multiple methods and contexts that aimed to
test emotional security hypothesis as mediators between marital discord and children’s
internalized and externalized problem, it was found that emotional reactivity mediated the
relations between marital discord and internalized and externalized problems. Hostile internal
representations also mediated relations between marital discord and internalized problems.
Children’s regulation of exposure, on the other hand, was found to be unrelated to both marital
discord and children’s dysfunction. The two aspects of emotion security accounted for a little
less than 50% of the total variance (Davies & Cummings, 1998).
Another important explanatory model accounting for the relations between marital
conflict and children’s and adolescents’ adjustment was the cognitive-contextual framework
proposed by Grych and Fincham (1990). The model conceptualized interparental conflict as a
source of stress and highlighted the role of children’s cognitive appraisal that might influence
their overall functioning. To deal with the stressor, children made various attempts to first of all
understand the meaning of conflict and then cope with it. Children’s coping behavior was
proposed to be influenced by characteristics of interparental conflict (intensity, content, duration,
and resolution) and context factors. Both cognition and affect were involved in the process and
provided guidance to children’s coping behavior.
Two types of cognitive processing were involved: primary processing referred to
children’s emotional reactions when exposed to interparental conflict and lead to secondary
processing when children engaged in cognitive thinking about the reasons of the conflicts as well
as what they should do in response. Two processes interacted with each other: secondary
processing was influenced by the emotional arousal level that occurred in primary processing,
53
and modified the affect through interpretation and efficacy of coping generated from this stage.
Both processes were influenced by distal and proximal factors. Distal factors referred to stable
factors including previous experience with conflict, children’s perceptions of family emotional
climate, and temperament, whereas proximal context included children’s expectations of the
course of conflict and their current mood (Grych & Fincham, 1990).
Based on the cognitive-contextual model, Grych and his colleagues developed a measure
of assessing interparental conflicts from the children’s perspective. The Children’s Perception of
Interparental Conflict Scale (CPICS) (see Grych et al., 1992 for details) measured the conflict
properties of interparental conflict, as well as providing assessment of interpretation of the
conflict (perceived threat, self-blame attributions) as well as their coping behavior and appraisal
(triangulation and coping efficacy). The cognitive-contexual model has been widely tested and
supported by the accumulative efforts of researchers (DeBoard-Lucas et al., 2010; Grych,
Harold, & Miles, 2003; Grych, Raynor, & Fosco, 2004). In particular, most studies have focused
on examining how the cognitive appraisal (attributions such as self-blame, perceived threat) and
coping strategies provided another important research direction for the field. Interparental
conflict was found to be associated with heightened level of children’s psychological distress,
through their use of maladaptive but not adaptive coping behaviors one year later (Shelton &
Harold, 2007). Adolescents, who perceived interparental conflicts as threating and made selfblame attributions, indicated different coping behaviors one year later. Those with high
perceived threats tended to display avoidance whereas those who blamed themselves engaged
into over-involvement of the conflict, and both of these two coping strategies led to heightened
level of internalized and externalized problems (Shelton & Harold, 2008). Children’s (6th
graders) self-blame and perceived threat also mediated the relations between their perceptions of
54
interparental conflict and both externalized and internalized problems (Gerard, Buehler, Franck,
& Anderson, 2005). In another longitudinal study with early adolescents from U.K., children’s
appraisals of self-blame fully mediated interparental conflict and academic achievement
(indicated by standardized test scores), even when negative parenting behavior was considered
(Harold, Aitken, & Shelton, 2007).
Although proposed by different groups of researchers with seemingly distinct terms, the
emotional security hypothesis (EST) and the cognitive-contextual model essentially dealt with
similar processes experienced by children exposed to interparental conflicts. The immediate or
first stage of reaction to interparental conflicts has been identified as emotional reaction in both
models, which lead to the following or second stage of processes including higher levels of
cognitive appraisal as well as generated coping strategies (in EST, this has been referred to as
internal representations of conflict and regulation of exposure). In particular, both models could
be viewed as dealing with children’s and adolescents’ emotion regulation in the specific context
of interparental conflict. The current study, however, focuses on general emotion regulation as an
outcome, and explores how parenting practices might impact the relations between interparental
conflict and emotion regulation.
Accumulated findings from research studies have evidenced that emotion regulation has
been influenced by parental factors inside families, including parenting practices, parental-child
relations, and emotional climate of the family, especially interparental conflict. Most
interparental literature as well as emotion regulation studies focus on children and adolescents as
targeted population, thus ignore older participants such as late adolescents/emerging adults. The
resultant consequence is that we know rather little about how emotion regulation has been
55
impacted in the family context (interparental conflict, parent-child relations and practices, etc.)
among this age group.
Emerging Adulthood
Arnett (2000, 2004) proposed a theory of emerging adulthood to refer to young people
between the ages of 18 to 25. According to Arnett, emerging adults are distinctly different from
adolescents and young adults. As a special developmental period characterized by identity
explorations, instability, and various possibilities, emerging adulthood is entitled with more
autonomy and freedom than adolescence, and the lack of constraints imposed by commitments
and responsibilities common in the adult years (Arnett, 2007; Tanner & Arnett, 2009). Emerging
adulthood is distinct “demographically, subjectively and in terms of identity explorations”
(Arnett, 2000, p. 469). There are huge variations in demographic features for emerging adults,
and most of them have left their parents’ homes either for school attendance or independent
living. Young people of this age also have various opinions of when they reach adulthood with
age serving as only a rough estimation. The top three criteria for youth’s self-perceptions of
becoming adults were accepting responsibilities for selves, making independent decisions, and
establishing equal relationships with parents (Arnett, 1997). A lot of identity explorations take
place in this period, much more than that of adolescence, targeting three main outcomes: love,
work, and worldviews (Arnett, 2000), and it is not unusual that such explorations will lead to
disappointment and emotional fluctuation.
Given that emerging adulthood might provide a lot of possible setbacks and struggles, it
is reasonable to believe that emotion regulation might be especially important during this time.
Emerging adulthood is characterized by numerous cognitive, emotional, and behavioral
characteristics (Tanner & Arnett, 2009). Emerging adults are more sensitive than adults when
56
dealing with socio-emotional stimuli, especially negative ones. Research studies have shown that
emotional stability improved with age, with the period of emerging adulthood still linking with
high sensitivity to negative stimuli (Williams et al., 2006). Emerging adulthood also is a time
when personality is most likely to change (Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006).
Emerging adults engage in more risky behaviors than do adolescents. Research studies
have shown that the highest rate of several risky behaviors (e.g., unprotected sex, substance use,
risky driving) occurred not during adolescent years, but in emerging adulthood (Arnett, 1992;
Bachman, Johnston, O’Malley, & Schulenberg, 1996). Two factors might account for such
phenomena. First, a great deal of identity exploration might lead to a greater onset of risky
behaviors; second, emerging adults are more autonomous than adolescents, and less constrained
by responsibilities, making risky behaviors be more likely to happen (Arnett, 2000). Whatever
the reasons, self-regulated emotion abilities might become more prominent in buffering risky
behaviors for this age period.
Mental health is another problem that endangers emerging adults. The prevalence of
psychiatric disorder peaks among people of this age period (Tanner et al., 2007). Mental health
and psychopathology increased tremendously in emerging adulthood (Schulenberg & Zarrett,
2006). Undoubtedly, this is a time of “high sensitivity” as well as one with great potential for
individuals to change and develop (Tanner & Arnett, 2009). In addition, transformations in
family relations have not been completed by the end of adolescence and continue to develop
during emerging adulthood, despite a greater physical distance between parents and their
emerging adult children (Arnett, 2007). Parents continue to exert their influences on emerging
adults’ socialization outcomes. In terms of emotional self-regulation, parents continue to provide
emotional and psychological support to their children, though in most cases, such assistance is
57
based on greater autonomy support, with the aim to increase emerging adults capabilities of
regulating their own emotions and behaviors (Arnett, 2007). Emerging adults’ abilities of selfregulation improve as they move out of their parents’ homes since they are allowed more
autonomy to regulate their own behaviors, with continued support and guidance from parents
(Arnett, 2007). In addition, the nature of the attachment relationship continues to be an important
indicator of young adults’ behavioral and emotional health.
Given the characteristics of emerging adulthood and the importance of self-emotional
regulation for this period, the current study targeted this population. Some researchers have
pointed out that attending college provides some crucial context for the development of young
emerging adults (Tanner, 2006), and research should further explore these developmental
processes. The role of various dimensions of parenting (psychological and behavior control and
autonomy support) as well as the attachment relationship, in the maintenance of emerging adults’
emotion-regulation abilities needs more attention. Therefore, the purpose of the current study is
to examine how college students’ self emotion-regulation ability may be influenced by the
family context, including interparental conflict and parenting behaviors and parent-child
attachment.
58
CHAPTER III
Method
Participants
Participants were 361 undergraduate students recruited from the Educational Psychology
research pool at Ball State University, the procedure for which is described in the procedures.
There were 292 female and 69 male participants with an average age of 20.23 (SD = 1.39) years.
The racial makeup was as follows: Caucasian (n = 322, 89.2%), African American (n = 24,
6.6%), Asian American (n = 4, 1.1%), Hispanic (n = 5, 1.4%), and Other (n = 6, 1.7%). Ninetyone participants were freshmen (25.2%), 95 were sophomores (26.3%), 106 were juniors
(29.4%), and 69 were seniors (19.1%). Of all the participants, 237 reported living at their
primary residence with both parents (65.7%), 100 lived with one parent (27.7%), 14 lived with
another relative (e.g., grandparents, aunt; 3.9%), and 10 of them did not report their living status
(2.8%). Participants also reported their frequency of visiting with parents. The majority of them
met with parents at least once a month (n = 163, 45.2%) or more than once a semester (n = 105,
29.1%), 51 of them met with parents at least once a week (14.1%), 30 of them every day (8.3%),
and 12 of them less than once a semester (3.3%). Participants came from a wide range of majors,
including elementary education, secondary education, nursing, health sciences, criminal justice,
sociology, and speech pathology (see Table 1).
59
Table 1
Demographic Information of Participants
Frequency
Percentage (%)
Gender
Male
69
19.1
292
80.9
24
6.6
4
1.1
322
89.2
Hispanic
5
1.4
Other
6
1.7
Freshman
91
25.2
Sophomore
95
26.3
Junior
106
29.4
Senior
69
19.1
Both Parents
237
65.7
One Parent
100
27.7
Relative
14
3.9
Missing
10
2.8
Less than once a semester
12
3.3
More than once a semester
105
29.1
Once a month
163
45.2
Once a week
51
14.1
Every day
30
8.3
Female
Race
African American
Asian American
Caucasian
Year in School
Living Status
Frequency Visit Parents
N = 361
60
Measures
Demographics. The survey included items that asked participants to self-report their
gender (male, female), age (in years), racial identification (Caucasian, African American, Asian
American, Hispanic, Other), and year in school (freshman, sophomore, junior, senior).
Participants also were asked whether they lived at their primary residence with both parents, one
parent, or a relative/other; and how often they visited their parents (less than once a semester,
more than once a semester, once a month, once a week, or every day).
Emotion regulation. The Self-Report Emotional Intelligence Test (SREIT) (Schutte et
al., 1998) was used to measure emotion regulation. The scale has thirty-three items that measure
the conceptual model of emotional intelligence proposed by Salovey and Mayer (1990) (see
Appendix A). Although there are three categories in Salovey and Mayer (1990)’s model, namely
appraisal and expression of emotion, regulation of emotion, and utilization of emotions, Schutte
et al. (1998) extracted four factors out of the initial 62 items, but retained only the first factor
because of its strong eigenvalue (10.79) and conceptual parsimony (Ng et al., 2010). The SREIT
scale was developed using participants from an urban area in the southeastern part of United
States including college students and people from local communities. About two-thirds of the
participants were females and one-third were males with an average age of 29.27 years (SD =
10.23). No racial information was provided.
Validity was established through positive correlations between EI and attention to
feelings and emotions, clarity of feelings, mood repair, optimism and negative correlations with
pessimism, depression, impulsivity and alexithymia. In addition, between-group differences were
found in which psychotherapists scored higher than prisoners and substance abusers, and female
participants scored higher than males. As an indicator of predictive validity, SREIT was found to
61
be predictive of college students’ first-year GPA. Divergent validity was demonstrated by
finding no correlations between SAT scores as indicators of cognitive abilities and SREIT scores
(Schutte et al., 1998). In addition, SREIT was significantly related to only one of the big five
personality dimensions, openness to experience, but not to neuroticism, extraversion,
agreeableness, and conscientiousness. The internal consistency of SREIT was good, with a
Cronbach’s alpha of above .90 (Schutte et al., 1998; Brackett & Mayer, 2003). A cross-check of
internal consistency indicated a value of .87 for Cronbach’s alpha and a test-retest reliability of
.78 (Schutte et al., 1998).
Although Schutte et al. (1998) proposed a one-factor model for the 33 items, later
empirical studies failed to support a general EI factor. For example, Petrides and Furnham (2000)
tested the structure model via confirmatory and exploratory factor analyses and found that the
scale was not unifactorial, but had a four-factor structure identified as optimism/mood regulation,
appraisal of emotions, social skills, and utilization of emotions. They suggested future
researchers pursue their own factor structure analysis because of the instability of the model.
Other researchers tested the factor structure of the scale via confirmatory factor analysis. Gignac,
Pamler, Manocha and Stough (2005) conducted a qualitative analysis of all 33 items, and
decided that five of the items were not loaded on any factor. They then conducted a confirmatory
factor analysis on a proposed model but failed to obtain a good model fit. The proposed model
was modified, and items measuring the emotion expression and emotion regulation of others
factors were deleted, and another confirmatory factor analysis was conducted with the remaining
21 items, which had a reasonable model fit. Ng and colleagues (2010) conducted a series of
confirmatory factor analyses based on previous studies, and found that the model proposed by
Gignac et al. (2005) fit their data (see Figure 1), but was not parsimonious; and they proposed a
62
new model that fit the data well (see Figure 2). For the current study, four models were tested
with confirmatory factor analysis: the one factor model by Schutte et al. (1998); the four factor
model proposed by Petrides and Furnham (2000) based on their results from exploratory factor
analysis; Gignac et al.’s final model (2005) (see Figure 1), and the model proposed by Ng et al.
