How Much Can We Generalize from Impact Evaluations? Eva Vivalt

advertisement
How Much Can We Generalize from Impact Evaluations?
Eva Vivalt∗
New York University
April 30, 2015
Abstract
Impact evaluations aim to predict the future, but they are rooted in particular
contexts and results may not generalize across settings. I founded an organization to
systematically collect and synthesize impact evaluation results on a wide variety of
interventions in development. These data allow me to answer this and other questions
for the first time using a large data set of studies. I examine whether results predict
each other and whether variance in results can be explained by program characteristics,
such as who is implementing them, where they are being implemented, the scale of
the program, and what methods are used. I find that when regressing an estimate
on the hierarchical Bayesian meta-analysis result formed from all other studies on the
same intervention-outcome combination, the result is significant with a coefficient of
0.5-0.7, though the R2 is very low. The program implementer is the main source of
heterogeneity in results, with government-implemented programs faring worse than and
being poorly predicted by the smaller studies typically implemented by academic/NGO
research teams, even controlling for sample size. I then turn to examine specification
searching and publication bias, issues which could affect generalizability and are also
important for research credibility. I demonstrate that these biases are quite small;
nevertheless, to address them, I discuss a mathematical correction that could be applied
before showing that randomized controlled trials (RCTs) are less prone to this type of
bias and exploiting them as a robustness check.
∗
E-mail: eva.vivalt@nyu.edu. I thank Edward Miguel, Bill Easterly, David Card, Ernesto Dal Bó, Hunt Allcott,
Elizabeth Tipton, David McKenzie, Vinci Chow, Willa Friedman, Xing Huang, Michaela Pagel, Steven Pennings,
Edson Severnini, seminar participants at the University of California, Berkeley, Columbia University, New York
University, the World Bank, Cornell University, Princeton University, the University of Toronto, the London School
of Economics, among others, and participants at the 2015 ASSA meeting and 2013 Association for Public Policy
Analysis and Management Fall Research Conference for helpful comments. I am also grateful for the hard work put
in by many at AidGrade over the duration of this project, including but not limited to Jeff Qiu, Bobbie Macdonald,
Diana Stanescu, Cesar Augusto Lopez, Jennifer Ambrose, Naomi Crowther, Timothy Catlett, Joohee Kim, Gautam
Bastian, Christine Shen, Taha Jalil, Risa Santoso and Catherine Razeto.
1
1
Introduction
In the last few years, impact evaluations have become extensively used in development eco-
nomics research. Policymakers and donors typically fund impact evaluations precisely to figure out
how effective a similar program would be in the future to guide their decisions on what course
of action they should take. However, it is not yet clear how much we can extrapolate from past
results or under which conditions. Further, there is some evidence that even a similar program,
in a similar environment, can yield different results. For example, Bold et al. (2013) carry out an
impact evaluation of a program to provide contract teachers in Kenya; this was a scaled-up version
of an earlier program studied by Duflo, Dupas and Kremer (2012). The earlier intervention studied
by Duflo, Dupas and Kremer was implemented by an NGO, while Bold et al. compared implementation by an NGO and the government. While Duflo, Dupas and Kremer found positive effects,
Bold et al. showed significant results only for the NGO-implemented group. The different findings
in the same country for purportedly similar programs point to the substantial context-dependence
of impact evaluation results. Knowing this context-dependence is crucial in order to understand
what we can learn from any impact evaluation.
While the main reason to examine generalizability is to aid interpretation and improve predictions, it would also help to direct research attention to where it is most needed. If generalizability
were higher in some areas, fewer papers would be needed to understand how people would behave
in a similar situation; conversely, if there were topics or regions where generalizability was low,
it would call for further study. With more information, researchers can better calibrate where to
direct their attentions to generate new insights.
It is well-known that impact evaluations only happen in certain contexts. For example, Figure
1 shows a heat map of the geocoded impact evaluations in the data used in this paper overlaid by
the distribution of World Bank projects (black dots). Both sets of data are geographically clustered, and whether or not we can reasonably extrapolate from one to another depends on how much
related heterogeneity there is in treatment effects. Allcott (forthcoming) recently showed that site
selection bias was an issue for randomized controlled trials (RCTs) on a firm’s energy conservation
programs. Microfinance institutions that run RCTs and hospitals that conduct clinical trials are
also selected (Allcott, forthcoming), and World Bank projects that receive an impact evaluation
2
Figure 1: Growth of Impact Evaluations and Location Relative to Programs
The figure on the left shows a heat map of the impact evaluations in AidGrade’s database overlaid by black dots
indicating where the World Bank has done projects. While there are many other development programs not done
by the World Bank, this figure illustrates the great numbers and geographical dispersion of development programs.
The figure on the right plots the number of studies that came out in each year that are contained in each of three
databases described in the text: 3ie’s title/abstract/keyword database of impact evaluations; J-PAL’s database of
affiliated randomized controlled trials; and AidGrade’s database of impact evaluation results data.
are different from those that do not (Vivalt, 2015). Others have sought to explain heterogeneous
treatment effects in meta-analyses of specific topics (e.g. Saavedra and Garcia, 2013, among many
others for conditional cash transfers), or to argue they are so heterogeneous they cannot be adequately modelled (e.g. Deaton, 2011; Pritchett and Sandefur, 2013).
Impact evaluations are still exponentially increasing in number and in terms of the resources
devoted to them. The World Bank recently received a major grant from the UK aid agency DFID
to expand its already large impact evaluation works; the Millennium Challenge Corporation has
committed to conduct rigorous impact evaluations for 50% of its activities, with “some form of
credible evaluation of impact” for every activity (Millennium Challenge Corporation, 2009); and
the U.S. Agency for International Development is also increasingly invested in impact evaluations,
coming out with a new policy in 2011 that directs 3% of program funds to evaluation.1
Yet while impact evaluations are still growing in development, a few thousand are already
complete. Figure 1 plots the explosion of RCTs that researchers affiliated with J-PAL, a center for development economics research, have completed each year; alongside are the number of
development-related impact evaluations released that year according to 3ie, which keeps a direc1
While most of these are less rigorous “performance evaluations”, country mission leaders are supposed to identify
at least one opportunity for impact evaluation for each development objective in their 3-5 year plans (USAID, 2011).
3
tory of titles, abstracts, and other basic information on impact evaluations more broadly, including
quasi-experimental designs; finally, the dashed line shows the number of papers that came out in
each year that are included in AidGrade’s database of impact evaluation results, which will be
described shortly.
To summarize, while we do impact evaluation to figure out what will happen in the future,
many issues have been raised about how well we can extrapolate from past impact evaluations,
and despite the importance of the topic, previously we were unable to do little more than guess or
examine the question in narrow settings as we did not have the data. Now we have the opportunity
to address speculation, drawing on a large, unique dataset of impact evaluation results.
I founded a non-profit organization dedicated to gathering this data. That organization, AidGrade, seeks to systematically understand which programs work best where, a task that requires
also knowing the limits of our knowledge. To date, AidGrade has conducted 20 meta-analyses
and systematic reviews of different development programs.2 Data gathered through meta-analyses
are the ideal data to answer the question of how much we can extrapolate from past results, and
since data on these 20 topics were collected in the same way, coding the same outcomes and other
variables, we can look across different types of programs to see if there are any more general trends.
Currently, the data set contains 637 papers on 210 narrowly-defined intervention-outcome combinations, with the greater database containing 14,993 estimates.
The data further allow me to examine a second set of questions revolving around specification
searching and publication bias. Specification searching refers to the practice whereby researchers
artificially select results that meet the criterion for being considered statistically significant, biasing
results. It has been found to be a systematic problem by Gerber and Malhotra in the political science
and sociology literature (2008a; 2008b); Simmons and Simonsohn (2011) and Bastardi, Uhlmann
and Ross (2011) in psychology; and Brodeur et al. (2012) in economics. I look for evidence of
specification searching and publication bias in this large data set, both for its own importance as
well as because it is possible that results may appear to be more “generalizable” merely because
they suffer from a common bias.
I pay particular attention to randomized controlled trials (RCTs), which are considered the
2
Throughout, I will refer to all 20 as meta-analyses, but some did not have enough comparable outcomes for
meta-analysis and became systematic reviews.
4
“gold standard” in the sciences and on which development economics has increasingly relied. It is
possible that the method may reduce specification searching due to its emphasis on rigor or the
increased odds of publication independent of results. It is also possible that RCTs may be done in
more selected settings, leading to results not generalizing as well. I will shed light on both of these
potential issues.
The outline of this paper is as follows. First, I define generalizability, present some basic statistics about it, and use leave-one-out cross-validation to check what kinds of study characteristics
can help predict another study’s results better than placebo data. I also conduct leave-one-out hierarchical Bayesian meta-analyses of all but one result within an intervention-outcome combination
and check to what extent these meta-analysis results, which theoretically might provide the best
estimates of a given program’s effects, predict the result left out. Since some of the analyses will
draw upon statistical methods not commonly used in economics, I will use the concrete example of
conditional cash transfers (CCTs), which are relatively well-understood and on which many papers
have been written, to elucidate the issues.
Regarding specification searching, I first conduct caliper tests on the distribution of z-statistics,
seeing whether there is a disproportionate number of papers just above the threshold for statistical
significance compared to those just below the threshold. The data contain both published papers
and unpublished working papers, and I examine how much publication bias there appears to be for
results that are significant.
After examining how much these biases are present, I discuss how one might correct for them
when considering generalizability. There is a simple mathematical adjustment that could be made
if one were willing to accept the constraints of a fixed effects meta-analysis model.3 However, I
show this would not be an appropriate model for these data. Instead, I turn to using RCTs, which
I show do not suffer as much from these biases, as a robustness check.
While this paper focuses on results for impact evaluations of development programs, this is only
one of the first areas within economics to which these kinds of methods can be applied. In many of
the sciences, knowledge is built through a combination of researchers conducting individual studies
and other researchers synthesizing the evidence through meta-analysis. This paper begins that
natural next step.
3
In particular, Simonsohn et al. (2014) make the assumption of fixed effects.
5
2
Theory
2.1
Heterogeneous Treatment Effects
I model treatment effects as potentially depending on the context of the intervention. Each
impact evaluation is on a particular intervention and covers a number of outcomes. The relationship
between an outcome, the inputs that were part of the intervention, and the context of the study
is complex. In the simplest model, we can imagine that context can be represented a “contextual
variable”, C, such that:
Zj “ α ` βTj ` δCj ` γTj Cj ` εj
(1)
where j indexes the individual, Z represents the value of an aggregate outcome such as “enrollment
rates”, T indicates being treated, and C represents a contextual variable, such as the type of agency
that implemented the program.4
In this framework, a particular impact evaluation might explicitly estimate:
Zj “ α ` β 1 Tj ` εj
(2)
but, as Equation 1 can be re-written as Zj “ α ` pβ ` γCj qTj ` δCj ` εj , what β 1 is really capturing
is the effect β 1 “ β ` γC. When C varies, unobserved, in different contexts, the variance of β 1
increases.
This is the simplest case. One can imagine that the true state of the world has “interaction
effects all the way down”.
Interaction terms are often considered a second-order problem. However, that intuition could
stem from the fact that we usually look for interaction terms within an already fairly homogeneous
dataset - e.g. data from a single country, at a single point in time, on a particularly selected sample.
Not all aspects of context need matter to an intervention’s outcomes. The set of contextual
variables can be divided into a critical set on which outcomes depend and an set on which they do
not; I will ignore the latter. Further, the relationship between Z and C can vary by intervention or
outcome. For example, school meals programs might have more of an effect on younger children,
4
Z can equally well be thought of as the average individual outcome for an intervention. Throughout, I take high
values for an outcome to represent a beneficial change unless otherwise noted; if an outcome represents a negative
characteristic, like incidence of a disease, its sign will be flipped before analysis.
6
but scholarship programs could plausibly affect older children more. If one were to regress effect size
on the contextual variable “age”, we would get different results depending on which intervention
and outcome we were considering. Therefore, it will be important in this paper to look only at a
restricted set of contextual variables which could plausibly work in a similar way across different
interventions. Additional analysis could profitably be done within some interventions, but this is
outside the scope of this paper.