(2010). The Ng et al. (2010) model fit the data best (see Table 2). According to the model, eight
items loaded onto the emotion regulation factor (see Figure 1). Students responded to the scale
items using a 5-point Likert-type scale from 1 “not at all true” to 5 “very true.” Scores were then
averaged across these items with higher scores indicative of greater emotion regulation. The
reliability of the emotion regulation subscale indicated by Cronbach’s α was .77.
Table 2
Fit Indices for Four Confirmatory Factor Analysis Models of SREIT Scale
Models
χ2
df
χ2/df
NNFI
CFI
RMSEA
AIC
Schutte et al. (1998)
1581.38
495
3.20
0.68
0.72
0.08
1779.38
Petrides & Furnham (2000)
1376.91
490
2.81
0.73
0.77
0.07
1584.91
Gignac et al. (2005)
672.55
304
2.21
0.84
0.88
0.06
932.55
Ng et al. (2010)
362.89
181
2.01
0.89
0.92
0.05
560.82
Note. df = degrees of freedom; NNFI = non-normed fit index; CFI = comparative fit index;
RMSEA = root of mean square error of approximation; AIC = Akaike information criterion.
63
Figure 1. Confirmatory Factor Analysis of SREIT Model One EI 2
EI 3
EI 10
EI 12
ER
EI 14
EI 23
EI 28r
EI 31
EI 11
EI 1
EI 13
EI
EI 30
EI 24
EI 16
EI 4
EI 15
EI 32
EI 5
AEO
EI 29
EI 25
EI 18
EI 33
EI 9
AES
EI 22
EI 17
EI 27
UEPS
EI 7
EI 20
Acquiescence
Note. Gignac et al. 2005 Model. EI: a first-order general EI factor, ER: emotion regulation of self, AEO:
appraisal of emotions of others, AES: Appraisal of emotions of the self, UEPS: using emotions for
problem solving, Acquiescence: a first-order acquiescence factor. Figure adapted from Gignac et al. 2005.
64
Figure 2. Confirmatory Factor Analysis of SREIT Model Two
ER
Reverse
EI
AEO
AES
UEPS
EI 2
EI 3
EI 10
EI 12
EI 14
EI 23
EI 28r
EI 31
EI 5r
EI 15
EI 18
EI 25
EI 29
EI 32
EI 33r
EI 9
EI 22
EI 7
EI 17
EI 27
EI 20
Note. Ng et al. 2010 two-level factor model. ER: emotion regulation of self, AEO: appraisal of emotions
of others, AES: Appraisal of emotions of the self, UEPS: using emotions for problem solving, Reverse: a
unique factor for reverse coding. Figure adapted from Ng et al. 2010.
65
Interparental conflict (IPC). The Conflict Properties subscale of the Children’s
Perceptions of Interparental Conflict scale (CPIC) (Grych et al., 1992) was used to measure
interparental conflict. The Conflict Properties scale has 19 items measuring the frequency,
intensity, and resolution of interparental conflict (see Appendix B). Students responded to the
scale questions using a 3-point Likert-type scale with 1 = “true,” 2 = “sort of true,” and 3 =
“false.” Scores were averaged across items with higher scores indicative of greater perceived
conflict, that is, higher frequency of conflict, higher intensity of conflict, and lower level of
resolution.
The CPIC was originally developed among two samples of predominantly Caucasian
early adolescents (fourth- and fifth-grade children with mean age of 131 months) with roughly
equal numbers of males and females (124 boys and 98 girls in sample 1, 52 boys and 62 girls in
sample 2) with the second sampling aiming to replicate initial findings. Reliability of the
Conflict Properties scale was reported by internal consistency (.90 for sample 1 and.89 for
sample 2) and a two-week test-retest reliability (.70 for the Conflict Properties subscale).
Validity was assessed through correlating the CPIC subscale scores with established measures of
marital conflict (OPS by Porter & O’Leary, 1980; and the Conflict Tactics Scale by Straus,
1979). The CPIC subscale, especially the Conflict Properties subscale was significantly
correlated with OPS (r = .30), and the Conflict Tactics Scale (r = .39). The validity of CPIC also
was examined by assessing whether children’s perceptions of interparental conflict were
associated with their internalized and externalized problems, and results showed that the Conflict
Properties subscale was significantly related to child report internalizing problems for boys (r =
.49) and teacher/peer report of internalizing behavior for both boys (r = .18), and girls (r = -.23),
child report aggression for boys (r = .30) and girls (r = .26), and teacher/peer report of
66
externalized problems for boys (r = .20) and girls (r = .27). Several studies have been conducted
using the CPIC, and all showed a satisfying reliability of around .85 (e.g., DeBoard-Lucas et al.,
2010; O’Donnell et al., 2010). Although the original sample used was different from the target
population in our study, research has demonstrated that the CPIC could be used among late
adolescents (college students) with good internal consistency and 2-week internal test-retest
reliability (both were. 95 for the Conflict Properties subscale) (Bickham & Fiese, 1997). For the
current dataset, the reliability as indicated by Cronbach’s α was .87 for Frequency, .87 for
Intensity, and .90 for Resolution of interparental conflict.
Parental psychological control. Psychological control was measured with the
Psychological Control Scale-Youth Self-Report (PCS-YSR) developed by Barber (1996; see
Appendix C). Eleven items were created to measure various aspects of psychological control
including constraining verbal expression, invalidating feelings, and personal attack, and used
with five items from the revised Children’s Report of Parent Behavior (CRPBI; Schaefer, 1965;
Schludermann & Schludermann, 1988) that measured guilt induction and love withdrawal. A
total of 933 families with adolescents from fifth and eighth grades recruited through a stratified
random sampling responded to the survey. There were an equal number of males and females,
and 71% of participants were White (16% Hispanic), 84% from middle income families, and
46% were Mormon. Factor analysis indicated that eight items loaded onto one single factor and
should be retained based on the standard that items must have a coefficient of at least .50 for
primary loading and at least .20 discrepancy between primary and secondary loading to be
retained. Reported alphas ranged from .72 to .86 (different subsamples, e.g., Hispanic females, or
eighth-grade males). These eight items measured various aspects of parental psychological
control including invalidating feelings, constraining verbal expressions, personal attack, and love
67
withdrawal. Parental psychological control also was found to be significantly related to
depression and delinquency behaviors among adolescents (Barber, 1996). Participants respond to
these items on a 5-point Likert-type scale from “not at all true” to “very true.” Scores are
averaged across items with higher scores indicative of greater psychological control. For the
current dataset, the Cronbach’s α was .84, indicating good reliability.
Parental autonomy support. Parental autonomy support was measured using an 8-item
psychological autonomy granting scale (see Appendix D). These items were selected by Silk and
colleagues (2003) from a parenting questionnaire developed by Steinberg, Lamborn, Dornbusch,
and Darling (1992) and used as part of a large study with 9,564 adolescents from grades 9 to 12.
Nineteen items that were conceptually related to autonomy granting and psychological control
were selected and four graduate students rated the relevance of items based on the definition of
the constructs with an internal rater reliability of .88 (kappa). A confirmatory factor analysis
indicated that eight items loaded onto one latent factor -- parental autonomy granting. The eight
items in the scale measured parental encouragement of adolescents’ independent decisionmaking and thinking (e.g., “My parents emphasize that every member of the family should have
some say in family decisions”). Reported internal consistency reliability was .69. Validity of the
scale was shown through examining the relations between autonomy granting and various
outcomes. Consistent with the literature, parental autonomy granting measured with the scale
was positively associated with adolescents’ self-concept, and negatively related to internalized
problems and problem behaviors. Participants responded to the items on a 5-point Likert-type
scale from “not at all true” to “very true.” Scales scores are computed by averaging responses
across items, with higher scores indicative of greater autonomy granting. The reliability as
indicated by the Cronbach’s α was .69 for the current dataset.
68
Parental behavioral control. Parents’ behavior control was measured with a sixteenitem parent monitoring scale used by Wang et al. (2007; see Appendix E) based on the measures
developed by Stattin and Kerr (2000; Kerr & Stattin, 2000). Participants responded to these
questions on a 5-point Likert-type scale from “never” to “very often.” Scales scores were
computed by averaging responses across items, with higher scores indicative of greater behavior
control. Reported internal consistency reliability ranged from .85 to .92 (different waves). The
original Stattin and Kerr scale (2000) was developed with 763 eighth-graders in Sweden with
seventy-six percent of them from intact families. A pilot study was conducted among 36 eighthgraders from a different cohort in which participants responded to the study at two occasions that
were two months apart. The test-retest reliability was .83 and the internal consistency reliability
for the whole sample was .86 (Stattin & Kerr, 2000). Behavior control measured with this scale
was found to link with increased academic functioning among adolescents (Wang et al., 2007).
For the current dataset, Cronbach’s α was .90.
Parent-child attachment. The Inventory for Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA,
Armsden & Greenberg, 1987) was used for assessing parent-child attachment (see Appendix F).
The IPPA was originally developed with an overwhelmingly Caucasian college sample (75%) to
assess their attachment with parents and peers with about two-thirds of them were females and
one-third males. Ten items assess emerging adults’ trust towards parents (e.g., “My parents trust
my judgment”), ten items measure communication with parents (e.g., “I tell my parents about my
problems and troubles”), and eight items assess alienation from parents (e.g., “My parents don’t
understand what I am going through these days). Cronbach’s alpha was reported as internal
consistency reliability for the three subscales, with a .91 value for Trust, .91 for Communication,
and .86 for Alienation. The validity of IPPA was assessed by examining its association to
69
psychological well-being, family climate, and support-seeking from others. Attachment with
parents was significantly correlated with family climate (more cohesion, expressiveness,
organization, less conflict and control), and moderately associated with seeking parents when
needed. Better attachment with parents also was correlated with higher self-esteem and life
satisfaction, and lower levels of depression/anxiety, resentment/alienation (Armsden &
Greenberg, 1987).
Participants responded to the items on a 5-point Likert-type scale from “almost never or
never true” to “almost always or always true.” Scales scores were computed by averaging
responses across items, with higher scores indicative of higher trust, more communication, and
higher level of alienation, respectively. For the current dataset, the reliabilities for the three
subscales as indicated by the Cronbach’s α were .91 for Trust, .90 for Communication, and .89
for Alienation.
Procedure
Participants were recruited through the Educational Psychology research pool. Students
in many of the classes that participated in the research pool were required to complete
professional development activities of which research is one option. Instructors of other classes
in the department also offered research participation to students as a way to provide students an
understanding of the research process. Some instructors offered extra credit for participation in
the study. The researcher first sent an email to instructors requesting that they forward the link of
the study to their students and requesting an opportunity to visit each individual classroom. Some
instructors responded back and invited the researcher to visit their classrooms. Student
participants obtained a “receipt” at the end of the online survey that they sent to their instructor
to earn the respective credit.
70
The survey was posted online. Students volunteered their participation, and before they
moved on to the actual survey questions, an explanation of the nature of the study was provided
and students were informed that, in order to proceed to the survey, they needed to indicate that
they were willing to be a participant. The complete on-line survey took about 45 minutes,
although some students chose to finish it in couple of time periods. Over the course of a onemonth period, email reminders were sent to students through instructors every Monday. Onemonth later, data were downloaded into an SPSS data file for analysis.
Data Analysis
Preliminary analysis. Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations among all
predictors and the outcome (frequency, intensity, resolution of IPC, psychological control,
autonomy support, behavioral control, trust, communication, alienation, emotion regulation)
were calculated to provide some preliminary analysis for the data. Less than 5% of data were
missing and demonstrated a completely random pattern, therefore, the listwise method was used
to remove missing data in subsequent analyses.
Regression analysis. A simultaneous multiple regression analysis was used to answer the
first research question: How are interparental conflict (frequency, intensity, and resolution),
parenting (parental psychological control, autonomy support, and behavioral control), and
parent-child relations (parent-child attachment: trust, communication, alienation) related to
emerging adults’ emotion regulation? All predictors were entered in one layer simultaneously to
predict emotion regulation (see Figure 3).
Regression is a statistical technique that allows users to access the relationship between
dependent and independent variables (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). The method of least squares
71
is used in regression to find the “line of best fit” between the model and the observed data. The
selected line fits best because it has the lowest sum of squared differences (Field, 2005).
Several assumptions were checked to ensure the appropriate use of regression. First, the
variance of residuals (difference between the model predicted and actual values of the dependent
variable) should remain the same across all predicted values of the dependent variable
(homoscedasticity), and all residuals in the model should be normally distributed with a mean of
zero (normally distributed errors). Finally, all of the residuals should be independent of each
other. In addition, the analysis that is utilized should model a linear relationship between the
dependent variable and residuals.
The possibility of multicollinearity was checked. Multicollinearity refers to strong
correlations between the predictors, high levels of which can lead to biased estimates of
regression coefficients and standard errors, and inflated Type II error. Multicollinearity could be
detected through very low tolerance (1-SMC, SMC: squared multiple correlation among
independent variables) and high variance inflation factor (VIF) predictors. A tolerance lower
than .1, and/or a VIF value over 10 were used as indicators of multicollinearity (Field, 2005).
Path analysis. A path analysis was conducted (see Figure 4) to answer the second
research question: Do parenting (parental psychological control, behavioral control, and
autonomy support), and parent-child relations (parent-child attachment: trust, communication,
alienation) mediate the relations between interparental conflict and emotion regulation? Path
analysis is widely used for analyzing causal relations among observed variables. It could be
viewed as an extension of multiple regression for it can estimate the casual relations between
various variables and test the assumed theory with empirical data (Kline, 2005). Maximum
likelihood was used to estimate the parameter coefficients.
72
Figure 3. Proposed Model for Research Question One
Frequency of
Conflict
Intensity of Conflict
Resolution of
Conflict
Psychological
Control
Autonomy Support
Behavioral Control
Trust
Communication
Alienation
Emotion Regulation
73
Figure 4. Proposed Model for Research Question Two
Psychological Control
Frequency of
Conflict
Autonomy Support
Behavioral Control
Intensity of
Conflict
Emotion Regulation
Trust
Resolution of
Conflict
Communication
Alienation
.
74
CHAPTER IV
Results
Preliminary Analysis
Means, standard deviations (see Table 3), as well as intercorrelations (see Table 4)
among all observed variables (frequency, intensity, resolution of IPC; parental psychological
control, behavioral control, and autonomy support; trust, communication, and alienation of
parent-child attachment; and emotion regulation) were calculated to provide some preliminary
analysis for the data. The results showed positive parental relations among participants as they
reported moderate interparental conflicts, relatively low psychological control and behavioral
control, moderate autonomy support, high trust and communication, and low alienation (see
Table 3).