Generalizability will ultimately depend on the heterogeneity of treatment effects. The next
section formally defines generalizability for use in this paper.
2.2
Generalizability: Definitions and Measurement
Definition 1 Generalizability is the ability to predict results accurately out of sample.
Definition 2 Local generalizability is the ability to predict results accurately in a particular outof-sample group.
Any empirical work, including this paper, will only be able to address local generalizability.
However, I will argue we should not be concerned about this. First, the meta-analysis data were
explicitly gathered using very broad inclusion criteria, aiming to capture the universe of studies.
Second, by using a large set of studies in various contexts and repeatedly leaving out some of the
studies when generating predictions, we can estimate the sensitivity of results to the inclusion of
particular papers. In particular, as part of this paper I will systematically leave out one of the
studies, predict it based on the other studies, and do this for each study, cycling through the studies. As a robustness check I do this for different subsets of the data.
There are several ways to measure predictive power. Most measures of predictive power rely on
building a model on a “training” data set and estimating the fit of that model on an out-of-sample
“test” data set, or estimating on the same data set but with a correction. Gelman et al. (2013)
provides a good summary. I will focus on one of these methods - cross-validation. In particular,
I will use the predicted residual sum of squares (PRESS) statistic, which is closely related to the
mean squared error and can also be used to generate an R2 -like statistic. Specifically, to calculate
it one follows this procedure:
7
1. Start at study i “ 1 within each intervention-outcome combination.
2. Generate the predicted value of effect size Yi , Ypi , by building a model based on Y´i , the effect
sizes for all observations in that intervention-outcome except i. For example, regress Y´i “
α`βC´i `ε, where C represents a predictor of interest, and then use the estimated coefficients
to predict Ypi . Alternatively, generate Ypi as the meta-analysis result from synthesizing Y´i .
3. Calculate the squared error, pYi ´ Ypi q2 .
4. Repeat for each i ‰ 1.
5. Calculate PRESS “
ř
n pYi
´ Ypi q2
To aid interpretation, I also calculate the PRESS statistic for placebo data in simulations. The
models for predicting Yi are intentionally simple, as both economists and policymakers often do
not build complicated models of the effect sizes when drawing inferences from past studies. First,
I simply see if interventions, outcomes, intervention-outcomes, region or implementer have any
explanatory power in predicting the resultant effect size. I also try using the meta-analysis result
M´i , obtained from synthesizing the effect sizes Y´i , as the predictor of Yi . This makes intuitive
sense; we are often in the situation of wanting to predict the effect size in a new context from a
meta-analysis result.
When calculating the PRESS statistic, I correct the results for attenuation bias using a firstorder approximation described in Gelman et al. (2013) and Tibshirani and Tibshirani (2009).
y “ CV ´ CV.
Ě
Representing the cross-validation squared error pYi ´ Ypi q2 as CV, bias
While predictive power is perhaps the most natural measure of generalizability, I will also
show results on how impact evaluation results correlate with each other. The difference between
correlation and predictive power is clear: it is similar to the difference between an estimated
coefficient and an R2 . Impact evaluation results could be correlated so that regressing Yi on
explanatory variables like M´i could result in a large, significant coefficient on the M´i term while
still having a low R2 . Indeed, this is what we will see in the data.
To create the meta-analysis result M´i , I use a hierarchical Bayesian random effects model with
an uninformative prior, as described in the next section on meta-analysis.
8
2.3
Models Used in Meta-Analyses
This paper uses meta-analysis as a tool to synthesize evidence.
As a quick review, there are many steps in a meta-analysis, most of which have to do with the
selection of the constituent papers. The search and screening of papers will be described in the
data section; here, I merely discuss the theory behind how meta-analyses combine results.
One of two main statistical models underlie almost all meta-analyses: the fixed-effect model
or the random-effects model. Fixed-effect models assume there is one true effect of a particular
program and all differences between studies can be attributed simply to sampling error. In other
words:
Yi “ θ ` ε i
(3)
where θ is the true effect and εi is the error term.
Random-effects models do not make this assumption; the true effect could potentially vary from
context to context. Here,
Yi “ θi ` εi
“ θ̄ ` ηi ` εi
(4)
(5)
where θi is the effect size for a particular study i, θ̄ is the mean true effect size, ηi is a particular
study’s divergence from that mean true effect size, and εi is the error.
When estimating either a fixed effect or random effects model through meta-analysis, a choice
must be made: how to weight the studies that serve as inputs to the meta-analysis. Several
weighting schemes can be used, but by far the most common to use are inverse-variance weights.
As the variance is a measure of how certain we are of the effect, this ensures that those results about
which we are more confident get weighted more heavily. The variance will contain a between-studies
term in the case of random effects. Writing the weights as W , the summary effect is simply:
řk
M “ ři“1
k
W i Yi
i“1 Wi
9
(6)
with standard error
b
řk 1
i“1
Wi
.
To build a hierarchical Bayesian model, I first assume the data are normally distributed:
Yij |θi „ N pθi , σ 2 q
(7)
where j indexes the individuals in the study. I do not have individual-level data, but instead can
use sufficient statistics:
Yi |θi „ N pθi , σi2 q
(8)
where Yi is the sample mean and σi2 the sample variance. This provides the likelihood for θi .
I also need a prior for θi . I assume between-study normality:
θi „ N pµ, τ 2 q
(9)
where µ and τ are unknown hyperparameters.
Conditioning on the distribution of the data, given by Equation 8, I get a posterior:
θi |µ, τ, Y „ N pθˆi , Vi q
where
θˆi “
Yi
σi2
1
σi2
`
`
µ
τ2
1
τ2
, Vi “
1
σi2
1
`
(10)
1
τ2
(11)
I then need to pin down µ|τ and τ by constructing their posterior distributions given noninformative priors and updating based on the data. I assume a uniform prior for µ|τ , and as the
Yi are estimates of µ with variance pσi2 ` τ 2 q, obtain:
µ|τ, Y „ N pµ̂, Vµ q
where
Yi
i σi2 `τ 2
ř
1
i σi2 `τ 2
(12)
ř
µ̂ “
, Vµ “
10
ÿ
1
i
1
σi2 `τ 2
(13)
For τ , note that ppτ |Y q “
ppµ,τ |Y q
ppµ|τ,Y q .
The denominator follows from Equation 12; for the numer-
ator, we can observe that ppµ, τ |Y q is proportional to ppµ, τ qppY |µ, τ q, and we know the marginal
distribution of Yi |µ, τ :
Yi |µ, τ „ N pµ, σi2 ` τ 2 q
(14)
I use a uniform prior for τ , following Gelman et al. (2005). This yields the posterior for the
numerator:
ppµ, τ |Y q9ppµ, τ q
ź
N pYi |µ, σi2 ` τ 2 q
(15)
i
Putting together all the pieces in reverse order, I first simulate τ , then generate ppτ |Y q using
τ , followed by µ and finally θi .
Unless otherwise noted, I rely on this hierarchical Bayesian random effects model to generate
meta-analysis results.
3
Data
This paper uses a database of impact evaluation results collected by AidGrade, a U.S. non-profit
research institute that I founded in 2012. AidGrade focuses on gathering the results of impact evaluations and analyzing the data, including through meta-analysis. Its data on impact evaluation
results were collected in the course of its meta-analyses from 2012-2014 (AidGrade, 2015).
AidGrade’s meta-analyses follow the standard stages: (1) topic selection; (2) a search for relevant papers; (3) screening of papers; (4) data extraction; and (5) data analysis. In addition, it
pays attention to (6) dissemination and (7) updating of results. Here, I will discuss the selection of
papers (stages 1-3) and the data extraction protocol (stage 4); more detail is provided in Appendix
B.
3.1
Selection of Papers
The interventions that were selected for meta-analysis were selected largely on the basis of there
being a sufficient number of studies on that topic. Five AidGrade staff members each independently
made a preliminary list of interventions for examination; the lists were then combined and searches
done for each topic to determine if there were likely to be enough impact evaluations for a meta-
11
analysis. The remaining list was voted on by the general public online and partially randomized.
Appendix B provides further detail.
A comprehensive literature search was done using a mix of the search aggregators SciVerse,
Google Scholar, and EBSCO/PubMed. The online databases of J-PAL, IPA, CEGA and 3ie were
also searched for completeness. Finally, the references of any existing systematic reviews or metaanalyses were collected.
Any impact evaluation which appeared to be on the intervention in question was included,
barring those in developed countries.5 Any paper that tried to consider the counterfactual was
considered an impact evaluation. Both published papers and working papers were included. The
search and screening criteria were deliberately broad. There is not enough room to include the full
text of the search terms and inclusion criteria for all 20 topics in this paper, but these are available
in an online appendix as detailed in Appendix A.
3.2
Data Extraction
The subset of the data on which I am focusing is based on those papers that passed all screening
stages in the meta-analyses. Again, the search and screening criteria were very broad and, after
passing the full text screening, the vast majority of papers that were later excluded were excluded
merely because they had no outcome variables in common or did not provide adequate data (for
example, not providing data that could be used to calculate the standard error of an estimate, or for
a variety of other quirky reasons, such as displaying results only graphically). The small overlap of
outcome variables is a surprising and notable feature of the data. Ultimately, the data I draw upon
for this paper consist of 14,993 results (double-coded and then reconciled by a third researcher)
across 637 papers covering the 20 types of development program listed in Table 1.6 For sake of
comparison, though the two organizations clearly do different things, at present time of writing this
is more impact evaluations than J-PAL has published, concentrated in these 20 topics. Unfortunately, only 318 of these papers both overlapped in outcomes with another paper and were able to
5
High-income countries, according to the World Bank’s classification system.
Three titles here may be misleading. “Mobile phone-based reminders” refers specifically to SMS or voice reminders for health-related outcomes. “Women’s empowerment programs” required an educational component to be
included in the intervention and it could not be an unrelated intervention that merely disaggregated outcomes by
gender. Finally, micronutrients were initially too loosely defined; this was narrowed down to focus on those providing
zinc to children, but the other micronutrient papers are still included in the data, with a tag, as they may still be
useful.
6
12
be standardized and thus included in the main results which rely on intervention-outcome groups.
Outcomes were defined under several rules of varying specificity, as will be discussed shortly.
Table 1: List of Development Programs Covered
2012
Conditional cash transfers
Deworming
Improved stoves
Insecticide-treated bed nets
Microfinance
Safe water storage
Scholarships
School meals
Unconditional cash transfers
Water treatment
2013
Contract teachers
Financial literacy training
HIV education
Irrigation
Micro health insurance
Micronutrient supplementation
Mobile phone-based reminders
Performance pay
Rural electrification
Women’s empowerment programs
73 variables were coded for each paper. Additional topic-specific variables were coded for some
sets of papers, such as the median and mean loan size for microfinance programs. This paper
focuses on the variables held in common across the different topics. These include which method
was used; if randomized, whether it was randomized by cluster; whether it was blinded; where it
was (village, province, country - these were later geocoded in a separate process); what kind of
institution carried out the implementation; characteristics of the population; and the duration of
the intervention from the baseline to the midline or endline results, among others. A full set of
variables and the coding manual is available online, as detailed in Appendix A.
As this paper pays particular attention to the program implementer, it is worth discussing
how this variable was coded in more detail. There were several types of implementers that could
be coded: governments, NGOs, private sector firms, and academics. There was also a code for
“other” (primarily collaborations) or “unclear”. The vast majority of studies were implemented
by academic research teams and NGOs. This paper considers NGOs and academic research teams
together because it turned out to be practically difficult to distinguish between them in the studies,
especially as the passive voice was frequently used (e.g. “X was done” without noting who did
it). There were only a few private sector firms involved, so they are considered with the “other”
category in this paper.