As expected, emotion regulation was significantly correlated with intensity and resolution
of interparental conflict, parental psychological control, autonomy support, behavioral control,
and parent-child attachment. To be more specific, lower levels of resolution of conflict, parental
psychological control, and alienation from parents were negatively correlated with emotion
regulation, whereas parental autonomy support, behavioral control, trust, and communication
were positively correlated with emotion regulation. In addition, frequency of interparental
conflict was positively correlated with parental psychological control and alienation, and
negatively correlated with trust and communication. Intensity and resolution of interparental
conflict were both positively correlated with psychological control and alienation, and negatively
correlated with autonomy support, behavioral control, trust, and communication. Psychological
control was negatively correlated with autonomy support, trust, and communication, and
positively correlated with behavior control and alienation.
75
Table 3
Mean, Standard Deviations, and Reliability of Variables
Mean (SD)
Possible Range
α
IPC Frequency
1.88 (.62)
1.00 - 3.00
0.87
IPC Intensity
1.78 (.58)
1.00 - 3.00
0.87
IPC Resolution
1.74 (.63)
1.00 - 3.00
0.90
Psychological Control
2.20 (.88)
1.00 - 5.00
0.84
Autonomy Support
3.48 (.70)
1.00 - 5.00
0.69
Behavioral Control
2.98 (.78)
1.00 - 5.00
0.90
Att Trust
4.19 (.81)
1.00 - 5.00
0.91
Att Communication
3.78 (.91)
1.00 - 5.00
0.90
Att Alienation
2.32 (.93)
1.00 - 5.00
0.89
Emotion Regulation
4.11 (.55)
1.00 - 5.00
0.77
Note. Frequency, intensity and resolution are three subscales of interparental conflict (IPC);
trust, communication, and alienation are three subscales of parent-child attachment (Att).
76
Table 4 Inter-­‐Correlations Among Variables 1.00
.32**
.72**
1.00
−.14*
.26**
1.00
−.20**
1.00
.19**
1.00
1.00
7
.77**
−.14*
.11*
1.00
6
1. Frequency
.77**
−.14** −.23**
.22**
.83**
5
2. Intensity
−.07
4. Psychological Control .30**
3. Resolution
−.07
.61**
.31**
4
5. Autonomy Support
−.35** −.38** −.38** −.59**
.57**
3
6. Behavioral Control
−.34** −.37** −.37** −.46**
−.33** −.11*
2
7. Trust
.42**
1
8. Communication
.42**
.25**
.26**
8
1.00
.36**
9
10
−.29** 1.00
−.70** −.70** 1.00
.40**
−.21** −.14**
.71**
9. Alienation
−.09
.22**
10. Emotion Regulation −.09
Note. * p < .05; **p < .01 77
Research Question One
Before running the regression analysis, several assumptions were checked. The
assumption of homoscedasticity was examined through the residuals scatterplot (Figure 5),
which indicated that the variance of the residuals was the same across all independent variables.
In addition, the residuals were normally distributed as indicated by the histogram, and P-P Plot
of residuals (Figure 6 and 7), and there were no linear relationships between the residuals and the
dependent variable, as indicated by a series of scatterplots (Figure 8 to 16).
In addition, although strong correlations were observed between some independent
variables (IPC frequency with IPC intensity: r = .77; IPC frequency with IPC resolution: r = .77;
IPC intensity with IPC resolution: r = .72; psychological control and attachment alienation: r =
.71; attachment trust and communication: r = .83; attachment alienation and trust: r = -.70;
attachment alienation and communication: r = -.70), none of the tolerances approached .1, and
the VIF values were lower than 10 (Field, 2005). Therefore, no multicollinearity problems were
detected in the dataset.
78
Figure 5. Scatterplot for Checking Homoscedasticity Assumption
Figure 6. P-P Plot for Checking Normality Assumption
79
Figure 7. Histogram for Checking Normality Assumption
Figure 8. Linearity Relationship Checking: Frequency of IPC with ER
80
Figure 9. Linearity Relationship Checking: Intensity of IPC with ER
Figure 10. Linearity Relationship Checking: Resolution of IPC with ER
81
Figure 11. Linearity Relationship Checking: Psychological Control with ER Figure 12. Linearity Relationship Checking: Autonomy Support with ER
82
Figure 13. Linearity Relationship Checking: Behavioral Control with ER
Figure 14. Linearity Relationship Checking: Attachment Trust with ER
83
Figure 15. Linearity Relationship Checking: Attachment Communication with ER
Figure 16. Linearity Relationship Checking: Attachment Alienation with ER
84
To answer the first research question, a simultaneous regression (see Figure 17) was
conducted with all predictors entered in one step including frequency, intensity, and resolution of
interparental conflict; parental psychological control, behavioral control, autonomy support; and
trust, communication and alienation of parent-child attachment; emotion regulation was used as
the outcome. A total of 21 percent of the variances in emotion regulation could be explained by
the model, R2 = .21. Examination of standardized beta coefficients (see Figure 17 and Table 5)
showed that resolution of interparental conflict, b = −.29, p = .002, and attachment alienation
from parents, b = −.20, p = .049, were negatively related to emotion regulation; and behavioral
control, b = .12, p = .045, and attachment communication, b = .25, p = .019, were positively
related to emotion regulation. That is, lower levels of interparental conflict resolution and higher
levels of alienation from parents predicted lower levels of emotion regulation, whereas higher
levels of behavior control and communication with parents predicted higher levels of emotion
regulation.
85
Table 5
Simultaneous Regression Analysis Predicting Emotion Regulation From Interparental Conflict,
Parenting Practices, and Parent-Child Attachment
Predictor
β
SD
t
IPC Frequency
.14
.09
1.40
IPC Intensity
.10
.09
1.12
-.29
.08
-3.11
Psychological Control
.05
.05
0.56
Autonomy Support
.12
.06
1.69
Behavioral Control
.12
.05
2.01
-.15
.08
-1.35
.25
.07
2.35
-.20
.06
-1.98
IPC Resolution
Att Trust
Att Communication
Att Alienation
p
.164
.263
.002
.576
.092
.045
.180
019
.049
Note. Frequency, Intensity and Resolution: three subscales of interparental conflict (IPC); Trust,
Communication and Alienation: three subscales of parent-child attachment (Att); All regression
coefficients were standardized; SD=unstandardized standard deviation.
86
Figure 17. Results of Multiple Regression Analysis
Frequency of Conflict
Intensity of Conflict
Resolution of Conflict
-.2
9*
*
Psychological Control
2
R =.21
Emotion Regulation
Autonomy Support
.12*
.25
*
Behavioral Control
-.2
Communication
0*
Trust
Alienation
Note. Only significant paths were drawn. * p < .05 ** p < .01
87
Research Question Two
To address the second research question, a path analysis was conducted using the AMOS
17. The path analysis (see Figure 18) revealed that twenty percent of the variation in emotion
regulation was explained by the model, R2 = .20. Although in the simultaneous regression only
interparental conflict resolution made a unique contribution to the variation in emotion
regulation, the path model revealed indirect paths for both frequency and intensity of conflict
through the parenting and attachment factors. Specifically, frequency of conflict, b = .30, p =
.001, in conjunction with higher resolution of conflict, b = -.39, p < .001, significantly predicted
parental behavioral control, R2 = .08, and in turn greater parental behavioral control was
significantly related to participants’ emotion regulation, b = .11, p = .036. In addition, lower
intensity of conflict in conjunction with higher levels of conflict resolution was significantly
related to higher levels of attachment trust, R2 = .17, and communication, R2 = .15, and lower
levels of attachment alienation, R2 = .21. In turn, higher levels of attachment communication, b =
.30, p < .001, and lower levels of alienation, b = -.16, p = .006, were significantly related to
higher emotion regulation. Surprisingly, lower levels of trust, b = -.17, p = .002, were related to
higher emotion regulation. However, given that trust did not directly predict a unique proportion
of variance in emotion regulation in the simultaneous regression (see Figure 18 and Table 6) and
given its high correlations with both communication, r = .83, p < .001, and alienation, r = -.70, p
< .001, a suppression effect may have caused a unusual reversal of signs. Cohen and Cohen
(1983) explained that an inconsistent mediation effect could occur through the suppression of
error variance in communication and alienation created by their significant correlations with
trust. Greater intensity of IPC also significantly predicted psychological control, b = .23, p =
.007; R2 = .11, however, that path was not complete in predicting emotion regulation.
88
Higher levels of interparental conflict resolution significantly predicted attachment trust,
communication, and alienation, which in turn predicted emotion regulation. In addition, greater
parental autonomy support was significantly related to higher levels of IPC resolution, R2 = .03, b
= -.19, p = .035, which in turn significantly predicted participants’ emotion regulation, b = .10, p
= .041. However, higher resolution of conflict continued to significantly predict greater emotion
regulation even after controlling for all parenting and attachment factors, b = -.26, p = .003 (see
Figure 18).
The significance of the indirect effects of frequency, intensity, and resolution of
interparental conflict on emotion regulation through parenting related factors were calculated
using bootstrap methods in SPSS (Preacher & Hayes, 2008) (see Table 7). Intensity and
resolution of interparental conflict, but not frequency of conflict, had significant indirect effects
on emotion regulation. Specifically, the effect of intensity of conflict on emotion regulation was
significantly mediated by attachment communication and alienation. In addition behavioral
control, communication, and alienation functioned as significant mediators between resolution of
conflict and emotion regulation (see Table 7).
89
Table 6
Results of Path Analyses: Parenting Practices and Parent-Child Attachment as Mediators
Between Interparental Conflict and Emotion Regulation
Paths (Predictor – Outcome)
Predicting Psychological Control
CR
R2 = .11
.12
.13
1.32
IPC Intensity−Psychological Control
.23**
.13
2.69
IPC Resolution−Psychological Control
.01
.12
0.09
.12
3.26
IPC Frequency− Behavioral Control
R2 = .08
.30**
IPC Intensity− Behavioral Control
-.09
.12
-1.00
IPC Resolution− Behavioral Control
-.39***
.11
-4.54
Predicting Autonomy Support
R2 = .03
IPC Frequency−Autonomy Support
.17
.11
1.73
IPC Intensity− Autonomy Support
-.12
.11
-1.31
IPC Resolution− Autonomy Support
-.19*
.10
-2.10
R2 = .17
Predicting Attachment Trust
SE
IPC Frequency−Psychological Control
Predicting Behavioral Control
β
IPC Frequency−Att Trust
-.04
.12
-0.45
IPC Intensity− Att Trust
-.19*
.12
-2.31
IPC Resolution− Att Trust
-.22**
.11
-2.68
90
Table 6 continued
Predicting Attachment Communication
R2 = .15
IPC Frequency− Att Comm
−.02
.13
−0.19
IPC Intensity− Att Comm
−.19*
.13
−2.28
IPC Resolution− Att Comm
−.22*
.12
−2.59
Predicting Attachment Alienation
R2 = .21
IPC Frequency− Att Alienation
.09
.13
1.04
IPC Intensity− Att Alienation
.19*
.13
2.41
IPC Resolution− Att Alienation
.22**
.12
2.64
.03
−0.03
Predicting Emotion Regulation
Psy Control− Emotion Regulation
R2 = .20
−.00
Autonomy Support− Emotion Reg
.10*
.04
2.04
Behavioral Control- Emotion Reg
.11*
.04
2.09
−.17**
.04
−3.12
.03
5.48
Att Trust− Emotion Regulation
Att Communication− Emotion Reg
.30***
Att Alienation− Emotion Regulation
−.15**
.03
−2.77
IPC Frequency− Emotion Regulation
.11
.08
1.23
IPC Intensity− Emotion Regulation
.15
.08
1.73
−.26**
.08
−2.99
IPC Resolution− Emotion Regulation
Note. Frequency, intensity, and resolution: three subscales of interparental conflict (IPC); Trust,
Communication (Comm), and Alienation: three subscales of parent-child Attachment (Att); Psy
Control: Psychological Control; Emotion Reg: Emotion Regulation. All beta coefficients are
standardized. SE: standard error of regression weight; CR: critical ratio for regression weight.
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
91
Table 7
Total and Specific Indirect Effects of Frequency, Intensity, and Resolution of Interparental
Conflict on Emotion Regulation
IPC Frequency
IPC Intensity
IPC Resolution
Total Indirect Effect
.04
−.09*
−.11*
Psychological Control
.00
.01
.00
Autonomy Support
.02
−.02
−.02
Behavioral Control
.03
−.01
−.04*
Att Trust
.00
.03
.03
Att Communication
.00
−.06*
−.05*
Att Alienation
−.01
−.05*
−.04*
Note. Frequency, intensity and resolution: three subscales of interparental conflict (IPC); Trust,
Communication (Comm) and Alienation: three subscales of parent-child Attachment (Att); * p<
.05.
92
Figure 18. Path Analysis Model with Parenting Practices and Parent-Child Attachment as One Level of Mediation
2
R =.11
Psychological Control
2
R =.08
.30**
Frequency of
Conflict
Behavioral Control
2
R =.03
-­‐.19* Intensity of
Conflict
.1
9*
9*
-­‐.1
-­‐.1
9
2 **
-­‐.2
2
R =.17
Emotion Regulation
-­‐.17** ** -­‐.26*
2
R =.15
Communication
*
.30
-­‐.1
-­‐.22* Resolution of
Conflict
2
R =.20
Trust
* .10* .21**
2
R =.21
Alienation
Note. Only significant paths were drawn. *p <.05, **p <.01, ***p < .001
**
* Autonomy Support
-.3
9
**
*
.2
3*
.11
* 5*
93
Post-hoc Analysis
In noting the significant role of the attachment factors in the path model and the literature
that would suggest that attachment may in fact mediate the relations of parenting on adolescent
outcomes (Brenning et al., 2012; Karavasilis et al., 2003; Scott et al., 2011; Shpigel et al., 2012),
a new model was proposed in which the parent-child attachment factors served as mediators
between parenting practices and emotion regulation. The new model was tested in AMOS 17
(see Figure 19), and the results indicated that the attachment factors of communication and
alienation indeed mediated the relations of behavior control, psychological control, and
autonomy support with emotion regulation. Specifically, all three parenting factors significantly
predicted all three attachment factors and their relations with emotion regulation were fully
mediated by higher parent-child communication and lower alienation. However, given its strong
relations with communication and alienation, trust was not related to emotion regulation in this
model (the suppression effect in the original model disappeared).