13
Studies tend to report results for multiple specifications. AidGrade focused on those results
least likely to have been influenced by author choices: those with the fewest controls, apart from
fixed effects. Where a study reported results using different methodologies, coders were instructed
to collect the findings obtained under the authors’ preferred methodology; where the preferred
methodology was unclear, coders were advised to follow the internal preference ordering of prioritizing randomized controlled trials, followed by regression discontinuity designs and differences-indifferences, followed by matching, and to collect multiple sets of results when they were unclear
on which to include. Where results were presented separately for multiple subgroups, coders were
similarly advised to err on the side of caution and to collect both the aggregate results and results
by subgroup except where the author appeared to be only including a subgroup because results
were significant within that subgroup. For example, if an author reported results for children aged
8-15 and then also presented results for children aged 12-13, only the aggregate results would be
recorded, but if the author presented results for children aged 8-9, 10-11, 12-13, and 14-15, all
subgroups would be coded as well as the aggregate result when presented. Authors only rarely
reported isolated subgroups, so this was not a major issue in practice.
When considering the variation of effect sizes within a group of papers, the definition of the
group is clearly critical. Two different rules were initially used to define outcomes: a strict rule,
under which only identical outcome variables are considered alike, and a loose rule, under which
similar but distinct outcomes are grouped into clusters.
The precise coding rules were as follows:
1. We consider outcome A to be the same as outcome B under the “strict rule” if outcomes
A and B measure the exact same quality. Different units may be used, pending conversion.
The outcomes may cover different timespans (e.g. encompassing both outcomes over “the
last month” and “the last week”). They may also cover different populations (e.g. children
or adults). Examples: height; attendance rates.
2. We consider outcome A to be the same as outcome B under the “loose rule” if they do not
meet the strict rule but are clearly related. Example: parasitemia greater than 4000/µl with
fever and parasitemia greater than 2500/µl.
14
Clearly, even under the strict rule, differences between the studies may exist, however, using two
different rules allows us to isolate the potential sources of variation, and other variables were coded
to capture some of this variation, such as the age of those in the sample. If one were to divide the
studies by these characteristics, however, the data would usually be too sparse for analysis.
Interventions were also defined separately and coders were also asked to write a short description of the details of each program. Program names were recorded so as to identify those papers
on the same program, such as the various evaluations of PROGRESA.
After coding, the data were then standardized to make results easier to interpret and so as
not to overly weight those outcomes with larger scales. The typical way to compare results across
different outcomes is by using the standardized mean difference, defined as:
SM D “
µ 1 ´ µ2
σp
where µ1 is the mean outcome in the treatment group, µ2 is the mean outcome in the control
group, and σp is the pooled standard deviation. When data are not available to calculate the
pooled standard deviation, it can be approximated by the standard deviation of the dependent
variable for the entire distribution of observations or as the standard deviation in the control group
(Glass, 1976). If that is not available either, due to standard deviations not having been reported
in the original papers, one can use the typical standard deviation for the intervention-outcome. I
follow this approach to calculate the standardized mean difference, which is then used as the effect
size measure for the rest of the paper unless otherwise noted.
This paper uses the “strict” outcomes where available, but the “loose” outcomes where that
would keep more data. For papers which were follow-ups of the same study, the most recent results
were used for each outcome.
Finally, one paper appeared to misreport results, suggesting implausibly low values and standard
deviations for hemoglobin. These results were excluded and the paper’s corresponding author
contacted. Excluding this paper’s results, effect sizes range between -1.5 and 1.8 SD, with an
interquartile range of 0 to 0.2 SD. So as to mitigate sensitivity to individual results, especially
with the small number of papers in some intervention-outcome groups, I restrict attention to those
standardized effect sizes less than 2 SD away from 0, dropping 1 additional observation. I report
15
main results including this observation in the Appendix.
3.3
Data Description
Figure 2 summarizes the distribution of studies covering the interventions and outcomes considered in this paper. Attention will typically be limited to those intervention-outcome combinations
on which we have data for at least three papers, with an alternative minimum of four papers in the
Appendix.
Table 12 in Appendix C lists the interventions and outcomes and describes their results in a bit
more detail, providing the distribution of significant and insignificant results. It should be emphasized that the number of negative and significant, insignificant, and positive and significant results
per intervention-outcome combination only provide ambiguous evidence of the typical efficacy of a
particular type of intervention. Simply tallying the numbers in each category is known as “vote
counting” and can yield misleading results if, for example, some studies are underpowered.
Table 2 further summarizes the distribution of papers across interventions and highlights the
fact that papers exhibit very little overlap in terms of outcomes studied. This is consistent with
the story of researchers each wanting to publish one of the first papers on a topic. We will indeed
see that later papers on the same intervention-outcome combination more often remain as working
papers.
A note must be made about combining data. When conducting a meta-analysis, the Cochrane
Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions recommends collapsing the data to one observation per intervention-outcome-paper, and I do this for generating the within intervention-outcome
meta-analyses (Higgins and Green, 2011). Where results had been reported for multiple subgroups
(e.g. women and men), I aggregated them as in the Cochrane Handbook’s Table 7.7.a. Where
results were reported for multiple time periods (e.g. 6 months after the intervention and 12 months
after the intervention), I used the most comparable time periods across papers. When combining
across multiple outcomes, which has limited use but will come up later in the paper, I used the
formulae from Borenstein et al. (2009), Chapter 24.
16
Figure 2: Within-Intervention-Outcome Number of Papers
17
Table 2: Descriptive Statistics: Distribution of Narrow Outcomes
Intervention
Number of
outcomes
Mean papers
per outcome
Max papers
per outcome
Conditional cash transfers
Contract teachers
Deworming
Financial literacy
HIV/AIDS Education
Improved stoves
Insecticide-treated bed nets
Irrigation
Micro health insurance
Microfinance
Micronutrient supplementation
Mobile phone-based reminders
Performance pay
Rural electrification
Safe water storage
Scholarships
School meals
Unconditional cash transfers
Water treatment
Women’s empowerment programs
10
1
12
1
3
4
1
2
1
5
23
2
1
3
1
3
3
3
2
2
16
3
13
5
8
2
9
2
2
4
27
4
3
3
2
2
3
2
5
2
28
3
18
5
10
2
9
2
2
5
47
5
3
3
2
2
3
2
6
2
Average
4.2
5.9
8.0
Table 3: Differences between vote counting and meta-analysis results
Vote counting result
Negative
Insignificant
Positive
Total
Meta-analysis result
Negative
Insignificant
Positive
and significant
and significant
0
0
0
1
30
14
0
1
21
1
31
35
18
Total
0
45
22
67
4
Generalizability of Impact Evaluation Results
4.1
Method
The first thing I do is to report basic summary statistics. I ask: given a positive, significant
result - the kind perhaps most relevant for motivating policy - what proportion of papers on the
same intervention-outcome combination find a positive, significant effect, an insignificant effect,
or a negative, significant effect? How much do the results of vote counting and meta-analysis diverge? Another key summary statistic is the coefficient of variation. This statistic is frequently
used as a measure of dispersion of results and is defined as
σ
µ,
where µ is the mean of a set of
results and σ the standard deviation.7 The set of results under consideration here is defined by
the intervention-outcome combination; I also separately look at variation within papers. While the
coefficient of variation is a basic statistic, this is the first time it is reported across a wide variety of
impact evaluation results. Finally, I look at how much results overlap within intervention-outcome
combinations, using the raw, unstandardized data.
I then regress the effect size on several explanatory variables, including the leave-one-out metaanalysis result for all but study i within each intervention-outcome combination, M´i . Whenever
I use M´i to predict Yi , I adjust the estimates for sampling variance to avoid attenuation bias. I
also cluster standard errors at the intervention-outcome level to guard against the case in which
an outlier introduces systematic error within an intervention-outcome. While the main results are
based on any intervention-outcome combination covered by at least three papers, as mentioned I try
increasing this minimum number of papers in robustness checks that are included in the Appendix.
Finally, I construct the PRESS statistic to measure generalizability, as discussed in Section 2.2.
In order to be able to say whether the result is large or small, I also calculate an R2 -like statistic
for prediction and conduct simulations using placebo data for comparison. The R2 -like statistic is
the RP2 r from prediction:
RP2 r “ 1 ´
7
Absolute values are always taken.
19
PRESS
SST ot
(16)
where SST ot is the total sum of squares. In the simulations, I randomly assign the real effect
size data to alternative interventions, outcomes, or other variables and then generate the PRESS
statistic and the predicted RP2 r for these placebo groups.
4.2
4.2.1
Results
Summary statistics
Summary statistics provide a first look at how results vary across different papers.
The average intervention-outcome combination is comprised 37% of positive, significant results;
58% of insignificant results; and 5% of negative, significant results. To gauge how stable results are,
suppose we know that one study in a particular intervention-outcome combination found a positive,
significant result (the kind of result one might think could influence policy); drawing another study
at random from the set, there is a 60% chance the new result will be insignificant and a 8% chance
it will be significant and negative, leaving only about a 32% chance it will again be positive and
significant.
The differences between meta-analysis results and vote counting results are shown in Table 3.
Only those intervention-outcomes which had a vote counting “winner” are included in this table; in
other words, it does not include ties. That the meta-analysis result was often positive and significant or negative and significant when the vote counting result was insignificant is likely a function
of many impact evaluations being underpowered.
In the methods section, I discussed the coefficient of variation, a measure of the dispersion of
the impact evaluation findings. Values for the coefficient of variation in the medical literature tend
to range from approximately 0.1 to 0.5. Figure 3 shows its distribution in the economics data,
across papers within intervention-outcomes as well as within papers.
Each of the across-paper coefficients of variation was calculated within an intervention-outcome
combination; for example, the effects of conditional cash transfer programs on enrollment rates.
When a paper reports multiple results, the previously described conventions work to either select
one of them or aggregate them so that there is one result per intervention-outcome-paper that is
used to calculate the within-intervention-outcome, across-paper coefficient of variation. The across-
20
Figure 3: Distribution of the Coefficient of Variation
paper coefficient of variation is thus likely lower than it might otherwise be due to the aggregation
process reducing noise.
The within-paper coefficients of variation are calculated where the data include multiple results
from the same paper on the same intervention-outcome. There are several reasons a paper may have
reported multiple results: multiple time periods were examined; the author used multiple methods;
or results for different subgroups were collected. In each of these scenarios, the context is more
similar than it typically is across different papers. Variation within a single paper due to different
specifications and subgroups has often been neglected in the literature but constituted on average
approximately 67% of the variation across papers within a single intervention-outcome combination.
As Figure 3 makes clear, the coefficient of variation within the same paper within an interventionoutcome combination is much lower than that across papers. The mean coefficient of variation across
papers in the same intervention-outcome combination is 1.9; the mean coefficient of variation for
results within the same paper in the same intervention-outcome combination is lower, at 1.2, a
difference that is significantly different in a t-test at pă0.05.8
The coefficient of variation clearly depends on the set of results being considered. Outcomes
were extremely narrowly defined, as discussed; interventions varied more. For example, a school
meals program might disburse different kinds of meals in one study than in another. The contexts also varied, such as in terms of the implementing agency, the age group, the underlying rates
8
All these results are based on truncating the coefficient of variation at 10 as in Figure 3; if one does so at 20,
the across-paper coefficient of variation rises to 2.1 and the within-paper to 1.5.
21
of malnutrition, and so on. The data are mostly too sparse to use this information, however, a
few papers considered the same programs; the average coefficient of variation within the same
intervention-outcome-program combination, across papers, was 1.5.
To aid in interpreting the results, I return to the unstandardized values within interventionoutcome combinations. I ask: what is the typical gap between a study’s point estimate and the
average point estimate within that intervention-outcome combination? How often do the confidence
intervals around an estimated effect size overlap within intervention-outcomes?
Table 4 presents some results, excluding risk ratios and rate ratios, which are on different
scales. The mean absolute difference between a study’s point estimate and the average point
estimate within that intervention-outcome combination is about 90%. Regarding the confidence
intervals, a given result in an intervention-outcome combination will, on average, have a confidence interval that overlaps with about 85% of the confidence intervals of the other results in that
intervention-outcome. The point estimate will be contained in the confidence interval of the other
studies approximately half of the time.
4.2.2
Regression results
Do results exhibit any systematic variation? This section examines whether generalizability is
associated with study characteristics such as the type of program implementer.
I first present some OLS results. As Table 5 indicates, there is some evidence that studies with
a smaller number of observations have greater effect sizes than studies based on a larger number of
observations. This is what we would expect if specification searching were easier for small datasets;
this pattern of results would also be what we would expect if power calculations drove researchers
to only proceed with studies with small sample sizes if they believed the program would result in
a large effect size or if larger studies are less well-targeted. Interestingly, government-implemented
programs fare worse even controlling for sample size (the dummy variable category left out is
“Other-implemented”, which mainly consists of collaborations and private sector-implemented interventions). Studies in the Middle East / North Africa region may appear to do slightly better
than those in Sub-Saharan Africa (the excluded region category), but not much weight should be
put on this as very few studies were conducted in the former region.
I then turn to the PRESS statistic in Table 6. In this table, all C represent dummy variables
22
on the RHS of the regression that is fit; for example, the first row uses the fitted Ypi from regressř
ing Yi “ α ` n βn Interventionin ` εi where Intervention comprises dummy variables indicating
different interventions. The PRESS statistic and RP2 r from each regression of Y on assorted C is
listed, along with the average PRESS statistic and RP2 r from the corresponding placebo simulations.
It should be noted that, unlike R2 , RP2 r need not have a lower bound of zero. This is because the
predicted residual sum of squares, which is by definition greater than the residual sum of squares,
can also be greater than the total sum of squares. The p-value gives how likely it is the PRESS
statistic is from the distribution of simulation PRESS statistics, using the standard deviation from
the simulations.9 As Table 6 shows, one can distinguish the interventions, outcomes, interventionoutcomes, and regions in the data better than chance. The implementer dummy does not have
significant predictive power here.
One might believe the relatively poor predictive power is due to too many diverse interventions
and outcomes being grouped together. I therefore separate out the two interventions with the
largest number of studies, CCTs and deworming, to see if patterns are any different within each of
these interventions.
Results are weaker here (Table 7). While this could partially due to reduced sample sizes, it
also suggests that there are many different sources of heterogeneity in the data.
Table 8 presents more PRESS statistics, this time using the leave-one-out meta-analysis result
from within intervention-outcome combinations to predict the result left out. The significance of
the meta-analysis results is striking compared to that of the earlier results, but the low RP2 r should
be noted.
The regressions in Table 9 show some of the key takeaways of this paper in an easily digested
format. The relationship between the PRESS statistics and a regression model can also be seen by
comparing Tables 8 and 9. In both cases, the meta-analysis result is a significant predictor of the
effect size, but the R2 or RP2 r is also low in both tables. Table 9 also suggests that governmentimplemented programs do not fare as well on average.
9
Simulations were run 100 times for each regression.
23
24
Intervention
Conditional Cash Transfers
Conditional Cash Transfers
Conditional Cash Transfers
Conditional Cash Transfers
Conditional Cash Transfers
Conditional Cash Transfers
Conditional Cash Transfers
Conditional Cash Transfers
Conditional Cash Transfers
Conditional Cash Transfers
Conditional Cash Transfers
Conditional Cash Transfers
Conditional Cash Transfers
Conditional Cash Transfers
Conditional Cash Transfers
Contract Teachers
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Financial Literacy
Financial Literacy
Financial Literacy
HIV/AIDS Education
HIV/AIDS Education
HIV/AIDS Education
HIV/AIDS Education
HIV/AIDS Education
Irrigation
Irrigation
Micro Health Insurance
Micro Health Insurance
Micro Health Insurance
Table 4: Differences in Point Estimates
Outcome
Mean estimate
Attendance rate
0.07
Birth in a medical facility
0.07
Enrollment rate
0.08
Height
0.99
Height-for-age
-0.01
Labor force participation
-0.03
Labor hours
-1.99
Pregnancy rate
-0.03
Probability skilled attendant at delivery
0.12
Probability unpaid work
-0.06
Retention rate
-0.01
Test scores
0.06
Unpaid labor hours
-1.51
Weight-for-age
0.19
Weight-for-height
0.12
Test scores
0.18
Attendance rate
0.03
Birthweight
0.00
Height
0.09
Height-for-age
0.16
Hemoglobin
0.06
Malformations
0.05
Mid-upper arm circumference
0.08
Test scores
0.08
Weight
0.14
Weight-for-age
0.14
Weight-for-height
0.09
Has savings
0.02
Probability has taken loan
0.03
Savings
-17.40
Pregnancy rate
-0.01
Probability has multiple sex partners
0.23
Probability sexually active
-0.02
STD prevalence
0.09
Used contraceptives
0.04
Consumption
838.55
Total income
-6.20
Household health expenditures
0.17
Probability of inpatient visit
0.00
Probability of outpatient visit
0.00
Mean difference
0.03
0.06
0.06
0.03
0.17
0.03
1.64
0.04
0.06
0.07
0.01
0.09
0.97
0.25
0.09
0.06
0.02
0.01
0.28
0.21
0.11
0.05
0.22
0.03
0.25
0.21
0.18
0.01
0.05
67.18
0.01
0.22
0.02
0.10
0.07
887.56
89.13
0.18
0.01
0.03
Units
percentage points
percentage points
percentage points
cm
z-score
percentage points
hours/week
percentage points
percentage points
percentage points
percentage points
standard deviations
hours/week
z-score
z-score
standard deviations
percentage points
kg
cm
z-score
g/dL
percentage points
cm
standard deviations
kg
z-score
z-score
percentage points
percentage points
current US$
percentage points
percentage points
percentage points
percentage points
percentage points
current US$
current US$
current US$
percentage points
percentage points
25
Microfinance
Microfinance
Microfinance
Microfinance
Microfinance
Microfinance
Micronutrients
Micronutrients
Micronutrients
Micronutrients
Micronutrients
Micronutrients
Micronutrients
Micronutrients
Performance Pay
Rural Electrification
Rural Electrification
Rural Electrification
Scholarships
Scholarships
Scholarships
School Meals
School Meals
School Meals
Unconditional Cash Transfers
Unconditional Cash Transfers
Unconditional Cash Transfers
Women’s Empowerment
Women’s Empowerment
Assets
Consumption
Probability of owning business
Profits
Savings
Total income
Height
Height-for-age
Hemoglobin
Mid-upper arm circumference
Test scores
Weight
Weight-for-age
Weight-for-height
Test scores
Enrollment rate
Study time
Total income
Attendance rate
Enrollment rate
Test scores
Enrollment rate
Height-for-age
Test scores
Enrollment rate
Test scores
Weight-for-height
Savings
Total income
21.81
-3.52
0.05
-14.41
41.06
23.54
0.07
0.06
0.08
0.04
0.01
0.09
0.05
0.03
0.13
0.08
1.10
42.46
0.01
0.01
0.11
0.11
0.15
0.12
0.12
0.06
0.02
24.16
6.83
79.83
4.27
0.03
55.06
56.02
18.20
0.36
0.11
0.30
0.11
0.11
0.13
0.09
0.08
0.06
0.01
0.88
42.23
0.00
0.00
0.08
0.10
0.06
0.12
0.12
0.02
0.04
21.69
1.04
current US$
current US$
percentage points
current US$
current US$
current US$
cm
z-score
g/dL
cm
standard deviations
kg
z-score
z-score
standard deviations
percentage points
hours/week
current US$
percentage points
percentage points
standard deviations
percentage points
z-score
standard deviations
percentage points
standard deviations
z-score
current US$
current US$
Table 5: Regression of Effect Size on Study Characteristics
(1)
Effect size
b/se
Number of
observations (100,000s)
Government-implemented
(2)
Effect size
b/se
(3)
Effect size
b/se
-0.018***
(0.01)
(4)
Effect size
b/se
(5)
Effect size
b/se
-0.019***
(0.00)
-0.015**
(0.01)
-0.150**
(0.06)
-0.075
(0.05)
-0.163***
(0.06)
-0.070
(0.04)
Academic/NGO-implemented
RCT
0.049
(0.04)
East Asia
0.120***
(0.00)
0.199***
(0.04)
0.080***
(0.03)
-0.015
(0.03)
-0.006
(0.04)
0.284**
(0.11)
0.009
(0.04)
0.114***
(0.02)
534
0.19
634
0.23
634
0.22
534
0.22
Latin America
Middle East/North
Africa
South Asia
Constant
Observations
R2
0.201***
(0.04)
534
0.20
Table 6: PRESS statistics and RP2 r : All interventions
C dummies
Intervention
Outcome
Intervention & Outcome
Region
Implementer
P RESS
46.1
45.6
46
45.5
46.3
P RESSSim
47.2
49
49.9
46
46.4
p-value
0.063
<0.001
<0.001
0.021
0.764
RP2 r
-0.01
0.01
0.00
0.00
-0.01
RP2 rSim
-0.03
-0.07
-0.09
-0.01
-0.01
RP2 r -RP2 rSim
0.02
0.07
0.09
0.01
0.00
One may be concerned that low-quality papers are either inflating or depressing the degree
of generalizability that is observed. There are infinitely many ways to measure paper “quality”;
I consider two. First, I consider only those papers that were randomized controlled trials. Second, I use the most widely-used quality assessment measure, the Jadad scale (Jadad et al., 1996).
The Jadad scale asks whether the study was randomized, double-blind, and whether there was a
description of withdrawals and dropouts. A paper gets one point for having each of these charac-
26
Table 7: Within-intervention PRESS statistics and RP2 r
CCTs
C dummies
Outcome
Region
Implementer
Deworming
C dummies
Outcome
Region
Implementer
P RESS
6.9
6.5
6.6
P RESSSim
7.1
6.8
6.6
p-value
0.676
0.215
0.762
RP2 r
-0.09
-0.02
-0.04
RP2 rSim
-0.12
-0.07
-0.04
RP2 r -RP2 rSim
0.03
0.05
0.00
P RESS
10.7
10.4
10.4
P RESSSim
10.8
10.5
10.5
p-value
0.924
0.309
0.543
RP2 r
-0.05
-0.02
-0.02
RP2 rSim
-0.06
-0.03
-0.03
RP2 r -RP2 rSim
0.01
0.01
0.01
Table 8: PRESS statistics and RP2 r using Within-Intervention-Outcome Meta-Analysis Results to
Predict Estimates from Different Implementers
Meta-Analysis Result
M (on full sample)
M (on govt only)
M (on acad/NGO only)
P RESS
41.9
9.3
32.4
P RESSSim
46
9.9
35.7
p-value
<0.001
0.028
<0.001
RP2 r
0.09
0.02
0.09
RP2 rSim
0.00
-0.04
-0.01
RP2 r -RP2 rSim
0.09
0.06
0.09
teristics; in addition, a point is added if the method of randomization was appropriate, subtracted
if the method is inappropriate, and similarly added if the blinding method was appropriate and
subtracted if inappropriate. This results in a 0-5 point scale. Given that the kinds of interventions
being tested are not typically readily suited to blinding, I consider all those papers scoring at least
a 3 to be “high quality”. Tables 13 and 14 in the Appendix provide robustness checks using these
two quality measures. Table 15 also considers those intervention-outcome combinations with at
least four papers; Table 16 includes the one observation previously dropped for having an effect
size more than 2 SD away from 0; in Table 17, I use a fixed effect meta-analysis to create the
leave-one-out meta-analysis results.
To illustrate the mechanics more clearly, Figures 4 and 5 show the density of results according
to a hierarchical Bayesian model with an uninformative prior.10 Figure 4 shows the results within
one intervention-outcome combination: conditional cash transfers and enrollment rates. The dark
dots correspond to the aggregated estimates of the government versions of the interventions; the
light, the academic/NGO versions. The still lighter dots found in figures in the Appendix repre10
Code adapted from Hsiang, Burke and Miguel (2013), based on Gelman et al., 2013).