Moreover, relations of both frequency and intensity of interparental conflict with
attachment were fully mediated by parenting. Specifically, frequency of conflict was mediated
by behavioral control and intensity of conflict by psychological control. Resolution of conflict
was partially mediated by its significant relations with behavioral control and autonomy support;
however, higher conflict resolution continued to predict less alienation, which in turn was
significantly related to higher emotion regulation. In addition, conflict resolution continued to
have a direct relationship with emotion regulation beyond both parenting and attachment.
The total variance in emotion regulation explained by the model was similar to that in the
previous model (R2 = .19). However, in the first mediation model, AIC=1099.87 whereas the
value of AIC in the second mediation model was 381.68, indicating that the second model fits
94
the data better than does the first. Because this new model was exploratory in nature, more future
work needs to be done to validate the model with other samples.
95
Figure 19. Path Analysis Model with Parenting Practices and Parent-Child Attachment as Two Levels of Mediators
2
R =.08
*
.31*
Frequency of
Conflict
*
Behavioral
Control
.17***
.25
2
R =.63
Trust
**
-.1
*
1*
**
2
*
.28***
*
*
R =.60
-.15
Alienation
***
Autonomy
Support
Note. Only significant paths were drawn. *p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001
7*
**
**
.14*
R =.03
Communication
2
-.26**
-.19
-.35***
-.1
-.40
***
.64
Resolution of
Conflict
R =.52
**
Psychological
Control
2
*
.44
.23**
8
-.4
R =.19
.48
*
R =.12
Intensity of
Conflict
2
*
2
Emotion
Regulation
96 Chapter V
Discussion
The purpose of the current study was twofold: first, to examine how the overall emotional
environment of a family (defined as interparental conflict between parents), parenting practices,
(psychological control, autonomy support and behavioral control) and parent-child relations
(attachment) impact emerging adults’ emotion regulation capabilities; and second, to examine
how parenting practices and parent-child relations functioned as mechanisms in the relations
between interparental conflict and emerging adults’ emotion regulation. The first hypothesis was
that interparental conflict and parental psychological control would be negatively associated with
emerging adults’ emotion regulation whereas autonomy support, behavioral control, and parentchild attachment be positively related to emotion regulation. The hypothesis was partially
supported by the results. Of all the predictors, resolution of interparental conflict, behavioral
control, and two aspects of parental-child attachment (communication and alienation) were
significantly correlated with emotion regulation. Higher conflict resolution, more behavioral
control, better parent-child communication, and lower levels of alienation were associated with
better emotion regulation. The second hypothesis regarding whether parenting practices and
parent-child relations mediated the relations between interparental conflict and emotion
regulation also was partially supported. Parental autonomy support, behavioral control, and
parent-child attachment partially mediated the relations between resolution of interparental
conflict and emerging adults’ emotion regulation. The direct relation between resolution of
conflict and emotion regulation remained significant even when autonomy support, behavioral
control, and parent-child attachment were controlled. In addition, the analysis testing the role of
attachment in mediating parenting and emotion regulation showed that communication and
alienation fully mediated the influence of psychological control, behavioral control, and
97 autonomy support on emotion regulation. Specific patterns of direct and mediated effects are
discussed in turn.
Direct Relations Between Family Factors and Emotion Regulation
When considering all of the family factors together, an interesting pattern of direct effects
emerged. First, lower resolution of interparental conflict was associated with lower levels of
emotion regulation, however, frequency and intensity of interparental conflict were not related to
emotion regulation. Previous research has suggested that resolution of interparental conflict
would be particularly associated with children’s feelings of emotional security. In Cummings
and Davies’ (1994) review of 11 studies, they found that resolution after interparental conflict
significantly reduced children’s negative reactions to the conflict, as children were very sensitive
to resolution and were able to sense how much a conflict had been resolved. Although the
frequency of interparental conflict has been associated with children’s maladjustment, such
results might be due to its high covariance with other dimensions of interparental conflict
(Cummings & Davies). In addition, one meta-analysis showed that, when examined alone,
frequency of interparental conflict was only weakly related to children’s maladjustment
(Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000). Although the frequency of interparental conflict has been
found to be related to a sense of insecurity and distress among children (Kerig, 1996) and both
frequency and intensity of interparental conflict have been related to late adolescents’ sensitivity
to conflicts and emotional negativity (David & Murphy, 2004); hostile conflict has been shown
to be a stronger predictor of adolescents’ problem behaviors than the frequency of conflict
(Buehler et al., 1998). However, Buehler et al. (1998) found that parental use of covert conflict,
but not overt conflict, predicted adolescents’ internalized problems. It is possible that because the
measure of intensity of conflict in this study assesses only overt conflict (e.g., argue, yell, break
98 things, push or shove each other), but not covert conflict, it failed to present an association with
emotion regulation. Ultimately, the resolution of the conflict appeared to be the best predictor of
emotional regulation, especially among these emerging adults who might not have a fully
accurate perception of either the frequency or intensity of conflict when not living at home.
There appears to be something about the ability of parents to resolve their conflicts that enhances
their adult children’s ability to regulate their own emotions.
The fact that only resolution of conflict was related to emerging adults’ emotion
regulation might echo the social learning model. Parents who acted verbally and physically
aggressive towards each other, and were unable to find effective ways to resolve their disputes,
might fail to provide positive modeling to their children about effective ways of problem solving
and how to regulate emotions in time of distress. It is reasonable to believe that every family has
disagreements now and then, but what really differentiates one family from another, and impacts
their emerging adult children’s emotion regulation abilities, is how well disagreement and
conflicts are resolved afterwards (Cummings & Davies, 2010; Kerig, 1996). When parents failed
in their endeavors to negotiate and settle disputes and conflicts, they allowed a negative
emotional atmosphere to impact the family, making them less likely to engage in effective
parenting, and ultimately dampened their children’s emotion regulation capabilities.
Next, most of the parenting and attachment factors were either directly or indirectly
related to emotion regulation. Behavioral control as a dimension of parenting involves parents’
provision of structure and guidance about children’s behavior and has been associated with
adaptive outcomes among children and adolescents (Bean et al., 2006; Pomerantz & Wang,
2009). Psychological control, however, involves parents’ intrusion into children’s emotional and
psychological development and has been related to a number of measures of maladjustment
99 (Manzeske & Stright, 2009; McEwen & Flouri, 2009). It has been found that parental
psychological control, but not behavioral control, is related to difficulties in emotion regulation
among adolescents (McEwen & Flouri, 2009). In the current study, behavioral control was
positively associated with emotion regulation, but psychological control showed no influence on
emotion regulation. These findings imply that although most young emerging adults have left
their parents’ house and live independently, they still benefit from parental supervision and
guidance, especially during times of struggle. Parents still play an important role in assisting
emerging adults in regulating their behavior, mostly through the provision of psychological and
emotional support and encouragement (Arnett, 2007). Psychological control on the other hand is
greatly diminished during emerging adulthood even if it had been present during adolescence
(Arnett, 2000, 2007; Hill & Holmbeck, 1986). In the current sample, relatively low levels of
psychological control and limited variability (as would be expected within the normal
population) make it difficult to detect any impact it might have on emotion regulation.
In addition, two dimensions of attachment (better communication and less alienation)
were associated with more positive emotion regulation demonstrating that secure parent-child
attachment predicted better emotion regulation among these emerging adults. Many researchers
believed that an important characteristic of secure attachment was open discussions about
negative emotions between parents and children through verbal and non-verbal means (Bowlby,
1980; Etzion-Carasso & Oppenheim, 2000; Laible & Panfile, 2009). According to the spillover
hypothesis, parents occupied with the negative emotions brought by interparental conflict with
spouses might transfer these emotions to their relations with children, arousing feelings of
alienation between parent and child (Erel, & Burman, 1995; Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000).
These findings that more behavioral control, better communication, and less alienation were
100 associated with better emotion regulation among emerging adults demonstrated that, although
socialization of affective regulation is mostly completed at this stage, young emerging adults’
capabilities of emotion regulation could still be fostered through a positive parent-child
emotional bonding; and parental knowledge about their college children’s whereabouts,
associations, and activities with peers provided a basis for a functional parent-child relations.
Indirect Effects of Interparental Conflict with Emotion Regulation
Although only resolution of conflict had a direct impact on emotion regulation, all three
aspects of interparental conflict demonstrated different patterns of relations with parenting
practices, which in turn were related to emotion regulation. Taken together, the current study
indicated that interparental conflict explained a significant proportion of variance in parenting
practices, similar to what was reported by Krishnakumar and Buehler (2000). Overall,
interparental conflict was positively associated with negative parenting practices, supporting the
spillover hypothesis.
Higher frequency of interparental conflict was related to more behavioral control and
greater intensity of conflict was associated with higher psychological control. It appears that
parents who engage in more conflict are more likely to attend to the needed supervision of their
children’s behavior; and parents who engage in more intense conflict are more likely to impose
psychological control over their children, demonstrating the unique roles of the frequency vs.
intensity of parents’ conflict in how they parent. Although the positive association between
frequency of interparental conflict and emotion regulation was somewhat surprising, it provided
empirical support for Grych’s (2002) articulation that research studies tended to oversimplify the
complicated relations between interparental conflict and parenting, which indicated that “good
things go together,” in other words, interparental conflict should always predict ineffective
101 parenting. The findings of the current study, however, demonstrated that different dimensions of
interparental conflict were associated with both effective and ineffective parenting practices. It is
likely for parents of emerging adults, frequent conflicts with spouse might prompt them to
inquire about their young adult children’s whereabouts and activities. In turn, more behavioral
control prompts emerging adults’ emotion regulation. These findings support a previous study
that found that more destructive interparental conflicts were associated with emotion-related
parenting practices, but not others (Katz & Windecker-Nelson, 2006). In fact, these findings are
consistent with a type of compensatory hypothesis, when parents tend to become overly
dedicated to the parent-child relations to compensate for the loss of intimacy in the marital
relationship (Cummings & Davies, 2010; Erel & Burman, 1995). As such, parents who
frequently yell, break, or throw things, or act physically violent and aggressive during marital
conflict, may feel more guilty and blame themselves for not being able to provide an emotionally
and physically secure environment for their children, thus become overly involved and overprotective to children afterwards to compensate. The results of one study found that interparental
conflict became associated with higher levels of maternal psychological control and lower level
of parental monitoring and maternal acceptance among early to late adolescents (Krishnakumar
et al., 2003). Likewise, Cummings and Davies (2010) found that in families stressed by higher
interparental conflict, parents were more likely to become psychologically controlling in an
attempt to compensate for the unhappy family environment. Unfortunately, this enmeshed and
intrusive type of parenting served to jeopardize a secure parent-child attachment (Cummmings &
Davies, 2010), as likewise demonstrated in the current study. Although not directly related to
emotion regulation, psychological control was associated with an insecure parent-child
102 attachment (less communication and greater alienation), which in turn was related to less
emotion regulation among these emerging adults.
Less resolution of conflict, on the other hand, was associated with less parental autonomy
support and less behavior control, demonstrating that children from high conflict families with
low resolution were exposed to two levels of stressors: one directly from the interparental
conflict, and the other from less parental promotion of autonomy because of a dissonant family
emotional environment (Michael, Torres, & Seemann, 2007). These findings also provided more
evidence for the spillover hypothesis, which argued that the negative effect induced from a
disturbed marital realm spilled into parent-child interaction, making parents less likely to engage
in adaptive parenting practices (Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000). According to the findings,
parents who had problems settling their own disputes and conflicts with spouses were less likely
to support and encourage their children to be autonomous and develop individuality, and less
likely to provide adequate behavior regulation. This might reflect some consistent personal
characteristics within a parent. For example, husbands and wives who find it difficult to reach an
agreement or resolve a dispute with their spouses may have less tolerance of different opinions,
or they are very rigid with their own point of view. If that is the case, how could one expect such
parents to be “generous” in granting autonomy to their children? In addition, drained by the
frequent disputes and conflicts, emotionally distracted parents were less likely to have time to
regulate their children’s behaviors (Erel & Burman, 1995). In turn, the current study showed the
negative impact of less effective behavioral control and autonomy support on the emotion
regulation of these emerging adults.
It was the two aspects of parent-child attachment (high communication and low
alienation), however, that appeared to serve as the most significant predictors of better emotion
103 regulation. Communication between parents and children was related to better emotion
regulation whereas a feeling of alienation was associated with lower levels of emotion regulation
among emerging adults. Previous studies have demonstrated that in the face of emotional stress
imposed by interparental conflict, parents’ availability and support could buffer the negative
influence and serve as protectors (Bowlby, 1973; Grych & Fincham, 1990), whereas parental
insensitivity might produce an even more challenging environment for children and deteriorate
their abilities to regulate emotions (Cummings & Davies, 1994). Parent-child attachment has
been shown to provide a source of security in time of distress and promote better emotion
regulation (Cummings & Davies, 1994; Kobak, Cole, Fleming, Ferenz-Gillies, & Gamble, 1993).
Parents who were sensitive and emotionally available were found to be more able to discern if
children were emotionally disturbed (e.g., in the face of interparental conflict) (DeBoard-Lucas
et al., 2010), and were engaged in open discussion with children thus actually buffered the
negative impact of interparental conflict. Children were more likely to turn to parents with whom
they felt securely attached for clarification and comfort, and parents could guide them through
the misrepresentations of the unclear family future, and provided reassurance (Davies, Harold,
Goeke-Morey, & Cummings, 2002). When children perceived their parents to be responsive and
emotionally available, they were more likely to turn to them for affective guidance after family
discord, or in other times of distress (Davies et al., 2002). In addition, middle to late adolescents
who were more securely attached to their mothers perceived interparental conflict as less
threatening, as reported by Grych et al. (2004).