27
Table 9: Regression of Effect Size on Hierarchical Bayesian Meta-Analysis Results
Meta-analysis result
Observations
R2
(1)
Effect size
b/se
(2)
Effect size
b/se
(3)
Effect size
b/se
0.595***
(0.09)
0.530***
(0.05)
0.709***
(0.14)
566
0.08
103
0.15
439
0.08
The meta-analysis result in the table above was created by synthesizing all but one observation within an
intervention-outcome combination; that one observation left out is on the left hand side in the regression. All
interventions and outcomes are included in the regression, clustering by intervention-outcome. Column (1) shows
the results on the full data set; column (2) shows the results for those effect sizes pertaining to programs
implemented by the government; column (3) shows the results for academic/NGO-implemented programs.
sent those papers with “other” implementers (collaborations or private sector implementers). The
dashed black line shows the overall weighted mean.
The two panels on the right side of Figure 4 give the weighted distribution of effects according
to a hierarchical Bayesian model. In the first panel, all intervention-implementer pairs are pooled;
in the second, they are disaggregated. This figure graphically depicts what a meta-analysis does.
We can make a similar figure for each of the 48 intervention-outcome combinations on which we
have sufficient papers (available online; for details, see Appendix A).
To summarize results further, we may want to aggregate up to the intervention-implementer
level. Figure 5 shows the effects if the meta-analysis were to aggregate across outcomes within
an intervention-implementer as if they were independent. This figure is based on only those interventions that have been attempted by both a government agency and an academic team or
NGO.
The standard errors here are exceptionally small for a reason: each dot has aggregated all the
results of all the different papers and outcomes under that intervention-implementer. Every time
one aggregates in this fashion, even if one assumes that the multiple observations are correlated and
corrects for this, the standard errors will decrease. The standard errors are thus more indicative
of how much data I have on those particular programs - not much, for example, in the case of
academic/NGO-implemented unconditional cash transfers.
Figure 5 is simply for illustrative purposes, as collapsing across outcomes may not make sense if
28
Effect size (in SD)
Figure 4: Example of Hierarchical Bayesian Meta-Analysis Results: Conditional Cash Transfers
and Enrollment Rates
2.0
2.0
1.5
1.5
−
−
1.0
1.0
−
−
−
−
−
−
−
−
0.5
0.0
−
−
−
−
−
−
−
−
0.0
Academic
(light) vs.
Government
(dark)
All studies
Mo et al. (2013)
Baird et al. (2011)
Edmonds and Schady (2012)
Ferreira, Filmer and Schady (2009)
Behrman, Parker and Todd (2004)
Akresh, de Walque and Kazianga (2013)
Angelucci et al. (2010)
Maluccio, Murphy and Regalia (2009)
Galasso (2006)
Galiani and McEwan (2013)
Arraiz and Rozo (2011)
de Brauw and Gilligan (2011)
Davis et al. (2002)
de Janvry, Finan and Sadoulet (2012)
Chaudhury, Friedman and Onishi (2013)
Ham (2010)
Perova (2010)
Garcia and Hill (2009)
Dubois, de Janvry and Sadoulet (2012)
Ward et al. (2010)
Olinto and Souza (2005)
Attanasio et al. (2010)
Fuwa (2001)
Ferro, Kassouf and Levison (2007)
Rubio−Codina (2003)
Gitter and Barham (2008)
Barrera−Osorio et al. (2008)
−0.5
Glewwe and Kassouf (2008)
−0.5
0.5
This figure provides the point estimate and confidence interval for each paper’s estimated effect of a conditional
cash transfer program (intervention) on enrollment rates (outcome). The first grey box on the right hand side of the
figure shows the aggregate distribution of results using the hierarchical Bayesian procedure, and the second grey
box farther to the right shows the distributions for the government-implemented and academic/NGO-implemented
studies separately. Government-implemented programs are denoted by dark grey, while
academic/NGO-implemented studies are in lighter grey. The even lighter dots in other figures found in the
Appendix represent those papers with “other” implementers (collaborations or private sector implementers). The
data have been converted to standard deviations for later comparison with other outcomes. Conventions were
followed to isolate one result per paper. These are detailed in the Appendix and in the online coding manual, but
the main criteria were to use the result with the fewest controls; if results for multiple time periods were presented,
the time period closest to those examined in other papers in the same intervention-outcome was selected; if results
for multiple subgroups were presented, such as different age ranges, results were aggregated as these data were
typically too sparse to do subgroup analyses. Thus, the data have already been slightly aggregated in this figure.
the outcomes are not comparable. Still, unless one expects a systematic bias affecting governmentimplemented programs differently vis-a-vis academic/NGO-implemented programs, it is clear that
the distribution of the programs’ effects looks quite different, and the academic/NGO-implemented
programs routinely exhibit higher effect sizes than their government-implemented counterparts.
Figure 5 also illustrates the limitations of meta-analyses: while the academic/NGO-implemented
29
1.5
1.5
1.0
1.0
0.5
−
−
−
−
−
−
0.0
−
−
−
−
−
−
0.5
0.0
Academic
(light) vs.
Government
(dark)
All studies
HIV/AIDS Education
Deworming
School meals
−1.5
Financial literacy
−1.5
Performance pay
−1.0
Micronutrient supplementation
−1.0
Contract teachers
−0.5
Microfinance
−0.5
Conditional cash transfers
Effect size (in SD)
Figure 5: Government- and Academic/NGO- Implemented Projects Differ Within the Same Interventions
This figure focuses only on those interventions for which there were both government-implemented and
academic/NGO-implemented studies. All outcomes and papers are aggregated within each
intervention-implementer combination. While it appears that the academic/NGO-implemented studies do better
overall, the higher weighting of some of the better government-implemented interventions (in particular, conditional
cash transfer programs, which tend to have very large sample sizes) disguises this. This figure both illustrates that
academic/NGO-implemented programs seem to do better than government-implemented programs and shows why
caution must be taken in interpreting results.
studies do better overall, the higher weighting of some of the better government-implemented
interventions hides this in the results in the side panel. This again points to the fact that what one
is aggregating over and the weighting scheme used is important.
5
Specification searching and publication bias
Results on generalizability could be biased in the presence of specification searching and publi-
cation bias. In particular, if studies are systematically biased, they could speciously appear to be
more generalizable. In this section, I examine these issues. First, I test for specification searching
and publication bias, finding that these biases are quite limited in my data, especially among randomized controlled trials. I then suggest a mathematical correction that could be applied, much in
30
the spirit of Simonsohn et al.’s p-curve (2014), which looks at the distribution of p-values one would
expect given a true effect size. I run some simulations that show that my mathematical correction
would recover the correct distribution of effect sizes. However, since that approach depends on a
key assumption that there is one true effect size and all deviations are noise, and one might want
to weaken that assumption, I show how one might do that. Finally, I restrict attention to just the
subset of studies that had been RCTs, as I showed they did not appear subject to the same biases,
and repeat the earlier analyses.
5.1
5.1.1
Specification searching: how bad is it?
Method
To examine the issue of specification searching, I start by conducting a series of caliper tests,
following Gerber and Malhotra (2008a). As they describe (2008b), even if results are naturally
concentrated in a given range, one should expect to see roughly comparable numbers of results just
on either side of any threshold when restricting attention to a narrow enough band. I consider
the ranges 2.5%, 5%, 10%, 15% and 20% above and below z=1.96, in turn, and examine whether
results follow a binomial distribution around 1.96 as one would expect in the absence of bias.
I do these tests on the full data set - here there is no need to consider only those results for
intervention-outcome combinations covered by a certain number of papers, for example - but then
also break it down in several ways, such as by RCT or non-RCT and government-implemented or
non-government-implemented.
When doing this kind of analysis, one should also carefully consider the issues arising from
having multiple coefficients coming from the same papers. Gerber and Malhotra (2008a; 2008b)
address the issue by breaking down their results by the number of coefficients contributed by
each paper, so as to separately show the results for those papers that contribute one coefficient,
two coefficients, and so on. I also do this, but in addition use the common statistical method of
aggregating the results by paper, so that, for example, a paper with four coefficients below the
threshold and three above it would be counted as “below”. The approach followed in Gerber and
Malhotra (2008a; 2008b) retains slightly different information. While it preserves the number of
coefficients on either side of the threshold, it does not reduce the bias that may be present if one
31
or two of the papers are responsible for much of the effect. By presenting a set of results collapsed
by paper, I can test if results are sensitive to this.
5.1.2
Results
I begin by simply plotting the distribution of z-statistics in the data for different groups.
The distributions, shown in Figure 6, are consistent with specification searching, particularly for
government-implemented programs and non-RCTs: while noise remains, mostly from using multiple results from the same study, which tend to be clustered, there appears to be a bit of a deviation
from the downward trend around 1.96, the threshold for statistical significance at the 5% level for a
two-sided test. These are “de-rounded” figures, accounting for the fact that papers may have presented results which were imprecise; for example, specifying a point estimate of 0.03 and a standard
error of 0.01. Since these results would artificially cause spikes in the distribution, I re-drew their
z-statistics from the uniform range of possible results ( 0.025
0.015 ´
0.035
0.05 ),
as in Brodeur et al. (2012).
Figure 6: Distribution of z-statistics
This figure shows histograms of the z-statistics, by implementer and whether the result was from an RCT. A jump
around 1.96, the threshold for significance at the 5% level, would suggest that authors were wittingly or unwittingly
selecting significant results for inclusion.
32
Table 10: Caliper Tests: By Result
All studies
2.5% Caliper
5% Caliper
10% Caliper
15% Caliper
20% Caliper
RCTs
2.5% Caliper
5% Caliper
10% Caliper
15% Caliper
20% Caliper
Non-RCTs
2.5% Caliper
5% Caliper
10% Caliper
15% Caliper
20% Caliper
Over Caliper
Under Caliper
p-value
119
190
353
519
675
92
176
357
526
706
<0.10
79
123
253
376
498
74
135
274
404
543
40
67
100
143
177
18
41
83
122
163
<0.01
<0.05
Table 11: Caliper Tests: By Paper
All studies
2.5% Caliper
5% Caliper
10% Caliper
15% Caliper
20% Caliper
RCTs
2.5% Caliper
5% Caliper
10% Caliper
15% Caliper
20% Caliper
Non-RCTs
2.5% Caliper
5% Caliper
10% Caliper
15% Caliper
20% Caliper
Over Caliper
Under Caliper
54
71
81
100
104
43
71
101
119
129
35
51
59
71
72
36
57
80
91
96
19
20
22
29
32
7
14
21
28
33
33
p-value
<0.10
<0.10
<0.05
Overall, these figures look much better than the typical ones in the literature. I designed
AidGrade’s coding conventions partially to minimize bias,11 which could help explain the difference.
The government z-statistics are perhaps the most interesting. While they may reflect noise
rather than bias, it would be intuitive for governments to exert pressure over their evaluators to
find significant, positive effects. Suppose there are two ways of obtaining a significant effect size:
putting effort into the intervention (such as increasing inputs) or putting effort into leaning on
the evaluators. For large-scale projects, it would seem much more efficient to target the evaluator.
While this story would be consistent with the observed evidence, many other explanations remain
possible.
Turning to the caliper tests, I still find little evidence of bias (Tables 10 and 11). For the
caliper tests, I use the raw rather than de-rounded data, as de-rounding could mask subtle jumps
at z=1.96. Whether considering results independently or collapsed by paper, non-RCTs appear to
suffer from bias, but RCTs perform much better. It should be recalled that as the distribution
of the z-statistics is skewed, we should expect to see fewer results just over as opposed to just
under the threshold for significance for a wide enough band, which is indeed what we see for RCTs.
The results, especially for RCTs, mark a great difference from Gerber and Malhotra’s results on
the political science (2008a) or sociology (2008b) literature. I reproduce one of their tables in the
Appendix to illustrate (Table 19), as the difference between my results and their row of mostly
p ă 0.001 results is striking.
5.2
Publication bias
Turning to publication bias, published impact evaluations are more likely to have significant
findings than working papers, as we can see in Table 12. RCTs are also greatly selected for publication. However, once controlling for whether a study is an RCT, publication bias is reduced.
As discussed, an attempt was made to be very comprehensive in the data gathering process,
and both published and unpublished papers were searched for and included. I can therefore re-run
the main regressions separately for published and unpublished papers (Table 18 in the Appendix).
Results are fairly similar. The coefficient on the meta-analysis term is marginally not significant
11
Such as by focusing on the specifications with fewest controls and only collecting subgroup data where results
for all subgroups were reported.
34
Table 12: Publication Bias
(1)
Published
b/se
Published
RCT
(2)
Published
b/se
(3)
Published
b/se
1.855***
(0.27)
2.796***
(1.11)
1.227
(0.37)
1.690
(0.59)
9381
9381
4.429***
(0.85)
Significant
RCT*Significant
Observations
637
Exponentiated coefficients
Each column presents the results of a logistic regression of whether a study or result was published on different
characteristics. Column (1) considers papers; Columns (2) and (3) consider results for simplicity, though the
decision of whether or not a paper will be published could be a more complicated function of the individual results’
significance.
for unpublished papers (p=0.12), but this appears likely influenced by the small sample size; the
coefficient is of a similar magnitude, and if I run the meta-analyses on the full data set and then
restrict attention to the subset of unpublished papers (reducing noise), rather than first restricting
attention to the unpublished papers and then generating the meta-analysis results, the coefficient
is again highly significant (p¡0.001).12 In short, published and unpublished papers appear roughly
comparable in terms of generalizability; it therefore can be hoped that if any papers were not only
not published but also “file drawered”, i.e. not even available as a working paper, this may not be
too much of a concern. Since I cannot say with certainty that “file drawered” studies are similar
to unpublished studies, or that “file drawered” studies are necessarily to unpublished studies what
unpublished studies are to published studies, this paper ultimately can only speak to the results of
papers that do come out in some form.
Finally, we might believe that earlier or later papers on the same intervention-outcome combination might show systematically different results. On the one hand, later authors might face
pressure to find large or different results from earlier studies in order to publish better. In other
disciplines, one can also sometimes observe a “decline effect”, whereby early, published results were
12
Results are available upon request.
35
Figure 7: Variance of Results Over Time, Within Intervention-Outcome
much more promising than later replications, possibly due to publication bias.
To investigate this, I generate the absolute value of the percent difference between a particular
result and the mean result in that intervention-outcome combination. I then compare this with the
chronological order of the paper relative to others on the same intervention-outcome, scaled to run
from 0 to 1. For example, if there were 5 papers on a particular intervention-outcome combination,
the first would take the value 0.2, the last, 1.
Figure 7 provides a scatter plot of the relationship between the absolute percent difference and
the chronological order variables, restricting attention to those percent differences less than 1000%.
There is a weak positive relationship between them, indicating that earlier results tend to be closer
to the mean result than the later results, which are more variable, but this is not significant. Further, the relationship varies according to the cutoff used. Table 20 in Appendix C illustrates.
One could also look at how results vary by journal rankings, but there are relatively few papers
published in economics journals that have a ranking assigned to them as most rankings focus on
e.g. the top 500, as in RePEc, so the data would not yet be sufficient; future work can address this
issue.
36
5.3
Identifying bias vs. heterogeneous effects
Generalizability and specification searching and publication bias are intimately related. This
can be seen by considering how one might correct the bias among the papers in a single interventionoutcome combination.
If one were to consider these papers to be estimating the same true effect, as in the fixed effects
model,13 one would immediately know the variance of the effect sizes. In a fixed effects model, it
is always the same. Yi is normally distributed, with variance equal to 2{n. This can be seen by
considering that the only variation in Yi is assumed to be due to sampling variance, σ 2 {n, and since
Yi is standardized, σ 2 =1, resulting in 2{n for two samples.
This implies that if one believed the fixed effects model were true, one would not even need to
correct the effect size estimates Yi in order to know their variance. For some of the measures of
generalizability previously discussed, such as a simple PRESS statistic which tried to see how well
the group mean effect size, excepting i, predicted Yi , we can pin down the correct statistic using
only the sample size.14
This serves to underscore the inappropriateness of the fixed effect model when considering
generalizability. Bias and generalizability are not separately identified by themselves. If one brought
data from a random effects model and tried to fit it to a fixed effects model, the real differences in
effect size would appear as “bias”.
There are a few possible solutions. A random effects model would seem to be better as it allows
the possibility of heterogeneous treatment effects. However, one would still have to be confident
that one could distinguish bias from heterogeneous effects; one needs another lever.
One possibility would be to exploit different levels of the data. For example, perhaps if one
looked within very narrow groups of results that could plausibly share a true effect size, one could
reasonably consider each of those groups of results as fitting a separate fixed effects model and
correct them before aggregating up in a hierarchical model. Concretely, if the goal were to have an
unbiased random effects model within a particular intervention-outcome combination, one could
correct the data at the paper level, supposing there were enough results within a paper on the
13
Used in e.g. Simonsohn et al. (2014).
Other measures, such as the coefficient of variation, would also require adjusting θ, but this could also be easily
obtained as in Simonsohn et al. (2014).
14
37
same outcome. I do not take this approach, because apart from being data-intensive, this approach
requires having homogenous subgroups and, as previously shown, effect size estimates vary quite a
bit even within the same intervention-outcome-paper in the data. Still, if one wanted to take this
approach, I include simulations in Appendix D that show that one can indeed recover the original
distribution of effect sizes to a close approximation.
Instead, I turn to a different robustness check. Since I previously found that not only are
the biases quite small overall, but RCTs exhibit even fewer signs of specification searching and
publication bias, I thus re-run the main regression relating to generalizability on the subset of the
data that were RCTs (Table 13, Appendix C).
6
Conclusion
How much impact evaluation results generalize to other settings is an important topic, and data
from meta-analyses are the ideal data with which to answer this question. With data on 20 different
types of interventions, all collected in the same way, we can begin to speak a bit more generally
about how results tend to vary across contexts and what that implies for impact evaluation design
and policy recommendations.
I started by defining generalizability and relating it to heterogeneous treatment effects models.
After examining key summary statistics, I conducted leave-one-out hierarchical Bayesian metaanalyses within different narrowly-defined intervention-outcome combinations, separating the metaanalyses into different specifications by type of implementer. The results of these meta-analyses
were significantly associated with the effect size left out at each iteration, with the typical coefficient
on the meta-analysis result being approximately 0.5-0.7; it would be 1 if the estimate were identical
to the meta-analysis result. However, the meta-analysis results were not significantly associated
with the results of studies implemented by governments. Further, the effect sizes of governmentimplemented programs appeared to be lower even after controlling for sample size. This points to
a potential problem when results from an academic/NGO-implemented study are expected to scale
through government implementation.
Specification searching and publication bias could affect both impact evaluation results and their
generalizability, and I next turned to examine these topics. While each issue is present, neither
38
turns out to be particularly important in the data. RCTs fare better than non-RCTs.
Overall, impact evaluation results were very heterogeneous. Even within the same paper, where
one might expect contexts to be very similar, there was a high degree of variation within the same
intervention-outcome combination. When considering the coefficient of variation, a unitless figure
that can be compared across outcomes, within-paper variation was 67% of across-paper variation.
Both, however, were quite high, at 1.2 and 1.9, respectively; in comparison, results in the medical
literature might have coefficients of variation of about 0.1-0.5. The average across-paper, withinintervention-outcome coefficient of variation for the set of papers covering the same program was
1.5.
There are some steps that researchers can take that may improve the generalizability of their
own studies. First, just as with heterogeneous selection into treatment (Chassang, Padró i Miquel
and Snowberg, 2012), one solution would be to ensure one’s impact evaluation varied some of
the contextual variables that we might think underlie the heterogeneous treatment effects. Given
that many studies are underpowered as it is, that may not be likely; however, large organizations
and governments have been supporting more impact evaluations, providing more opportunities to
explicitly integrate these analyses. Efforts to coordinate across different studies, asking the same
questions or looking at some of the same outcome variables, would also help. The framing of
heterogeneous treatment effects could also provide positive motivation for replication projects in
different contexts: different findings would not necessarily negate the earlier ones but add another
level of information.
In summary, generalizability is not binary but something that we can measure. This paper
showed that past results have significant but limited ability to predict other results on the same
topic and this was not seemingly due to bias. Knowing how much results tend to extrapolate
and when is critical if we are to know how to interpret an impact evaluation’s results or apply its
findings.
39
References
AidGrade (2013). “AidGrade Process Description”, http://www.aidgrade.org/methodology/
processmap-and-methodology, March 9, 2013.
AidGrade (2015). “AidGrade Impact Evaluation Data, Version 1.1”.
Alesina, Alberto and David Dollar (2000).
“Who Gives Foreign Aid to Whom and Why?”,
Journal of Economic Growth, vol. 5 (1).
Allcott, Hunt (forthcoming). “Site Selection Bias in Program Evaluation”, Quarterly Journal of
Economics.
Bastardi, Anthony, Eric Luis Uhlmann and Lee Ross (2011). “Wishful Thinking: Belief, Desire,
and the Motivated Evaluation of Scientific Evidence”, Psychological Science.
Becker, Betsy Jane and Meng-Jia Wu (2007). “The Synthesis of Regression Slopes in MetaAnalysis”, Statistical Science, vol. 22 (3).
Bold, Tessa et al. (2013). “Scaling-up What Works: Experimental Evidence on External Validity
in Kenyan Education”, working paper.
Borenstein, Michael et al. (2009). Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Wiley Publishers.
Boriah, Shyam et al. (2008). “Similarity Measures for Categorical Data: A Comparative Evaluation”, in Proceedings of the Eighth SIAM International Conference on Data Mining.
Brodeur, Abel et al. (2012). “Star Wars: The Empirics Strike Back”, working paper.
Cartwright, Nancy (2007). Hunting Causes and Using Them: Approaches in Philosophy
and Economics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cartwright, Nancy (2010). “What Are Randomized Controlled Trials Good For?”, Philosophical
Studies, vol. 147 (1): 59-70.
Casey, Katherine, Rachel Glennerster, and Edward Miguel (2012). “Reshaping Institutions: Evidence on Aid Impacts Using a Preanalysis Plan.” Quarterly Journal of Economics, vol. 127 (4):
1755-1812.
Chassang, Sylvain, Gerard Padr I Miquel, and Erik Snowberg (2012). “Selective Trials: A PrincipalAgent Approach to Randomized Controlled Experiments.” American Economic Review, vol. 102
(4): 1279-1309.
Deaton, Angus (2010). “Instruments, Randomization, and Learning about Development.” Journal
40
of Economic Literature, vol. 48 (2): 424-55.
Duflo, Esther, Pascaline Dupas and Michael Kremer (2012). “School Governance, Teacher Incentives and Pupil-Teacher Ratios: Experimental Evidence from Kenyan Primary Schools”, NBER
Working Paper.
Evans, David and Anna Popova (2014). “Cost-effectiveness Measurement in Development: Accounting for Local Costs and Noisy Impacts”, World Bank Policy Research Working Paper, No.
7027.
Ferguson, Christopher and Michael Brannick (2012). “Publication bias in psychological science:
Prevalence, methods for identifying and controlling, and implications for the use of meta-analyses.”
Psychological Methods, vol. 17 (1), Mar 2012, 120-128.
Franco, Annie, Neil Malhotra and Gabor Simonovits (2014). “Publication Bias in the Social Sciences: Unlocking the File Drawer”, Working Paper.
Gerber, Alan and Neil Malhotra (2008a). “Do Statistical Reporting Standards Affect What Is Published? Publication Bias in Two Leading Political Science Journals”, Quarterly Journal of Political
Science, vol 3.
Gerber, Alan and Neil Malhotra (2008b). “Publication Bias in Empirical Sociological Research:
Do Arbitrary Significance Levels Distort Published Results”, Sociological Methods &Research, vol.
37 (3).
Gelman, Andrew et al. (2013). Bayesian Data Analysis, Third Edition, Chapman and Hall/CRC.
Hedges, Larry and Therese Pigott (2004). “The Power of Statistical Tests for Moderators in MetaAnalysis”, Psychological Methods, vol. 9 (4).
Higgins JPT and S Green, (eds.) (2011). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of
Interventions, Version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration. Available from
www.cochrane-handbook.org.
Hsiang, Solomon, Marshall Burke and Edward Miguel (2013). “Quantifying the Influence of Climate on Human Conflict”, Science, vol. 341.