Most interesting was that parent-child attachment served to mediate the effects of
parenting on emotion regulation. To be more specific, parental psychological control was
negatively associated with parent-child attachment, whereas parental autonomy support and
104 behavioral control were positively associated with parent-child attachment. Previous research
studies showed that interparental conflict was negatively associated with parent-child relations
(Cummings & Davies, 1994), our findings demonstrated that the relations were actually realized
through parenting practices such as psychological control, autonomy support, and behavioral
control. Research studies demonstrated that ineffective parenting practice could exacerbate the
negative effects of interparental conflict (Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000) and the current study
indicated that such aggravation is fulfilled through a worsened parent-child attachment. In
previous research, children’s feelings of attachment were significantly predicted by their parents’
parenting practices (Brenning et al., 2012; Karavasilis et al., 2003), and attachment styles
influenced children’s developmental outcomes (Scott et al., 2003). More specifically, parental
warmth, autonomy support, and behavioral control were found to be associated with children’s
and adolescents’ secure attachment (Karavasilis et al., 2003; Shpigel et al., 2012) whereas
negligent, unresponsive parenting together with low autonomy support were related to insecure
attachment among children and adolescents (Brenning et al., 2012; Karavasilis et al., 2003). In
one study, late adolescents’ attachment with parents measured by trust, communication and
alienation fully mediated the relations between parenting practices and aggression (Gallarin &
Alonso-Arbiol, 2012). The current study showed that when parents’ do not inquire about their
college children’s whereabouts, activities, and associations with friends; do not support their
autonomy, and are psychologically controlling the emotional bonds between parent and children
were diminished; and in turn emotion regulation was negatively affected.
Indeed, higher levels of communication and lower levels of alienation were associated
with positive emotion regulation. When children have secure affective relations with parents,
they may feel less threatened by the presence of interparental conflict, and might be less
105 distressed. Even in the presence of disagreements and fights between parents, children who
perceive the family bonding as strong, due to the feelings of security in the parent-child
subsystem, can learn to deal with emotions positively. Although the security obtained from the
parent-child relations could not entirely offset the insecurity in the interparental realm, it at least
served as buffer to protect emerging adults’ overall adjustment to some degree.
Even after controlling for parenting, poor resolution of interparental conflict continued to
have a direct impact on emerging adult children’s perceptions of alienation from parents.
Moreover, resolution of conflict had a direct impact on emotion regulation above and beyond all
other family factors. Perhaps children can learn how to regulate their emotions by modeling
parents’ constructive ways of handling conflict (Thompson & Meyer, 2007), or perhaps parents
who possess the temperament required to readily resolve conflicts have children who possess a
similar ability to regulate their emotions in times of distress. These findings suggest that there are
numerous aspects of the family system not explained simply with parenting practices, or even
with the security of the parent-child relationship. Certainly the ability of emerging adults to
regulate their emotions is a complex developmental process worthy of further study.
Importance of the Current Study
The association between interparental conflict and emotional dysfunction among children
has been well documented (Rhoades, 2008). In exploration of how several family subsystems
influenced emerging adults’ emotion regulation, the current study examined the role of three
dimensions of interparental conflict on emerging adults’ emotion regulation. Most research
studies focused on the impact of frequency of conflict on youth development, and ignored other
aspects such as intensity and resolution, thus were unable to provide an accurate picture of which
aspects of interparental conflict were more detrimental on youth various outcomes (Grych &
106 Fincham, 1990). Although it was clear that marital conflict predicted ineffective parenting, the
majority of studies used a general measure (or composite score) for marital conflict thus
provided little information about which dimension of IPC impacted which aspect of parenting
(Grych, 2002). The current study demonstrated that different dimensions of interparental conflict
made various contributions to parenting practices, parent-child relations, and emotion regulation
among emerging adults. In particular, resolution of conflict demonstrated the strongest effect on
parenting and emotion regulation, which could be viewed as the most significant finding of the
current study. Indeed, the observation of conflict between parents impacts and changes children’s
emotion regulation capabilities (Fincham et al., 1994), and such impact can extend into emerging
adulthood. In fact, previous research has shown that the effect of exposure to interparental
conflict from divorced families posed risks that primarily emerged in late adolescents and the
early adult years (Amato & Keith, 1991). The current study was the first to examine how
interparental conflict, parenting practices and parent-child attachment impacted emotion
regulation among emerging adults. The findings indicated that interparental conflict, especially
resolution of conflict, was closely associated with young adults’ emotion regulation capabilities,
and at least part of the relations were achieved through parent-child dynamics including
parenting practices and parent-child attachment.
The influence of family extends into emerging adult years (David & Murphy, 2004).
Although the influence of interparental conflict has been widely investigated in children, little is
known about how older adolescents and emerging adults could be impacted by interparental
conflict (David & Murphy, 2004; Parke & Buriel, 1998). Research studies have demonstrated
that interparental conflict led to less responsive, consistent, and more intrusive parenting (Katz &
Gottman, 1996; Shelton & Harold, 2008), and was related to an insecure parent-child
107 relationship among children and adolescents (Doyle & Markiewicz, 2005). However, it is
unclear whether similar patterns exist for emerging adults. The current study supported the
proposition that interparental conflict was associated with ineffective parenting practices and less
secure emotional connections between parents and their emerging adult children. In the current
study, a significant proportion of variance in parent-child attachment was explained,
demonstrating that the emotional discord between parents greatly impacts the affective
relationships between parents and children, and the impact was mostly realized through
dimensions of parenting practices. In particular, whether parents could effectively resolve
disagreement directly impacted whether their children felt related to parents emotionally.
Limitations of the Study
There were several limitations in the current study. In the first place, a college sample
was used to represent emerging adults, which as a population is composed of both college
students and young individuals living in communities who chose not to pursue higher education.
The current sample limits the generalizability of these findings to all emerging adults. In
addition, the participants were recruited from a research pool, with an unbalanced number of
males and females and an overwhelming proportion of Caucasians. To prompt the data
collection, the researcher inquired about opportunities to meet with potential participants and
worked with course instructors to gain access to the sample. It should be acknowledged that the
varied support from instructors might have introduced some bias in the sample. Further, because
the data were collected through an online survey, the researcher was not able to spot possible
problems during the data collection process and could not address participants’ questions in a
prompt manner.
108 Directions for Future Study
The complex family dynamic contains three subsystems: interparental, parent-child, and
sibling relations (Erel & Burman, 1995). The current study focused only on how two of the three
realms, that is, how relations between parents and parenting practices and parent-child
attachment impact emerging adults’ emotion regulation. Future studies should also examine how
sibling relations might influence individuals’ emotion regulation capabilities. In previous
research, marital conflict and ineffective parenting were associated with disturbed sibling
relationships (Deal, Hagan, Bass, Hetherington, & Clingempeel, 1999). Some researchers
suggested that “meta-emotion coaching” proposed by Gottman and colleagues (1996) could be
viewed as a new dimension of parenting (Cowan & Cowan, 2002), which focuses on how
parents regulate emotions of their own and their children. “Emotionally coaching” parents are
good at labeling, recognizing and utilizing their and their children’s emotions, they accept and
validate children’s emotion expression, especially negative ones, and use it as optimal
opportunity to teach emotion regulation strategies and enhance parent-child closeness. “Emotion
dismissing” parents, on the other hand, refuse to recognize and accept the existence of negative
emotions in children, invalidate such emotions, and induce shame and doubt to discourage the
expression of negative emotions. Future researchers may consider examining how such parenting
dimensions might be related to parent-child relations and impact emerging adults’ emotion
regulation capabilities. In the current study, the measure of interparental conflict mainly focused
on overt conflict, and neglected how covert conflict might influence emerging adults’ outcomes.
Future researchers might consider measuring both overt and covert conflict and examine the
various patterns between types of conflicts and emerging adults’ emotional outcomes.
109 In the current study, college students reported their perceptions of interparental conflicts,
parenting practices, parent-child attachment, and their emotion regulation capacities. Although
some people might argue that such subjective perceptions may not be accurate, youth
perceptions’ of interparental conflict reflect what they actually observe and feel, thus would be
considered a better predictor of their developmental outcomes than parent-reported information
(Emery & O’Leary, 1982). In fact, child reported dimensions of interparental conflict were
consistently associated with their adjustment as reported by other informants (e.g., teachers,
parents, peers), whereas parent-reported data were only related to children’s adjustment
problems reported by parents (Grych et al., 1992; Fincham et al., 1994). However, future
researchers might consider assessing interparental conflict using multiple informants, and
compare and contrast the possible different patterns in youths’ social and emotional outcomes.
Historically, the conceptualization of marital functioning included marital satisfaction and
interparental conflict (Buehler et al., 1998); but, in the current study, only interparental conflict
was assessed for it has been found to be a better predictor of youth outcomes (Cummings,
Davies, & Simpson, 1994; Katz & Gottman, 1993).
In the current study, the impact of interparental conflict on various aspects of parenting
was examined, however, the relations between interparental conflict and parenting could be
reciprocal (Grych, 2002). When parents could not reach agreement on how to discipline children,
their disagreement might escalate into serious conflicts. Parenting practices also are under the
influence of many other factors (e.g. parental personalities, external stress) (Grych, 2002), and
future research might take such factors into consideration when examining the relations between
interparental conflict and parenting, as well as their effects on emerging adults’ emotion
regulation. Researchers should also consider examining how interparental conflict, parenting
110 practices, and parent-child relations could impact young emerging adults’ adjustment in blended
families, cohabiting families, and single-parent families. In the current study, no differentiation
was made for participants from divorced and intact families, since what really impacts children is
conflict, not necessarily the marital status itself (Deal et al., 1999). The emotional problem
differences that existed between children from divorced and non-divorced groups could be traced
back to early years when their parents were still married (Cherlin, Chase-Lansdale, & McRae,
1998). In other words, when exposed to interparental conflict, children in intact families suffer
similarly as those in divorced families (Amato & Keith, 1991).
Conclusion
The current study as a “process-oriented’ research (Cummings & Davies, 2002)
explicated the multiple protective and risk factors in the parent-child realm, and demonstrated
how various parenting practices and parent-child attachment might contribute to emerging
adults’ emotion regulation. The conceptual model of the current study was derived from
frameworks proposed by Morris et al. (2007) and Cummings and Davies (2002) for a processoriented direction in examining how interparental conflict impact children’s outcomes.
Based on the results, interparental conflict negatively impacted parenting practices, which in turn
influenced parent-child attachment. Indeed research studies have consistently shown that stresses
such as marital conflict induced negative emotions which in turn influenced the quality of
parenting (Dix, 1991; Doyle & Markiewicz, 2005; Owen & Cox, 1997). As the “barometers of
parenting,” parents’ emotions predicted a healthy or dysfunctional parent-child relationship (Dix,
1991, p. 4). In essence, this is in line with the spillover hypothesis, that the negative affect
generated from a disturbed marital conflict, could be transferred to the parent-child subsystem. 111 References
Alberto, A. (2011). Parenting styles and children’s emotional intelligence: what do we know?
The Family Journal, 19, 56-62.
Akimoto, S. A., & Sanbonmatsu, D. M. (1999). Differences in self-effacing behavior between
European and Japanese Americans: Effect on competence evaluations. Journal of CrossCultural Psychology, 30, 159-177.
Amato, P. R. (1986). Marital conflict, the parent-child relationship and child self-esteem. Family
Relations, 35, 403-410.
Amato, P. R., & Keith, B. (1991). Parental divorce and the well-being of children: A metaanalysis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 26-46.
Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The inventory of parent and peer attachment:
Individual differences and their relationship to psychological well-being in adolescence.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16(5), 427-454.
Arnett, J. (1992). Reckless behavior in adolescence: A developmental perspective.
Developmental Review, 12, 339-373.
Arnett, J. J. (1997). Young people’s conceptions of the transition to adulthood. Youth and
Society, 29, 1-23.
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through
the twenties. American Psychologist, 55, 469-480.
Arnett, J. J. (2004). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from the late teens through the
twenties. New York: Oxford University Press.
Arnett, J. J. (2007). Socialization in emerging adulthood: From the family to the wider world,
from socialization to self-socialization. In J. Grusec & P. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of
socialization (pp. 208-231). New York: Guilford.
Bachman, J. G., Johnston, L. D., O ' Malley, P., & Schulenberg, J. (1996). Transitions in drug
use during late adolescence and young adulthood. In J. A. Graber, J. Brooks-Gunn, & A.
C. Petersen (Eds.), Transitions through adolescence: Interpersonal domains and context
(pp.111-140). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: revisiting a neglected construct. Child
Development, 67, 3296-3319.
112 Barber, B. K., & Harmon, E. L. (2002). Violating the self: Parental psychological control of
children and adolescents. In B. K. Barber (Ed.), Intrusive parenting: How psychological
control affects children and adolescents (pp. 15-52). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Barber, B. K., Olsen, J. E., & Shagle, S. C. (1994). Associations between parental psychological
and behavioral control and youth internalized and externalized behaviors. Child
Development, 65 (4), 1120-1136.
Bar-On, R. (1997). The Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i): Technical manual.
Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology
Monographs, 4(1).
Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parenting style on adolescent competence and substance
use. Journal of Early Adolescence, 11(1), 56-95.
Bean, R. A., Barber, B. K., & Crane, R. D. (2006). Parental support, behavioral control, and
psychological control among African American youth: The relationships to academic
grades, delinquency, and depression. Journal of Family Issues, 27, 1335-1355.
Becker, W. C. (1964). Consequences of different types of parental discipline. In M.L. Hoffman
& L.W. Hoffman (Eds.), Review of Child Development Research. NY: Russell Sage
Foundation.
Belsky, J., Youngblade, L., Rovine, M., & Volling, B. (1991). Patterns of marital change and
parent-child interaction . Journal of Marriage and Family, 53, 487-498.
Bickham, N. L. & Fiese, B. H. (1997). Extension of the Children’s Perceptions of Interparental
Conflict Scale for use with late adolescents. Journal of Family Psychology, 11(2), 246250.
Biringen, Z., Emde, R., Campos, J., & Appelbaum, M. (1995). Affective reorganization in the
infant, the mother, and the dyad. Child Development, 66, 499-514.
Biringen, Z., & Robinson, J. (1991). Emotional availability in mother-child interactions: A
conceptualization for research. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 61, 258-271.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss, Vol. 2: Separation. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (I980). Attachment and loss, Vol. 3: Loss, sadness and depression. New York: Basic
Books.
113 Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd ed.) New York: Basic Books.
(Original work published 1969)
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development.
New York: Basic Books.
Brackett, M. A., & Mayer, J. D. (2003). Convergent, discriminant, and incremental validity of
competing measures of emotional intelligence. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 29, 1147-1158.
Brenning, K., Soenens, B., Braet, C., & Bal, S. (2012). The role of parenting and motheradolescent attachment in the intergenerational similarity of depressive symptoms. Journal
of Youth and Adolescence, 41, 802-816.
Brody, G. H., & Ge, X. (2001). Linking parenting processes and self-regulation to psychological
functioning and alcohol use during early adolescence. Journal of Family Psychology, 15,
82-94.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Crouter, A.C. (1983). The evolution of environmental models in
developmental research. In P. Mussen (Ed.), The handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1.
Theories of development (pp. 358-414). New York: Wiley.