Independent Evaluation Group (2012). “World Bank Group Impact Evaluations: Relevance and
Effectiveness”, World Bank Group.
Jadad, A.R. et al. (1996). “Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials: Is blinding
necessary?” Controlled Clinical Trials, 17 (1): 112.
41
Millennium Challenge Corporation (2009). “Key Elements of Evaluation at MCC”, presentation
June 9, 2009.
Page, Matthew, McKenzie, Joanne and Andrew Forbes (2013). “Many Scenarios Exist for Selective
Inclusion and Reporting of Results in Randomized Trials and Systematic Reviews”, Journal of
Clinical Epidemiology, vol. 66 (5).
Pritchett, Lant and Justin Sandefur (2013). “Context Matters for Size: Why External Validity
Claims and Development Practice Don’t Mix”, Center for Global Development Working Paper
336.
Rodrik, Dani (2009). “The New Development Economics: We Shall Experiment, but How Shall We
Learn?”, in What Works in Development? Thinking Big, and Thinking Small, ed. Jessica Cohen
and William Easterly, 24-47. Washington, D.C.: Brookings Institution Press.
Saavedra, Juan and Sandra Garcia (2013). “Educational Impacts and Cost-Effectiveness of Conditional Cash Transfer Programs in Developing Countries: A Meta-Analysis”, CESR Working Paper.
Simmons, Joseph and Uri Simonsohn (2011). “False-Positive Psychology: Undisclosed Flexibility
in Data Collection and Analysis Allows Presenting Anything as Significant”, Psychological Science,
vol. 22.
Simonsohn, Uri et al. (2014). “P-Curve: A Key to the File Drawer”, Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General.
Tibshirani, Ryan and Robert Tibshirani (2009). “A Bias Correction for the Minimum Error Rate
in Cross-Validation”, Annals of Applied Statistics, vol. 3 (2).
Tierney, Michael J. et al. (2011). “More Dollars than Sense: Refining Our Knowledge of Development Finance Using AidData”, World Development, vol. 39.
Tipton, Elizabeth (2013). “Improving generalizations from experiments using propensity score subclassification: Assumptions, properties, and contexts”, Journal of Educational and Behavioral
Statistics, 38: 239-266.
RePEc (2013). “RePEc h-index for journals”, http://ideas.repec.org/top/
top.journals.hindex.html.
Vivalt, Eva (2015). “Selection Bias in Impact Evaluations: Evidence from the World Bank”, Working Paper.
Walsh, Michael et. al. (2013). “The Statistical Significance of Randomized Controlled Trial Results
42
is Frequently Fragile: A Case for a Fragility Index”, Journal of Clinical Epidemiology.
USAID (2011). “Evaluation: Learning from Experience”, USAID Evaluation Policy, Washington,
DC.
43
Appendices
A
Guide to Appendices
A.1
Appendices in this Paper
B) Excerpt from AidGrade’s Process Description (2013).
C) Additional results.
D) Simulations showing the recoverability of the distribution of unbiased data in fixed effect models.
A.2
Further Online Appendices
Having to describe data from twenty different meta-analyses and systematic reviews, I must rely
in part on online appendices. The following are available at http://www.evavivalt.com/research:
E) The search terms and inclusion criteria for each topic.
F) The references for each topic.
G) The coding manual.
H) Figures showing hierarchical Bayesian meta-analysis results for each intervention-outcome combination.
44
B
Data Collection
B.1
Description of AidGrade’s Methodology
The following details of AidGrade’s data collection process draw heavily from AidGrade’s Process Description (AidGrade, 2013).
Figure 8: Process Description
Stage 1: Topic Identification
AidGrade staff members were asked to each independently make a list of at least thirty international development programs that they considered to be the most interesting. The independent
lists were appended into one document and duplicates were tagged and removed. Each of the
45
remaining topics was discussed and refined to bring them all to a clear and narrow level of focus.
Pilot searches were conducted to get a sense of how many impact evaluations there might be on
each topic, and all the interventions for which the very basic pilot searches identified at least two
impact evaluations were shortlisted. A random subset of the topics was selected, also acceding to
a public vote for the most popular topic.
Stage 2: Search
Each search engine has its own peculiarities. In order to ensure all relevant papers and few irrelevant papers were included, a set of simple searches was conducted on different potential search engines. First, initial searches were run on AgEcon; British Library for Development Studies (BLDS);
EBSCO; Econlit; Econpapers; Google Scholar; IDEAS; JOLISPlus; JSTOR; Oxford Scholarship
Online; Proquest; PubMed; ScienceDirect; SciVerse; SpringerLink; Social Science Research Network (SSRN); Wiley Online Library; and the World Bank eLibrary. The list of potential search
engines was compiled broadly from those listed in other systematic reviews. The purpose of these
initial searches was to obtain information about the scope and usability of the search engines to
determine which ones would be effective tools in identifying impact evaluations on different topics.
External reviews of different search engines were also consulted, such as a Falagas et al. (2008)
study which covered the advantages and differences between the Google Scholar, Scopus, Web of
Science and PubMed search engines.
Second, searches were conducted for impact evaluations of two test topics: deworming and
toilets. EBSCO, IDEAS, Google Scholar, JOLISPlus, JSTOR, Proquest, PubMed, ScienceDirect,
SciVerse, SpringerLink, Wiley Online Library and the World Bank eLibrary were used for these
searches. 9 search strings were tried for deworming and up to 33 strings for toilets, with modifications as needed for each search engine. For each search the number of results and the number
of results out of the first 10-50 results which appeared to be impact evaluations of the topic in
question were recorded. This gave a better sense of which search engines and which kinds of search
strings would return both comprehensive and relevant results. A qualitative assessment of the
search results was also provided for the Google Scholar and SciVerse searches.
Finally, the online databases of J-PAL, IPA, CEGA and 3ie were searched. Since these databases
are already narrowly focused on impact evaluations, attention was restricted to simple keyword
46
searches, checking whether the search engines that were integrated with each database seemed to
pull up relevant results for each topic.
Ultimately, Google Scholar and the online databases of J-PAL, IPA, CEGA and 3ie, along with
EBSCO/PubMed for health-related interventions, were selected for use in the full searches.
After the interventions of interest were identified, search strings were developed and tested using
each search source. Each search string included methodology-specific stock keywords that narrowed
the search to impact evaluation studies, except for the search strings for the J-PAL, IPA, CEGA
and 3ie searches, as these databases already exclusively focus on impact evaluations.
Experimentation with keyword combinations in stages 1.4 and 2.1 was helpful in the development of the search strings. The search strings could take slightly different forms for different search
engines. Search terms were tailored to the search source, and a full list is included in an appendix.
C# was used to write a script to scrape the results from search engines. The script was programmed to ensure that the Boolean logic of the search string was properly applied within the
constraints of each search engines capabilities.
Some sources were specialized and could have useful papers that do not turn up in simple
searches. The papers listed on J-PAL, IPA, CEGA and 3ies websites are a good example of this.
For these sites, it made more sense for the papers to be manually searched and added to the relevant
spreadsheets. After the automated and manual searches were complete, duplicates were removed
by matching on author and title names.
During the title screening stage, the consolidated list of citations yielded by the scraped searches
was checked for any existing meta-analyses or systematic reviews. Any papers that these papers
included were added to the list. With these references added, duplicates were again flagged and
removed.
Stage 3: Screening
Generic and topic-specific screening criteria were developed. The generic screening criteria are
detailed below, as is an example of a set of topic-specific screening criteria.
The screening criteria were very inclusive overall. This is because AidGrade purposely follows
a different approach to most meta-analyses in the hopes that the data collected can be re-used
by researchers who want to focus on a different subset of papers. Their motivation is that vast
47
resources are typically devoted to a meta-analysis, but if another team of researchers thinks a
different set of papers should be used, they will have scour the literature and recreate the data
from scratch. If the two groups disagree, all the public sees are their two sets of findings and their
reasoning for selecting different papers. AidGrade instead strives to cover the superset of all impact
evaluations one might wish to include along with a list of their characteristics (e.g. where they
were conducted, whether they were randomized by individual or by cluster, etc.) and let people
set their own filters on the papers or select individual papers and view the entire space of possible
results.
48
Figure 9: Generic Screening Criteria
Category
Methodologies
Inclusion Criteria
Impact evaluations that have counterfactuals
Publication status
Time period of study
LocationGeography
Quality
Peer-reviewed or working paper
Any
Any
Any
Exclusion Criteria
Observational studies,
strictly qualitative studies
N/A
N/A
N/A
N/A
Figure 10: Topic-Specific Criteria Example: Formal Banking
Category
Intervention
Outcomes
Inclusion Criteria
Formal banking services specifically including:
- Expansion of credit and/or savings
- Provision of technological innovations
- Introduction or expansion of financial education,
or other program to increase financial literacy
or awareness
- Individual and household income
- Small and micro-business income
- Household and business assets
- Household consumption
- Small and micro-business investment
- Small, micro-business or agricultural output
- Measures of poverty
- Measures of well-being or stress
- Business ownership
- Any other outcome covered by multiple papers
Exclusion Criteria
Other formal banking services
Microfinance
N/A
Figure 11 illustrates the difference.
For this reason, minimal screening was done during the screening stage. Instead, data was
collected broadly and re-screening was allowed at the point of doing the analysis. This is highly
beneficial for the purpose of this paper, as it allows us to look at the largest possible set of papers
and all subsets.
After screening criteria were developed, two volunteers independently screened the titles to determine which papers in the spreadsheet were likely to meet the screening criteria developed in Stage
3.1. Any differences in coding were arbitrated by a third volunteer. All volunteers received training
before beginning, based on the AidGrade Training Manual and a test set of entries. Volunteers’
training inputs were screened to ensure that only proficient volunteers would be allowed to continue.
49
Figure 11: AidGrade’s Strategy
50
Of those papers that passed the title screening, two volunteers independently determined whether
the papers in the spreadsheet met the screening criteria developed in Stage 3.1 judging by the paper
abstracts. Any differences in coding were again arbitrated by a third volunteer. The full text was
then found for those papers which passed both the title and abstract checks. Any paper that proved
not to be a relevant impact evaluation using the aforementioned criteria was discarded at this stage.
Stage 4: Coding
Two AidGrade members each independently used the data extraction form developed in Stage
4.1 to extract data from the papers that passed the screening in Stage 3. Any disputes were arbitrated by a third AidGrade member. These AidGrade members received much more training than
those who screened the papers, reflecting the increased difficulty of their work, and also did a test
set of entries before being allowed to proceed. The data extraction form was organized into three
sections: (1) general identifying information; (2) paper and study characteristics; and (3) results.
Each section contained qualitative and quantitative variables that captured the characteristics and
results of the study.
Stage 5: Analysis
A researcher was assigned to each meta-analysis topic who could specialize in determining which
of the interventions and results were similar enough to be combined. If in doubt, researchers could
consult the original papers. In general, researchers were encouraged to focus on all the outcome
variables for which multiple papers had results.
When a study had multiple treatment arms sharing the same control, researchers would check
whether enough data was provided in the original paper to allow estimates to be combined before
the meta-analysis was run. This is a best practice to avoid double-counting the control group; for
details, see the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (2011). If a paper did
not provide sufficient data for this, the researcher would make the decision as to which treatment
arm to focus on. Data were then standardized within each topic to be more comparable before
analysis (for example, units were converted).
The subsequent steps of the meta-analysis process are irrelevant for the purposes of this paper.
51
It should be noted that the first set of ten topics followed a slightly different procedure for stages
(1) and (2). Only one list of potential topics was created in Stage 1.1, so Stage 1.2 (Consolidation
of Lists) was only vacuously followed. There was also no randomization after public voting (Stage
1.7) and no scripted scraping searches (Stage 2.3), as all searches were manually conducted using
specific strings. A different search engine was also used: SciVerse Hub, an aggreator that includes
SciVerse Scopus, MEDLINE, PubMed Central, ArXiv.org, and many other databases of articles,
books and presentations. The search strings for both rounds of meta-analysis, manual and scripted,
are detailed in another appendix.