Buehler, C., & Gerard, J. (2002). Marital conflict, ineffective parenting, and children’s and
adolescent’s maladjustment. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 64, 78-92.
Buehler, C., Lange, G., & Franck, K. L. (2007). Adolescents’ cognitive and emotional responses
to marital hostility. Child Development, 78, 775-789.
Buehler, C, Krishnakumar, A., Anthony, C., Tittsworth, S., & Stone, G. (1994). Hostile
interparental conflict and youth maladjustment. Family Relations, 43, 409-416.
Buehler, C., Krishnakumar, A., Stone, G., Anthony, C., Pemberton, S., Gerard, J., & Barber, B.
K. (1998). Interparental conflict styles and youth problem behaviors: A two-sample
replication study. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 119-132.
Calkins, S. D, & Hill, A. (2007). Caregiver influences on emerging emotion regulation:
Biological and environmental transactions in early development. In Gross J. J.
(Ed.), Handbook of Emotion Regulation (pp. 3-26). New York: The Guilford Press.
Calkins, S. D., & Johnson, M. C. (1998). Toddler regulation of distress to frustrating events:
Temperamental and maternal correlates. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 379-395.
114 Calkins, S. D., Smith, C. L., Gill, K. L., & Johnson, M. C. (1998). Maternal interactive style
across contexts: Relations to emotional, behavioral, and physiological regulation during
toddlerhood. Social Development, 7, 350-369.
Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E., Witherington, D. C., &
Hertenstein, M. (2000). Travel broadens the mind, Infancy, 1, 149-219.
Camras, L. A., Oster, H., Campos, J., Campos, R., Ujiie, T., Miyake, K., Wang, L., & Meng, Z.
(1998). Production of emotional facial expressions in European American, Japanese, and
Chinese infants. Developmental Psychology, 34, 616-628.
Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Influences of attachment relationships. In N. Fox (Ed.),
The development of emotion regulation. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Child Development, 59(2-3, Serial No. 240), 228-249.
Chan, S. M. (2011). Social competence of elementary-school children: Relationships to maternal
authoritativeness, supportive maternal responses and children’s coping strategies. Child:
Care, Health, and Development, 37(4), 524-532.
Cherlin, A. 1., Chase-Lansdale. P. L., & McRae. C. (1998). Effects of divorce on mental health
through the life course. American Sociological Review, 63, 239-249.
Clark, T. R. (2004). Feelings in the family: Interparental conflict, anger, and expressiveness in
families with older adolescents. The Family Journal, 12, 129-138.
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the
behavioral sciences (2nd Ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cole, P. M. (1985). Display rules and the socialization of affective displays. In G. Zivin (Ed.),
The development of expressive behavior (pp. 269–290). New York: Academic Press.
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T.A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific
construct: Challenges and directions for child development research. Child
Development, 75, 317-333.
Cole, P. M., Michel, M. K., & Teti, L. O. (1994) The development of emotion regulation and
dysregulation: A clinical perspective. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development, 59, 73-100.
Compas, B. E., Malcarne, V. L., & Fondacaro, K. M. (1988). Coping with stressful events in
order children and young adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
56, 405-411.
115 Contreras, J. M., Kerns, K. A., Weimer, B. L., Gentzler, A. L., & Tomich, P. L. (2000). Emotion
regulation as a mediator of associations between mother-child attachment and peer
relationships in middle childhood. Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 111-124.
Costanzo, P., & Woody, E. (1985). Domain-specific parenting styles and their impact on
the child's development of particular deviance: The example of obesity
proneness. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 3(4), 425-445.
Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (2002). Interventions as tests of family systems theories: Marital
and family relationships in children's development and psychopathology. Development
and Psychopatholology. Special Issue on Interventions as Tests of Theories., 14, 731-760.
Cox, M. J., Paley, B., & Harter, K. (2001). Interparental conflict and parent-child relationships.
In J. H. Grych & F. D. Fincham (Eds.), Interparental conflict and child development (pp.
249-272). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Crockenberg, S. & Leerkes, E. (2000). Infant social and emotional development in family
context. In C. Zeanah (Ed.). The Handbook of Infant Mental Health, 2nd Edition (pp. 6090). New York: Guilford.
Crockenberg, S. & Litman, C. (1990). Autonomy as competence in 2-year-olds: Maternal
correlates of child defiance, compliance, and self-assertion. Developmental
Psychology, 26, 961-971.
Cummings, E. M., Braungart-Rieker, J., & Du Rocher Schudlich, T. (2003). Emotion and
personality development in childhood. In R. M. Lerner, M. A. Easterbrooks, & J. Mistry
(Eds.), Comprehensive Handbook of Psychology: Volume 6: Developmental Psychology
(pp. 211-239). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. T. (1994). Children and marital conflict: The impact of family
dispute and resolution. New York: Guilford Press.
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. T. (2002). Effects of marital discord on children: Recent
advances and emerging themes in process-oriented research. Journal of Child
Psychologyand Psychiatry, 43, 31-63.
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. T. (2010). Marital conflict and children: An emotional security
perspective. New York: Guilford Press.
Cummings, E. M., Davies, P. T., & Campbell, S. B. (2000). Developmental psychopathology and
family process: Theory, research, and clinical implications. New York: Guilford Press.
116 Cummings, E. M., Davies, P. T., & Simpson, K. S. (1994). Marital conflict, and children’s
appraisals and coping efficacy as mediators of child adjustment. Journal of Family
Psychology, 8, 141-149.
Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as context: An integrative model.
Psychological Bulletin, 113(3), 487-496.
David, K. M. & Murphy, B. C. (2004). Interparental conflict and late adolescents' sensitization
to conflict: The moderating effects of emotional functioning and gender. Journal of
Youth and Adolescence, 33, 187-200.
Davies, P. & Cummings, E. M. (1998). Exploring children's emotional security as a mediator of
the link between marital relations and child adjustment. Child Development, 69, 124-139.
Davies, P. T., Forman, E. M., Rasi, J. A., & Stevens, K. I. (2002). Assessing children’s
emotional security in the interparental relationship: The security in the interparental
subsystem scales. Child Development, 73, 544-562.
Davies, P. T., Harold, G. T., Goeke-Morey, M. C., & Cummings, E. M. (2002). Child emotional
security and interparental conflict. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development, 67(3, vii-viii, Serial No. 270), 1-131.
Deal, J. E., Hagan, M. S., Bass, B. L., Hetherington, M., & Clingempeel, G. (1999). Marital
interaction in dyadic and triadic settings: Continuities and discontinuities. Family
Process, 38, 105-115.
DeBoard-Lucas, R. L., Fosco, G. M., Raynor, S. M., & Grych, J. H. (2010). Interparental
Conflict in Context: Exploring Relations Between Parenting Processes and Children's
Conflict Appraisals. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39, 163-175.
Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers:
Contextual validation. Child Development, 57, 194-201.
Denham, S. A. (1989). Maternal affect and toddlers’ social-emotional competence. American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 59, 368-376.
Denham, S. A., & Kochanoff, A. T. (2002). Parental contributions to preschoolers' understanding
of emotion. Marriage & Family Review, 34, 311-343.
Diamond, A., Werker, J. F., & LaLonde, C. (1994). Toward understanding commonalities in the
development of object search, detour navigation, categorization, and speech perception.
117 In G. Dawson & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain (pp.
380 – 426). New York: Guilford Press.
Dix T. (1991). The affective organization of parenting: Adaptive and maladaptive
processes. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 3–25.
Doyle, A. B., & Markiewicz, D. (2005). Parenting, marital conflict and adjustment from early- to
mid-adolescence: Mediated by adolescent attachment style? Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 34, 97-110.
Dunn, J., & Brown, J. (1993). Early conversations about causality: Content, pragmatics, and
developmental change. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 107-123.
El-Sheikh, M., Buckhalt, J. A., Keller, P., Cummings, M., & Acebo, C. (2007). Child emotional
insecurity and academic achievement: The role of sleep disruptions. Journal of Family
Psychology, 21, 29-38.
Eisenberg, N. A., Fabes, R. A., & Guthrie, I. K. (1997). Coping with stress: The roles of
regulation and development. In S. A. Wolchik & I. N. Sandler (Eds.), Handbook of
children's coping: Linking theory and intervention (pp. 41 -70). New York: Plenum
Press.
Eisenberg, N., Gershoff, E. T., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Cumberland, A. J., & Losoya, S. H.
(2001). Mother's emotional expressivity and children's behavior problems and social
competence: Mediation through children's regulation. Developmental Psychology, 37,
475-490.
Eisenberg, N., & Morris, A. S. (2002). Children’s emotion-related regulation. In H. Reese & R.
Kail (Eds.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 30, 189-229.
Eisenberg, N., Morris, A. S., & Spinrad, T. L. (2005). Emotion-related regulation: The construct
and its measurement. In D. Teti (Ed.), Handbook of Research Methods in Developmental
Science (pp. 423-442). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.
Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004). Emotion-related regulation: Sharpening the definition.
Child Development, 75, 334-339.
Eisenberg, N., & Sulik, M. J. (2012). Emotion-related self-regulation in children. Teaching of
Psychology, 39(1), 77-83.
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face: Guidelines for
research and an integration of findings. New York: Pergamon Press.
118 Emde, R. N., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (1985). Assessing emotional availability in early
development. In W. K. Frankenburg, R. N. Emde, & J. Sullivan (Eds.), Early
identification of the at-risk child: An international perspective (pp. 79-102). New York:
Plenum.
Emery, R. E. (1982). Interparental conflict and the children of discord and
divorce. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 310-330.
Emery, R. E., and O’Leary, K. D. (1982). Children’s perceptions of marital discord and behavior
problems of boys and girls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 10, 11-24.
Erel, O., & Burman, B. (1995). Interrelatedness of marital relations and parent-child relations: A
meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 108-132.
Etzion-Carasso, A., & Oppenheim, D. (2000). Open mother-pre-schooler communication:
Relations with early secure attachment. Attachment and Human Development, 2, 347370.
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., & Green, F. L. (2001). Development of children’s understanding of
connections between thinking and feeling. Psychological Science, 12, 430-432.
Field, A. (2005). Discovering statistics using SPSS. (2nd ed.). London: Sage.
Fincham, F. D. (1994). Understanding the association between marital conflict and child
adjustment: Overview. Journal of Family Psychology, 8,123-127.
Fincham, F. D., Grych, J. H., & Osborne, L. N. (1994). Does marital conflict cause child
maladjustment? Directions and challenges for longitudinal research. Journal of Family
Psychology, 8, 128-140.
Frederickson, N., Petrides, K. V., & Simmonds, E. (2012). Trait emotional intelligence as a
predictor of socioemotional outcomes in early adolescence. Personality and Individual
Differences, 52, 317-322.
Galambos, N. L., & Costigan, C. L. (2003). Emotional and personality development in
adolescence. In I. B. Weiner (Series Ed.), & R. M. Lerner, M. A. Easterbrooks, & J.
Mistry (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Vol. 6. Developmental psychology (pp. 351372). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Gallarin, M., & Alonso-Arbiol, I. (2012). Parenting practices, parental attachment and
aggressiveness in adolescence: A predictive model. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 16011610.
119 Garber, J., Braafladt, N., & Zeman, J. (1991). The regulation of sad affect: An information
processing perspective. In J. Garber & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), The development of emotional
regulation and dysregulation (pp. 208-240). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Garner P. W. (1995). Toddler’s Emotion Regulation Behavior: The Roles of Social Context and
Family Expressiveness. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 156(4), 417-430.
Gerard, J. M., Buehler, C., Franck, K. L., & Anderson, O. (2005). In the eye of the beholder: The
functional roles of perceived appraisals associated with interparental conflict. Journal of
Family Psychology, 19, 376-384.
Gianino, A., & Tronick, E. (1988). The mutual regulation model: The infant's self and interactive
regulation and coping and defensive capacities. In T. Field, P. McCabe, & N.
Schneiderman (Eds.), Stress and coping (pp. 47-68). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Inc.
Gignac, G. E., Palmer, B. R., Manocha, R., Stough, C. (2005). An examination of the factor
structure of the Schutte Self-Report Emotional Intelligence (SSREI) scale via
confirmatory factor analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 1029-1042.
Gilliom, M., Shaw, D. S., Beck, J. E., Schonberg, M. A., & Lukon, J. L. (2002). Anger
regulation in disadvantaged preschool boys: Strategies, antecedents, and the development
of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 38, 222-235.
Gomez, R., & McLaren, S. (2007). The inter-relations of mother and father attachment, selfesteem and aggression during late adolescence. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 160-169.
Gottman, J., Fainsilber-Katz, L. & Hooven, C. (1996). Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the
emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of Family
Psychology, 10(3), 243-268.
Grolnick, W. S., Bridges, L. J., & Connell, J. P. (1996). Emotional self-regulation in two-yearolds: Strategies and emotional expression in four contexts. Child Development, 67, 928941.
Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent
consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 74, 224-237.
120 Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes:
Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 85, 348-362.
Grych, J. H. (2002). Marital relationships and parenting. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of
parenting (2nd edition) (pp. 203-226). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Grych, J. H., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Marital conflict and children’s adjustment: cognitivecontextual framework. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 267-290.
Grych, J. H., Harold, G. T., & Miles, C. J. (2003). A prospective investigation of appraisals as
mediators of the link between interparental conflict and child adjustment. Child
Development, 74, 1176-1193.
Grych, J. H., Raynor, S. R., & Fosco, G. M. (2004). Family processes that shape the impact of
interparental conflict on adolescents. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 649-665.
Grych, J. H., Seid, M., & Fincham, F. D. (1992). Assessing marital conflict from the child's
perspective: The Children's Perception of Interparental Conflict Scale. Child
Development, 63, 558-572.
Grych, J. H., Wachsmuth-Schlaefer, T., & Klockow, L. L. (2002). Interparental aggression and
young children’s representations of family relationships. Journal of Family Psychology,
16, 259-272.
Gullone, E., Hughes, E. K., King, N.J., & Tonge, B. (2010). The normative development of
emotion regulation strategy use in children and adolescents: A 2-year follow-up
study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(5), 567-674.
Hall, G. S. (1905). Adolescence, Its Psychology and its Relations to Physiology, Anthropology,
Sociology, Sex, Crime, Religion and Education. New York: Appleton.
Hardy, D., Power, T., & Jaedicke, S. (1993). Examining the relation of parenting to children’s
coping with everyday stress. Child Development, 64, 1829-1841.
Harold, G., Aitken, J., & Shelton, K. (2007). Inter--parental conflict and children's academic
attainment: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 48(12),
1223-1232.
Harold, G., Shelton, K., Goeke-Morey, M. & Cummings, E. M. (2004). Marital conflict, child
emotional security about family relationships and child adjustment. Social Development,
13, 350-376.
121 Harter, S., & Whitesell, N. R. (1989). Developmental changes in children's understanding of
single, multiple, and blended, emotion concepts. In C. Saarni & P. L. Harris (Eds.),
Children's understanding of emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hill, J. P. (1983). Early adolescence: A framework. Journal of Early Adolescence, 3, 1-21.
Hill, J. P., & Holmbeck, G. N. (1986). Attachment and autonomy during adolescence. In G. J.
Whitehurst (Ed.), Annals of child development (Vol. 3, pp. 145-189). Greenwich, CT: JAI
Press.
Hofmann, S. G., Heering, S., Sawyer, A. T., & Asnaani, A. (2009). How to handle anxiety: the
effects of reappraisal, acceptance, and suppression strategies on anxious arousal.
Behavior Research Therapy, 47(5), 389-394.
Howes, P., & Markman, H. J. (1989). Marital quality and child functioning: A longitudinal
investigation. Child Development, 60, 1044-1051.
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality
processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of
Personality, 72, 1301-1333.
Karavasilis, L., Doyle, A. B., & Markiewicz, D. (2003). Associations between parenting style
and attachment to mother in middle childhood and adolescence. International Journal of
Behavioral Development, 27(2), 153-164.
Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1993). Patterns of marital conflict predict children's internalizing
and externalizing behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 29, 940-950.
Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1996). Spillover effects of marital conflict: In search of parenting
and coparenting mechanisms. New Directions in Child Development, 74, 57-76.
Katz, L. F., & Windecker-Nelson, B. (2006). Domestic violence, emotion coaching, and child
adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 56-67.
Kerig, P. K. (1996). Assessing the links between interparental conflict and child adjustment: The
Conflicts and Problem-Solving Scales. Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 454–473.
Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2000). What parents know, how they know it, and several forms of
adolescent adjustment: Further support for a reinterpretation of monitoring.
Developmental Psychology, 36, 366-380.
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed). New York:
Guilford.
122 Kobak, R., Cole, H., Fleming, W., Ferenz-Gillies, R. & Gamble, W. (1993). Attachment and
emotion regulation during mother-teen problem-solving: A control theory analysis. Child
Development, 64, 231-245.
Kopp, C. (1989). Regulation of distress and negative emotions: A developmental view.
Developmental Psychology, 25, 343-354.
Krishnakumar, A., & Buehler, C. (2000). Interparental conflict and parenting behaviors: A meta
analytic review. Family Relations, 49, 25-44.
Krishnakumar, A., Buehler, C., & Barber, B. K. (2003). Youth perceptions of interparental
conflict, ineffective parenting, and youth problem behaviors in European-American and
African-American families. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 20, 239-260.
Laible, D., & Panfile, T. (2009). Mother-child reminiscing in the context of secure attachment
relationships: Lessons in understanding and coping with negative emotions. In J. A. Quas
& R. Fivush (Eds). Emotion and memory in development: Biological, cognitive, and
social considerations (pp. 166-195). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Lamborn, S. D., Mounts, N. S., Steinberg, L., & Dornbusch, S. M. (1991). Patterns of
competence and adjustment from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent and neglectful
families. Child Development, 62,1049-1065.
Langlois, J. H. (2004). Emotion and emotion regulation: From another perspective. Child
Development, 75(2), 315-316.
Larson, R., & Lampman-Petraitis, C. (1989). Daily emotional states associated with the onset of
adolescence. Child Development, 60, 1250-1260.
Larson, R., & Richards, M. H. (1991). Daily companionship in late childhood and early
adolescence: Changing developmental contexts. Child Development, 62, 284-300.
Laumakis, M. A., Margolin, G., & John, R. S. (1998). The emotional, cognitive, and coping
responses of preadolescent children to different dimensions of conflict. In G. W. Holden,
R. Geffner, & E. N. Jouriles (Eds.), Children exposed to marital violence: Theory,
research, and applied issues (pp. 257-288). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Lazarus, R. (1999). Stress and Emotion: A new synthesis. NY: Springer Publishing Company.
123 Lazarus, R. S. & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York: Springer.
Levine, L. J. (1995). Young children’s understanding of the causes of anger and sadness.
Child Development, 66, 697-709.
Lewis, M. (1995). Embarrassment: The emotion of self-exposure and evaluation. In J. P.
Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of shame,
guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 198-218). New York: Guilford.
Levine, S.C. (1995) Individual Difference in Characteristic Arousal Asymmetry: Implications for
Cognitive Functioning. In F.L. Kitterle, Ed., Hemispheric Communication: Mechanisms
and Models. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Litovsky, V. G., & Dusek, J. B. (1985). Perceptions of child rearing and self-concept
development during the early adolescent years. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 14,
373-387.
Lopes, P. N., Brackett, M. A., Nezlek, J. B., Schütz, A., Sellin, I., & Salovey,
P. (2004). Emotional intelligence and social interaction. Personality and Social
Psychological Bulletin, 30, 1018-1034.
Luyckx, K., Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Goossens, L., & Berzonsky, M. (2007). Parental
psychological control and dimensions of identity formation in emerging
adulthood. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 546-550.
Izard, C. E., Ackerman, B. P., & Schultz, D. (1999). Independent emotions and consciousness:
Self-consciousness and dependent emotions. In J. A. Singer & P. Singer (Eds.), At play in
the fields of consciousness: Essays in honor of Jerome L. Singer (pp. 83-102). Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent–child
interaction. In P. H. Mussen & E. M. Hetherington, Handbook of child psychology: Vol.
4. Socialization, personality, and social development (4th ed.). New York: Wiley.
Malatesta, C. Z., Culver, C., Tesman, J. R., & Shepard, B. (1989). The development of emotion
expression during the first two years of life. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Child Development, 54(1-2, Serial No. 219).
Main, M. & Hesse, E. (1990). Parents' Unresolved Traumatic Experiences are related to Infant
Disorganized Attachment Status: Is frightened/frightening parental behavior the linking
124 mechanism? In M. Greenberg, D. Cichetti & M. Cummings 5 (Eds.), Attachment in the
preschool years (pp. 161-182). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Manzeske, D. & Stright, A. D. (2009). Parenting styles and emotion regulation: The role of
behavioral and psychological control during young adulthood. Journal of Adult
Development, 16, 223-229.
Mavroveli, S. Petrides. K. V., Rieffe, C., & Bakker, F. (2007). Trait emotional intelligence,
psychological well-being, and peer-rated social competence in adolescence. British
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 263-275.
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey & D. Sluyter
(Eds.), Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Implications for educators
(pp. 3-31). New York: Basic Books.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2002). Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional
Intelligence Test: Manual. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
McDowell, D. J., Kim, M., O’Neil, R. & Parke, R. D. (2002). Children’s emotional regulation
and social competence in middle childhood: The role of maternal and paternal interactive
style. Marriage & Family Review, 34, 345-364.
McDowell, D. J., O'Neil, R., & Parke, R. D. (2000). Display rule application in a disappointing
situation and children’s emotional reactivity: Relations with social competence. MerrillPalmer Quarterly, 46(2).
McEwen, C., & Flouri, E. (2009). Fathers’ parenting, adverse life events, and adolescents’
emotional and eating disorder symptoms: the role of emotion regulation. European Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 18(4), 206-216.
Memedovic, S., Grisham, J. R., Denson, T. F., Moulds, M. L. (2010). The effects of trait
appraisal and suppression on anger and blood pressure in response to provocation.
Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 540-543.
Michael, K., Torres, A., & Seemann, E. (2007). Adolescents’ health habits, coping styles and
self-concept are predicted by exposure to interparental conflict. Journal of Divorce &
Remarriage, 30(1), 5-12.
Moilanen, K. L. (2007). The Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory: The development and
validation of a questionnaire of short-term and long-term self-regulation. Journal of
Youth and Adolescence, 36, 835-848.
125 Morris, A., Silk, J., Steinberg, L., Myers, S., & Robinson, L. (2007). The role of the family
context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16, 361-388.
Mumme, D. L., Fernald, A., & Herrera, C. (1996). Infants’ responses to facial and vocal
emotional signals in a social referencing paradigm. Child Development, 67, 3219-3237.
Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of interactions between two-montholds and their mothers. In T. M. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.) Social perception in infants.
(pp. 177-197). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Murray, L., de Rosnay, M., Pearson, J., Bergeron, C., Schofield, E., Royal-Lawson, M., et al.
(2008). Intergenerational transmission of social anxiety: The role of social referencing
processes in infancy. Child Development, 79, 1049-1064.
Ng, K., Wang, C., Kim, D., & Bodenhorn, N. (2010). Factor structure analysis of the Schutte
Self-report Emotional Intelligence Scale on international students. Educational and
Psychological Measurement, 70(4), 695-709.
O'Brien, M., Margolin, G., John, R. & Krueger, L., (1991). Mothers' and
sons' cognitive and emotional reactions to simulated marital and family
conflict. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59(5), 692-703.
O’Connor, R. M. & Little, I. S. (2003). Revisiting the predictive validity of emotional
intelligence: self-report versus ability-based measures. Personality and Individual
Differences, 35, 1893-1902.
O'Donnell, E.H., Moreau, M., Cardemil, E.V., & Pollastri, A. (2010). Interparental conflict,
parenting, and childhood depression: The role of general cognitive style. Journal of Youth
and Adolescence, 39, 12-22.
Ortner, C. N. M., & Koning, M. (2013) Effects of Regulating Positive Emotions through
Reappraisal and Suppression on Verbal and Non-Verbal Recognition Memory. PLOS
ONE 8(4): e62750. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0062750
Owen, M. T., & Cox, M. J. (1997). Marital conflict and the development of infant-parent
attachment relationships. Journal of Family Psychology, 11(2), 152-164.
Page, M., Wilhelm, M. S., Gamble, W. C., & Card, N. A. (2010). Comparison of maternal
sensitivity and verbal stimulation as unique predictors of infant social-emotional and
cognitive development. Infant Behavior and Development, 33(1), 101-110.
126 Parke, R. D. & Buriel, R. (1998). Socialization in the family: Ethnic and ecological perspectives.
In N. Eisenberg (Ed.) The handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and
personality development, Fifth Edition, Vol. 3, (pp. 463-552). New York: Wiley.
Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2000). On the dimensional structure of emotional intelligence.
Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 313-320.
Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2003). Trait emotional intelligence: Behavioural validation in
two studies of emotion recognition and reactivity to mood induction. European Journal
of Personality, 17, 39-57.
Petrides, K. V., Sangareau, Y., Furnham, A., & Frederickson , N. (2006). Trait
emotional intelligence and children’s peer relations at school. Social Development, 15,
537-547.
Pettit, G. S., & Laird, R. D. (2002). Psychological control and monitoring in early adolescence:
The role of parental involvement and earlier child adjustment. In B. K. Barber (Ed.),
Intrusive parenting: How psychological control affects children and adolescents (pp. 97123). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.
Pomerantz, E. M., & Wang, Q. (2009). The role of parents' control in children's development in
Western and East Asian countries. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 285289.
Porter, B., & O’Leary, K. D. (1980). Marital discord and childhood behavior problems. Journal
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 8, 287-295.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008) Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and
comparing indirect effects in multiple models. Behavioral Research Methods, 40(3), 879891.
Repacholi, B. M. (1998). Infants’ use of attentional cues to identify the referent of another
person’s emotional expression. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1017-1025.
Rhoades, K. A. (2008). Children's responses to interparental conflict: a meta-analysis of their
associations with child adjustment. Child Development, 79(6), 1942-1956.
Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (1999). Composure at any cost? The cognitive consequences of
emotion suppression. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1033-1044.
Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: The cognitive costs of
keeping one’s cool. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 410-424.
127 Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in
personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies.
Psychological Bulletin, 132, 1-25.
Rochat, P., Striano, T., & Blatt, L. (2002). Differential effect of happy, neutral, and sad stillfaces on 2-, 4-, and 6-month-old infants. Infant and Child Development, 11(4), 289-303.
Russo, P. M., Mancini, G., Trombini, E., Baldaro, B., Mavroveli, S., & Petrides, K. V. (2012).
Trait emotional intelligence and the Big Five: a study on Italian children and
preadolescents. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 30(3), 274-283.
Saarni, C. (1984). An observational study of children's attempts to monitor their expressive
behavior. Child Development, 55, 1504-1513.
Saarni, C., Mumme, D. L., & Campos, J. J. (1998). In William Damon & Nancy Eisenberg
(Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality
development. New York: Wiley.
Saarni, C. (2000). The social context of emotional development. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland
(Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 306–322). New York: Guilford Press.
Saarni, C., Campos, J. J., Camras, L., & Witherington, D. (2006). Emotional development:
Action, communication, and understanding. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (series eds.)
and N. Eisenberg (vol. ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and
personality development (6th ed., pp. 226- 299). New York: Wiley.
Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, and
Personality, 9, 185-211.
Schaefer, E. S. & Bell, R. Q. (1958). Development of a parental attitude research instrument.
Child Development, 29, 339-361.
Schaefer, E. S. (1965). A configurational analysis of children's reports of parent behavior.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 29, 552-557.
Schludermann, E., & Schludermann, S. (1988). Questionnaire for children and youth.
Unpublished manuscript.
Schulenberg, J. E. & Zarrett, N. (2006). Mental health during emerging adulthood: continuity
and discontinuity in courses, causes, and functions. In J. J. Arnett and J. L. Tanner (Eds.),
Emerging adults in America: Coming of age in the 21st century. Washington, DC:
American Psychiatric Association.
128 Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J., et al.
(1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Personality
and Individual Differences, 25, 167-177.
Scott, S., Briskman, J., Woolgar, M., Humayun, S., & O’Connor, T. G. (2011). Attachment in
adolescence: Overlap with parenting and unique prediction of behavioral adjustment.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(10), 1052-1062.
Sheeber, L., Allen, N., Davis, B., & Sorensen, E. (2000). Regulation of negative affect during
mother-child problem-solving interactions: adolescent depressive status and family
processes. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 467-479.
Shelton, K. H., & Harold, G. T. (2007). Marital conflict and children's adjustment: The
mediating and moderating role of children's coping strategies. Social Development, 16(3),
487-512.