52
C
Additional Results
53
54
Intervention
Conditional cash transfers
Conditional cash transfers
Conditional cash transfers
Conditional cash transfers
Conditional cash transfers
Conditional cash transfers
Conditional cash transfers
Conditional cash transfers
Conditional cash transfers
Conditional cash transfers
HIV/AIDS Education
HIV/AIDS Education
HIV/AIDS Education
Unconditional cash transfers
Unconditional cash transfers
Unconditional cash transfers
Insecticide-treated bed nets
Contract teachers
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Deworming
Financial literacy
Improved stoves
Improved stoves
Improved stoves
Improved stoves
Irrigation
Irrigation
Microfinance
Microfinance
Microfinance
Table 12: Descriptive Statistics: Standardized Narrowly Defined Outcomes
Outcome
# Neg sig papers # Insig papers
Attendance rate
0
6
Enrollment rate
1
6
Height
0
1
Height-for-age
0
6
Labor force participation
1
12
Probability unpaid work
1
0
Test scores
1
2
Unpaid labor
0
2
Weight-for-age
0
2
Weight-for-height
0
1
Pregnancy rate
0
2
Probability has multiple sex partners
0
2
Used contraceptives
1
5
Enrollment rate
0
2
Test scores
0
1
Weight-for-height
0
2
Malaria
0
3
Test scores
0
1
Attendance rate
0
1
Birthweight
0
2
Diarrhea incidence
0
1
Height
3
10
Height-for-age
1
9
Hemoglobin
0
13
Malformations
0
2
Mid-upper arm circumference
2
0
Test scores
0
0
Weight
3
8
Weight-for-age
1
6
Weight-for-height
2
7
Savings
0
2
Chest pain
0
0
Cough
0
0
Difficulty breathing
0
0
Excessive nasal secretion
0
1
Consumption
0
1
Total income
0
1
Assets
0
3
Consumption
0
2
Profits
1
3
# Pos sig papers
9
21
1
1
5
4
2
3
0
1
0
0
4
0
1
0
6
2
1
0
1
4
4
2
0
5
2
7
5
2
3
2
2
2
1
1
1
1
0
1
# Papers
15
28
2
7
18
5
5
5
2
2
2
2
10
2
2
2
9
3
2
2
2
17
14
15
2
7
2
18
12
11
5
2
2
2
2
2
2
4
2
5
55
Microfinance
Microfinance
Micro health insurance
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Micronutrient supplementation
Mobile phone-based reminders
Mobile phone-based reminders
Performance pay
Rural electrification
Rural electrification
Rural electrification
Safe water storage
Scholarships
Scholarships
Scholarships
School meals
School meals
School meals
Water treatment
Water treatment
Women’s empowerment programs
Savings
Total income
Enrollment rate
Birthweight
Body mass index
Cough prevalence
Diarrhea incidence
Diarrhea prevalence
Fever incidence
Fever prevalence
Height
Height-for-age
Hemoglobin
Malaria
Mid-upper arm circumference
Mortality rate
Perinatal deaths
Prevalence of anemia
Stillbirths
Stunted
Test scores
Triceps skinfold measurement
Wasted
Weight
Weight-for-age
Weight-for-height
Appointment attendance rate
Treatment adherence
Test scores
Enrollment rate
Study time
Total income
Diarrhea incidence
Attendance rate
Enrollment rate
Test scores
Enrollment rate
Height-for-age
Test scores
Diarrhea incidence
Diarrhea prevalence
Savings
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
0
0
1
3
5
7
0
2
0
1
0
0
0
1
1
0
4
1
0
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
3
3
1
4
1
3
5
5
2
2
22
23
11
2
9
12
5
6
4
5
2
0
2
19
23
18
0
3
2
1
1
2
1
1
2
2
3
2
2
1
1
1
0
2
1
3
4
0
5
1
0
2
7
8
29
0
7
0
0
9
0
0
7
1
0
13
10
8
2
1
1
2
2
0
1
1
0
0
0
0
1
1
5
1
3
5
2
7
5
3
11
6
2
5
32
36
47
2
18
12
6
15
4
5
10
2
2
36
34
26
3
5
3
3
3
2
2
2
2
2
3
2
3
2
6
2
Women’s empowerment programs
Average
Total income
0
0.6
0
4.2
2
2.9
2
7.7
56
Table 13: Regression of Effect Size on Hierarchical Bayesian Meta-Analysis Results: RCTs Only
Meta-analysis result
Observations
R2
(1)
Effect size
b/se
(2)
Effect size
b/se
(3)
Effect size
b/se
0.570***
(0.14)
0.290
(0.33)
0.654***
(0.15)
477
0.05
45
0.04
415
0.06
The meta-analysis result in the table above was created by synthesizing all but one observation within an
intervention-outcome combination; that one observation left out is on the left hand side in the regression. All
interventions and outcomes are included in the regression, clustering by intervention-outcome. Column (1) shows
the results on the full set of RCTs; column (2) shows the results for those effect sizes pertaining to programs
implemented by the government; column (3) shows the results for academic/NGO-implemented programs.
57
Table 14: Regression of Effect Size on Hierarchical Bayesian Meta-Analysis Results: Minimum
Quality Score
Meta-analysis result
Observations
R2
(1)
Effect size
b/se
(2)
Effect size
b/se
(3)
Effect size
b/se
0.534***
(0.13)
0.442
(0.38)
0.555***
(0.16)
417
0.05
22
0.10
381
0.04
The meta-analysis result in the table above was created by synthesizing all but one observation within an
intervention-outcome combination; that one observation left out is on the left hand side in the regression. All
interventions and outcomes are included in the regression, clustering by intervention-outcome. Column (1) shows
the results on the full set of “high-quality” papers according to the Jadad scale; column (2) shows the results for
those effect sizes pertaining to programs implemented by the government; column (3) shows the results for
academic/NGO-implemented programs.
58
Table 15: Regression of Effect Size on Hierarchical Bayesian Meta-Analysis Results: Minimum Four
Papers per Intervention-Outcome
Meta-analysis result
Observations
R2
(1)
Effect size
b/se
(2)
Effect size
b/se
(3)
Effect size
b/se
0.682***
(0.12)
0.544
(0.45)
0.703***
(0.14)
539
0.07
92
0.04
427
0.07
The meta-analysis result in the table above was created by synthesizing all but one observation within an
intervention-outcome combination; that one observation left out is on the left hand side in the regression. All
interventions and outcomes are included in the regression, clustering by intervention-outcome. Column (1) shows
the results for all the intervention-outcome combinations covered by at least four papers; column (2) shows the
results for those effect sizes pertaining to programs implemented by the government; column (3) shows the results
for academic/NGO-implemented programs.
59
Table 16: Regression of Effect Size on Hierarchical Bayesian Meta-Analysis Results: Including All
Observations
Meta-analysis result
Observations
R2
(1)
Effect size
b/se
(2)
Effect size
b/se
(3)
Effect size
b/se
0.580***
(0.09)
0.466***
(0.08)
0.708***
(0.14)
567
0.06
104
0.05
439
0.08
The meta-analysis result in the table above was created by synthesizing all but one observation within an
intervention-outcome combination; that one observation left out is on the left hand side in the regression. All
interventions and outcomes are included in the regression, clustering by intervention-outcome. Column (1) shows
the results on the full data set; column (2) shows the results for those effect sizes pertaining to programs
implemented by the government; column (3) shows the results for academic/NGO-implemented programs.
60
Table 17: Regression of Effect Size on Fixed Effect Meta-Analysis Results
Meta-analysis result
Observations
R2
(1)
Effect size
b/se
(2)
Effect size
b/se
(3)
Effect size
b/se
0.657***
(0.08)
0.543***
(0.05)
0.814***
(0.07)
566
0.11
103
0.18
439
0.10
The meta-analysis result in the table above was created by synthesizing all but one observation within an
intervention-outcome combination in a fixed effect meta-analysis; that one observation left out is on the left hand
side in the regression. All interventions and outcomes are included in the regression, clustering by
intervention-outcome. Column (1) shows the results on the full data set; column (2) shows the results for those
effect sizes pertaining to programs implemented by the government; column (3) shows the results for
academic/NGO-implemented programs.
61
Table 18: Regression of Effect Size on Hierarchical Bayesian Meta-Analysis Results: Published vs.
Unpublished Papers
Meta-analysis result
(1)
Published
b/se
(2)
Unpublished
b/se
0.600***
(0.14)
0.461
(0.30)
480
0.06
73
0.16
Observations
R2
The meta-analysis result in the table above was created by synthesizing all but one observation within an
intervention-outcome combination; that one observation left out is on the left hand side in the regression. All
interventions and outcomes are included in the regression, clustering by intervention-outcome. Column (1) shows
the results using the set of papers that were published; column (2) shows the results using only unpublished or
working papers.
62
Table 19: Caliper Tests for Political Science, Reproduced from Gerber and Malhotra (2008a) for
Comparison
A. APSR
Vol. 89-101
10% Caliper
15% Caliper
20% Caliper
Vol. 96-101
10% Caliper
15% Caliper
20% Caliper
Vol. 89-95
10% Caliper
15% Caliper
20% Caliper
B. AJPS
Vol. 39-51
10% Caliper
15% Caliper
20% Caliper
Vol. 46-51
10% Caliper
15% Caliper
20% Caliper
Vol. 39-45
10% Caliper
15% Caliper
20% Caliper
Over Caliper
Under Caliper
p-value
49
67
83
15
23
33
ă0.001
ă0.001
ă0.001
36
46
55
11
17
21
ă0.001
ă0.001
ă0.001
13
28
21
4
12
6
0.02
0.008
0.003
90
128
165
38
66
95
ă0.001
ă0.001
ă0.001
56
80
105
25
45
66
ă0.001
0.001
0.002
34
48
60
13
21
29
0.002
ă0.001
ă0.001
63
Table 20: Regression of Studies’ Absolute Percent Difference from the Within-InterventionOutcome Mean on Chronological Order
Chronological order
Observations
R2
(1)
ă500%
b/se
(2)
ă1000%
b/se
(3)
ă1500%
b/se
(4)
ă2000%
b/se
0.053
(0.21)
0.699
(0.47)
0.926
(0.61)
0.971
(0.67)
480
0.13
520
0.13
527
0.12
532
0.12
Each column restricts attention to a set of results a given maximum percentage away from the mean result in an
intervention-outcome combination.
64
D
Simulations
I show that the distribution of standardized effect sizes is recoverable from a range of biases,
including selection bias (dropping insignificant results), data-peeking (adding data until getting
the results one wants), cherry-picking dependent variables (choosing dependent variables so as to
get significance), and selectively excluding outliers. I use the same specifications (fixed effects vs.
random effects, etc.) as Simonsohn et al. (2014). They take a vector of estimated effect sizes and
return a scalar unbiased estimate of the effect size; I focus on the distribution of effect sizes.
Each measures the PRESS statistic, defined as:
PRESS “
n
ÿ
pYi ´ Ypi q2
(17)
i“1
where Ypi is the predicted value for the effect size Yi based on all observations except i. Here, Ypi
represents the leave-one-out mean effect size.
Code is available at http://www.evavivalt.com/research.
65
Figure 12: Three selection bias simulations.
Specifications, left to right:
1. Selectively reporting significant studies: N=40, 20 observations per cell, fixed effects.
2. Selectively reporting significant studies: Predetermined sample size between N=10 and N=70,
5-35 observations per cell, fixed effects.
3. Selectively reporting significant studies: Predetermined sample size between N=10 and N=70,
5-35 observations per cell, random effects: Yi „ N pθ̄, 0.2q.
In each case, only significant values are kept. Figures contain 500 simulated studies.
66
Figure 13: Three p-hacking simulations.
Specifications, left to right:
1. Data-peeking: If pą0.05 with N=20, add 10 observations.
2. Cherry-picking: If pą0.05 with one dependent variable, try another two.
3. Selectively excluding outliers: If pą0.05, drop observations more than 2 SD from the mean,
first from one group, then the other, or both if necessary.
In each case, only significant values are kept. Present figures contain 500 simulated studies.
67
Download