Shelton, K. H., & Harold, G.T. (2008). Pathways between interparental conflict and adolescent
psychological adjustment: Bridging links through children’s cognitive appraisals and
coping strategies. Journal of Early Adolescence, 28, 555-582.
Shoben, E. J. (1949). Psychotherapy as a problem in learning theory. Psychological Bulletin, 46,
366-392.
Shortt, J. W., Stoolmiller, M., Smith-Shine, J. N., Eddy, J. M., & Sheeber, L. (2010). Maternal
emotion coaching, adolescent anger regulation, and siblings' externalizing symptoms.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(7), 799-808.
Shpigel, M. S., Diamond, G. M., & Diamond, G. S. (2012). Changes in parenting behaviors,
attachment, depressive symptoms, and suicidal ideation in attachment-based family
therapy for depressive and suicidal adolescents. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy,
38(1), 271-283.
Silk, J. S., Morris, A.S., Kanaya, T., & Steinberg, L. (2003). Psychological control and autonomy
granting: Opposite ends of a continuum or distinct constructs? Journal of Research on
Adolescence, 13(1), 113-128.
Silk, J. S., Shaw, D. S., Skuban, E., Oland, A., & Kovacs, M. (2006). Emotion regulation
strategies in children of mothers with childhood-onset depression. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 69-78.
129 Skinner, E. A., Johnson, S. J., & Snyder, T. (2005). Six dimensions of parenting: A motivational
model. Parenting: Science and Practice, 2, 175-235.
Smrtnik Vitulic, H. (2009). The development of understanding of basic emotions from middle
childhood to adolescence. Studia Psychologica, 51(1), 3-20.
Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2010). A theoretical upgrade of the concept of parental
psychological control: Proposing new insights on the basis of self-determination theory.
Developmental Review, 30, 74-99.
Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2000). Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation. Child Development, 71,
1072-1085.
Stein, N. L., & Levine, L. J. (1999). The early emergence of emotional understanding and
appraisal: Implications for theories of development. In T. Dalgleish & M. J. Power
(Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 383-408). Chichester, England and New
York: Wiley.
Stein, N. L., & Trabasso, T. (1989). Children's understanding of changing emotional states. In C.
Saarni & P. L. Harris (Eds.), Children's understanding of emotion. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Steinberg, L. (1990). Autonomy, conflict, and harmony in the family relationship. In Feldman, S.
S. and Elliott, G. R. (Eds.). At the threshold: The developing adolescent (pp. 255-276).
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S., Dornbusch, S., & Darling, N. (1992). Impact of parenting practices
on adolescent achievement: Authoritative parenting, school involvement, encouragement
to succeed. Child Development, 63, 1266-1281.
Stephanie, B., Stephanie, T., Geoffrey, B., & Sarah-Jayne, B. (2011). Pubertal development of
the understanding of social emotions: Implications of education. Learning and Individual
Differences, 21(6), 681-689.
Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The Conflict Tactics Scales.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75-88.
Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. (2004). Empathy and observed anger and aggression in five-year-olds.
Social Development, 13(1), 1-13.
Sturge-Apple, M.L., Davies, P.T., Winter, M.A., Cummings, E.M., & Schermerhorn, A. (2008).
Interparental conflict and children's school adjustment: The explanatory role of children's
130 internal representations of interparental and parent-child relationships. Developmental
Psychology, 44, 1678-1690.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using Multivariate Statistics (5th ed). Boston: Allyn
and Bacon.
Tanner, J. L. (2006). Recentering during emerging adulthood: A critical turning point in life span
human development. In J. J. Arnett and J. L. Tanner (Eds.), Emerging adults in America:
Coming of age in the 21st century. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Tanner, J. L., & Arnett, J. J. (2009). The emergence of “emerging adulthood”: The new life stage
between adolescence and young adulthood, In A. Furlong (Ed.), Handbook of Youth and
Young Adulthood. London, UK: Routledge.
Tanner, J. L., Reinherz, H. Z., Beardslee, W. R., Fitzmaurice, G. M., Leis, J. A., & Berger, S. R.
(2007). Change in prevalence of psychiatric disorders from ages 21 to 30 in a community
sample. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195, 298-306.
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. In N. Fox (Ed.),
The development of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2-3, Serial 240).
Thompson, R. A. (2000). Childhood anxiety disorders from the perspective of emotion
regulation and attachment. In M. W. Vasey & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The developmental
psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 160-182). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Thompson, R., Easterbrooks, M.A., & Padilla-Walker, L. (2003). Social and emotional
development in infancy. In I.B. Weiner (Ed.), Handbook of psychology: Vol. 6.
Developmental Psychology. (pp. 91-112). New York: Wiley.
Thompson, R.A. & Meyer, S. (2007). The socialization of emotion regulation in the family. In J.
Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp.249-268). New York: Guilford.
Walden, T.A. (1991). Infant social referencing. In J. Garber and K.A. Dodge (Eds). The
development of emotion regulation and dysregulation, pp. 69-87. Cambridge University
Press.
Walton, A., & Flouri, E. (2010). Contextual risk, maternal parenting and adolescent externalizing
behaviour problems: the role of emotion regulation. Child: Care, Health and
Development, 36(2), 275-284.
131 Wang, Q., Pomerantz, E. M., & Chen, H. (2007). The role of parents' control in early
adolescents' psychological functioning: A longitudinal investigation in the United States
and China. Child Development, 78, 1592-1610.
Weinberg, M. K., Tronick, E. Z., Cohn, J. F., & Olson, K. L. (1999). Gender differences in
emotional expressivity and self-regulation during early infancy. Developmental
Psychology, 35(1), 175-188.
Williams, L. M., Brown, K. J., Palmer, D., Liddell, B. J., Kemp, A. H., Olivieri, G., et al. (2006).
The mellow years? Neural basis of improving emotional stability over age. The Journal
of Neuroscience, 26, 6422-6430.
Witherington, D.C., Campos, J.J., & Hertenstein, M.J. (2001). Principles of emotion and its
development in infancy. In G. Bremner & A. Fogel (Eds.), The Blackwell handbook of
infant development (pp. 427-464). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Wolfradt, U., Hempel, S. & Miles, J. (2003). Perceived parenting styles depersonalisation,
anxiety and coping behaviour in adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 34,
521-532.
Yap, M., Allen. N. B., & Ladouceur, C. (2008). Maternal socialization of positive affect: The
impact of “dampening” on adolescent emotion regulation and depressive
symptomatology. Child Development, 79, 1415-1431.
Zeman, J., Cassano, M., Perry-Parrish, C., & Stegall, S. (2006). Emotion regulation in children
and adolescents. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2), 155-168.
132 Appendix A
Emotion Regulation
(Emotional Intelligence Scale; Schutte et al., 1998)
In this section, you will be asked about yourself. Please indicate how true each statement is for
you.
1 = Not at all true, 2 = Slightly untrue, 3 = Neither true nor untrue, 4 = Slightly true, 5 = Very
true
1. I know when to speak about my personal problems to others.
2. When I am faced with obstacles, I remember times I faced similar obstacles and
overcame them.
3. I expect that I will do well on most things I try.
4. Other people find it easy to confide in me.
5. I find it hard to understand the non-verbal messages of other people.
6. Some of the major events of my life have led me to re-evaluate what is important and not
important.
7. When my mood changes, I see new possibilities.
8. Emotions are one of the things that make my life worth living.
9. I am aware of my emotions as I experience them.
10. I expect good things to happen.
11. I like to share my emotions with others.
12. When I experience a positive emotion, I know how to make it last.
13. I arrange events others enjoy.
14. I seek out activities that make me happy.
15. I am aware of the non-verbal messages I send to others.
16. I present myself in a way that makes a good impression on others.
17. When I am in a positive mood, solving problems is easy for me.
18. By looking at their facial expressions, I recognize the emotions people are experiencing.
19. I know why my emotions change.
20. When I am in a positive mood, I am able to come up with new ideas.
21. I have control over my emotions.
22. I easily recognize my emotions as I experience them.
23. I motivate myself by imagining a good outcome to tasks I take on.
24. I compliment others when they have done something well.
25. I am aware of the non-verbal messages other people send.
26. When another person tells me about an important event in his or her life, I almost feel as
though I have experienced this event myself.
27. When I feel a change in emotions, I tend to come up with new ideas.
28. When I am faced with a challenge, I give up because I believe I will fail.
133 29. I know what other people are feeling just by looking at them.
30. I help other people feel better when they are down.
31. I use good moods to help myself keep trying in the face of obstacles.
32. I can tell how people are feeling by listing to the tone of their voice.
33. It is difficult for me to understand why people feel the way they do.
Note. Items on the emotion regulation factor and used in the analyses are shown in boldface.
134 Appendix B
Interparental Conflict
(Children’s Perceptions of Interparental Conflict Scale; Grych, Seid, & Fincham, 1992)
I live with _____ both my mom and my dad
_____ only one of my parents
_____ another relative (e.g., grandparents, aunt)
How often do you see your parents?
_____less than once a semester
_____ more than once a semester
_____once a month
_____once a week
_____every day
In every family there are times when the parents don't get along. When their parents argue or
disagree, kids can feel a lot of different ways. We would like to know what kind of feelings you
have when your parents have arguments or disagreements. If your parents don't live together in
the same house with you, think about times that they are together when they don't agree or about
times when both of your parents lived in the same house, when you answer these questions.
1 = True, 2 = Sort of true, 3 = False
Conflict Properties
Frequency
1. I never see my parents arguing or disagreeing.
2. They may not think I know it, but my parents argue or disagree a lot.
3. My parents are often mean to each other even when I'm around.
4. I often see my parents arguing.
5. My parents hardly ever argue.
6. My parents often nag and complain about each other around the house.
Intensity
7. My parents get really mad when they argue.
8. When my parents have a disagreement they discuss it quietly.
9. When my parents have an argument they say mean things to each other.
10. When my parents have an argument they yell a lot.
11. My parents hardly ever yell when they have a disagreement.
12. My parents have broken or thrown things during an argument.
13. My parents have pushed or shoved each other during an argument.
135 Resolution
14. When my parents have an argument they usually work it out.
15. Even after my parents stop arguing they stay mad at each other.
16. When my parents disagree about something, they usually come up with a solution.
17. When my parents argue they usually make up right away.
18. After my parents stop arguing, they are friendly toward each other.
19. My parents still act mean after they have had an argument.
136 Appendix C
Psychological Control
(Barber, 1996)
In this section, you will be asked about your parents. Please indicate how true each statement is
for you.
1 = Not at all true, 2 = Slightly untrue, 3 = Neither true nor untrue, 4 = Slightly true, 5 = Very
true
1. My parents change the subject, whenever I have something to say.
2. My parents finish my sentences whenever I talk.
3. My parents often interrupt me.
4. My parents act like they know what I’m thinking or feeling.
5. My parents would like to be able to tell me how to feel or think about things all the time.
6. My parents are always trying to change how I feel or think about things.
7. My parents blame me for other family members’ problems.
8. My parents bring up my past mistakes when they criticize me.
137 Appendix D
Psychological Autonomy Granting
(Silk et al., 2003)
In this section, you will be asked about your parents. Please indicate how true each statement is
for you.
1 = Not at all true, 2 = Slightly untrue, 3 = Neither true nor untrue, 4 = Slightly true, 5 = Very
true
1. My parents emphasize that every member of the family should have some say in family
decisions.
2. My parents emphasize that it is important to get my ideas across even if others don’t like it.
3. My parents say that you should always look at both sides of the issue.
4. My parents talk at home about things like politics or religion, where one takes a different side
from others.
5. My parents keep pushing me to think independently.
6. My parents let me make my own plans for things I want to do.
7. When I get a good grade, my parents give me more freedom to make my own decisions.
8. My parents admit that I know more about some things than adults do.
138 Appendix E
Behavioral Control
(Stattin & Kerr, 2000)
In this section, you will be asked about your parents. Please indicate how true each statement is
for you.
1 = Never, 2 = Seldom, 3 = Sometimes, 4 = often, 5 = Very often
1. How often do your parents ask you about your activities outside of classes?
2. How often do your parents ask you to tell them what happens on campus?
3. How often do your parents initiate a conversation with you about things you do with your
friends?
4. How often do your parents ask you about how you spend your money?
5. How often do your parents initiate a conversation with you about what happens during your
free time?
6. How often do your parents ask you about places you go with your friends?
7. How often do your parents initiate a conversation with you about how your schoolwork is
going?
8. How often do your parents ask you about your grades?
9. How often do your parents require you to ask for their permission before you go out?
10. How often do your parents often require you to tell them where you are going and what you
are doing before you go out?
11. How often do your parents require you to speak with them before you decide on plans for
weekends with your friends?
12. How often do your parents require you to tell them where you are going and what you are
doing before you go out on weekends?
13. How often do your parents require you to explain where you went and what you did if you
come home late?
14. How often do your parents require you to let them know how you are doing in school?
15. How often do your parents require you to let them know how much time you spend studying?
16. How often do your parents require you to let them know your grades on your schoolwork?
139 Appendix F
Parent-Child Attachment
(The Inventory for Parent and Peer Attachment; Armsden & Greenberg, 1987)
Please indicate how true the following statements are for you.
1= Almost never or never true, 2= Seldom true, 3= Sometimes true, 4= Often true, 5= Almost
always or always true
Trust
1. My parents respect my feelings.
2. I feel my parents are successful as parents.
3. I wish I had different parents.
4. My parents accept me as I am.
13. When we discuss things, my parents consider my point of view.
14. My parents trust my judgment.
21. My parents understand me.
23. When I am angry about something, my parents try to be understanding.
24. I trust my parents.
Communication
6. I like to get my parents’ point of view on things I’m concerned about.
7. I feel it’s no use letting my feelings show.
8. My parents sense when I’m upset about something.
15. My parents have their own problems, so I don’t bother them with mine.
16. My parents help me to understand myself better.
17. I tell my parents about my problems and troubles.
20. My parents encourage me to talk about my difficulties.
26. I can count on my parents when I need to get something off my chest.
28. If my parents know something is bothering me, they ask me about it.
Alienation
5. I have to rely on myself when I have a problem to solve.
9. Talking over my problems with my parents makes me feel ashamed or foolish.
10. My parents expect too much from me.
11. I get upset easily at home.
12. I get upset a lot more than my parents know about.
18. I feel angry with my parents.
19. I don’t get much attention at home.
22. I don’t know whom I can depend on these days.
25. My parents don’t understand what I am going through these days.
27. I feel that no one understands me.
Download